Download Morphological Basis of Learning and Memory: Vertebrates

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Time perception wikipedia , lookup

Memory consolidation wikipedia , lookup

Premovement neuronal activity wikipedia , lookup

Optogenetics wikipedia , lookup

Clinical neurochemistry wikipedia , lookup

History of neuroimaging wikipedia , lookup

Neurophilosophy wikipedia , lookup

Brain wikipedia , lookup

Artificial general intelligence wikipedia , lookup

Limbic system wikipedia , lookup

Haemodynamic response wikipedia , lookup

Long-term potentiation wikipedia , lookup

Brain morphometry wikipedia , lookup

Human brain wikipedia , lookup

Neuroinformatics wikipedia , lookup

Neuropsychology wikipedia , lookup

Neurotransmitter wikipedia , lookup

Feature detection (nervous system) wikipedia , lookup

Long-term depression wikipedia , lookup

State-dependent memory wikipedia , lookup

Connectome wikipedia , lookup

Cognitive neuroscience wikipedia , lookup

Adult neurogenesis wikipedia , lookup

De novo protein synthesis theory of memory formation wikipedia , lookup

Brain Rules wikipedia , lookup

Neuroanatomy of memory wikipedia , lookup

Dendritic spine wikipedia , lookup

Donald O. Hebb wikipedia , lookup

Neuroesthetics wikipedia , lookup

Development of the nervous system wikipedia , lookup

Neural correlates of consciousness wikipedia , lookup

Eyeblink conditioning wikipedia , lookup

Epigenetics in learning and memory wikipedia , lookup

Neuroeconomics wikipedia , lookup

Neuropsychopharmacology wikipedia , lookup

Nervous system network models wikipedia , lookup

Aging brain wikipedia , lookup

Neuroanatomy wikipedia , lookup

Metastability in the brain wikipedia , lookup

Synaptic gating wikipedia , lookup

Neuroplasticity wikipedia , lookup

Nonsynaptic plasticity wikipedia , lookup

Holonomic brain theory wikipedia , lookup

Synaptogenesis wikipedia , lookup

Environmental enrichment wikipedia , lookup

Chemical synapse wikipedia , lookup

Activity-dependent plasticity wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
<title>Morphological Basis of Learning and Memory: Vertebrates</title>
<h1p>The central issue in morphology of learning and memory is how such constructs are stored
in the nervous system. The basic shape of neurons and synapses is illustrated in Figure 1. In the late
nineteenth century, Santiago Ramón y Cajal (Spanish histologist, 1852<en>1934) suggested that
learning might involve changes in the synaptic connections through which neurons communicate.
Such synaptic change could take at least four possible forms. First, forming new synapses or
removing existing synapses could alter the pattern of functional connections. Second, selectively
strengthening or weakening some synapses could alter the pattern of functional connections. Very
strong evidence exists for both of these possibilities during learning, and in models of learning
such as -<i>long-term potentiation</i> (LTP). Third, by forming new neurons (neurogenesis),
neuronal circuits could be modified; there is increasing evidencethat neurogenesis occurs in the
mature brain. Fourth, changes could occur in both the nonsynaptic regions of neurons and in
nonneural elements of the brain such as glial cells and the vascular system subserving the brain, for
which evidence exists as well.(/h1p)
[insert Figure 1]
<h1>Changes in Synapse Number</h1>
<p>Important roots of memory research lie in studies of the effects of experience upon brain
development. For example, visual experience is necessary to develop normal visual ability in
mammals. Searching for a basis for this in brain anatomy, Cragg (1975), and others, noted that
1
animals deprived of visual experience had fewer synaptic connections per nerve cell in the visual
cortex. These studies profoundly influenced thinking about the processes by which the brain stores
information, because they showed that (1) brain structure is malleable; (2) synaptic organization
can be orchestrated into different configurations by behavioral experience; (3) both forming new
connections and pruning existing connections are involved in altering brain organization; and (4)
differential experience can modify the structure of synapses, suggesting that synaptic efficacy (or
strength) also can change. These effects of experience on synaptic connections during
development led to proposals that such changes might underlie adult learning as well</p>.
<p>A separate developmental approach that was very fruitful in understanding brain substrates of
learning and memory involved enriching the lives of young animals with additional stimulation.
Donald Hebb (psychobiologist, 1904<en>1985) proposed ways in which synaptic change could be
incorporated meaningfully into functional circuitry. With his students, he showed that enriching
the rearing environment of rats with cagemates and toys improved the animals’ ability to solve
complex problems. Hebb concluded that behavior, and by implication brain organization, was
permanently altered by this early experience. Subsequently, Rosenzweig, Bennett and Diamond
(1972) found that regions of the cerebral cortex were thicker and heavier in rats reared in enriched
environments, compared with rats reared in solitary or group cages. Volkmar and Greenough
(1972) followed up these findings, reporting that visual cortical neurons of rats reared in enriched
environments had larger dendritic fields than did those of cage-housed controls. Dendrites of
neurons receive the bulk of the synaptic input (see Figure 1), so the implication was that new
synapses formed. Similar findings were subsequently reported in other areas of the cerebral cortex
and in brain regions such as the hippocampus, superior colliculus, and cerebellum. The enriched
2
environment changed brain anatomy in adult rats, which is important evidence for involvement in
learning and memory. Turner and Greenough (1985) found that rats reared in enriched
environments had more synapses per neuron in the visual cortex, compared with rats reared alone
or in pairs in standard laboratory cages. Moreover, similar changes occur in the striatum as well
(Comery, Shah and Greenough, 1995), suggesting that the experience-dependent changes in
neuronal morphology influence multiple levels or systems in the brain. The general conclusion
from these enriched environment studies is that when animals are placed in an environment in
which they store information that affects later behavior, they form new synapses. (The term
<i>enriched</i> is used in comparison with the typical laboratory environment and does not imply
superiority to the natural environment.)</p>
<p>Follow-up studies have explored the effects of specific learning tasks upon these same
measures. There is compelling evidence that many forms of learning change both the amount of
dendrite per neuron and the number of synapses per neuron. For example, dendritic branching is
increased in visual cortical neurons following twenty-five days of exposure to a series of maze
problems (Greenough, Juraska, and Volkmar, 1979). Subsequent work used split-brain rats,
severing the nerve fibers that allow the right and left hemispheres to communicate, and opaque
contact lenses that restricted visual input from training to one eye. Neurons on the trained side of
the brain selectively exhibited dendritic field size increases; thus these changes were not of the
general sort that might be due to stress or arousal associated with the task, which should affect both
sides of the brain equally. These studies, and others, indicated that the altered dendritic fields were
associated with neural input and output related to the training.</p>
3
<p>Synaptogenesis is also implicated in associative learning. Tsukahara (1981) investigated
associative limb flexion conditioning, using stimulation to the cerebral peduncle as the
conditioned stimulus and electric shock to the forelimb as the unconditioned stimulus. In this
paradigm, red nucleus lesions abolish the conditioned response, implicating the involvement of
this structure. Electrophysiological studies following conditioning indicated enhanced input to the
red nucleus from the cerebral cortex. Subsequently, Tsukahara’s coworkers (Murakami, Higashi,
Katsumaru, and Oda, 1987) reported morphological evidence for formation of new corticorubral
synapses in conditioned animals.</p>
<p>Similar anatomical effects of training have been observed in other behavioral tasks. Stewart
(1991) examined day-old chicks that learned to avoid pecking a bad tasting food particle and found
an elevated number of synapses in a forebrain region previously shown to be involved in the
learning. In another involved brain region there were also increases in the number of spines (see
Figure l), the dendritic component of one type of synapse. Likewise, the density of spines was
reported to be elevated in hippocampal area CA1 in adult rats following spatial learning (Moser,
Trommald, and Andersen, 1994). Comparable changes have been observed in numerous other
paradigms (e.g., bird song learning and imprinting in birds). Finally, while this discussion is
confined to vertebrates, excellent evidence for comparable synaptic number changes in
invertebrate plasticity paradigms also exists.</p>
<p>Neurogenesis presents yet another mechanism whereby brain organization could be modified
in response to experience. For example, increased numbers of new neurons have been reported in
the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus of animals exposed to a complex environment
4
(Kempermann, Kuhn, and Gage, 1998) or permitted access to an exercise wheel (Van Praag,
Christie, Sejnowski, and Gage, 1999). More specific to learning and memory, Gould et al. (1999a)
reported that the number of new neurons in rat dentate gyrus dramatically increased following
associative conditioning. While most reports of neurogenesis have been limited to rodents and to
relatively unique areas in the brain such as the hippocampus or olfactory bulb, neurogenesis has
been reported in the primate cortex (Gould, Reeves, Graziano, and Gross, 1999b) and disputed
(Kornack and Rakic, 2001). While the issues of the extent, location, and functional relevance of
neurogenesis has yet to be resolved, the mere possibility of such a mechanism in the mature central
nervous system has spurred a great deal of excitement and hope for many areas of neural
research.</p>
<h1>Role of Synapse Formation in Plastic Neural Change</h1>
<p>An issue that affects all of these studies is whether anatomical changes that are seen following
training result merely from increased neural activity involved in performing the learned task.
Muscles grow larger in response to exercise; perhaps neurons do too, such that these structural
changes have nothing to do with learning or memory per se. (This issue is, of course, not unique to
morphological studies; proposed molecular and other aspects of the cellular mechanisms of
memory may similarly be artifacts of activity.)-</p>
<p>There have been direct tests of the effects of neural activity versus learning on synapse change.
For example, Black et al. (1990) compared a cerebellar cortical region in rats that had learned a
complex series of motor tasks to that of rats that performed one of two forms of physical exertion
5
involving little learning: running on a treadmill or in an activity wheel. Exercise alone had no
effect on synapse number whereas rats that had learned increased the number of synapses per
neuron in the cerebellar cortex. Similar effects have been observed in areas such as the motor
cortex (Kleim et al., 1996). In contrast, blood vessel density was elevated in affected regions in rats
that had exercised, whereas the motor learning rats had the same blood vessel density as control
animals. These results indicate that activity and learning have very different effects on brain
tissue.</p>
<p>Additional support for the role of synapse formation in plastic neural change has come from
studies of long-term potentiation. LTP involves an increase in the response of postsynaptic
neurons following high frequency bursts of presynaptic firing. LTP induction has been shown to
increase spine density and modify dendritic morphology in the motor cortex (Ivanco, Racine and
Kolb, 2000) and in hippocampal subfield CA1 (Lee, Oliver, Schottler, and Lynch, 1981). Chang
and Greenough (1984) included a type of high frequency stimulation that did not induce LTP, and
subsequently did not alter synapse formation, suggesting that LTP-induced synaptogenesis was
not caused by high frequency stimulation alone. In contrast to CA1, induction of LTP in the
hippocampal dentate gyrus does not cause synapse formation but changes synapse structure
(Geinisman, de Toledo-Morrell, and Morrell, 1991). Recent advances in microscopy have
permitted near real time evaluation of changes in morphology of living neurons. Using these tools,
Engert and Bonhoeffer (1999) reported that postsynaptic spines are formed de novo in response to
stimulation. Taken together, these findings reinforce the view that numerous different cellular
changes may be involved in learning, and other forms of neural plasticity as well.</p>
6
<h1>Changes in Synapse Structure: Indications of Synapse Efficacy Change</h1>
<p>Several structural features of synapses have been found to be altered by behavioral experience.
One of the most obvious features is the size of synapses. Larger synapses may release more
neurotransmitter or have more receptors, such that a size change could indicate a strength change.
Early findings indicated smaller synapses in the visual cortex of animals visually deprived during
development, and Tieman (1985) reported smaller geniculocortical synapses in monocularly
deprived cats. Conversely, synapse size increased following imprinting in day-old chicks, and
similar size changes were found after avoidance learning. Larger synapses were also observed in
layer IV of the visual cortex of rats reared in enriched environments, compared with individually
caged controls. Likewise, Van Harreveld and Fifkova (1975) described changes in the size of
synaptic spine heads and necks (see Figure 1) following LTP induction in the dentate gyrus. Koch
and Zador (1993) have suggested that larger spine components, and the associated spine neck
restriction, may permit activation of focalized intracellular cascades and reduce electrical
resistance, thereby facilitating the passage of synaptic current into the dendrite (Harris and Kater,
1994).</p>
<p><i>Synaptic vesicles</i> are believed to contain neurotransmitters, and changes in their
numbers could indicate changes in synapse strength. Synaptic vesicle numbers have been reported
to decrease with visual deprivation (e.g., Tieman, 1985) and there are reports of both decreased
vesicle density and altered vesicle location within the presynaptic terminal following LTP
induction (e.g., Applegate, Kerr, and Landfield, 1987; Fifkova and Van Harreveld, 1977). In
contrast, vesicle numbers have been shown to increase in rats reared in enriched environments
7
(Nakamura, Kobayashi, Ohashi, and Ando, 1999; Sirevaag and Greenough, 1991).</p>
<p>At least three other synapse features appear to be sensitive to experience. First, small
discontinuities in the postsynaptic density, termed <i>perforations,</i> have been found to
increase in number following complex environment exposure and to decrease in affected synapses
subsequent to sensory deprivation (Greenough, West, and DeVoogd, 1978). Moreover, Vrensen
and Cardozo (1981) found that the number of perforated synapses increased in the visual cortex
following visual discrimination learning. The function of these perforations is unknown. Second,
the incidence of <i>multiple synaptic boutons</i> (presynaptic elements that synapse with
multiple postsynaptic components) is elevated in numerous brain areas following exposure to a
complex environment and in animals that have been trained in an associative learning paradigm
(Geinisman et al., 2001; Jones et al., 1997). Third, the cellular organelles that synthesize protein,
<i>polyribosomal aggregates,</i> are frequently found in the heads and necks of spines during
periods of synapse formation (Steward and Falk, 1985). They are also found more frequently in
spines of animals in complex environments, possibly reflecting greater rates of synapse formation.
Protein synthesis at the synapse has been proposed as a memory mechanism (Steward and
Schuman, 2001).</p>
<h1>Changes in Nonneural Elements</h1>
<p>The enriched environment work indicated from its earliest days that morphological changes
were not restricted to neurons. Glial cells, supportive elements that maintain ionic, metabolic, and
neurotransmitter homeostasis, respond similarly to environmental complexity. Sirevaag and
8
Greenough (1991) reported that astrocytes grow larger and extend additional processes into the
tissue during the first phase of their response to the animal’s housing in an enriched environment.
In a second phase, astrocytes divide, increasing their numbers, and shrink, on average, toward their
preexposure size. These stages are qualitatively comparable with those of gliosis, the glial reaction
to injury, yet protracted. Moreover, Anderson et al. (1994) have shown an increase in the volume
of glia per Purkinje cell in the cerebellum of animals that learned a complex motor skill, but not
those who simply exercised. Likewise, blood vessel density increased in rats placed in an enriched
environment at the age of weaning. In animals that are older at the time they are first exposed to
enrichment, this blood vessel response diminishes with increasing age.</p>
<h1>Conclusions</h1>
<p>Morphological research has provided strong evidence for both forms of synaptic change that
have been proposed to underlie learning and memory. Formation, and occasionally loss, of
synapses occurs both during periods of development when the brain is storing information and
during exposure to specific learning tasks. Various control procedures have largely ruled out the
possibility that these synaptic changes are artifactual results arising from factors other than
learning. Changes in the structure of synapses, such as in the size or shape of synaptic components,
also occur during learning and in other situations in which functional brain organization is altered,
such as LTP. Many of these structural changes have been associated with synapse strength
differences in other research. Developing research implicates addition of new neurons via
neurogenesis and changes in nonneuronal elements of the brain. Thus the weight of the evidence
indicates that both synapse formation/removal and synapse strength changes are involved in
9
learning and memory, but additional mechanisms are also likely.</p>
<seealso><i>See also:</i> <sc>Morphological Basis of Learning and Memory: Vertebrates;
Protein Synthesis in Long-Term Memory in Vertebrates<sc></seealso>
10
<bib>Bibilography</bib>
<bibcit>Andersen, B. J., Li, X., Alcantara, A. A., Isaacs, K. R., Black, J. E., and Greenough, W. T.
(1994). Glial hypertrophy is associated with synaptogenesis following motor-skill learning, but
not with angiogenesis following exercise. <i> Glia 11,</i>73<en>80.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Applegate, M. D., Kerr, D. S., and Landfield, P. W. (1987). Redistribution of synaptic
vesicles during long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. <i>Brain Research 401,</i>
401<en>406.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Black, J. E., Isaacs, K. R., Anderson, B. J., Alcantara, A. A., and Greenough, W. T.
(1990). Learning causes synaptogenesis, whereas motor activity causes angiogenesis, in cerebellar
cortex of adult rats. <i> Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 87, </i>
5,568<en>5,572.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Chang, F.-L. F., and Greenough, W. T. (1984). Transient and enduring morphological
correlates of synaptic activity and efficacy change in the rat hippocampal slice. <i>Brain Research
309,</i>35<en>46.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Comery, T. A., Shah, R., and Greenough, W. T. (1995). Differential rearing alters spine
density on medium-sized spiny neurons in the rat corpus striatum: Evidence for the association of
morphological plasticity with early response gene expression. <i>Neurobiology of Learning and
Memory 63,</i> 217<en>219.</bibcit>
11
<bibcit>Cragg, B. G. (1975). The development of synapses in kitten visual cortex during visual
deprivation. <i>Experimental Neurology 46,</i> 445<en>451.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Engert, F., and Bonhoeffer, T. (1999). Dendritic spine changes associated with
hippocampal long-term synaptic plasticity. <i> Nature 399,</i> 66<en>70.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Fifkova, E., and Van Harreveld, A. (1977). Long-lasting morphological changes in
dendritic spines of dentate granular cells following stimulation of the entorhinal area. <i>Journal
of Neurocytology 6,</i> 211<en>230.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Geinisman, Y., Berry, R. W., Disterhoft, J. F., Power, J. M., and Van der Zee, E. A.
(2001). Associative learning elicits the formation of multiple-synapse boutons. <i>Journal of
Neuroscience 21,</i> 5,568<en>5,573.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Geinisman, Y., de Toledo-Morrell, L., and Morrell, F. (1991). Induction of long-term
potentiation is associated with an increase in the number of axospinous synapses with segmented
postsynaptic densities. <i>Brain Research 566,</i> 77<en>88.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Gould, E., Beylin, A., Tanapat, P., Reeves, A., and Shors, T. J. (1999a). Learning
enhances adult neurogenesis in the hippocampal formation. <i>Nature Neuroscience 2, </i>
260<en>265.</bibcit>
12
<bibcit>Gould, E., Reeves, A. J., Graziano, M. S., and Gross, C. G. (1999b). Neurogenesis in the
neocortex of adult primates. <i>Science 286,</i> 548<en>552.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Greenough, W. T., Juraska, J. M., and Volkmar, F. R. (1979). Maze training effects on
dendritic branching in occipital cortex of adult rats. <i>Behavioral and Neural Biology 26,</i>
287<en>297.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Greenough, W. T., West, R. W., and DeVoogd, T. J. (1978). Sub-synaptic plate
perforations: Changes with age and experience in the rat. <i>Science
202,</i>1,096<en>1,098.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Harris, K. M., and Kater, S. B. (1994). Dendritic spines: Cellular specializations
imparting both stability and flexibility to synaptic function. <i>Annual Review of Neuroscience
17,</i> 341<en>371.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Ivanco, T. L., Racine, R. J., and Kolb, B. (2000). Morphology of layer III pyramidal
neurons is altered following induction of LTP in sensorimotor cortex of the freely moving rat.
<i>Synapse 37</i>, 16<en>22.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Jones, T. A., Klintsova, A. Y., Kilman, V. L., Sirevaag, A. M., and Greenough, W. T.
(1997). Induction of multiple synapses by experience in the visual cortex of adult rats.
<i>Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 68,</i> 13<en>20. </bibcit>
13
<bibcit>Kempermann, G., Kuhn, H. G., and Gage, F. H. (1998). Experience-induced neurogenesis
in the senescent dentate gyrus. <i>Journal of Neuroscience 18,</i> 3,206<en>3,212.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Kleim, J. A., Lussnig, E., Schwarz, E. R., Comery, T. A., and Greenough, W. T. (1996).
Synaptogenesis and FOS expression in the motor cortex of the adult rat after motor skill learning.
<i>Journal of Neuroscience 16,</i> 4,529<en>4,535.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Koch, C., and Zador, A. (1993). The function of dendritic spines: Devices subserving
biochemical rather than electrical compartmentalization. <i>Journal of Neuroscience 13,</i>
413<en>422.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Kornack, D. R., and Rakic, P. (2001). Cell proliferation without neurogenesis in adult
primate neocortex. <i>Science 294,</i> 2,127<en>2,130.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Lee, K. S., Oliver, M., Schottler, F., and Lynch, G. (1981). Electron microscopic studies
of brain slices: The effects of high frequency stimulation on dendritic ultrastructure. In G. A.
Kerkut, and H. V. Wheal, eds. <i>Electrophysiology of isolated mammalian CNS
preparations.</i> New York: Academic Press.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Moser, M.-B., Trommald, M., and Andersen, P. (1994). An increase in dendritic spine
density on hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells following spatial learning in adult rats suggests the
formation of new synapses. <i>Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 91,</i>
14
12,673<en>12,675 </bibcit>
<bibcit>Murakami, F., Higashi, S., Katsumaru, H., and Oda, Y. (1987). Formation of new
corticorubral synapses as a mechanism for classical conditioning in the cat. <i>Brain Research
437,</i> 379<en>382.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Nakamura H., Kobayashi, S., Ohashi, Y., and Ando, S. (1999). Age-changes of brain
synapses and synaptic plasticity in response to an enriched environment. <i>Journal of
Neuroscience Research 56,</i> 307<en>315.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Rosenzweig, M. R., Bennett, E. L., and Diamond, M. C. (1972). Chemical and anatomical
plasticity of brain: Replications and extensions. In J. Gaito, ed. <i>Macromolecules and
behavior,</i> 2nd edition. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Sirevaag, A. M., and Greenough, W. T. (1991). Plasticity of GFAP-immunoreactive
astrocyte size and number in visual cortex of rats reared in complex environments. <i>Brain
Research 540,</i> 273<en>278.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Steward, O., and Falk, P. M. (1985). Polyribosomes under developing spine synapses:
Growth specializations of dendrites at sites of synaptogenesis. <i>Journal of Neuroscience
Research 13,</i> 75<en>88.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Steward O., and Schuman E. M. (2001). Protein synthesis at synaptic sites on dendrites.
15
<i>Annual Review of Neuroscience 24,</i> 299<en>325.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Stewart, M. G. (1991). Changes in dendritic and synaptic structure in chick forebrain
consequent on passive avoidance learning. In R. J. Andrew, ed. <i>Neural and behavioral
plasticity.</i> London: Oxford University Press.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Tieman, S. B. (1985). The anatomy of geniculocortical connections in monocularly
deprived cats. <i>Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology 5,</i> 35<en>45.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Tsukahara, N. (1981). Sprouting and the neuronal basis of learning. <i>Trends in
Neurosciences 4,</i> 234<en>237.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Turner, A. M., and Greenough, W. T. (1985). Differential rearing effects on rat visual
cortex synapses. I. Synaptic and neuronal density and synapses per neuron. <i>Brain Research
329,</i> 195<en>203.</bibcit>
<bibcit>Van Harreveld, A., and Fifkova, E. (1975). Swelling of dendritic spines in the fascia
dentata after stimulation of the perforant fibers as a mechanism of post-tetanic potentiation.
<i>Experimental Neurology 49,</i> 736<en>749.</bibcit>
Van Praag, H., Christie, B. R., Sejnowski, T. J., and Gage, F. H. (1999). Running enhances
neurogenesis, learning, and long-term potentiation in mice. <i>Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences 96,</i> 13,427<en>13,431.</bibcit>
16
Volkmar, F. R., and Greenough, W. T. (1972). Rearing complexity affects branching of dendrites
in the visual cortex of the rat. <i>Science 176,</i> 1,445<en>1,447.</bibcit>
Vrensen, G., and Cardozo, J. N. (1981). Changes in size and shape of synaptic connections after
visual training: An ultrastructural approach of synaptic plasticity. <i>Brain Research 218, </i>
79<en>97.</bibcit>
<byline>William T. Greenough</byline>
<byline>Revised by William T. Greenough and James D. Churchill</byline>
17