Download BOX 42.2 WHY BRAIN SIZE IS IMPORTANT Larger brains are

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Neural oscillation wikipedia , lookup

Molecular neuroscience wikipedia , lookup

Neural coding wikipedia , lookup

Blood–brain barrier wikipedia , lookup

Axon wikipedia , lookup

Lateralization of brain function wikipedia , lookup

Donald O. Hebb wikipedia , lookup

Axon guidance wikipedia , lookup

Environmental enrichment wikipedia , lookup

Neuroscience and intelligence wikipedia , lookup

Neuroinformatics wikipedia , lookup

Mirror neuron wikipedia , lookup

Neurogenomics wikipedia , lookup

Biochemistry of Alzheimer's disease wikipedia , lookup

Development of the nervous system wikipedia , lookup

Single-unit recording wikipedia , lookup

Neurolinguistics wikipedia , lookup

Craniometry wikipedia , lookup

Selfish brain theory wikipedia , lookup

Neurophilosophy wikipedia , lookup

Haemodynamic response wikipedia , lookup

Premovement neuronal activity wikipedia , lookup

Neuroesthetics wikipedia , lookup

Activity-dependent plasticity wikipedia , lookup

Clinical neurochemistry wikipedia , lookup

History of anthropometry wikipedia , lookup

Brain morphometry wikipedia , lookup

Aging brain wikipedia , lookup

Optogenetics wikipedia , lookup

Artificial general intelligence wikipedia , lookup

Neuroeconomics wikipedia , lookup

Brain wikipedia , lookup

Mind uploading wikipedia , lookup

Circumventricular organs wikipedia , lookup

Neural correlates of consciousness wikipedia , lookup

Human brain wikipedia , lookup

History of neuroimaging wikipedia , lookup

Evolution of human intelligence wikipedia , lookup

Cognitive neuroscience wikipedia , lookup

Neuropsychology wikipedia , lookup

Channelrhodopsin wikipedia , lookup

Brain Rules wikipedia , lookup

Synaptic gating wikipedia , lookup

Feature detection (nervous system) wikipedia , lookup

Connectome wikipedia , lookup

Neurotoxin wikipedia , lookup

Holonomic brain theory wikipedia , lookup

Neuroplasticity wikipedia , lookup

Metastability in the brain wikipedia , lookup

Nervous system network models wikipedia , lookup

Neuropsychopharmacology wikipedia , lookup

Neuroanatomy wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
BOX 42.2
WHY BRAIN SIZE IS IMPORTANT
Larger brains are generally thought to be computationally better because they usually have
more neurons. However, growing bigger brains with more neurons creates a need for modifications
in brain organization, and some solutions are likely to be common across taxa, allowing predictions
about brain organization with brain size (Kaas, 2000). Enlarging a brain by having more neurons
creates two related problems. If brain functions depend on each neuron maintaining the same
proportion of connections with other neurons, then more neurons means more connections, which
would be longer and take up more space. In addition, the conduction times of longer axons and
dendrites would be longer, unless the thickness of the axons and dendrites were increased with the
added length (Bekkers & Stevens, 1970). Thus, larger brains would have larger neurons with longer,
thicker axons and dendrites. This suggests that brains would reach a maximum size, perhaps not
much larger than that of present-day humans, because brain connections and glia would take up so
much space with the addition of more neurons that such additions would be impractical. However, a
design solution for such connection problems is for brains to become increasingly modular with
larger size, so that short distance connections predominate, and only a few neurons have the long
connections that are needed to coordinate the modular parts. Rodents do seem to increase the
average neuron size with brain size, so that neuron densities greatly decrease as proportionately
more space is devoted to glia and connections, but primates maintain neuron densities with
increasing brain size, implying that average neuron size does not increase (Herculano-Houzel,
Collins, Wong, & Kass, 2007). No other mammal, including elephants and whales with much larger
brains, has the same number of neurons as the 1.5-kg human brain. A rodent with 86 billion
neurons would need a 35-kg brain, well beyond the largest known brain mass of 9 kg for the blue
whale (Herculano-Houzel, 2009). Whales and elephants may have evolved more modular brains as
they acquired large brains, but they do not seem to have done it to the extent that seems to have
occurred in the evolution of the large human brain. Over the course of the evolution of the large
human brain, the neocortex of primates, which makes up the bulk of the human brain, acquired
more cortical areas and more modular subdivisions of areas as brain sizes increased. As an example,
the hemispheric specializations of the human brain presumably reduce the need for long
connections between the two hemispheres.
A related issue is that large cortical areas are unlikely to function in the same manner as small
cortical areas. Unless neurons compensate with larger dendrites and longer intrinsic connections as
areas get larger, the computational window or scope of neurons will decrease (Fig. 42.10). For
example, as a visual area gets bigger, individual neurons would evaluate information from less and
less of the total visual field. This implies that as areas get bigger, their neurons become less capable
of global center-surround comparisons and more devoted to local center-surround comparisons.
Thus, some of the integrative functions of large areas must be displaced to smaller areas. The large
V1 of the human brain preserves the detail of visual scenes, but perception depends on other,
smaller visual areas. It is also apparent that changes in the sizes of dendritic arbors and the lengths
of intrinsic axons in smaller areas would have more impact on the sizes of computational windows
of neurons. Comparable changes in dendrites and axons would enlarge or reduce receptive field
sizes more in a small than a large visual area. Because their functions are more modifiable by smaller
structural modifications, smaller areas may be specialized more easily for different functions.
Neurons in large areas typically do not have larger dendritic arbors and longer intrinsic connections,
and indeed, they may have smaller dendritic arbors. Thus, V1 of primates has overall the smallest
and most densely packed neurons of all cortical areas, and this is especially the case in large-brained
anthropoid primates (Collins, Airey, Young, Leitch, & Kaas, 2010). In addition, although primary
sensory areas are often larger in larger brains, they are not enlarged in proportion to the rest of
cortex. Thus, in surface area primary visual cortex is less than three times larger in human brains
than in the brains of macaque monkeys, whereas the neocortex as a whole is over 10 times larger.
Neocortex in chimpanzee brains is one-fourth the size of the human neocortex, while primary visual
cortex (V1) is about the same size. The general lack of proportional growth of cortical areas with
brain size reduces the impact of the changing functions of areas with size, and reflects the addition
of other smaller cortical areas.