* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download View PDF - CiteSeerX
Functional magnetic resonance imaging wikipedia , lookup
Neuroplasticity wikipedia , lookup
Endocannabinoid system wikipedia , lookup
Holonomic brain theory wikipedia , lookup
Neuroeconomics wikipedia , lookup
Bird vocalization wikipedia , lookup
Axon guidance wikipedia , lookup
State-dependent memory wikipedia , lookup
Haemodynamic response wikipedia , lookup
Convolutional neural network wikipedia , lookup
Neuroethology wikipedia , lookup
Psychophysics wikipedia , lookup
Electrophysiology wikipedia , lookup
Artificial general intelligence wikipedia , lookup
Response priming wikipedia , lookup
Eyeblink conditioning wikipedia , lookup
Neurotransmitter wikipedia , lookup
Types of artificial neural networks wikipedia , lookup
Perception of infrasound wikipedia , lookup
Multielectrode array wikipedia , lookup
Activity-dependent plasticity wikipedia , lookup
Nonsynaptic plasticity wikipedia , lookup
Mirror neuron wikipedia , lookup
Caridoid escape reaction wikipedia , lookup
Clinical neurochemistry wikipedia , lookup
Biological neuron model wikipedia , lookup
Molecular neuroscience wikipedia , lookup
Olfactory bulb wikipedia , lookup
Single-unit recording wikipedia , lookup
Evoked potential wikipedia , lookup
Development of the nervous system wikipedia , lookup
Neural correlates of consciousness wikipedia , lookup
Neural oscillation wikipedia , lookup
Metastability in the brain wikipedia , lookup
Circumventricular organs wikipedia , lookup
Premovement neuronal activity wikipedia , lookup
Central pattern generator wikipedia , lookup
Neuroanatomy wikipedia , lookup
Neural coding wikipedia , lookup
Pre-Bötzinger complex wikipedia , lookup
Neuropsychopharmacology wikipedia , lookup
Optogenetics wikipedia , lookup
Nervous system network models wikipedia , lookup
Synaptic gating wikipedia , lookup
Stimulus (physiology) wikipedia , lookup
J Comp Physiol A (1999) 185: 565±576 Ó Springer-Verlag 1999 ORIGINAL PAPER B. GruÈnewald Physiological properties and response modulations of mushroom body feedback neurons during olfactory learning in the honeybee, Apis mellifera Accepted: 9 September 1999 Abstract Mushroom bodies are central brain structures and essentially involved in insect olfactory learning. Within the mushroom bodies c-aminobutyric acid (GABA)-immunoreactive feedback neurons are the most prominent neuron group. The plasticity of inhibitory neural activity within the mushroom body was investigated by analyzing modulations of odor responses of feedback neurons during olfactory learning in vivo. In the honeybee, Apis mellifera, feedback neurons were intracellularly recorded at their neurites. They produced complex patterns of action potentials without experimental stimulation. Summating postsynaptic potentials indicate that their synaptic input region lies within the lobes. Odor and antennal sucrose stimuli evoked excitatory phasic-tonic responses. Individual neurons responded to various odors; responses of dierent neurons to the same odor were highly variable. Response modulations were determined by comparing odor responses of feedback neurons before and after one-trial olfactory conditioning or sensitisation. Shortly after pairing an odor stimulus with a sucrose reward, odor-induced spike activity of feedback neurons decreased. Repeated odor stimulations alone, equally spaced as in the conditioning experiment, did not aect the odor-induced excitation. A single sensitisation trial also did not alter odor responses. These ®ndings indicate that the level of odorinduced inhibition within the mushroom bodies is speci®cally modulated by experience. Key words Olfactory learning á Sensitisation á Inhibitory feedback neurons á Insect á Intracellular recording B. GruÈnewald (&) Institut fuÈr Neurobiologie, Freie UniversitaÈt Berlin, KoÈnigin-Luise-Str. 28±30 D-14195 Berlin, Germany e-mail: [email protected] Tel.: +49-30-838-4298; Fax: +49-30-838-5455 Abbreviations CS conditioned stimulus á GABA c-aminobutyric acid á MB mushroom body á PER proboscis extension re¯ex á US unconditioned stimulus Introduction Studies on the neural correlates of learning behavior in the honeybee, Apis mellifera, focused on the analysis of the proboscis extension re¯ex, PER. This appetitive re¯ex can be conditioned by a single forward pairing of an odor stimulus (conditioned stimulus, CS) with a sucrose reward (unconditioned stimulus, US; Kuwabara 1957; Vareschi 1971; Menzel et al. 1974; Bitterman et al. 1983). A single sucrose stimulation sensitises the response (Menzel et al. 1991; Hammer et al. 1994). Within the insect brain, the mushroom bodies (MB) are essentially involved in olfactory learning and memory formation (Apis: Menzel et al. 1974; Erber et al. 1980, 1987; reviews: Hammer and Menzel 1995; Menzel and MuÈller 1996; Drosophila: Heisenberg et al. 1985; De Belle and Heisenberg 1994; reviews: Davis 1993; Dubnau and Tully 1998). The anatomical organization of the MB, its connectivity within the insect brain and behavioural development have been described in detail (reviewed by Mobbs 1984, 1985; SchuÈrmann 1987; Menzel et al. 1994; Fahrbach and Robinson 1995), and the physiology of several of its neuronal elements have been analyzed (Erber 1978; Homberg and Erber 1979; Schildberger 1981, 1983, 1984; Homberg 1984; Gronenberg 1986, 1987; Mauelshagen 1993; Laurent and Naraghi 1994; SchaÈfer et al. 1994; Menzel et al. 1994; Li and Strausfeld 1997; Stopfer et al. 1997; Goldberg et al. 1999). The present study analyses the physiology of c-aminobutyric acid (GABA)-immunoreactive feedback neurons, which are major components of the insect MB (ants: Goll 1967; crickets: SchuÈrmann 1973; ¯ies: Strausfeld 1976; locusts: Weiss 1978; Schildberger 1983; Leitch and Laurent 1996; moths: Homberg et al. 1987; 566 honeybees: Mobbs 1982; Bicker et al. 1985). In the honeybee MB approximately 50 feedback neurons connect the dorsal and median a-lobe, the b-lobe, and the pedunculus with all ipsilateral calycal subcompartments, lip, collar, and basal ring (GruÈnewald 1999). Initial physiological studies have shown that feedback neurons respond to olfactory, visual, gustatory and mechanical stimuli; often a single neuron responds to a variety of stimuli of dierent sensory modalities (Homberg and Erber 1979; Schildberger 1981; Gronenberg 1987). However, it was unknown whether feedback neurons change their response behavior during olfactory learning. In the honeybee MB, the individual, identi®ed MB output neuron, PE1, undergoes modulations of its odor responses, which are speci®c for associative and nonassociative stimulus paradigms (Mauelshagen 1993). A single conditioning trial results in a decrease, a single antennal sensitisation trial in a transient increase of odor evoked spike frequency, indicative for a dierential neural representation of associative and non-associative events at the MB level. The precise mechanisms that underlie modulations of odor-evoked activity in MB neurons are still unknown. Modulatory input from the VUMmx1 neuron, which mediates the reinforcing function of rewards during olfactory conditioning (Hammer 1993, 1997) is probably involved. Since Mauelshagen (1993) suggested that response modulations of the PE1-neuron may be due to altered input from presynaptic Kenyon cells, one may hypothesise that altering the amount of inhibition within the MB regulates Kenyon cell excitation and thus aects odor responses of MB output neurons like the PE1-neuron. Therefore, the present study analyses responses of feedback neurons to olfactory and gustatory stimuli that are relevant for olfactory learning. It further investigates whether a single sensitisation or conditioning trial modulates the odor responses of feedback neurons. The results indicate that the level of odor-induced inhibition within the MB is altered in an experience-dependent fashion. Materials and methods Animals and preparation Worker honeybees (Apis mellifera carnica) were caught between 9:00 and 10:00 a.m. at the hive entrance. After immobilisation by cooling, they were ®xed in small metal tubes with a strip of sticky tape between head and thorax. After recovery animals were fed with one drop of sucrose solution (25%). Only those bees which readily showed proboscis extension upon feeding were selected for experiments, which began at least 1 h after feeding. To achieve stable intracellular recordings the head preparation developed by Mauelshagen (1993) was used. For this the isolated head of the bee was mounted on a perfusion chamber which was supplied continuously with aerated saline containing (in mmol á l)1): 135 NaCl, 5 KCl, 10 MgCl2, 1.6 CaCl2, and 80 tris(hydroxymethyl)aminomethane (TRIS), pH 7.25. After stabilising the brain by carefully dissecting the oesophagus with its connecting muscles, the anterior surface of the brain was exposed around the recording site. As a consequence of the preparation the proboscis extension re¯ex was irreversibly impaired. Recording and labeling of neurons Recording electrodes (borosilicate glass, 1.0 mm outer diameter, 0.58 mm inner diameter; Hilgenberg, Germany) were pulled with a horizontal micropipette puller (P87, Sutter Instruments, Novato, Calif.). Electrodes were back®lled with Neurobiotin (2% in 1 mol á l)1 KCl, Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, Calif.) or Lucifer Yellow (5% in 0.1 mol á l)1 LiCl, Molecular Probes, Eugene, Ore.), and had resistances in the tissue between 40±90 MW and between 100 and 180 MW, respectively. Electrodes were positioned in the medio-lateral a-lobe, which was easy to recognise visually. They were lowered vertically into the brain and their depth was monitored by using a potentiometer connected to the ®ne adjustment of the micromanipulator (Leitz, Wetzlar, Germany). After each experiment neurons were iontophoretically ®lled by either injecting Neurobiotin (2±4 nA depolarising current) or Lucifer yellow (2±4 nA hyperpolarising current) for as long as the cell could be held stabile (2±15 min). After dissection in saline, brains were ®xed in paraformaldehyde (4% in phosphate-buered saline (PBS), for 12 h at 4 °C). Biotin-®lled brains were dehydrated in graded ethanol, lipophilic substances solubilized in xylene, and rehydrated in PBS. The ganglion sheath was permeabilized with collagenase/hyaluronidase (1 mg ml)1, 1 mmol á l)1 CaCl2 added, pH 7.4, 60 min, 37 °C; Sigma Chemicals, St. Louis, Mo.), endogenous peroxidases were blocked by H2O2 (0.03% in PBS). After several buer rinses, specimens were incubated in biotinylised Avidin-HRP-complex (Vektor Laboratories, 5 h, 37 °C, 1% Triton added). Following preincubation with 0.02% di-aminobenzidine (Sigma, 45 min, 20 °C), di-azonium salt was precipitated by adding 0.001% H2O2. After dehydration in graded ethanol, specimens were cleared with methyl salicylate. Lucifer Yellow-stained specimens were dehydrated in graded ethanol, cleared up in methyl salicylate, and examined using a ¯uorescence microscope (Wild-Polyvar, Leica, Germany) under epi-illumination. During this study, intracellular labeling of neurons was performed exclusively for morphological identi®cation of a recorded cell. Due to the extended periods of up to 20 min needed for all test protocols, time for subsequent dye injection was limited and therefore most neurons were not completely ®lled. Only those recordings were analysed in which a single neuron was stained and anatomically identi®ed as a feedback neuron. Thus, 52 recorded feedback neurons were included in this study. Stimuli Gustatory stimuli were manually applied and consisted of brie¯y (<1 s) touching the distal tip of the antennae and/or the proboscis with a wooden tooth stick soaked in sucrose solution (25%). Olfactory stimuli (duration 2 s) were carnation and orange blossom, citral and geraniol, which are components of ¯oral odors and are frequently used as conditioned stimuli (CS) in olfactory conditioning (e.g. Menzel et al. 1991). Pus of odors and plain air were delivered to the antennae by a custom-made olfactometer. Glass cylinders (1 ml volume), each ®lled with 0.4 ml of pure odor substances, were placed into tightly closed brass containers (height 3 cm, diameter 2.5 cm), positioned ca. 50 cm from the head preparation. A continuous air¯ow (¯ow rate approximately 3 l min)1) transported the odor streams via Te¯on tubes to the exhaust. Switching a 3-way-magnetic valve directed the selected odor stream through a nozzle (2 cm in front of the preparation) to the antennae. The scented air was quickly exhausted through a large tube (diameter 5 cm, positioned 10 cm behind the recording chamber). Stimulus onset and oset (indicated by switching the magnetic valves) were stored separately on tape. Stimulus latencies between switching the valves and arrival at the antenna were estimated with a thermistor and ranged between 80 ms and 120 ms. Experimental design To determine response modulations three experimental groups of animals were formed. The conditioning group (n = 11) received a single forward pairing of the CS (carnation) with a sucrose reward 567 (stimulation of both antennae and proboscis). Animals of the sensitisation group (n = 11) were stimulated with a sucrose solution applied to both antennae without olfactory stimulation. Animals of a control group (n = 8) were stimulated with an odor alone, without sucrose application. In all groups the odor responses were tested 2 min prior to the CS/US pairing or applying the sensitising stimulus (reference response). After conditioning or sensitisation responses during the presentation of the same odor were tested at 30 s, 1 min, and 2 min (test odor responses). Responses during the tests were compared with their corresponding reference responses. Data analysis Intracellular signals were preampli®ed (d.c.-coupled, 10´; Simmonds Ampli®er, Cambridge, UK), visualized on a storage oscilloscope, and stored on a digital tape recorder (Biologic DTR1800). The signal and stimulus traces were digitized (signal sample rate 4000 Hz, stimulus sample rate 500 Hz) and stored on a computer using a CED1401plus interface and Spike2 software (version 2.24, Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK). To determine the spontaneous frequency the total number of action potentials was counted during a 2 s interval immediately before the ®rst odor response. Spike parameters (amplitude, duration, time-to-peak) were analyzed by measuring 4 individual spikes per neuron and measures of several neurons were averaged. For analyzes of odor responses (latency, peristimulus spike frequency, maximum instantaneous frequency, burst depolarization, etc.), two responses per neuron were analyzed and data from several neurons averaged. The number of neurons analyzed is indicated in Table 1. Response frequencies were calculated as the number of spikes during the whole stimulus duration (2 s) and during peristimulus time intervals of various durations (100±500 ms) after stimulus onset. The spike frequency during a 2 s period prior to each stimulus onset was subtracted from its corresponding response frequency to correct for background spontaneous activity. Relative response frequencies during the tests were expressed as the relative deviation from the corresponding reference response frequencies, Table 1 Physiological properties of feedback neurons Parameter General Membrane potential Spontaneous frequency Action potential Amplitude Duration Time-to-peak Sucrose response Duration Membrane depolarization Spike frequency Maximum frequency Odor responses Latency Membrane depolarization Phasic response Tonic response Spike frequency Phasic response Tonic response Maximum frequency Phasic response Tonic response O-responses Latency Duration Spike frequency ND ± Not determined which was set to 1. Thus, a value of 1 indicates no change; 0 means 100% spike frequency reduction, and 2 indicates a 100% increase. Statistics All data are expressed as means with standard errors of means. A 2-way ANOVA for repeated measures was employed for comparison of odor and air pu responses, and to evaluate dierences in relative frequencies and response latencies between the experimental groups. For subsequent post-hoc analyses, a NewmanKeuls test was chosen. Data were analysed using STATISTICA for Windows (version 5.1; StatSoft, Tulsa, Okla.). Results Morphological identi®cation of feedback neurons Intracellularly stained feedback neurons were identi®ed according to several morphological features, that were previously described in detail (GruÈnewald 1999). Their somata are located ventrally at the anteriolateral protocerebrum, close to the median border of the lobula (Figs. 1, 2A). The primary neurite projects dorsomedially and bifurcates at the dorsolateral margin of the a-lobe. One branch projects dorsally and posteriorly within the protocerebral-calycal tract towards the calyces. The other branch loops ventrally, penetrates the a-lobe at the a-exit point (Mobbs 1982), and arborizes within the dorsal and median a-lobe layers. It sends o branches posteriorly, which bifurcate at the posterior border of the a-lobe, and arborize in the pedunculus and the b-lobe. The protocerebral collateral Range Mean SEM n )35 to )65 mV 0±24 Hz )41.3 2.2 mV 5.2 1.3 Hz 23 23 20±55 mV 2.55±4.67 ms 0.87±1.85 ms 33.3 1.7 mV 3.38 0.12 ms 1.16 0.06 ms 23 23 23 105±1890 ms 2.5±10 mV 8±75 Hz 24±160 Hz 673 102 ms 5.5 0.58 mV 29.0 3.9 Hz 86.7 10.7 Hz 16 16 16 16 75±128 ms 96.6 3.2 ms 19 2.4±11.7 mV 1.2±5.8 mV 3.3±48.0 Hz 1.5±35 Hz 16.1±219.3 Hz ND 95±175 ms 160±750 ms 8±31 Hz 6.8 0.69 mV 3.2 0.35 mV 19 19 21.5 2.8 Hz 12.3 2.3 Hz 19 19 101.2 13.5 Hz ND 19 166 14.1 ms 434 97 ms 16.6 3.6 Hz 6 6 6 568 Fig. 1A±F Morphology of feedback neurons. A±C Lucifer yellow-stained group of neurons, which connect the pedunculus with the lip of the calyces. Neurons send several branches (arrowheads in B) toward the pedunculus. Three focal planes (85, 100, 135 lm below the frontal brain surface; wholemount). Arrow in A, primary neurites; arrowhead points to aexit. Outlines of the a-lobe indicated by broken lines in B, D. Arrows in C point to inner-ring tract, arrowhead to protocerebro-calycal tract. D, E Staining of another, single neuron with arborizations in the dorsal a-lobe (D) and in the basal ring of the calyces (E; broken lines indicate calycal outlines; frontal view). F Fine branches of yet another singlestained feedback neuron in the collar region of the lateral calyx (25 lm frontal section). No stained pro®les in the lip and basal ring. This neuron did not respond to olfactory stimuli. LC lateral calyx; MC median calyx; Li lip; Co collar; Br basal ring; d dorsal; l lateral. Scale bars in A±E, 100 lm, in F, 50 lm bifurcates ventrally of the calyces and innervates the median and the lateral calyx. All calycal subcompartments, the lip, collar, and basal ring, are innervated by feedback neurons. For this study only neurons were selected that responded to olfactory stimuli. All these neurons arborized either in the basal ring or the lip region. General physiological characteristics Spontaneous activity Recordings from neurites were performed at the a-exit point. Impalement of neurons was indicated by a sudden drop of the potential to )35 to )65 mV (see Table 1 for 569 Fig. 2A±C Wiring scheme and spike activity of feedback neurons. A Schematic diagram of feedback neurons within the honeybee mushroom body. Only the right brain hemisphere is drawn; arrows represent the putative ¯ow of information within feedback neurons. The calyces and the medio-dorsal a-lobe are shaded dark gray; star, somata cluster. The recording site is indicated. a, a-lobe; b, b-lobe; MC, median calyx; LC, lateral calyx; Pe, pedunculus; AL, antennal lobe; LO, lobula; ME, medulla; CB, central body; OC, lateral ocellus. Dorsal (d) and lateral (l) directions are indicated, scale bar = 100 lm. B Spontaneous discharge pattern is a series of action potentials. In most neurons relatively low frequency periods are interrupted by high frequency, input activity (top three traces). Some neurons generate continuous series of regularly spaced action potentials (lower recording). Each trace represents a 10 s recording period from a single neuron. C Typical shapes of action potentials generated by four dierent cells; asterisks in B, C summating excitatory postsynaptic potentials (epsps); ®lled circles in B, ipsps a list of physiological parameters). Most neurons (95%) generated action potentials without experimental stimulation (Fig. 2B). The spontaneous background discharge frequency ranged between 0 and 24 Hz (Table 1) and varied considerably among feedback neurons (Fig. 2B). The spontaneous discharge activity of most neurons was typically a complex temporal pattern, which was intermitted by short periods of higher frequency episodic phasic activity rather than a continuous series of equally spaced action potentials. This may indicate sensory input activity without experimental stimulation (probably via antennal movements or airborne odors in the experimental environment). In most neurons the action potentials (duration <5 ms) lacked pronounced after-hyperpolarizations (Fig. 2C). Sum- mating postsynaptic potentials (amplitude 2±10 mV) occurred frequently at the recording site (asterisks in Fig. 2B, C). Responses to sucrose solution Brie¯y touching the tip of the ipsilateral antenna with sucrose solution evoked excitatory responses in 92% of all recorded feedback neurons. Excitatory responses were never observed after touching or closely approaching the antennal tip with a dry tooth pick. The sucrose reaction tested by stimulating the ipsilateral antenna lasted between 105 ms and 1890 ms (Fig. 3, Table 1). Sucrose responses typically consisted of a rapid membrane depolarisation that gradually declined to baseline potential. During responses, spike frequency was increased to 29.0 3.9 Hz above resting values. Stimulation of the contralateral antenna evoked responses in 78% of all neurons that responded to ipsilateral stimulation. Contralateral responses were weaker and shorter than during ipsilateral stimulation (Fig. 3A), sucrose solution applied to the glossa rarely elicited any response. Repeated stimulation of the ipsilateral antenna at short intervals (2±4 s, Fig. 3B) but not at long intervals (30 s to 2 min, Fig. 3C) led to a gradual decline of response strength as indicated by successively lower response frequency (31.4±22 Hz) and shorter duration (688±362 ms) after 5 stimulations. 570 Fig. 3A±C Responses of three dierent feedback neurons to sucrose stimulation. A Responses are excitatory and are stronger when sucrose solution is applied to the ipsilateral (right) antenna than to the contralateral (left) antenna. B Repeated ipsilateral antennal stimulation results in response decrement when the intervals between stimulations are short (<5 s). Higher temporal resolution of the ®rst and ®fth response are given above. C Spaced presentation (2 min interval) of antennal sucrose solution did not result in response decrement. The spike frequencies during the responses and the response durations are indicated for each trace Odor responses Application of the test odors evoked either phasic or phasic-tonic excitatory responses in feedback neurons (Figs. 4±6). The phasic response component (duration <500 ms) consisted of a rapid and steep rise of the membrane potential with response latencies between 75 ms and 128 ms (Fig. 4, Table 1). The spike frequency during the phasic response was increased to 3.3±48.0 Hz above background activity, the instantaneous spike frequency may reach maximum values of up to 219 Hz. The burst typically consisted of 1±40 spikes with reduced amplitudes (measured from the burst potential). The end of the phasic response component is indicated by an abrupt drop of the membrane potential and the spike frequency to values still above resting potential and spontaneous frequency (Fig. 4A, B). During the tonic response component (Fig. 4A, C, D), which occurred in 74% of the recorded neurons, the membrane remained slightly depolarized and the spike frequency increased; summating postsynaptic potentials were frequently observed (Fig. 4A). Towards the end of the odor stimulus both parameters declined to resting values; the odor responses usually did not exceed stimulus duration. However, some neurons (22%) generated excitatory oresponses after stimulus oset (Fig. 4C, D, Table 1). These o-responses, which were not speci®c for a particular odor, were short with a latency of Fig. 4A±D Odor responses of feedback neurons. A Burst responses consist of almost instantaneous membrane depolarisation and high spike frequency (often exceeding 100 Hz). Marked area of A enlarged below. The membrane remains depolarized after the burst and during the tonic response component. Summating epsps (asterisks); and ipsps (®lled circle) occur during odor responses. B Pure phasic response. C, D O-responses (C shows same neuron as in A). Stimulus durations (orange in A; carnation in B, D; geraniol in C) are indicated as a bar below each signal trace; response frequencies and durations of the response component are given for the traces in C, D 166 14.1 ms (measured from stimulus oset), which was substantially longer than during the on-responses of the same neurons during the same responses (99.8 2.3 ms, n = 6). Stimulations with pus of plain air (duration 2 s) evoked purely phasic, excitatory responses (Figs. 5B, 6) with spike frequencies between 0.5 Hz and 14.5 Hz above spontaneous activity. Carnation stimuli induced stronger excitatory responses in the very same neurons. Comparing spike activity during odor and during air pu responses (Fig. 5) revealed signi®cant dierences between the tests (df = 1; F = 6.83; P < 0.02; 2-way MANOVA): Post-hoc tests showed weaker responses during air pus then during odor stimuli (P < 0.02, Newman-Keuls post-hoc test), but no signi®cant changes in background activity (P = 0.65, n.s.). Obviously, a portion of the phasic odor response component was 571 Fig. 6A±C Variability of odor responses among dierent feedback neurons. Each column (A±C) represents responses of one individual neuron to the various odors and to a plain air pu (top to bottom rows, stimulus indicated at the right) third neuron, that are almost indistinguishable from airpu stimulation (column C). Modulations of odor responses Fig. 5A, B Comparison of responses to odor (A) and air-pu stimulation (B) of neurons that responded to both stimuli. Peristimulus time histograms starting 1 s prior to stimulus onset (error bars, SEM; bin width 100 ms). The odor (carnation) induced a spike frequency of 13.4 2.8 Hz above spontaneous activity (n = 16, measured for the whole stimulus duration). The very same neurons responded to stimulation with plain air pus with an increased frequency of 3.3 1.14 Hz. C Subtraction of A-B yielded odor component of the whole response. Stimulus duration is indicated by the horizontal bar caused by activation of mechanosensory aerences at stimulus onset. Various test odors evoked excitatory responses in a given feedback neuron (Fig. 6, columns). The responses of a given neuron to the dierent test odors dier slightly, but dierences in odor responses were more pronounced between dierent neurons (Fig. 6, rows). One given odor (e.g., geraniol, row 4 of Fig. 6) may elicit weak phasic responses, hardly above the spontaneous activity in one neuron (e.g., column A of Fig. 6); in another neuron the same odor may evoke high-frequency, phasic-tonic responses with an o-component (column B), and almost purely phasic responses in a During these experiments spike activity of feedback neurons was recorded during olfactory learning and sensitisation. The results indicate that one-trial conditioning induces changes in odor responses of MB feedback neurons. Examples of original recording traces show similar temporal patterns of spike activity during odor responses of feedback neurons before and after conditioning or sensitisation (Fig. 7A, B). After a single pairing of an odor with a sucrose reward most feedback neurons generated weaker CS responses during the 30 s test and the 1 min test as compared to the reference test (91% and 82% of neurons, respectively; n = 11; Table 2). The mean relative frequencies indicated a response decrement during the tests. A single antennal sensitising stimulus led to stronger odor responses in 55% of feedback neurons during the 30 s test (n = 11) and in 78% during the 1 min test (n = 9). The mean relative frequencies indicated a response enhancement after sensitisation. Repeated presentations of a given odor (control group) did not systematically change response frequencies of feedback neurons, since almost the same number of neurons responded stronger or weaker during the tests as compared to the reference test. Statistical analyses revealed dierences in relative response frequencies (calculated for the whole stimulus duration) between the experimental groups (Fig. 8A, Table 2). Thus, the relative frequency of the CS response 30 s test after conditioning was decreased as compared to the odor control group (P < 0.002; n = 8; Newman- 572 Fig. 7A±C Recording traces of feedback neurons during conditioning (A), and sensitisation (B), and stimulus con®guration (C). Responses of feedback neurons to the odor before ()2 min car, carnation) either the pairing with compound sucrose stimulation (A), or a single sensitisation trial (B), and during subsequent tests (0.5 min, 1 min). Response decrement during the tests in A was not due to an unspeci®c weakening of the sensory input, since subsequent response to orange stimulation (or, 3 min after conditioning) illustrates that the neurons is still capable of generating stronger excitation. Also given for comparison is the response during presentation of geraniol in B (ger, 5.5 min after sensitisation). These other odors were not tested prior to sensitisation or conditioning and responses may, therefore, not be compared quantitatively. C Time course and sequence of olfactory (open rectangles) and gustatory stimuli ( ®lled rectangles) used in the three experimental groups. Each experiment consisted of the three phases, reference test (ref.), training (train., conditioning, sensitisation, odor), and tests. Occasionally, after the last test additional odors were presented at various intervals. The time scale is given below Keuls post-hoc test), but not during the 1 min test (see Table 2 for statistical details). Relative frequencies during odor responses after sensitisation dier from those after conditioning, but not from those of the control group. Similar results were obtained by comparing relative frequencies during the phasic response component, determined during the 1st 500 ms interval after stimulus onset (Table 2, Fig. 8B). Relative frequencies during the 30 s test are reduced after conditioning as compared to the odor control group (P < 0.05; n = 8; Newman-Keuls post-hoc test). Sensitisation did not induce modulations of the phasic odor response as compared to repeated odor presentations. Thus, conditioning produces a transient odor response decrement, whereas antennal sensitisation does not induce any signi®cant response modulations. Response latencies did not change signi®cantly in any experimental group (Table 2). No dierences were observed between groups (df = 2; F = 1.24; P = 0.31; n.s.; two-way ANOVA for repeated measures), nor within groups between references and tests (df = 2; F = 0.06; P = 0.94; n.s.). Discussion The present study showed that mushroom body feedback neurons in the honeybee receive excitatory olfactory input. Individual feedback neurons respond to various odors. The odor-induced inhibitory activity in the mushroom body can be modulated by a single classical conditioning trial, but not by a single sensitisation trial. Physiological properties of feedback neurons Previous studies presented morphological evidences that the information ¯ow within feedback neurons is directed from the lobes toward the calyces, i.e. from the MB main output toward its main input regions (Mobbs 1982; Rybak and Menzel 1993; GruÈnewald 1999). This view can now be con®rmed by the physiological ®ndings presented here. First, the occurrence of summating postsynaptic potentials at the recording site indicated that feedback neurons possess synaptic input regions close to the a-exit, probably within the lobes and pedunculus. This is generally consistent with the appearance of spiny processes at the feedback neuron terminals (honeybees: Mobbs 1982; Gronenberg 1987; Rybak and Menzel 1993) and ultrastructural evidence for output synapses from Kenyon cells onto feedback neurons in these areas (locust: Leitch and Laurent 1996). Second, the amplitudes of action potentials imply that spikes are passively propagated to the recording site. The spike generating zone should be close to the a-exit, however, 573 Fig. 8A±C Quanti®cation of learning experiments. A Relative frequencies were calculated for a 2 s peristimulus interval during the 30 s (left) and the 1 min (right) test. B Relative frequencies during the 1st 500 ms interval after stimulus onset, representing the phasic response component. Response frequencies during the reference test were set to 1; thus, downward bars indicate response decrements, upward bars indicate response enhancements. Stars indicate statistically signi®cant (P > 0.05, Newman-Keuls post-hoc test) dierences between experimental groups since afterhyperpolarisations were recorded in some neurons. Thus, feedback neurons probably form dendrites within the output regions of the MB, where they receive input from Kenyon cells and recurrently transmit this information to the MB input sites. There, in the calyces, they probably synapse onto dendrites of Kenyon cells, which express GABA receptor-mediated Cl) currents in vitro (honeybees: Rosenboom et al. 1994; crickets: Cayre et al. 1999). In locusts Kenyon cells generate ipsps, which are evoked by spike activity in feedback neurons (Laurent and Naraghi 1994) and Kenyon cells of ¯ies express GABA receptors (Drosophila: Harrison et al. 1996; Calliphora: Brotz et al. 1997). Odour stimuli evoked excitatory responses in most feedback neurons. A given odor evokes a wide variety of responses in dierent feedback neurons ranging from weak phasic to high frequency phasic-tonic activity. Assuming that odors activate a large number of feedback neurons, olfactory stimuli induce a complex spatiotemporal pattern of inhibitory activity that counteracts the odor-induced excitation within the MB. This massive odor-induced activity of feedback neurons is striking, because antennal information is processed mainly within the ventral a-lobe (Mobbs 1982), whereas the dendritic ®elds of feedback neurons within the a-lobe are restricted to its median and dorsal portions (GruÈnewald 1999). The question of where the feedback neurons receive olfactory information is yet unsolved (cf. discussion in GruÈnewald 1999). Individual feedback neurons do not show a pronounced odor speci®city, since they respond to various odors, which was also previously observed (Gronenberg 1987) and they respond similarly to olfactory, mechanical, and gustatory stimuli. This may be due to their extended dendritic ®elds in the lobes and pedunculus (GruÈnewald 1999), where, like many MB output neurons Table 2 Response modulations of feedback neurons Parameter Test Conditioning Sensitization Odor Statistics1 Number of ¯/ neurons2 30 s 1 min 10/1 (11)3 9/2 (11) 4/6 (11) 2/7 (9) 3/3 (8) 2/3 (8) n.t. 30 1 30 1 30 1 30 1 0.58 0.67 0.66 0.77 0.58 0.61 85.2 89.6 1.58 1.94 1.23 1.36 1.06 1.52 90.0 89.1 Relative frequencies Whole stimulus 1st 500 ms 2nd 500 ms Latencies s min s min s min s min Post-hoc analyses5 Cond. vs Odor Sens. vs Odor Cond. vs Sens. 30 1 30 1 30 1 s min s min s min 0.08 0.16 0.08 0.08 0.16 0.02 3.2 ms 4.4 ms 0.35 0.44 0.21 0.27 0.4 0.4 5.9 ms 5.4 ms 1.28 1.38 1.23 1.00 0.62 0.75 95.8 95.4 0.28 0.33 0.14 0.21 0.13 0.13 5.4 ms 4.2 ms Whole stimulus duration 1st 500 ms interval P P P P P P P P P P P P < = = = < < 0.002*, n = 8 0.051, n = 8 0.18, n = 8 0.14, n = 8 0.05*, n = 11 0.002*, n = 9 1 Two-way ANOVA for repeated measures; n.t., not tested; n.s., not signi®cant 2 Number of neurons showing weaker (¯) or stronger () odor responses during the 30 s and 1 min test as compared to the reference test. Responses were de®ned as weaker (stronger) as reference test, if they showed a more than 10% reduction (increase) < = = = < = df = 2; F = 6.64; P < 0.005* df = 2; F = 4.83; P < 0.02* n.t.4 df = 2; F = 1.24; P = 0.31; n.s. 0.05*, n = 8 0.25, n = 8 0.54, n = 8 0.27, n = 8 0.015*, n = 11 0.09, n = 9 of response strength 3 Total number of observations in brackets 4 Number of observations to low, because ca. 30% of the neurons do not show tonic responses 5 Newman-Keuls post-hoc test on relative frequencies 574 (Rybak and Menzel 1993, 1998), the feedback neuron may integrate information from dierent Kenyon cell populations. Such multimodal sensitivity of MB extrinsic neurons has been reported in a variety of systems (honeybees: Erber 1978; Homberg and Erber 1979; Gronenberg 1987; Mauelshagen 1993; Rybak and Menzel 1998; crickets: Schildberger 1981, 1984; cockroaches: Li and Strausfeld 1997). Experience-dependent response modulations Feedback neurons show experience-dependent modulations of their odor responses. These ®ndings can be directly compared with intracellular studies by Mauelshagen (1993) and with behavioral analyses (e.g. Bitterman et al. 1983; Hammer et al. 1994), because the experimental design and stimulus con®guration were designed according to those previous experiments. The head preparation allowed frequent impalements and stable intracellular recordings of central neurons in the honeybee brain, while sensory aerences and sensory processing are left intact. Thus, intracellular activity of MB neurons can be recorded during simple learning tasks, such as one-trial conditioning and sensitisation. The response frequency of feedback neurons to an odor stimulus (CS) decreased after a single olfactory conditioning trial, which was similarly observed for the PE1-neuron (Mauelshagen 1993). The initial hypothesis, that inhibitory activity of feedback neurons may mediate the learning-dependent decrement of response frequency of the MB output neuron PE1, must therefore be modi®ed. Although the feedback neurons showed learning-dependent response modulations after one-trial conditioning, these are not antagonistic to those of the PE1-neuron as was expected, but rather similar, and cannot explain in a simple model the response decrement in the PE1-neuron. How then may the response modulations of MB output neurons be explained? First, activity of inhibitory feedback neurons may not aect odor processing of MB output neurons. The experience-dependent frequency modulations of output neurons and feedback neurons may then be accomplished further upstream of both neuron groups at the level of olfactory relay neurons within the antennal lobes or the MB calyces. Second, feedback neurons might presynaptically control transmitter release of the VUMmx1 neuron in the calyces. This modulatory neuron mediates the US reinforcing stimulus in appetitive olfactory conditioning (Hammer 1993). A reduced odor-induced presynaptic inhibition via feedback neurons in the lip region of the calyces may enable increased release of modulatory transmitter from the VUMmx1 neuron onto Kenyon cells. Output neurons like the PE1 and feedback neurons receive convergent synaptic input from thousands of Kenyon cells. Therefore, activity in these MB extrinsic neurons probably re¯ects the overall level of odorevoked excitation within the MB, which is transiently reduced after a single olfactory conditioning trial. Behavioural studies on the proboscis extension re¯ex (PER) have shown that a single pairing of an odor with a sucrose reward immediately enhances the probability of odorinduced PER for hours (Bitterman et al. 1983; Menzel 1990). This increased responsiveness is not paralleled by an increased odor-evoked activity within the MB. Rather, odor-induced MB neural activity is transiently reduced after single-trial conditioning. Interestingly, Hammer and Menzel (1998) showed recently that pairing an odor with octopamine injection into the calyces produces long-term enhancement of PER. However, during acquisition, the probability of odor-evoked PER was signi®cantly lower than during consecutive memory tests, which indicates inhibitory phenomena within the MB during acquisition. This would be consistent with a transiently decreased neural activity within MB neurons, like the PE1-neuron and the feedback neurons. Functional roles of inhibitory feedback neurons Inhibitory feedback connections are essential components in most neuronal systems, where they often prevent overshooting excitation by controlling the stimulusinduced excitatory level during information processing. If this also holds true for the insect MB, the balance between excitatory and inhibitory neural activity can be shifted by experience. It may, therefore, be one role of feedback neurons during olfactory learning in insects to regulate odor-induced MB excitation. Alternatively, feedback neurons may be involved in olfactory information processing. Odor stimuli induce coherent oscillatory activity in olfactory projection neurons (locusts: Laurent and Naraghi 1994; honeybees: Stopfer et al. 1997). This odor-evoked oscillatory activity is supposed to be necessary for ®ne odor discrimination and is maintained within the MB (MacLeod et al. 1998). Experience-dependent modulations of activity in inhibitory feedback neurons may modulate these odorinduced oscillations within the MB. Inhibitory feedback loops are involved in learningdependent plasticity in a variety of systems. During induction of hippocampal long-term potentiation, for example, dendritic GABAa inhibition is reduced, resulting in enhanced postsynaptic depolarisation by facilitation of the NMDA current component (Bliss and Collingridge 1993; Tomasulo et al. 1993). During eyeblink conditioning recurrent inhibition via GABAergic cerebello-olivary projections regulates processing of the US (Thompson and Krupa 1994; Hesslow and Ivarsson 1996) and mediates the behavioural phenomenon of blocking (Kim et al. 1998). In the somatosensory cortex the balance between GABAergic and cholinergic input may control neural plasticity of cortical neurons by regulating their receptive ®eld size (Dykes 1997). Whether inhibitory feedback neurons in the insect mushroom body play similar roles during memory formation awaits further exploration. 575 Acknowledgements This paper is dedicated to Dr. Juliane Mauelshagen. The author was grateful for her expert comments and continuous support. I thank Drs. Randolf Menzel and Richard B. Levine for helpful comments and critically reading the manuscript and Mary Wurm for correcting the English manuscript. This work was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (Pf 128/6-4 and SFB 515/C5). References Bicker G, SchaÈfer S, Kingan TG (1985) Mushroom body feedback interneurons in the honeybee show GABA-like immunoreactivity. Brain Res 360: 394±397 Bitterman ME, Menzel R, Fietz A, SchaÈfer S (1983) Classical conditioning of proboscis extension in honeybees (Apis mellifera). J Comp Psyc 97: 107±119 Bliss TVP, Collingridge GL (1993) A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature 361: 31±39 Brotz TM, Bochenek B, Aronstein K, Ffrench-Constant RH, Borst A (1997) gamma-Aminobutyric acid receptor distribution in the mushroom bodies of a ¯y (Calliphora erythrocephala): a functional subdivision of Kenyon cells? J Comp Neurol 383: 42±48 Cayre M, Buckingham SD, Yagodin S, Sattelle DB (1999) Cultured insect mushroom body neurons express functional receptors for acetylcholine, GABA, glutamate, octopamine, and dopamine. J Neurophysiol 81: 1±14 Davis RL (1993) Mushroom bodies and Drosophila learning. Neuron 11: 1±14 De Belle JS, Heisenberg M (1994) Associative learning in Drosophila abolished by chemical ablation of mushroom bodies. Science 263: 692±695 Dubnau J, Tully T (1998) Gene discovery in Drosophila. Annu Rev Neurosci 21: 407±444 Dykes RW (1997) Mechanisms controlling neuronal plasticity in somatosensory cortex. Can J Physiol Pharmacol 75: 535±545 Erber J (1978) Response characteristics and after eects of multimodal neurons in the mushroom body area of the honey bee. Physiol Entomol 3: 77±89 Erber J, Homberg U, Gronenberg W (1987) Functional roles of the mushroom bodies in insects. In: Gupta AP (ed) Arthropod brain: its evolution, development, structure, and functions. John Wiley and Sons, New York, pp 485±511 Erber J, Masuhr T, Menzel R (1980) Localization of short-term memory in the brain of the bee, Apis mellifera. Physiol Entomol 5: 343±358 Fahrbach SE, Robinson GE (1995) Behavioural development in the honey bee: toward the study of learning under natural conditions. Learn & Mem 2: 199±224 Goldberg F, GruÈnewald B, Rosenboom H, Menzel R (1999) Nicotinic acetylcholine currents of cultured Kenyon cells from the mushroom bodies of the honey bee Apis mellifera. J Physiol 514: 759±768 Goll W (1967) Strukturuntersuchungen am Gehirn von Formica. Z Morphol Oekol Tiere 59: 145±210 Gronenberg W (1986) Physiological and anatomical properties of optical input-®bres to the mushroom body in the bee brain. J Insect Physiol 32: 695±704 Gronenberg W (1987) Anatomical and physiological properties of feedback neurons of the mushroom bodies in the bee brain. Exp Biol 46: 115±125 GruÈnewald B (1999) Morphology of feedback neurons in the mushroom body of the honeybee, Apis mellifera. J Comp Neurol 404: 114±126 Hammer M (1993) An identi®ed neuron mediates the unconditioned stimulus in associative olfactory learning in honeybees. Nature 366: 59±63 Hammer M (1997) The neural basis of associative reward learning in honeybees. Trends Neurosci 20: 245±252 Hammer M, Braun G, Mauelshagen J (1994) Food-induced arousal and nonassociative learning in honeybees: dependence of sensitisation on the application site and duration of food stimulation. Behav Neural Biol 62: 210±223 Hammer M, Menzel R (1995) Learning and memory in the honeybee. J Neurosci 15: 1617±1630 Hammer M, Menzel R (1998) Multiple sites of associative odor learning as revealed by local brain microinjections of octopamine in honeybees. Learn & Mem 5: 146±156 Harrison JB, Chen HH, Sattelle E, Barker PJ, Huskisson NS, Rauh JJ, Sattelle DB (1999) Immunocytochemical mapping of a Cterminus anti-peptide antibody to the GABA receptor subunit, RDL in the nervous system of Drosophila melanogaster. Cell Tissue Res 284: 269±278 Heisenberg M, Borst A, Wagner S, Byers D (1985) Drosophila mushroom body mutants are de®cient in olfactory learning. J Neurogenet 2: 1±30 Hesslow G, Ivarsson M (1996) Inhibition of the inferior olive during conditioned responses in the decerebrate ferret. Exp Brain Res 110: 36±46 Homberg U (1984) Processing of antennal information in extrinsic mushroom body neurons of the bee brain. J Comp Physiol A 154: 825±836 Homberg U, Erber J (1979) Response characteristics and identi®cation of extrinsic mushroom body neurons of the brain. Z Naturf 34c: 612±615 Homberg U, Kingan TG, Hildebrand JG (1987) Immunocytochemistry of GABA in the brain and suboesophageal ganglion of Manduca sexta. Cell Tissue Res 248: 1±24 Kim JJ, Krupa DJ, Thompson RF (1998) Inhibitory cerebelloolivary projections and blocking eect in classical conditioning. Science 279: 570±573 Kuwabara M (1957) Bildung des bedingten Re¯exes von Pavlovs Typus bei der Honigbiene, Apis melli®ca. J Fac Sci Hokkaido Univ Ser VI Zool 13: 458±464 Laurent G, Naraghi M (1994) Odorant-induced oscillations in the mushroom bodies of the locust. J Neurosci 14: 2993±3004 Leitch B, Laurent G (1996) GABAergic synapses in the antennal lobe and mushroom body of the locust olfactory system. J Comp Neurol 372: 478±514 Li Y, Strausfeld NJ (1997) Morphology and sensory modality of mushroom body extrinsic neurons in the brain of the cockroach, Periplaneta americana. J Comp Neurol 387: 631± 650 MacLeod K, BaÈcker A, Laurent G (1998) Who reads temporal information contained across synchronized and oscillatory spike trains? Nature 395: 693±698 Mauelshagen J (1993) Neural correlates of olfactory learning paradigms in an identi®ed neuron in the honeybee brain. J Neurophysiol 69: 609±625 Menzel R (1990) Learning, memory, and ``cognition'' in honey bees. In: Kesner RP, Olton DS (eds) Neurobiology of comparative cognition. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey, pp 237±292 Menzel R, Durst C, Erber J, EichmuÈller S, Hammer M, Hildebrandt H, Mauelshagen J, MuÈller U, Rosenboom H, Rybak J, SchaÈfer S, Scheidler A (1994) The mushroom bodies in the honeybee: from molecules to behaviour. In: Schildberger K, Elsner N (eds) Fortschritte der Zoologie, vol 39. Neural basis of behavioural adaptations. Fischer, Stuttgart, pp 81±102 Menzel R, Erber J, Masuhr T (1974) Learning and memory in the honeybee. In: Barton Browne L (ed) Experimental analysis of insect behaviour. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 195±217 Menzel R, Hammer M, Braun G, Mauelshagen J (1991) Neurobiology of learning and memory in honeybees. In: Goodman LJ, Fisher RC (eds) The behaviour and physiology of bees. C.A.B. International, Wallingford, pp 323±353 Menzel R, MuÈller U (1996) Learning and memory in honeybees: from behaviour to neural substrates. Annu Rev Neurosci 19: 379±404 Mobbs PG (1982) The brain of the honeybee Apis mellifera. I. The connections and spatial organization of the mushroom bodies. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B 298: 309±354 576 Mobbs PG (1984) Neural networks in the mushroom bodies of the honeybee. J Insect Physiol 30: 43±58 Mobbs PG (1985) Brain structure. In: Kerkut GA, Gilbert LI (eds) Comprehensive insect physiology, biochemistry, and pharmacology. Pergamon Press, Oxford, pp 299±370 Rosenboom H, Goldberg F, SchaÈfer S, Menzel R (1994) Ionic currents of Kenyon cells possibly involved in olfactory learning in insects. Soc Neurosci Abstr 20: 333.19 Rybak J, Menzel R (1993) Anatomy of the mushroom bodies in the honey bee brain: the neuronal connections of the alpha-lobe. J Comp Neurol 334: 444±465 Rybak, J Menzel R (1998) Integrative properties of the Pe1 neuron, a unique mushroom body output neuron. Learn & Mem 5: 133± 145 SchaÈfer S, Rosenboom H, Menzel R (1994) Ionic currents of Kenyon cells from the mushroom body of the honeybee. J Neurosci 14: 4600±4612 SchuÈrmann F-W (1973) UÈber die Struktur der PilzkoÈrper des Insektengehirns III. Die Anatomie der Nervenfasern in den Corpora pedunculata bei Acheta domesticus L (Orthoptera): Eine Golgi-Studie. Z Zellforsch 145: 247±285 SchuÈrmann F-W (1987) The architecture of the mushroom bodies and related neuropils in the insect brain. In: Gupta AP (ed) Arthropod brain. Its evolution, development, structure, and functions. John Wiley and Sons, New York, pp 231±256 Schildberger K (1981) Some physiological features of mushroom-body linked ®bers in the house cricket brain. Naturwissenschaften 67: 623±624 Schildberger K (1983) Local interneurons associated with the mushroom bodies and the central body in the brain of Acheta domesticus. Cell Tissue Res 230: 573±587 Schildberger K (1984) Multimodal interneurons in the cricket brain: properties of identi®ed extrinsic mushroom body cells. J Comp Physiol A 154: 71±79 Stopfer M, Bhagavan S, Smith BH, Laurent G (1997) Impaired odour discrimination on desynchronization of odour-encoding neural assemblies. Nature (Lond) 390: 70±74 Strausfeld NJ (1976) Atlas of an insect brain. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York Thompson RF, Krupa DJ (1994) Organization of memory traces in the mammalian brain. Annu Rev Neurosci 17: 519±549 Tomasulo RA, Ramirez JJ, Steward O (1993) Synaptic inhibition regulates associative interactions between aerents during the induction of long-term potentiation and depression. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 90: 11578±11582 Vareschi E (1971) Odor discrimination in the honey bee ± single cell and behavioural response. Z Vergl Physiol 75: 143±173 Weiss MJ (1978) Giant ®ber associated with mushroom body neuropiles in an insect brain: a possible one-cell feedback loop. Soc Neurosci Abstr 4: 210