* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Amygdala oscillations and the consolidation of
Nervous system network models wikipedia , lookup
Human brain wikipedia , lookup
Brain Rules wikipedia , lookup
Nonsynaptic plasticity wikipedia , lookup
Cognitive neuroscience wikipedia , lookup
Neuroanatomy wikipedia , lookup
Sleep and memory wikipedia , lookup
Emotion perception wikipedia , lookup
Eyeblink conditioning wikipedia , lookup
Effects of sleep deprivation on cognitive performance wikipedia , lookup
Neuroeconomics wikipedia , lookup
Affective neuroscience wikipedia , lookup
Biology of depression wikipedia , lookup
Environmental enrichment wikipedia , lookup
Premovement neuronal activity wikipedia , lookup
Aging brain wikipedia , lookup
Cognitive neuroscience of music wikipedia , lookup
Hippocampus wikipedia , lookup
Activity-dependent plasticity wikipedia , lookup
Orbitofrontal cortex wikipedia , lookup
Feature detection (nervous system) wikipedia , lookup
Neuroplasticity wikipedia , lookup
Holonomic brain theory wikipedia , lookup
Emotion and memory wikipedia , lookup
De novo protein synthesis theory of memory formation wikipedia , lookup
Clinical neurochemistry wikipedia , lookup
Memory consolidation wikipedia , lookup
Optogenetics wikipedia , lookup
Neuropsychopharmacology wikipedia , lookup
Neural oscillation wikipedia , lookup
Traumatic memories wikipedia , lookup
Synaptic gating wikipedia , lookup
Neural correlates of consciousness wikipedia , lookup
Emotional lateralization wikipedia , lookup
Metastability in the brain wikipedia , lookup
306 Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.6 No.7 July 2002 Amygdala oscillations and the consolidation of emotional memories Denis Paré, Dawn R. Collins and Joe Guillaume Pelletier The amygdala receives multi-modal sensory inputs and projects to virtually all levels of the central nervous system. Via these widespread projections, the amygdala facilitates consolidation of emotionally arousing memories. How the amygdala promotes synaptic plasticity elsewhere in the brain remains unknown, however. Recent work indicates that amygdala neurons show theta activity during emotional arousal, and various types of oscillations during sleep. These synchronized neuronal events could promote synaptic plasticity by facilitating interactions between neocortical storage sites and temporal lobe structures involved in declarative memory. Even in the absence of sensory stimulation, the spontaneous activity of the brain is not random. Rhythmic population events, measurable in the extracellular space as currents, emerge from complex interactions between the intrinsic properties of neurons [1] and properties of the network in which they are embedded. Rhythms of various frequencies occur in different brain regions and these oscillations change depending on the behavioral state [2,3]. The importance of these oscillations derives from the fact that neuronal events underlying cognitive activity, from sensory perception to memory, are embedded in these endogenous population rhythms. In other words, the study of oscillations and coding in large neuronal ensembles are inextricable. Moreover, during sleep, when the brain is largely disconnected from the outside world, neurons generate patterns of oscillations and synchronized population bursts that are believed to play a crucial role in memory consolidation (see Box 1). Finally, because related parts of the brain tend to produce similar oscillations, the analysis of spontaneous oscillatory activity can reveal functional kinship among brain structures. This review focuses on the neuronal oscillations shown by the amygdala. The importance of this issue derives from data indicating that the amygdala facilitates consolidation of emotionally arousing memories [4]. Thus, oscillations in the amygdala might play a crucial role in this respect. Besides, the amygdala presents us with an interesting problem in that it receives inputs from parasensory neocortical areas as well as from phylogenetically older temporal cortices involved in declarative memory. Hence, the question emerges as to whether the amygdala exhibits spontaneous rhythms typical of only one, or both of these regions. Multiple functions of the amygdala As a group, amygdala nuclei receive inputs from and project to virtually all levels of the central nervous system (Box 2). Indeed, they have access to sensory inputs of all modalities, and they innervate cortical Box 1. Memory consolidation during sleep: the role of synchronized neuronal activity Denis Paré* Joe Guillaume Pelletier Center for Molecular and Behavioral Neuroscience, Aidekman Research Center, Rutgers State University, 197 University Ave, Newark, NJ 07102, USA. *e-mail: [email protected] Dawn R. Collins Dept of Physiology, University College London, Royal Free Campus, Rowland Hill Street, London, UK NW3 2PF. Much evidence suggests that sleep plays a pivotal role in synaptic plasticity and memory. For instance, slow-wave sleep (SWS) enhances cortical reorganization of ocular dominance columns in developing visual cortex following monocular deprivation [a]. Moreover, it has been shown that sleep is essential after visual training for the consolidation of some forms of procedural memory, such as visual discrimination skills [b,c]. However, the facilitating effect of sleep is not limited to procedural memory, as sleep deprivation produces marked impairments of episodic memory [d]. It was proposed that the facilitating effect of sleep on memory consolidation depends on the synchronized neuronal activity of SWS. For instance, one model of episodic memory [e] postulates that during waking, information is initially stored in the CA3 region of the hippocampus, through changes in the strength of connections between pyramidal neurons. Later, during SWS, synchronized population discharges of CA3 neurons in relation to events known as sharp waves (see main text) would ‘replay’ the representations stored in the CA3 network and, via the rhinal cortices, reactivate associative cortical neurons representing features of the event of interest. Ultimately, this replay of stored representations would lead to long-term synaptic changes in associative cortical networks. http://tics.trends.com Such models are consistent with neuropsychological data indicating that the medial temporal lobe plays a time-limited role in memory [f]. Moreover, they are supported by single-unit studies in which evidence of replay of waking activity patterns during sleep has been obtained [g]. References a Frank, M.G. et al. (2001) Sleep enhances plasticity in the developing visual cortex. Neuron 30, 275–287 b Gais, S. et al. (2000) Early sleep triggers memory for early visual discrimination skills. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 1335–1339 c Stickgold, R. et al. (2000) Visual discrimination learning requires sleep after training. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 1237–1238 d Plihal, W. and Born, J. (1999) Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on priming and spatial memory. Psychophysiology 36, 571–582 e Buzsáki, G. (1989) Two-stage model of memory formation: a role for noisy brain states. Neuroscience 31, 551–570 f Squire, L.R. and Cohen, N. (1979) Memory and amnesia: resistance to disruption develops for years after learning. Behav. Neural Biol. 25, 115–125 g Wilson, M.A. and McNaughton, B.L. (1989) Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. Science 265, 676–679 1364-6613/02/$ – see front matter © 2002 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. PII: S1364-6613(02)01924-1 Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.6 No.7 July 2002 307 Box 2. Connections of the amygdala References a McDonald, A.J. (1992) Cell types and intrinsic connections of the amygdala. In The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction (Aggleton, J.P., ed.), pp. 67–96, John Wiley & Sons b Swanson, L.W. and Petrovich, G.D. (1998) What is the amygdala? Trends Neurosci. 21, 323–331 c Russchen, F.T. (1986) Cortical and subcortical afferents of the amygdaloid complex. In Excitatory Amino Acids and Epilepsy (Schwarz, R. and Ben–Ari, Y., eds), pp. 35–52, Plenum Press d Amaral, D.G. et al. (1992) Anatomical organization of the primate amygdaloid complex. In The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction (Aggleton, J.P., ed.), pp. 1–66, John Wiley & Sons e Pitkänen, A. et al. (1997) Organization of intra-amygdaloid circuitries in the rat: an emerging framework for understanding functions of the amygdala. Trends Neurosci. 20, 517–523 f Paré, D. and Smith, Y. (1998) Intrinsic circuitry of the amygdaloid complex: common principles of organization in rats and cats. Trends Neurosci. 21, 240–241 g Davis, M. (2000) The role of the amygdala in conditioned and unconditioned fear and anxiety. In The Amygdala: a Functional Analysis (Aggleton, J.P., ed.), pp. 213–287, Oxford University Press h Pitkänen, A. (2000) Connectivity of the rat amygdaloid complex. In The Amygdala: a Functional Analysis (Aggleton, J.P., ed.), pp. 31–115, Oxford University Press The amygdala is a heterogeneous collection of interconnected nuclei located in the depth of the temporal lobe. Early anatomists divided the amygdala into three groups of nuclei (see Fig. I): the basolateral (BL) complex, comprising the lateral (LA), BL and basomedial (BM) nuclei; the corticomedial group including the central (CE), cortical and medial (ME) nuclei [a]; and a third, anterior group. Although the validity of this classification has been questioned [b], it is used in this article. Subsequent work showed that information about all sensory modalities is relayed to the amygdala from the thalamus and cerebral cortex [c,d]. Most of these sensory inputs end in the BL complex and are then conveyed to the corticomedial group by a large network of glutamatergic intra-amygdaloid projections [e,f]. In turn, the corticomedial group projects to brainstem and hypothalamic structures involved in autonomic control and in the elaboration of species-specific emotional behaviors [g]. In addition to these descending efferent projections, the amygdala has ascending projections to the cerebral cortex [h]. Amygdalocortical axons originate mostly from the BL complex and the majority terminate in higher-order cortical areas. Thus, as a function of the emotional significance of sensory events, amygdalofugal axons are able to modulate neuronal operations at nearly all CNS levels, from brainstem cardiovascular centers to the highest computational levels of the brain. Bed nucleus of Stria Terminalis Massive brain stem projections Lateral hypothalamus Basal forebrain Prefrontal cortex Thalamus (posterior and midline nuclei) Rhinal cortices, insula Prefrontal cortex Central (CE) Lateral (LA) Rhinal cortices, insula Hippocampal formation Accumbens PU CE OT ME LA Prefrontal cortex, insula Bed nucleus of Stria Terminalis Hypothalamus Medial (ME) BL BM AHA D L Hypothalamus Insula Brain Stem Bed nucleus of Stria Terminalis Olfactory system Bed nucleus of Stria Terminalis Hypothalamus Thalamus (midline nuclei) M V Prefrontal cortex Rhinal cortices, insula Temporal and cingulate cortex Hippocampal formation Thalamus (posteior and midline nuclei) Basolateral (BL) Prefrontal cortex Rhinal cortices, insula Hippocampal formation Various cortical fields Striatum Mediodorsal thalamus Bed nucleus of Stria Terminalis Light brainstem projections Prefrontal cortex Various temporal cortical fields Thalamus (posterior and midline) Hypothalamus Basomedial (BM) Rhinal cortices, insula Hippocampal formation Prefrontal cortex Striatum Bed nucleus of Stria Terminalis Hypothalamus (ventromedial) TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Fig. I. The main connections of the amygdala. Inputs are represented by green arrows; outputs by purple arrows. Note that for simplicity, inputs from and projections to neuromodulatory systems of the brainstem and basal forebrain have been omitted. AHA, amygdalohippocampal area; OT, optic tract; PU, putamen. D, dorsal; V, ventral; L, lateral, M, medial. http://tics.trends.com 308 Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.6 No.7 July 2002 Box 3. Consolidation of emotional memories Long-term memory for an event can be enhanced or reduced by manipulations performed in the hours after learning. Susceptibility of recently formed memories to post-learning manipulations was seen with electroconvulsive shocks, protein synthesis inhibitors, electrical stimulation or drug injections in discrete brain regions [a]. Various interpretations were proposed for these results. However, the observation that emotionally arousing events are remembered vividly whereas others are forgotten led Gold and McGaugh [b] to suggest that post-learning treatments might be interfering with, or potentiating, a mechanism that regulates memory consolidation. They reasoned that there would be a biological advantage in delaying memory consolidation until the significance of an experience could be evaluated. Thus, they hypothesized that the brain is endowed with modulatory systems that affect the development of memories, but do not store them. Consistent with this, administration of adrenal stress hormones after learning was found to facilitate retention in apetitively or aversively motivated tasks [c]. As predicted by the consolidation hypothesis, the effects of stress hormones were time-dependent, their impact on retention decreasing as the interval between training and treatment increased. Moreover, systemic administration of β-adrenergic-receptor antagonists blocked the effects of emotional arousal on long-term declarative memory [d]. These results raised the possibility that, when released during a stressful episode, these hormones could act retrogradely to affect memory of that event. In other words, the extent of consolidation would depend on the arousing consequences of an experience, as expressed by the release of stress hormones [b]. Later, it was shown that the basolateral (BL) amygdala mediates the effects of stress hormones on memory. Indeed, lesion or inactivation of the BL amygdala, but not of the central amygdaloid nucleus, blocked the memory modulating effects produced by adrenalectomy or peripheral administration of adrenaline [c]. In addition, post-learning treatments that presumably reduced or enhanced excitability of the BL neurons, respectively decreased or improved retention on a variety of emotionally charged learning tasks [c]. However, the memory modulating effects of these manipulations do not result from alterations of memory storage in the but in other structures that represent the storage site of particular forms of memories [e]. The modulation of memory by the BL amygdala can also be seen in humans. For instance, emotionally arousing stories are normally better recalled than neutral ones, but this effect is absent in subjects with amygdala lesions [f]. Moreover, imaging studies have found a high correlation between long-term recall of emotionally arousing versus neutral material and the degree of amygdala activation observed when these stimuli were first presented [g,h]. Thus, in emotionally arousing conditions, the amygdala facilitates memory-storage processes in brain areas that are involved in declarative memory. References a McGaugh, J.L. and Gold, P.E. (1976) Modulation of memory by electrical stimulation of the brain. In Neural Mechanisms of Learning and Memory (Rosenzweig, M.R. and Bennett, E.L., eds), pp. 549–560, MIT Press b Gold, P.E. and McGaugh, J.L. (1975) A single-trace, two process view of memory storage processes. In Short-Term Memory (Deutsch, D. and Deutsch, J.A., eds), pp. 355–378, Academic Press c McGaugh, J.L. (2000) Memory: a century of consolidation. Science 287, 248–251 d Cahill, L. et al. (1994) Beta-adrenergic activation and memory for emotional events. Nature 371, 702–704 e Cahill, L. and McGaugh, J.L. (1998) Mechanisms of emotional arousal and lasting declarative memory. Trends Neurosci. 21, 294–299 f Cahill, L. et al. (1995) The amygdala and emotional memory. Nature 377, 295–296 g Cahill, L. et al. (1996) Amygdala activity at encoding correlated with longterm, free recall of emotional information. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 93, 8016–8021 h Hamann, S.B. et al. (1999) Amygdala activity related to enhanced memory for pleasant and aversive stimuli. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 289–293 and subcortical structures involved in functions as diverse as memory, perception, behavioral state control and homeostatic regulation [5]. Accordingly, lesion and imaging studies indicate that the amygdala takes part in wide variety of functions. These include learning in positively and negatively motivated tasks, the expression of unconditioned fear responses, identification of the emotional content of sensory stimuli, and aspects of social cognition [6]. Moreover, much data suggest that the amygdala enhances memory consolidation in emotionally arousing conditions [4]. Briefly, neuromodulators released in emotionally arousing conditions appear to alter the activity of basolateral amygdala neurons in the hours after the learning episode. In turn, these changes would facilitate synaptic plasticity elsewhere in the brain (see Box 3). At present, the contribution of the amygdala to these multiple functions is unclear. One possibility is that the amygdala simply relays the results of computations performed by afferent structures to cortical and subcortical effectors. According to this view, the functional heterogeneity of the amygdala would reflect its promiscuous connections. Alternatively, the amygdala might perform computations that are critical to these functions. To gain insight into this issue, one approach is to examine how faithfully the amygdala reflects neuronal events taking place in its cortical and thalamic afferents. The former ‘relay’view would predict http://tics.trends.com close similarities between corticothalamic and amygdala activity. The latter view would be more consistent with the presence of relatively independent rhythms. Because most cortical and thalamic inputs end in the basolateral (BL) complex of the amygdala (namely the lateral, basolateral and basomedial nuclei), the rest of this review will focus on this group of nuclei. In terms of cortical connectivity, the BL complex is intriguing because it is reciprocally connected to cortical regions of differing phylogenetic origins. On the one hand, it derives inputs from high-order parasensory associative neocortical areas [7]. On the other hand, it also has strong connections with phylogenetically older cortical structures of the temporal lobe that are believed to play a crucial role in declarative memory. These include the rhinal cortices, and all fields of the hippocampus with the exception of the dentate gyrus [8]. Therefore, an intriguing question is to determine whether the BL complex exhibits spontaneous oscillations characteristic of parasensory neocortical areas or of these more ancient temporallobe structures. Before we turn to this issue, however, we will review the neocortical and hippocampal EEGs. EEG correlates of behavioral states in the neocortex and hippocampus The neocortex and hippocampus show contrasting activity patterns depending on the subject’s state of vigilance. Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.6 No.7 July 2002 Neocortex During the states of wakefulness and paradoxical sleep, the neocortical EEG is dominated by low-amplitude, high-frequency activity (so-called ‘activated EEG’). By contrast, slow waves of high amplitude dominate the neocortical EEG during slow-wave sleep (SWS) (called ‘synchronized EEG’). Although spectral analyses of the neocortical EEG reveal a continuum of frequencies during SWS, it includes readily identifiable components. The lowest frequency component (<1 Hz) is termed ‘slow oscillation’ [2]. This oscillation was first described in the neocortex of anesthetized animals [9] and, later, during SWS in cats [10] and humans [11]. Intracellular recordings have shown that the slow neocortical oscillation consists of an EEG depthnegative phase coinciding with increased firing in all classes of cortical cells, and an EEG depth-positive phase associated with neuronal silence [2]. This oscillation also occurs in subcortical structures, such as the thalamus and striatum [12,13], but is dependent on cortical inputs [14]. A second readily identifiable component are delta waves (1–4 Hz), which seem to become more prominent as SWS progresses [15,16]. The origin of delta waves remains unclear. Single thalamocortical cells can intrinsically oscillate at this frequency by virtue of the interplay between two of their ionic currents [17]. However, delta waves would not emerge in the EEG as a spatially synchronized phenomenon unless the activity of thalamocortical neurons were synchronized. Cortical feedback and activity of the reticular thalamic nucleus are among the likely synchronizing influences [18]. A third component of the neocortical EEG during SWS are sleep spindles. Spindles are brief periods (1–2 s) of waxing and waning oscillations at 7–14 Hz, which recur more or less synchronously every 3–10 s in much of the neocortex, but are especially obvious in Brodmann areas 4–6 and areas 5–7 [19,20]. Spindles are generated in the thalamus: they disappear from cortical areas whose thalamic inputs have been severed [19], and persist in the thalamus after decortication coupled to an upper brainstem transection [21]. Although the intrathalamic mechanisms underlying spindle genesis are still debated [2,22], they are believed to involve reciprocal interactions between GABAergic reticular thalamic neurons and thalamocortical cells [2]. The slow oscillation component can often be seen to entrain faster EEG components [23]. Indeed, the likelihood of observing sleep spindles or delta waves increases during the depth-negative phase of the slow oscillation, as if the synchronized discharge of cortical neurons triggered spindles and delta oscillation. In addition, even faster rhythms (beta and gamma oscillations) are also more readily observed during the depth-negative phase of the slow oscillation [10]. However, these faster oscillations seem more prominent in waking and http://tics.trends.com 309 paradoxical sleep, because of the disappearance of slow EEG components [2]. To the best of our knowledge, no attempt has been made to determine whether the various rhythms observed in SWS differentially recruit cortical cells that were involved in prior adaptive computations during the waking state. Because neuronal activity in SWS is hypothesized to play a crucial role in memory consolidation (Box 1), this issue constitutes an important challenge for future investigations. Hippocampus During arousal (e.g. produced by noxious stimuli), locomotion and paradoxical sleep, the hippocampal EEG is dominated by a high-amplitude oscillation in the theta range (4–8 Hz), which modulates the activity of essentially all types of hippocampal neurons [3,24]. Despite intense investigation, the mechanisms underlying theta generation remain unclear [25]. However, it is established that hippocampal theta requires intact connections with the medial septum [26], and that it depends on the rhythmic activity of entorhinal afferents and CA3 Schaffer collaterals [25]. Moreover, hippocampal theta is associated with cyclical amplitudemodulation of gamma waves in the hilus, entorhinal cortex and CA1 [27–29]. During SWS, the hippocampal EEG displays highamplitude slow waves of various frequencies but no spindles. In further contrast to the neocortical EEG, hippocampal recordings obtained during SWS, immobility and consummatory behaviors (e.g. eating, grooming) exhibit population events, termed ‘sharp waves’, believed to result from population bursts in CA3 and CA1 pyramidal neurons [30]. Sharp waves appear to trigger, or at least be associated with, an increase in the amplitude of fast oscillations (≈200 Hz), termed ‘ripples’ [31,32]. Ripples are believed to result from the synchronized discharges of inhibitory interneurons. A significant correlation between the occurrence of spindles in the neocortex and sharp waves/ripples in the hippocampus has been reported [33]. However, this correlation accounted for only a small change in firing probability. This is in contrast to the dramatic increases in firing probability evident in hippocampal neurons in relation to sharp-wave ripples, and in neocortical neurons in relation to spindles. Thus, different behavioral states have different EEG correlates in the neocortex and hippocampus. As the BL complex of the amygdala receives afferents from these two sets of structures, it has, in principle at least, the possibility of expressing both sets of EEG rhythms. If, as is currently believed (Box 1), neuronal oscillations do contribute to memory consolidation, understanding how neocortical and hippocampal rhythms interact with amygdala activity is of prime importance, as this interaction will shed light on how the amygdala facilitates consolidation of emotional memories. 310 Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.6 No.7 July 2002 (a) (c) LA1 0.5 mV LA LA2 12 1 2 PRH1 PRH 3 PRH2 PRH3 2s (d) (b) LA 1.2 0.8 0.4 0 0 1.2 40 80 PRH Delta waves 0.8 0.4 0 0.5 s 0 40 80 Frequency (Hz) TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Fig. 1. Synchronized slow oscillatory activity in the EEG in lateral amygdaloid (LA) nucleus and perirhinal (PRH) cortex. (a,b) Slow EEG oscillation shown at two different speeds. Data obtained in a cat anesthetized with ketamine and xylazine. In (b), the top trace in each of the five recordings shows the same bandwidth as in (a); the lower trace was digitally filtered between 20 and 35 Hz. This shows the systematic temporal relationship between the different phases of the slow oscillation and the amplitude of fast rhythms. (c) Position of recording sites on a ventral view of the cat brain. (d) Spectral composition of the focal activity recorded in the LA (top) and PRH cortex (bottom). (Units on the ordinate are arbitrary.) Oscillations in the BL amygdala We will now compare oscillatory activity observed in the neocortex, hippocampal formation and BL amygdala during different behavioral states. As the amygdala and hippocampus receive many of their neocortical inputs indirectly by way of the perirhinal cortex [34], we will also consider spontaneous EEG rhythms displayed by this cortical field. Slow sleep oscillation Although the slow oscillation has been observed in anesthetized rats in the hippocampal formation http://tics.trends.com (D. Henze, personal communication), the phase relationship existing between the hippocampal and neocortical rhythms was not determined. Such an analysis was, however, carried out for the slow oscillations of the neocortex and perirhinal cortices [35]. Although distant neocortical sites showed a coherent slow oscillation, the relationship between neocortical and perirhinal rhythms was irregular. By contrast, the lateral nucleus of the amygdala (LA) and perirhinal cortex generate a highly synchronized slow oscillatory activity despite the fact that the distance between the LA and different longitudinal levels of the perirhinal cortex varies widely (Fig. 1) [36]. In the light of previous findings [12–14], these results suggest that the rhinal cortices can generate slow oscillations independently of the neocortex and impose this slow rhythm on the amygdala. However, more work will be required to settle this issue. Importantly, both perirhinal and LA oscillations are associated with prominent fluctuations in firing probability (Fig. 2), which indicates that these focal oscillations are generated locally and are not volumeconducted from the cortex [36]. Moreover, as in the neocortex, the slow amygdala oscillation is associated with cyclical fluctuations in the amplitude of beta and gamma waves (Figs 1b,d and 2b). However, whereas the slow oscillation remains synchronized even at distant (up to 10 mm) amygdala and perirhinal sites, the coherence of gamma oscillations decreases abruptly with distance [36]. BL amygdala neurons exhibit a clear tendency to oscillate at the delta frequency during SWS [37]. Moreover, the phase relationship is tightly coupled to the delta oscillation of the rhinal cortices. Because a previous study revealed that correlation between rhinal and neocortical oscillations tended to be low [35], it is likely that amygdala delta oscillations also bear little relationship to those in the neocortex. Sleep spindles Whereas sleep spindles are prevalent in the neocortex, they are absent from the BL amygdala [37,38] and rhinal cortices [36]. During neocortical spindles, the amygdala exhibits trains of slow delta waves [37,38]. The lack of spindles in the rhinal cortices is consistent with the comparatively meager thalamic projections to these cortical fields [39]. Moreover, the dorsal thalamic nuclei that project to the entorhinal cortex, namely the reuniens and anterior thalamic nuclei [39,40], do not receive inputs from the reticular thalamic nucleus [41–43], which, as mentioned above, plays a crucial role in the genesis of spindles. Similarly, most of the thalamic nuclei that project to the BL complex [44] do not receive inputs from the reticular thalamic nucleus [41,43]. Thus, compared with the neocortex, the rhinal cortices and BL amygdala depend largely on cortico–cortical connections for the transfer of Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.6 No.7 July 2002 (a) (i) 1s (ii) 0.5 s Normalized frequency (iii) 3.0 nS = 3089 nR = 104 2.5 2.0 1.5 1.0 0.5 0 –500 0 500 1000 Time (ms) 1500 (b) (i) (ii) 0.1 s Normalized frequency 3.0 2.5 nS = 391 nR = 712 2.0 1.5 1.0 0.5 0 –100 –50 0 Time (ms) 50 100 TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Fig. 2. The slow focal oscillation of the lateral nucleus of the amygdala (LA). Data recorded in a cat anesthetized with ketamine and xylazine. (a) Unit activity and superimposed focal activity recorded in the LA shown with a slow (i) and a fast (ii) time base. (iii) Peri-event histogram (PEH) of neuronal discharges for the same cell using the negative peak of slow oscillations in focal waves as zero on the time axis. nS, number of spikes; nR, number of reference peaks. (b) Gamma-related modulation of firing probability in the LA. (i) Firing of a LA neuron (top trace) and fast focal activity (bottom trace) recorded simultaneously by the same microelectrode. (ii) PEH of neuronal discharges (1 ms bins) using the negative peak of gamma waves as zero time. http://tics.trends.com 311 thalamically generated spindle oscillations. In this context, the lack of spindle oscillations in these areas suggests that the rhythmic thalamic volleys accompanying spindles are gradually disorganized as they are transmitted through successive cortico–cortical links. Alternatively, it is possible that the neocortical inputs converging in the amygdala and rhinal cortices are insufficiently synchronized for EEG spindles to emerge. To our knowledge, the relationship between hippocampal sharp waves and amygdala activity has not been investigated. However, it has been found that during SWS, and under barbiturate anesthesia, the BL amygdala generates synchronized population bursts (Fig. 3a) [45]. These synchronized population discharges give rise to brief, large-amplitude potentials (termed ‘sharp potentials’) in the rhinal cortices [35,45] and, after a brief delay, in the dentate gyrus [35]. These sharp dentate potentials are associated with a marked increase in the amplitude of fast oscillations around 80 Hz in the dentate gyrus and rhinal cortices (Fig. 3b) [45]. These events might correspond to the dentate spikes previously described in rats [46]. Theta During paradoxical sleep, theta activity is readily observed in the BL amygdala [37] and perirhinal cortex [35], although in both structures it remains less prominent than the theta oscillation of the entorhinal cortex and hippocampus. For instance, Collins et al.’s, found that as many as 46% of entorhinal neurons displayed a statistically significant modulation in firing rate in this frequency range compared with only 16% in the perirhinal cortex [35]. In the perirhinal cortex at least, the theta oscillation is related to a cyclical modulation in the amplitude of gamma waves, as in the hippocampus. Moreover, perirhinal and amygdala theta is phase locked to entorhinal [35,37], and thus to hippocampal, theta. During the waking state, the theta oscillation is prominent in the amygdala only during periods of intense arousal, as produced by the anticipation of a noxious stimulus (Fig. 4) [47] or alimentary instrumental conditioning [48]. Moreover, it was reported that arousal was associated with an increased coherence of theta activity in the amygdala and frontal cortex [48]. Three non-exclusive factors probably contribute to the appearance of theta oscillations in the BL complex during anticipation of noxious stimuli. First, BL neurons are endowed with intrinsic membrane properties that predispose them to oscillate in this range of frequencies [49–51]. Second, the BL complex receives synaptic inputs from the rhinal cortices and hippocampal formation (reviewed in [7]) where rhythmic neuronal activity in the theta range has been observed [3,35,52,53]. Third, the BL amygdala receives inputs from thalamic nuclei, such as the anterior thalamic nuclei and nucleus reuniens, that 312 Fig. 3. Recordings from the basolateral amygdaloid nucleus (BL) during slow-wave sleep. (a) Simultaneously recorded BL neuron and bipolar entorhinal EEG. The firing rate of the BL neuron is closely correlated with the entorhinal sharp potentials. (b) Sharp potential recorded in the rhinal cortex shown here to coincide with an increase in the amplitude of the underlying fast rhythms. Entorhinal EEG was digitally filtered between 70 and 120 Hz. The traces were aligned with reference to the peak of sharp potentials. Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.6 No.7 July 2002 (a) (b) BL Entorhinal cortex EEG 0.1 s 0.1 s TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences could transmit the hippocampal theta rhythm to the amygdala [44,54]. Fast activities during arousal Several studies have reported that arousal is accompanied by increases in the amplitude of fast focal activities in general [55], and of ‘amygdala spindles’ in particular [56]. Although they have the typical spindle shape, these events should not be confused with sleep spindles because they occur during a different behavioral state and comprise much faster waves (in the beta/gamma range), as previously described in the adjacent pre-pyriform cortex [57]. Whether amygdala spindles are volume conducted from the pre-pyriform cortex or reflect a true amygdala rhythm remains unknown. Significance of oscillatory activity The findings reviewed above indicate that the amygdala reflects a unique pattern of statedependent oscillations. Like the rest of the brain, focal waves recorded in the amygdala during slow-wave sleep are dominated by waves of high amplitude and low frequencies. By contrast, faster activities of lower amplitude are predominant during wakefulness and paradoxical sleep. Similarities end here however. The lack of sleep spindles in the amygdala, the variable temporal relations seen between the slow amygdala and faster neocortical oscillations, and the presence of theta during EEG-activated states, all suggest that the amygdala is functionally closer to the hippocampal formation and rhinal cortices than to the neocortex. These findings take on a particular significance when considered in the light of data indicating that the amygdala facilitates consolidation of emotionally arousing memories [4]. Although previous work has emphasized the individual contributions of the amygdala and hippocampal system to distinct forms of memory, the functional similarities and reciprocal http://tics.trends.com connections between these structures suggest that they engage in cooperative interactions. Because theta oscillations are present in the amygdala only during emotional arousal, they represent a likely physiological substrate for the facilitated consolidation of emotional memories by the amygdala. Importantly, it should be noted that amygdala theta occurs in conditions that can have a negative [47] or a positive [48] valence, which is consistent with anecdotal reports of improved memory for circumstances surrounding both traumatic and happy events. Therefore, we propose that the theta activity of amygdala neurons during emotional arousal promotes memory by facilitating interactions between neocortical storage sites and the declarative memory system of the temporal lobe. How would periodic amygdala activity at the theta frequency (4–8 Hz) play this role? First, it should be noted that glutamatergic projection neurons of the BL complex have extremely low firing rates [37], even during emotional arousal [47]. Thus, the temporal clustering of neuronal discharges at the theta frequency greatly enhances the depolarization produced by BL activity on target structures. Second, much of the temporal lobe shows theta frequency oscillation during emotional arousal [37,53], and amygdala and hippocampal theta are highly correlated [37]. Third, coherent oscillations cause short recurring time windows that facilitate synaptic interactions between phase-locked oscillators. And fourth, coincident pre- and post-synaptic activity is crucial to synaptic plasticity [58]. Thus, we suggest that, by telescoping the periods of effective synaptic interactions in short time windows, amygdala oscillations at the theta frequency exert a depolarizing action that promotes synaptic plasticity in co-active structures of the temporal lobe and neocortex. Consistent with this idea, the conduction times of BL axons to the rhinal cortices adjust to compensate for variations in Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.6 No.7 July 2002 Tone on (a) 313 (b) Pre-tones Silence 160 Power Arterial pressure Tone off 120 80 0 10 15 20 Time (s) 25 0 30 Pre-tones 5 10 15 Frequency (Hz) 20 Silent period n = 59 nR = 442 nT = 517 6 4 2 Relative frequency Relative frequency (c) 5 n = 59 nR = 885 nT = 972 6 4 2 0 0 –400 –200 0 200 ms Cross-correlation 400 –400 –200 0 200 ms Cross-correlation 400 TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Acknowledgements We thank P. Giguère and D. Drolet for technical support. Supported by research grants to D.P. from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research and the Center for Molecular and Behavioral Neuroscience of Rutgers University. J.G.P. was supported by a scholarship from FCAR. consolidation of emotional memories. According to this view, despite storage facilitation by amygdala theta during wakefulness, representations would remain labile. Subsequent sleep activity would, through a still undefined mechanism, consolidate these representations. This view is analogous to the two-stage model of episodic memory (Box 1). However, in contrast with that model, we do not hypothesize that the amygdala stores a trace of waking activity patterns. Sleep events would not ‘replay’ waking activities, but would recruit cortical neurons randomly. Specificity of the consolidation process might be ensured by activity-dependent ‘labeling’ of particular groups of synapses in wakefulness. Fig. 4. Theta rhythm in the lateral amygdala (LA) associated with anticipation of a noxious stimulus. (a) Behavioral paradigm: cats learned that a series of tones interrupted by a period of silence (5 s) preceded the administration of a footshock. Changes in arterial blood pressure (mm Hg) observed in naive (lower trace) and trained cats (top trace). During the silent period, trained cats showed increases in blood pressure, indicating that they anticipated the noxious stimulus, a state anthropomorphically interpreted as fear. (b) Simultaneously recorded sites in the basolateral amygdala complex showing increased correlation in the theta band during fear. Cross-correlograms of local LA field potentials before the tones and during the silent period. Focal waves were digitally filtered from 2 to 55 Hz and divided in 1 s segments sliding in steps of 100 ms over the 5 s epoch preceding the tones or the silent period. Note marked increase in the theta frequency range (4–8 Hz). (c) Cross-correlation of neuronal discharges generated by simultaneously recorded LA neurons in control conditions (left) and during anticipation of the noxious stimulus (right). Note increased synchrony and theta rhythm in the anticipatory state. n, number of cell pairs; nR, number of spikes generated by the reference cells; nT, number of spikes generated by the test cells. Conclusion distance between the BL complex and distinct rostrocaudal rhinal sites [59]. As a result, BL neurons can generate simultaneous rhythmic depolarizations at spatially distributed rhinal sites and facilitate Hebbian associations between coincident activity patterns. In rats, intra-amygdala lidocaine injections up to six hours post-learning interfere with the facilitating effects of emotion on recall days later [60]. Because rats normally spend significant amounts of time sleeping, and undoubtedly would have slept in the six hours post-learning, this finding raises the possibility that the synchronized neuronal activity of amygdala neurons during sleep also contributes to the Compared with the neocortex and hippocampus, the study of neuronal oscillations in the amygdala is in its infancy. However, recent progress indicates that, like these better known structures, the amygdala exhibits a rich repertoire of oscillations. Understanding the role of these oscillations in synaptic plasticity is a major challenge. Undoubtedly, studying the relationship between population rhythms and coding by amygdala neurons will generate valuable insights into this issue. In addition, the study of neuronal oscillations might offer important clues about the much-debated affiliations of the various amygdala nuclei. That is, studying the coherence of oscillations in different amygdala nuclei might serve as a functional assay to examine the degree of kinship between them. http://tics.trends.com 314 Review References 1 Llinás, R.R. (1988) The intrinsic electrophysiological properties of mammalian neurons: insights into central nervous system function. Science 242, 1654–1664 2 Steriade, M. (1997) Synchronized activities of coupled oscillators in the cerebral cortex and thalamus at different levels of vigilance. Cereb. Cortex 7, 583–604 3 Buzsáki, G. et al. (1983) Cellular bases of hippocampal EEG in the behaving rat. Brain Res. Rev. 6, 139–171 4 Cahill, L. and McGaugh, J.L. (1998) Mechanisms of emotional arousal and lasting declarative memory. Trends Neurosci. 21, 294–299 5 Amaral, D.G. et al. (1992) Anatomical organization of the primate amygdaloid complex. In The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction (Aggleton, J.P., ed.), pp. 1–66, John Wiley & Sons 6 Aggleton, J.P., ed. (2000) The Amygdala: a Functional Analysis, Oxford University Press 7 McDonald, A.J. (1998) Cortical pathways to the mammalian amygdala. Prog. Neurobiol. 55, 257–332 8 Petrovich, G.D. et al. (2001) Combinatorial amygdalar inputs to hippocampal domains and hypothalamic behavior systems. Brain Res. Rev. 38, 247–289 9 Steriade, M. et al. (1993) A novel slow (<1 Hz) oscillation of neocortical neurons in vivo: depolarizing and hyperpolarizing components. J. Neurosci. 13, 3252–3265 10 Steriade, M. et al. (1996) Synchronization of fast (30–40 Hz) spontaneous cortical rhythms during brain activation. J. Neurosci. 16, 392–417 11 Achermann, P. and Borbély, A.A. (1997) Lowfrequency (<1 Hz) oscillations in the human sleep electroencephalogram. Neuroscience 81, 213–222 12 Steriade, M. et al. (1993) The slow (<1 Hz) oscillation in reticular thalamic and thalamocortical neurons: scenario of sleep rhythm generation in interacting thalamic and neocortical networks. J. Neurosci. 13, 3284–3299 13 Wilson, C.J. and Kawaguchi, Y. (1996) The origins of two-state spontaneous membrane potential fluctuations of neostriatal spiny neurons. J. Neurosci. 16, 2397–2410 14 Timofeev, I. and Steriade, M. (1996) Low frequency rhythms in the thalamus of intact-cortex and decorticated cats. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 4152–4168 15 Ball, G.J. et al. (1977) The cortical electromicrophysiology of pathological delta waves in the electroencephalogram of cats. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 43, 346–361 16 Petsche, H. et al. (1984) On the search for the sources of the electroencephalogram. Neuroscience 11, 1–27 17 McCormick, D.A. and Pape, H.C. (1990) Properties of a hyperpolarization-activated cation current and its role in rhythmic oscillation in thalamic relay neurones. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 431, 291–318 18 Steriade, M. et al. (1993) Thalamocortical oscillations in the sleeping brain. Science 262, 679–685 19 Andersen, P. and Andersson, S.A. (1968) Physiological Basis of the Alpha Rhythm, Appleton–Century–Crofts 20 Morison, R.S. and Dempsey, E.W. (1942) A study of thalamocortical relations. Am. J. Physiol. 135, 281–292 21 Morison, R.S. and Bassett, D.L. (1945) Electrical activity of the thalamus and basal ganglia in decorticate cats. J. Neurophysiol. 8, 309–314 http://tics.trends.com TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.6 No.7 July 2002 22 Von Krosigk, M. et al. (1993) Cellular mechanisms of a synchronized oscillation in the thalamus. Science 261, 361–364 23 Steriade, M. et al. (1993) Intracellular analysis of relations between the slow (<1 Hz) neocortical oscillation and other sleep rhythms of the electroencephalogram. J. Neurosci. 13, 3266–3283 24 Bland, B.H. et al. (1975) Two generators of hippocampal theta activity in rabbits. Exp. Brain Res. 94, 199–218 25 Buzsáki, G. (2002) Theta oscillations in the hippocampus. Neuron 33, 325–340 26 Petsche, H. et al. (1962) The significance of the rabbit’s septum as a relay station between midbrain and the hippocampus: I. The control of hippocampus arousal activity by the septum cells. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 450, 127–142 27 Bragin, A. et al. (1995) Gamma (40-100 Hz) oscillation in the hippocampus of the behaving rat. J. Neurosci. 15, 47–60 28 Chrobak, J.J. and Buzsáki, G. (1996) Highfrequency oscillations in the output networks of the hippocampal-entorhinal axis of the freely behaving rat. J. Neurosci. 16, 3056–3066 29 Chrobak, J.J. and Buzsáki, G. (1998) Gamma oscillations in the entorhinal cortex of the freely behaving rat. J. Neurosci. 18, 388–398 30 Buzsáki, G. (1986) Hippocampal sharp waves: their origin and significance. Brain Res. 398, 242–252 31 Buzsáki, G. et al. (1992) High-frequency network oscillation in the hippocampus. Science 256, 1025–1027 32 Ylinen, A. et al. (1995) Sharp wave-associated high-frequency oscillation (200 Hz) in the intact hippocampus: network and intracellular mechanisms. J. Neurosci. 5, 78–90 33 Siapas, A.G. and Wilson, M.A. (1998) Coordinated interactions between hippocampal ripples and cortical spindles during slow-wave sleep. Neuron 21, 1123–1128 34 Suzuki, W.A. (1996) The anatomy, physiology and functions of the perirhinal cortex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 6, 179–186 35 Collins, D.R. et al. (1999) Spontaneous activity of the perirhinal cortex in behaving cats. Neuroscience 89, 1025–1039 36 Collins, D.R. et al. (2001) Slow and fast (gamma) neuronal oscillations in the perirhinal cortex and lateral amygdala. J. Neurophysiol. 85, 1661–1672 37 Paré, D. and Gaudreau, H. (1996) Projection cells and interneurons of the lateral and basolateral amygdala: distinct firing patterns and differential relation to theta and delta rhythms in conscious cats. J. Neurosci. 16, 3334–3350 38 Forslid, A. et al. (1986) Observations on normal EEG activity in different brain regions of the unrestrained swine. Acta Physiol. Scand. 128, 389–396 39 Room, P. and Groenewegen, H.J. (1986) Connections of the parahippocampal cortex in the cat: II. Subcortical afferents. J. Comp. Neurol. 251, 451–473 40 Herkenham, M. (1978) The connections of the nucleus reuniens thalami: evidence for a direct thalamo-hippocampal pathway in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 177, 589–610 41 Steriade, M. et al. (1984) Thalamic projections of nucleus reticularis thalami of cat: a study using retrograde transport of horseradish peroxidase and double fluorescent tracers. J. Comp. Neurol. 229, 531–547 42 Paré, D. et al. (1987) Physiological characteristics of anterior thalamic nuclei, a group devoid of inputs from the reticular thalamic nucleus. J. Neurophysiol. 57, 1669–1685 43 Velayos, J.L. et al. (1989) Topographical organization of the projections from the reticular thalamic nucleus to the intralaminar and medial thalamic nuclei in the cat. J. Comp. Neurol. 279, 457–469 44 Turner, B.H. and Herkenham, M. (1991) Thalamoamygdaloid projections in the rat: a test of the amygdala’s role in sensory processing. J. Comp. Neurol. 313, 295–325 45 Paré, D. et al. (1995) Amygdalo-entorhinal relations and their reflection in the hippocampal formation: generation of sharp sleep potentials. J. Neurosci. 15, 2482–2503 46 Bragin, A. et al. (1995) Dentate EEG spikes and associated interneuronal population bursts in the hippocampal hilar region of the rat. J. Neurophysiol. 73, 1691–1705 47 Paré, D. and Collins, D.R. (2000) Neuronal correlates of fear in the lateral amygdala: multiple extracellular recordings in conscious cats. J. Neurosci. 20, 2701–2710 48 Aleksanov, S.S. (1983) Coherent functions of the electrical activity of the hippocampus, amygdala and frontal cortex during alimentary instrumental reflexes in the dog. Zh. Vyssh. Nerv. Deiat. Im. I. P. Pavlova 33, 694–699 49 Paré, D. et al. (1995) Bursting and oscillating neurons of the cat basolateral amygdaloid complex in vivo: electrophysiological properties and morphological features. J. Neurophysiol. 74, 1179–1191 50 Pape, H.C. and Driesang, R.B. (1998) Ionic mechanisms of intrinsic oscillations in neurons of the basolateral amygdaloid complex. J. Neurophysiol. 79, 217–226 51 Pape, H.C. et al. (1998) Two types of intrinsic oscillations in neurons of the lateral and basolateral nuclei of the amygdala. J. Neurophysiol. 79, 205–216 52 Mitchell, S. and Ranck, J.B. (1980) Generation of theta rhythm in medial entorhinal cortex of freely moving rats. Brain Res. 189, 49–66 53 Alonso, A. and García-Austt, E. (1987) Neuronal sources of theta rhythm in the entorhinal cortex of the rat. Exp. Brain Res. 67, 493–501 54 Vertes, R.P. et al. (2001) Theta-rhythmically firing neurons in the anterior thalamus: implications for mnemonic functions of Papez’s circuit. Neuroscience 104, 619–625 55 Pagano, R.R. et al. (1964) Amygdala activity: a central measure of arousal. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 17, 255–260 56 Feschenko, V.A. and Chilingaryan, L.I. (1990) Dependence of electrical activity of the amygdaloid complex on level of motivation and emotional state of the dog. Neurosci. Behav. Physiol. 20, 506–513 57 Freeman, W.J. (1959) Distribution in time and space of prepyriform electrical activity. J. Neurophysiol. 22, 644–665 58 Bliss, T. and Collingridge, G.L. (1993) A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature 361, 31–39 59 Pelletier, J.G. and Paré, D. (2002) Uniform range of conduction times from the lateral amygdala to distributed perirhinal sites. J. Neurophysiol. 87, 1213–1221 60 Parent, M.B. and McGaugh, J.L. (1994) Posttraining infusion of lidocaine into the amygdala basolateral complex impairs retention of inhibitory avoidance training. Brain Res. 661, 97–103