* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Similar Inhibitory Processes Dominate the Responses of Cat Lateral
Neuroplasticity wikipedia , lookup
Emotional lateralization wikipedia , lookup
Endocannabinoid system wikipedia , lookup
Response priming wikipedia , lookup
End-plate potential wikipedia , lookup
Transcranial direct-current stimulation wikipedia , lookup
Biological neuron model wikipedia , lookup
Development of the nervous system wikipedia , lookup
Neurotransmitter wikipedia , lookup
Psychophysics wikipedia , lookup
Metastability in the brain wikipedia , lookup
Apical dendrite wikipedia , lookup
Neuroanatomy wikipedia , lookup
Clinical neurochemistry wikipedia , lookup
Electrophysiology wikipedia , lookup
Synaptogenesis wikipedia , lookup
Neural oscillation wikipedia , lookup
Central pattern generator wikipedia , lookup
Activity-dependent plasticity wikipedia , lookup
Caridoid escape reaction wikipedia , lookup
Nervous system network models wikipedia , lookup
Neural correlates of consciousness wikipedia , lookup
Neural coding wikipedia , lookup
Molecular neuroscience wikipedia , lookup
Multielectrode array wikipedia , lookup
Hypothalamus wikipedia , lookup
Eyeblink conditioning wikipedia , lookup
Premovement neuronal activity wikipedia , lookup
Nonsynaptic plasticity wikipedia , lookup
Neuropsychopharmacology wikipedia , lookup
Neurostimulation wikipedia , lookup
Single-unit recording wikipedia , lookup
Spike-and-wave wikipedia , lookup
Chemical synapse wikipedia , lookup
Stimulus (physiology) wikipedia , lookup
Pre-Bötzinger complex wikipedia , lookup
Channelrhodopsin wikipedia , lookup
Optogenetics wikipedia , lookup
Synaptic gating wikipedia , lookup
Similar Inhibitory Processes Dominate the Responses of Cat Lateral Amygdaloid Projection Neurons to Their Various Afferents E. J. LANG AND D. PARÉ Département de Physiologie, Université Laval, Québec, Québec G1K 7P4, Canada INTRODUCTION The amygdala is critical for imbuing sensory stimuli with the appropriate emotional significance (Adolphs et al. 1995; Bechara et al. 1995). The lateral amygdaloid (LAT) nucleus, a major recipient of cortical and thalamic sensory pathways to the amygdala (Amaral et al. 1992; LeDoux et al. 1985; Romanski and LeDoux 1992; Russchen 1982b; Turner et al. 1980) and source of afferents to other amygdaloid nuclei (Krettek and Price 1978; Pitkänen et al. 1995; Smith and Paré 1994; Stefanacci et al. 1992), appears to be a necessary link for the development of auditory conditioned fear responses (LeDoux et al. 1986, 1990). Characterization of the mechanisms governing LAT neuronal activity therefore is required for understanding the neuronal basis of these responses and more generally of emotional expression. Recent evidence suggests that local inhibitory processes are major determinants of LAT neuronal activity and are central to its normal functioning as, for example, a decrease in GABAergic neurons within this structure is correlated with the development of kindled seizures (Callahan et al. 1991). In single unit studies, LAT neurons were found to have very low firing rates (Ben-Ari et al. 1974; Bordi et al. 1993), with the majority of projection neurons being virtually silent unless presented with a specific sensory stimulus (Gaudreau and Paré 1996). Moreover, in vitro studies have shown that synaptic responses in the basolateral amygdaloid (BL) complex are characterized by large inhibitory postsynaptic potentials (IPSPs) (Rainnie et al. 1991b; Sugita et al. 1993; Takagi and Yamamoto 1981; Washburn and Moises 1992). In agreement with these physiological findings, synaptic boutons immunoreactive for glutamic acid decarboxylase (GAD) are concentrated strategically around the soma, proximal dendrites and axonal initial segment of amygdaloid projection cells (Carlsen 1988). Inhibition within the LAT nucleus must arise largely from GABAergic neurons located within the LAT nucleus itself, as lesions deafferenting the BL complex lead to minor decreases in GAD levels (Le Gal La Salle et al. 1978). Moreover, although other amygdaloid nuclei contain GABAergic interneurons (McDonald 1985; Nitecka and Ben-Ari 1987; Paré and Smith 1993), internuclear projections linking the different nuclei of the BL complex appear to consist only of excitatory projections (Paré et al. 1995c; Smith and Paré 1994). An intriguing finding, therefore, was that both spontaneous and evoked g-aminobuturic acid-A (GABAA ) and GABAB responses could occur independently in LAT neurons recorded in vitro (Sugita et al. 1992), suggesting that the LAT nucleus contains two distinct GABAergic neuronal populations, each having access to different types of GABAergic receptors. Given the importance of inhibitory processes in the functioning of the amygdala, and to test the possibility that separate interneuronal populations are accessed differentially by 0022-3077/97 $5.00 Copyright q 1997 The American Physiological Society / 9k0b$$ja43 08-13-97 18:02:07 neupal LP-Neurophys 341 Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.1 on May 2, 2017 Lang, E. J. and D. Paré . Similar inhibitory processes dominate the responses of cat lateral amygdaloid projection neurons to their various afferents. J. Neurophysiol. 77: 341 – 352, 1997. To investigate the impact of inhibitory processes on responses of lateral amygdaloid ( LAT ) neurons, intracellular recordings were obtained from identified LAT projection neurons in barbiturateanesthetized cats. Synaptic responses evoked by perirhinal ( PRH ) , entorhinal ( ENT ) , basomedial, and LAT stimulation were investigated. Regardless of stimulation site, responses consisted of an excitatory postsynaptic potential (EPSP ) that either preceded and was truncated by an inhibitory postsynaptic potential ( IPSP) or occurred just after the IPSP onset. IPSPs were monophasic, lasted hundreds of milliseconds, and were of such large amplitude and rapid onset that they effectively opposed the EPSPs, generally preventing orthodromic spikes. All sites elicited IPSPs with relatively negative reversal potentials around 085 mV. Experiments analyzing the underlying ionic mechanisms are presented in the companion paper. Evoked responses were similar to synaptic potentials associated with spontaneous EEG events, known as simple ( small, monophasic ) and complex (large, triphasic ) ENT sharp potentials ( SPs) , with no difference between the reversals of evoked and SP-related IPSPs ( 083.2 { 2.7 mV ) . IPSPs coinciding with complex SPs truncated SP-related EPSPs more rapidly and had larger amplitudes and longer durations than those related to simple SPs. These differences reflected the fact that the amplitude and duration of SP-related IPSPs were correlated with SP amplitude. Similar variations were reproduced in evoked IPSPs by varying the stimulus intensity. Low intensities generated predominantly excitatory responses consisting of EPSPs sometimes followed by small IPSPs, whereas high intensities evoked predominantly inhibitory responses comprised of a large IPSP that truncated or occluded the EPSPs. Orthodromic spikes were elicited only in a narrow range of intermediate intensities. These changes in the evoked response primarily reflected increases in the IPSP evoked at high intensities. PRH stimulation at different rostro-caudal levels demonstrated that rostral sites elicited larger EPSPs and IPSPs with shorter latencies and longer durations than caudal sites. These differences probably reflect contrasting patterns of activity spread through the PRH cortex, suggesting that the intact cortical circuitry allowed a temporally distributed activation of inhibitory interneurons and thereby partly explains the long duration and monophasic nature of the IPSPs. Inhibition, thus, plays a primary role in shaping LAT neuronal responses. The profuse intrinsic connectivity of the LAT nucleus and parahippocampal cortices may underlie the relatively invariant response pattern of LAT neurons and suggests a common mode of information processing, based upon quantitative, rather than qualitative, differences in activation of LAT circuitry. Therefore we propose that effective transmission of signals through the LAT nucleus may require activation of specifically sized neuronal ensembles, rather than widespread afferent excitation. 342 E. J. LANG AND D. PARÉ particular amygdaloid afferents, we sought to characterize the IPSPs of intracellularly recorded LAT projection neurons in vivo. Our results demonstrate that powerful IPSPs regulate the response of LAT neurons to their synaptic inputs and that their response pattern is relatively constant irrespective of the stimulation site. Further, evidence was obtained that feedback inhibition within the LAT nucleus is mediated by similar IPSPs. This uniformity suggests that distinct GABAergic subpopulations cannot be independently accessed in vivo. Instead, the degree of activation within a particular afferent pathway was found to be critical in shaping the synaptic responses of LAT neurons. METHODS metrics, OR) data analysis software. For each cell, measurement of IPSP amplitudes were performed at the same time for each potential. This time corresponded to when the IPSP reached its maximal amplitude at the most depolarized tested level. The IPSP reversal potential was then determined by plotting the IPSP amplitude as a function of Vm . The input resistance (RIN ) before and during an IPSP was estimated by calculation of the slope resistance (the reciprocal of the slope conductance) (Johnston and Wu 1995). Here the change in voltage from the resting Vm was plotted against the DC current level, and the slope of the fitted line was used to estimate the RIN of the cell. Although I-V curves showed deviations from linearity, the comparison of the slope resistance before and during IPSPs remained useful for estimating the impact of IPSPs. Linear fits were performed with the least-squares method as calculated by the computer program IGOR (Wavemetrics, OR). Histology Intracellular recordings were obtained from adult cats (2.5–3.5 kg) anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital (Somnotol, 40 mg/kg ip), paralyzed with gallamine triethiodide (33 mg/kg iv), and ventilated artificially. The level of anesthesia was determined by continuously monitoring the electroencephalograph (EEG), and supplemental doses of Somnotol (5–7 mg/kg iv) were given as needed to maintain a synchronized EEG pattern. Lidocaine (2%) was applied to all skin incisions. End tidal CO2 concentration was kept at 3.7 { 0.2% (mean { SE), and the rectal temperature was maintained at 37–387C with a heating pad. To ensure recording stability, the cisterna magna was drained, the cat suspended, and a bilateral pneumothorax performed. The bone and dura mater overlying the parietal and temporal cortices were removed, allowing a lateral approach to the amygdala and a dorsal approach to the perirhinal (PRH) and entorhinal (ENT) cortices. Pairs of tungsten electrodes (0.5 MV ) whose tips were separated by 1.5 mm in the vertical axis, or concentric bipolar tungsten electrodes were used to record the PRH and ENT EEG activity and to apply local electrical stimuli (100–200 ms pulses, 100–1,500 mA, at 0.2–1 Hz). The tungsten electrodes were implanted stereotactically into the PRH and ENT cortices with one electrode of each pair in the superficial (I-II) and deep cortical layers (IV-V), respectively. The exact dorsoventral coordinate was obtained by lowering the ENT electrodes until they contacted the temporal bone and then raising them to their correct position using the EEG criteria of positive or negative going sharp potentials as indicative of superficial or deep cortical layers, respectively (Paré et al. 1995a). The dorsoventral position of the electrodes in the PRH cortex was corrected as a function of the difference between the stereotaxically predicted (Berman and Jones 1982) and actual positions of the ENT cortex. The ENT electrodes were placed into the ventromedial ENT area. Histological controls were performed to confirm the stimulation electrode positions and the morphology of the recorded cells. At the conclusion of the experiment, the animal was perfused with 500 ml of chilled saline (0.9%) followed by 1 l of a solution of 2% paraformaldehyde and 1% glutaraldehyde in 0.1 M phosphate buffered saline (PBS, pH 7.4). The brain was stored in 30% glucose solution overnight and then transferred to PBS. Coronal sections (80 mm) were cut on a freezing microtome. Neurobiotin filled cells were visualized by incubating the sections in the avidinbiotin-horseradish peroxidase (HRP) solution (ABC Elite Kit, Vector Labs) and processed to reveal the HRP staining (Horikawa and Armstrong 1988). The positions of stimulation electrodes were verified in thionin-stained sections. Recording procedure Intracellular recording electrodes consisted of glass capillary tubes pulled to a tip diameter of É0.5 mm (35–45 MV ). The electrodes were filled with K / -acetate (4 M) and Neurobiotin (14 mg/mL; Vector Labs). They were lowered obliquely 6 mm through the posterior sylvian gyrus to the border of the LAT nucleus using a piezoelectric manipulator. The exposed cortical surfaces then were covered with agar to improve recording stability. Intracellular recordings were made using a high impedance amplifier with active bridge circuitry (Neurodata, NY). Typically, cells were recorded for 30 min to 2 h. After penetration and stabilization of impalements, spontaneous and evoked synaptic potentials were recorded at different membrane potentials (Vms) induced by DC current injection. The intracellular and EEG signals were monitored using a digital oscilloscope, printed out on a chart recorder, and stored on VCR tape for off-line analysis using IGOR (Wave- / 9k0b$$ja43 RESULTS Stable intracellular recordings were obtained from 74 LAT neurons with stable resting potentials greater than or equal to 065 mV ( 072.6 { 1.3 mV; mean { SE; n Å 74), and spike amplitudes ranging between 60 and 90 mV. Eighteen percent of them were identified formally as projection neurons using morphological and/or physiological criteria (Fig. 1). In agreement with previous Golgi studies (for review, McDonald 1992), neurobiotin-filled neurons were considered as projection cells when they had spiny dendrites with a stellate or a modified pyramidal somatodendritic morphology. An example of a morphologically identified LAT neuron is shown in Fig. 1A1, along with a high-power photomicrograph of a spiny dendritic segment (Fig. 1A2). Physiological identification of LAT projection cells rested on eliciting antidromic responses by PRH or ENT stimulation (Fig. 1B). All neurons described in this study displayed similar electroresponsive properties known to be characteristic of LAT projection cells including lack of spontaneous discharges at rest, time- and voltage-dependent rectification, and generation of voltage-dependent oscillations in the 4– 10 Hz range upon steady membrane depolarization to around 065 mV (Pape and Paré 1995; Paré et al. 1995b). Recordings of spontaneous activity revealed synaptic events typically consisting of truncated excitatory postsynaptic potentials (EPSPs) followed by large (5–15 mV) hyperpolarizing potentials. These intracellular events often were related to spontaneous EEG waves in the PRH and ENT cortices, termed sharp potentials (SPs) (Paré et al. 1995a). For brevity, we will henceforth refer to evoked and SPrelated hyperpolarizing potentials as IPSPs; however, we demonstrate in the accompanying paper that they actually 08-13-97 18:02:07 neupal LP-Neurophys Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.1 on May 2, 2017 Surgery INHIBITION IN THE LATERAL AMYGDALOID NUCLEUS 343 FIG . 1. Morphological and physiological identification of lateral amygdaloid (LAT) projection neurons. A1: photomicrograph of a spiny stellate LAT neuron filled with Neurobiotin. A2: high magnification of a spiny dendritic segment. B1: perirhinal (PRH)-evoked antidromic spikes. Superimposed responses to 12 PRH stimuli (arrowhead) showing constant spike latency. Note that spike arises directly from baseline without a preceding excitatory postsynaptic potential (EPSP). B2: superimposed antidromic responses to high-frequency (250 Hz) trains of 3 and 5 PRH stimuli (arrowheads). Note ability of antidromic spikes to follow high-frequency stimulation. Cortical and intra-amygdaloid stimuli evoke similar IPSPs To test whether different afferents generate distinct IPSPs in LAT neurons, synaptic responses were evoked from several brain regions connected with this nucleus, including the PRH and ENT cortices as well as the basomedial (BM) nucleus. The latter was stimulated in an attempt to evoke recurrent inhibition, as this nucleus receives a massive input from the LAT nucleus but does not reciprocate this projection (Krettek and Price 1978). In addition, the LAT nucleus itself was stimulated to assess the influence of intrinsic connections. Histological controls confirmed the electrode placements in these regions (Fig. 3, A–D). In the PRH cortex, a series of four electrodes was placed along its rostrocaudal extent as shown schematically in Fig. 3A1. Cortical stimuli (PRH, Fig. 3A2; ENT, Fig. 3B) evoked similar responses with respect to the PSP sequence, IPSP amplitude, and reversal potential. The latency of cortically evoked IPSPs (from the site eliciting the shortest latency response) was 4.8 { 0.5 ms (range, 2.8–7.3 ms; n Å 8), whereas the BM-evoked IPSPs had a slightly longer latency of 6.6 { 0.9 ms (range, 2.1–10.8 ms; n Å 8). These differences did not reach significance (P õ 0.1). / 9k0b$$ja43 The monophasic IPSPs evoked from cortical and BM sites typically had reversal potentials ranging from 080 to 090 mV (cortex, 084.7 { 1.3 mV, n Å 17; BM, 082.1 { 0.9 mV, n Å 4) and were not significantly different (P õ 0.2). In particular, IPSPs evoked in the same cells by PRH and BM stimulation had closely matched reversal potentials. In the cell of Fig. 4, A and B, for example, PRH-evoked IPSPs (Fig. 4A1) reversed at 083.1 mV, whereas BM-evoked IPSPs (Fig. 4A2) reversed at 081.9 mV, nearly the same potential (Fig. 4A3). In this cell, BM stimulation evoked IPSPs of lower amplitude that were associated with a smaller drop in RIN (55%, Fig. 4B2) than PRH-evoked IPSPs (83%, Fig. 4B1). However, there were large variations between cells in the RIN drops related to IPSPs. In a further attempt to reveal a second, distinct type of inhibitory response, IPSPs were evoked by direct stimulation of the LAT nucleus. However, placement of stimulation electrodes in the LAT nucleus altered the properties of the cells (higher RINs and more depolarized resting potentials and IPSP reversals). Consequently, this stimulation site was used in only two experiments. These IPSPs had a latency of 4 { 0.5 ms (n Å 3), consistently shorter than BM- or cortically evoked IPSPs. However, their reversal potentials ( 080.4 { 0.6 mV, n Å 3) were similar to those of IPSPs elicited by cortical ( 078.9 { 0.7 mV, n Å 3) and BM ( 080.4 { 0.2 mV, n Å 3) stimulation in the same cells. Figure 4C illustrates LAT-evoked responses in a LAT projection cell. In this case, the IPSP reversed around 079.6 mV (Fig. 4C1) and was associated with a 71% drop in RIN (Fig. 4C3). Similarly, ENT- and BM-evoked IPSPs reversed at 077.5 and 080.5 mV and produced 32 and 68% decreases in RIN , respectively. SP-related IPSPs To determine whether the uniform properties of the evoked IPSPs reflected the artificial nature of electrical stimuli, we compared them with synaptic events occurring spontaneously in relation to simple (monophasic) and complex (triphasic) SPs that occur in the ENT and PRH cortices under barbiturate anesthesia (Fig. 2) and during slow-wave sleep (Paré and Gaudreau 1996; Paré et al. 1995a). The 08-13-97 18:02:07 neupal LP-Neurophys Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.1 on May 2, 2017 are generated by a combination of synaptic and synapticallyactivated intrinsic conductances (Lang and Paré 1997). An example is shown in Fig. 2B1 where the intracellular trace (INTRA) displays numerous IPSPs that occurred in association with SPs (Fig. 2B1, top; ENT) or in response to a PRH stimulus (arrowhead). Depolarization to 060 mV with /0.6 nA led to sustained spiking (Fig. 2A), which was interrupted by large SP-related IPSPs (curved arrows) lasting °500 ms. The IPSPs could occur in relation to either simple (monophasic, Fig. 2C1) or complex (triphasic, Fig. 2C2) SPs, as demonstrated by perievent averages of intracellular events using the negative peak (arrows in Fig. 2C) of SPs as a temporal reference. Cortically evoked responses were similar to SP-related intracellular potentials in amplitude and duration (Fig. 2B2). Like SP-related IPSPs, cortically evoked IPSPs often had small postsynaptic potentials (PSPs) embedded within them (Fig. 2D, arrowheads). 344 E. J. LANG AND D. PARÉ average reversal potential of SP-related IPSPs was 083.8 { 2.7 mV (n Å 7), which was not statistically different from the reversal potential of IPSPs evoked by cortical and BM stimuli (P ú 0.50). Furthermore, there was a high correlation between the reversal potential of evoked and SP-related IPSPs (r Å 0.87, P õ 0.05) in the same cells. Additionally, the reversal potentials of simple and complex SP-related IPSPs were compared and found to be nearly identical ( 086.3 { 5.9 mV, simple SP-related; 086.4 { 5 mV, complex SP-related; n Å 3). Simple and complex SP-related IPSPs recorded at different Vms in the same cell are shown in Fig. 5B. The IPSPs were aligned using the negative peak of the related SPs shown in Fig. 5A. In this cell, simple and complex SPrelated IPSPs reversed at similar values of 093.5 and 091 mV, respectively. ENT-evoked responses also reversed at a relatively negative value of 087.5 mV. It was found consistently that the amplitude of SP-related IPSPs was related to the size of the SP. This relationship was most clearly observed when comparing simple and complex SP-related IPSPs because of the large amplitude difference between the two SP types (Figs. 2, A and B, 5, and 6). As shown in the peri-SP averages of Fig. 2C, IPSPs related to simple SPs had a lower amplitude than those related to complex SPs (Simple, 2.2 { 0.2 mV; Complex, 6.9 { 0.5 mV, n Å 6 at 065 mV; P õ 0.00005, paired ttest). In addition to their larger amplitude, complex SPrelated IPSPs curtailed the initial EPSP more rapidly and / 9k0b$$ja43 had a longer duration. Superimposed traces of simple SPrelated synaptic potentials (Fig. 6B) and complex SP-related ones (Fig. 6C) demonstrate these differences. Thus, while the initial EPSPs are similar in amplitude, the ones related to complex SPs are significantly narrowed (Complex, 21.5 { 10.4 ms; Simple, 59.3 { 15.2 ms, n Å 6, P õ 0.02) and have a steeper falling phase than the simple SP-related EPSPs (Fig. 6, B–D). Further, comparison of the averaged potentials shows the longer duration of the complex SPrelated IPSP (Fig. 6D; Simple, 318.3 { 38.4 ms; Complex, 556.2 { 66.8 ms, n Å 6, P õ 0.002). This duration difference remained even after scaling the simple SP-related IPSP (Fig. 6E), suggesting that the complex SP triggers not only a larger amplitude potential, but also one that lasts longer. Synaptic responses of LAT neurons vary with stimulation intensity The parallel fluctuations between, on the one hand, the amplitude of SPs, and on the other, the nature of SP-related potentials, suggested that the balance between excitatory and inhibitory inputs converging on a LAT neuron might depend on the degree of activation in a LAT afferent pathway, additionally, so might the IPSP duration. To verify this, synaptic responses were evoked by PRH and BM stimulation at varying intensities, and a common response profile was observed (n Å 17). At very low intensities, depolarizing responses typically were observed. However, with increasing intensi- 08-13-97 18:02:07 neupal LP-Neurophys Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.1 on May 2, 2017 FIG . 2. Spontaneous activity of LAT projection cells is dominated by large amplitude inhibitory postsynaptic potentials (IPSPs). A and B: bipolar electroencephalographic recording of entorhinal (ENT) cortex (top) and simultaneously recorded LAT neuron (bottom). In A, depolarizing current injection (0.6 nA to approximately 060 mV) induced tonic firing except during large IPSPs that were related to spontaneous ENT sharp potentials (SPs). B: same cell at rest ( 072 mV). Note absence of spontaneous spikes and dominant IPSPs. Small EPSPs also were present, but often appeared truncated by large amplitude IPSPs. Arrowhead points to PRH stimulus artifact. Intracellular events labeled by asterisks in B1 are expanded in B2. C: peri-event average of intracellular potentials using negative peak (F ) of simple (C1) and complex (C2) SPs. D: ENT-evoked IPSPs from a depolarized level (D1) and from rest (D2). INHIBITION IN THE LATERAL AMYGDALOID NUCLEUS 345 are truncated more and more rapidly as the response becomes predominantly hyperpolarizing at higher intensities. The duration of the IPSP always was found to vary directly with stimulus intensity (n Å 17). Thus higher intensities produced IPSPs that could last several hundred milliseconds longer than IPSPs elicited at lower intensities. An example is shown in Fig. 7C, where IPSPs evoked by different intensities were scaled to have equal peak amplitudes. The responses evoked by the lower intensity stimuli, whereas peaking at a similar time as the responses evoked by the higher intensity shocks, had steeper decays leading to their more rapid termination (Fig. 7C). In addition, IPSP duration was measured from response onset to the time when the potential had decreased to 25% of the peak IPSP amplitude. A plot of duration as a function of stimulus intensity also showed the direct relation between IPSP duration and stimulus intensity (Fig. 7D). FIG . 3. Histological controls showing location of stimulation electrodes. A1: scheme showing placement of PRH stimulation electrodes (S1–S4) on a ventral view of cat brain. Remaining panels are thionin-stained coronal sections showing positions of stimulation electrodes placed in PRH (A2) and ENT (B) cortices, as well as in LAT ( C), and basal (D) amygdaloid nuclei. Curved arrows point to tips of stimulating electrodes. Scale bar in C and D is also valid for B. BL, basolateral amygdaloid nucleus; BM, basomedial amygdaloid nucleus; CEL, central lateral amygdaloid nucleus; CEM, central medial amygdaloid nucleus; DG, dentate gyrus; ENT, entorhinal cortex; EC, external capsule; GP, globus pallidus; L, lateral amygdaloid nucleus; OB, olfactory bulb; ON, optic nerve; OT, optic tract; PRH, perirhinal cortex; rs, rhinal sulcus. ties, these depolarizing responses decreased in size and then became dominated by large hyperpolarizing potentials. The shape of the depolarizing potential also varied with stimulus intensity, having a broad shape at lower intensities (lasting for 100–200 ms), but narrowing to 5–10 ms or completely disappearing at higher ones. The IPSP shape also was found to vary with stimulus intensity, but in contrast to the EPSP, both its amplitude and duration grew monotonically with stimulus intensity. A typical response, in this case evoked by PRH stimulation, is shown in Fig. 7A. Here the cell was depolarized to 062 mV with 0.21 nA DC current, and synaptic responses were evoked with intensities ranging from 0.23 to 1.5 mA (weaker shocks failed to elicit consistent responses). Depolarizing responses were evoked by the lower intensities (0.23, 0.25 mA), whereas the higher ones elicited hyperpolarizing responses; small depolarizing potentials embedded in the IPSP provided evidence that EPSPs continued to be elicited at the higher intensities (Fig. 7B). In cases where the EPSP preceded the IPSP onset (65%, n Å 11), evidence was obtained that the EPSPs would have continued to increase in size if not for the countering effects of the IPSP. For example, the PRH-evoked EPSPs in Fig. 8A increased in amplitude with stimulus intensity (compare EPSPs of 0.33, 0.50, and 0.83 mA traces), even though they / 9k0b$$ja43 Anatomic studies in cats have demonstrated that PRH projections to the LAT nucleus arise from the entire extent of the PRH cortex, although rostral levels contribute a denser projection (Russchen 1982a; Witter and Groenewegen 1986). To investigate the degree of PRH convergence onto individual LAT neurons, the PRH cortex was stimulated at different rostrocaudal sites (Fig. 9A). In all cells (n Å 10), synaptic responses could be evoked from the entire extent of the PRH cortex with all sites showing similar intensity dependent response profiles, IPSP reversal potentials, and difficulty in evoking orthodromic spikes. Nevertheless, some consistent differences emerged. First, stimulation of caudal PRH levels evoked smaller responses than were elicited from more rostral sites. For example, in the cell shown in Fig. 8, stimuli of identical strength (0.83 mA) evoked increasingly large EPSPs and IPSPs when applied at more rostral sites (compare S1, S2, and S3 in Fig. 8, A–C, respectively). Only very high stimulus intensities (1 mA) applied at the most caudal site (S4) produced IPSPs of comparable size. Further, the more rostral sites had lower thresholds for evoking EPSPs (compare minimal intensities for S1 and S2 versus S3 and S4 in Fig. 8). Yet, the generation of an orthodromic spike depended less on the absolute size of the synaptic potentials than on the balance between the competing EPSPs and IPSPs evoked by the stimulus. Thus orthodromic spikes could be elicited from any level of the PRH cortex, not just from the site with the largest EPSPs. However, each PRH site elicited spikes only within a narrow range of stimulus intensities. For example, S3 at 0.23 mA readily evoked spikes whereas other intensities produced subthreshold synaptic potentials (Fig. 8C). In the cell of Fig. 8, only subthreshold responses to S1, S2, and S4 are displayed for clarity. PSP onset and duration vary systematically with PRH stimulus site Onset latency, defined as the first sustained deflection from baseline, whether positive or negative (as the evoked IPSP sometimes preceded the EPSP), was found to increase as the distance between the LAT nucleus and the stimulation site increased (Fig. 9, A and D). The average response laten- 08-13-97 18:02:07 neupal LP-Neurophys Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.1 on May 2, 2017 Synaptic responses evoked from different PRH locations 346 E. J. LANG AND D. PARÉ cies to S1–S4 were 4.8 { 0.5, 5.9 { 0.7, 10.3 { 1.9, 29 { 3.5, respectively (n Å 8, P õ 0.05). An example of this latency shift is shown in Fig. 9B. The onset latency of PRH-evoked PSPs also was found to decrease with increasing stimulus intensity. The magnitude of this effect, however, depended on the stimulation site. Rostral sites had not only shorter onset latencies but also displayed less latency variations with changes in intensity. Thus in Fig. 9, the most caudal site evoked IPSPs at latencies of 17 and 6.2 ms with low and high intensities, compared with 6 and 2.5 ms with the rostral-most site. The duration of PRH-evoked IPSPs (as defined above) also varied systematically with stimulation location, with more rostral sites evoking longer duration PSPs compared with caudal sites. In the example shown in Fig. 9C, the IPSP duration evoked by S4 was 409 ms compared with 528 ms at S1 and intermediate values with S2 and S3. As with onset latency, the duration varied with stimulation distance (Fig. 9D); however, the change in duration was 13.2 ms/mm, some 30-fold greater than the latency variations (0.4 ms/mm). FIG . 4. IPSPs evoked by cortical and intra-amygdaloid stimuli have similar reversal potentials. A: IPSPs elicited at different Vms by PRH (A1) and BM (A2) stimulation in same LAT neuron. Each trace in this and the following figures is an average of 4–9 individual sweeps, unless otherwise stated. Evoked responses consist of a short latency EPSP followed by a long lasting IPSP. Although PRH stimulation evoked larger synaptic potentials than did BM stimuli in this cell, evoked IPSPs had similar reversal potentials. Rest Å 071 mV. A3: plot of IPSP amplitude ( DV) at IPSP peak vs. Vm as determined by DC current injection. Zero-crossing of fitted lines gives reversal potentials of IPSPs. B: plots of voltage change from resting potential ( DV) vs. DC current (I) before stimulation (RIN ) and at IPSP peak (RPEAK ) for PRH- (B1) and BM- (B2) evoked IPSPs. RIN estimated from slopes of fitted lines (slope resistance). C: IPSPs evoked by direct LAT stimulation in a different neuron. C1: IPSPs evoked from different Vm as determined by DC current injection. C2: plot of IPSP amplitude ( DV) at IPSP peak vs. Vm . IPSP reversal potential of 079.6 mV was similar to those obtained for ENT- and BM-evoked IPSPs in this cell ( 077.5 and 080.5 mV, respectively). See text. Rest Å 065 mV. C3: plots of voltage change from resting potential ( DV) vs. DC current (I) before stimulation (RIN ) and at IPSP peak (RPEAK ). At its peak, IPSP reduced RIN by Ç71%. / 9k0b$$ja43 08-13-97 18:02:07 neupal LP-Neurophys Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.1 on May 2, 2017 FIG . 5. Reversal potentials of simple and complex SP-related IPSPs. A: averaged bipolar recordings of ENT SPs and related intracellular events (B) recorded at different Vms ( 0106 to 061 mV) as determined by DC current injection. Simple SP-related IPSPs reversed at 093.5 mV whereas complex SP-related IPSPs reversed at a similar value ( 091 mV). ENTevoked IPSPs had a slightly more depolarized reversal potential ( 087.5 mV) in this cell. Rest Å 076 mV. INHIBITION IN THE LATERAL AMYGDALOID NUCLEUS 347 related synaptic potentials; there is a large degree of convergence onto LAT neurons from widespread regions of the PRH cortex; and synaptic response profiles are highly dependent on stimulus intensity, implying a competitive interaction between excitatory and inhibitory inputs in the LAT nucleus. Origin of inhibition within the LAT nucleus FIG . 6. Intracellular correlates of simple and complex SPs. A: average of 6 simple (A1) and 4 complex (A2) ENT SPs and related intracellular events (B and C, respectively). Same LAT neuron in B and C. Traces were aligned in relation to negative peak of the SPs. D: superimposition of averaged simple and complex SP-related synaptic potentials. Note larger size of complex SP-related IPSPs. E: superimposition of averaged complex and scaled-simple SP-related synaptic potentials. In D and E, arrows point to EPSPs and IPSPs related to simple SPs. Simple SP-related IPSP was scaled to have same peak amplitude as averaged complex SP-related IPSP. Calibration bars in C are for B–E. All postsynaptic potentials obtained at 065 mV with 0.6 nA. Rest Å 078 mV. DISCUSSION Previous work has shown that projection neurons of the LAT nucleus have very low firing rates (Gaudreau and Paré 1996; Paré and Gaudreau 1996), and that GABAergic inhibition may be an important factor in this respect. Thus the present investigation was undertaken to study how IPSPs affect the behavior of LAT projection neurons in vivo. Our results demonstrate that large amplitude, long-lasting monophasic IPSPs reversing around 085 mV dominate the activity of LAT cells; electrical stimuli applied in major input and output structures of the LAT nucleus evoke relatively invariant synaptic responses that are similar to spontaneous SP- / 9k0b$$ja43 FIG . 7. Synaptic response profiles vary with stimulation intensity. LAT neuron with resting Vm of 074 mV depolarized to 062 mV by current injection (0.21 nA). A: synaptic responses to PRH stimuli of different intensities (mA, numbers on right). Initial part of responses are shown at a faster sweep speed in B. Note transition from depolarizing to hyperpolarizing responses with increasing stimulus intensity. C: superimposition of several traces from A after scaling to the same peak IPSP amplitude. D: plot of IPSP duration versus stimulus intensity. IPSP duration was measured from response onset to time when potential had decreased to 25% of peak IPSP amplitude. 08-13-97 18:02:07 neupal LP-Neurophys Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.1 on May 2, 2017 Although glycinergic IPSPs of unknown origin have been observed in the LAT nucleus (Danober and Pape 1995), GABA appears to be the main inhibitory transmitter in the BL complex. For instance, in vitro studies of LAT and BL neurons have shown that synaptically evoked IPSPs are sensitive to GABAA- and/or GABAB-receptor antagonists (Danober and Pape 1995; Rainnie et al. 1991b; Sugita et al. 1992, 1993; Washburn and Moises 1992). These IPSPs presumably resulted from the activation of local GABAergic interneurons as lesions deafferenting the amygdala produce little if in any decreases in GAD levels (Le Gal La Salle et al. 1978) and internuclear connections linking the different BL nuclei only consist of excitatory projections (Paré et al. 1995c; Smith and Paré 1994). 348 E. J. LANG AND D. PARÉ Thus it is likely that in the present study, the hyperpolarizing potentials elicited by cortical and subcortical stimuli in LAT projection neurons were mediated mostly by GABAergic interneurons of the LAT nucleus. However, as shown in the companion paper (Lang and Paré 1997) and in a recent in vitro study (Danober and Pape 1996), a synaptically activated intrinsic conductance also contributed to these hyperpolarizing potentials. In vivo IPSP profile differs from that found in vitro In the in vitro studies of LAT and BL neurons (Rainnie et al. 1991a,b; Sugita et al. 1992, 1993; Washburn and Moises 1992), responses to afferent stimulation typically consisted of an early glutamatergic EPSP followed by a biphasic IPSP comprising an early Cl 0-mediated phase lasting Ç50 ms, and a late, longer-lasting, K / -mediated component. The pharmacological sensitivity of these two IPSP phases sug- / 9k0b$$ja43 08-13-97 18:02:07 neupal LP-Neurophys Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.1 on May 2, 2017 FIG . 8. Synaptic responses evoked from different PRH sites. Synaptic responses were evoked in same neuron from 4 different stimulation sites along rostrocaudal extent of PRH cortex: S1 (A), S2 (B), S3 (C), and S4 (D). See scheme in Fig. 3A1 for relative positions of stimulating electrodes. Each panel depicts responses to a range of intensities (mA, values on right). gested that they resulted from the activation of GABAA and GABAB receptors, respectively. The early and late IPSPs also could be distinguished by their reversal potentials (Rainnie et al. 1991b; Sugita et al. 1992, 1993; Washburn and Moises 1992). In LAT neurons for instance, the IPSP reversals were similar to those reported in other CNS neurons, i.e., around 070 mV for GABAA and 0110 mV for GABAB mediated IPSPs (Sugita et al. 1993). In addition, in vitro observations indicated that spontaneous and evoked GABAA and GABAB responses could occur independently in LAT neurons (Sugita et al. 1992), thus suggesting that the LAT nucleus contains two distinct GABAergic neuronal populations, each having access to different types of GABAergic receptors. Although the present results confirmed some of the basic in vitro findings, such as the near coincident activation of a fast EPSP and IPSP and the presence of a long-lasting IPSP, significant differences were observed. First, spontaneous and evoked IPSPs were monophasic. That is, a clear separation of the IPSP into distinct early and late components was not observed in vivo. Second, these monophasic IPSPs (measured at their peak) reversed around 085 mV, a value in between those typically reported for GABAA and GABAB IPSPs, suggesting that Cl 0 and K / conductances contributed to the IPSP. Third, the electrophysiological features of evoked IPSPs were constant, irrespective of the stimulation site. It could be argued that the use of barbiturates in our experiments has obscured the break between an early GABAA and late GABAB component because barbiturates prolonged the mean open time of GABAA channels (Barker and McBurney 1979), thus prolonging GABAA-mediated IPSPs (Thompson and Gähwiler 1992). However, biphasic GABAergic responses indistinguishable from those observed in vitro have been described in thalamocortical neurons intracellularly recorded in cats anesthetized with various drugs including urethane, sodium pentobarbital, and ketamine-xylazine (Contreras et al. 1996; Paré et al. 1991; Paré and Lang, unpublished results). Yet, pentobarbital perfusion has been shown to reduce GABAB IPSPs in neurons of the BL nucleus in vitro (Rainnie et al. 1991b). IPSPs evoked by stimulating various input and output structures of the LAT nucleus were studied in an attempt to test the possibility that the LAT nucleus contains two populations of GABAergic interneurons having access to either GABAA or GABAB receptors and being involved in distinct intra-amygdaloid circuits (Sugita et al. 1992). Stimulation of the PRH and ENT cortices, basal forebrain (results not shown) as well as intra-amygdaloid stimuli (BM nucleus) all evoked similarly shaped monophasic IPSPs that reversed around 085 mV. Moreover, IPSPs evoked by direct LAT stimulation were monophasic, and had nearly identical reversal potentials to those of the BM- and cortically evoked IPSPs in the same cells. BM stimulation was employed in an attempt to selectively evoke feedback inhibition, because this nucleus is a major target of LAT neurons (Krettek and Price 1978; Pitkänen et al. 1995; Russchen 1982b; Smith and Paré 1994; Wakefield 1979) and does not reciprocate this projection (with the exception of a minor projection to the ventromedial border of the LAT nucleus) (Paré et al. 1995c). Moreover, LAT projection neurons give off numerous collaterals before leav- INHIBITION IN THE LATERAL AMYGDALOID NUCLEUS 349 ing the LAT nucleus (McDonald 1992; Millhouse and DeOlmos 1983), which presumably contact projection cells and interneurons (Paré et al. 1995c). This attempt to demonstrate feedback inhibition was only partly successful in that the overlapping ranges of latencies of the BM- and PRH-evoked IPSP prevented us from ruling out the possibility that the BM-evoked IPSPs were mediated via an amygdalo-cortico-LAT loop rather than through antidromic activation of recurrent collaterals. Nevertheless, in at least some cells, the onset of the IPSP and/or EPSP was too fast (i.e., less than twice the shortest latency of a PRHevoked IPSP) for the initial part of the response to be mediated via a cortical loop. Thus in these cases at least, the initial components of BM-evoked PSPs were mediated by feedback excitation and inhibition. In light of these considerations, the finding that BM- and cortically evoked IPSPs were indistinguishable suggests that feedback IPSPs are mediated by the same interneurons responsible for the cortically evoked feed-forward inhibition. Again, this interpretation is subject to the caveat that a contribution from feed-forward inhibition due to cortical afferents cannot be excluded, particularly at later times during the IPSP. This indirect recruitment of cortical inputs may have obscured differences in shape or reversal potential. Conversely, it is possible that feedback inhibition, resulting from antidromic activation of cortically projecting LAT neurons, contributed to cortically evoked IPSPs. However, although the LAT nucleus projects to the entire rostro- / 9k0b$$ja43 caudal extent of the PRH in cats (Room and Groenewegen 1986), the projection to the ENT is limited to ventrolateral ENT area (Smith and Paré 1994), whereas we stimulated the more caudally situated ventromedial ENT area. Because no consistent differences between ENT- and PRH-evoked IPSPs were found, either the contribution of feedback inhibition to PRH-evoked IPSPs is minimal relative to the feedforward inhibition or both types of inhibition involve the same interneuronal population. In sum, the relatively invariant nature of the evoked IPSPs, regardless of stimulation site and their similarity to SP-related IPSPs, lead us to conclude that independent activation of interneuronal subpopulations contacting only GABAA or GABAB receptors, respectively, appears not to occur in vivo. This difference from the in vitro situation may be ascribed to the intactness of the cortical and amygdaloid circuitry in vivo, which probably links the activation of the different GABAergic populations. Given the reported barbiturate-sensitivity of GABAB IPSPs in some neurons (Rainnie et al. 1991b), this conclusion should be verified under other anesthetics. However, the results of the companion paper suggest that we were able to detect GABAB IPSPs and that they never occurred independently of GABAA IPSPs (Lang and Paré 1997). Convergence and divergence in corticoamygdaloid afferents The discrepancies between the in vivo and in vitro data appear to result from several factors, including the apparent 08-13-97 18:02:07 neupal LP-Neurophys Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.1 on May 2, 2017 FIG . 9. IPSP onset and duration vary with PRH stimulation site. A: scheme showing position of PRH stimulation electrodes on a ventral view of cat brain. B: initial part of responses are shown at fast sweep speed. Arrows indicate onset of responses. C: evoked response to identical stimuli applied at each site. Open circles, points where IPSP amplitudes have decreased to 25% of peak amplitudes. D: plot of IPSP duration and onset as a function of electrode location in terms of frontal plane level. Intracellular recordings were obtained at a frontal plane of Ç12 mm. E: schematics showing postulated flow of activity after stimulation of rostral (E1) or caudal (E2) PRH sites. 350 E. J. LANG AND D. PARÉ / 9k0b$$ja43 A balance of excitation and inhibition The large IPSPs observed here in vivo, and previously in vitro (Sugita et al. 1992, 1993; Takagi and Yamamoto 1981), must play an important role in shaping the responses of LAT neurons. Consistent with these intracellular observations, the large majority of LAT neurons have transient (i.e., 1–2 spikes) responses to sustained auditory stimuli (Bordi and LeDoux 1992), Moreover, in a double shock paradigm of medial geniculate inputs, LAT neurons were found to have a reduced response to stimuli for delays °150 ms (Clugnet et al. 1990). The present results suggest that this inhibition is due to activation of much the same intra-LAT circuitry and intrinsic conductances, regardless of the afferent source activated. Instead, the balance of inhibition and excitation evoked from a particular site, appears to depend most strongly on the stimulus intensity, with low intensities producing depolarizing responses and high intensities hyperpolarizing ones. Given the large degree of divergence in the PRH-LAT projection, this pattern may result from the more extensive dendritic trees of the projection cells as compared with those of interneurons (Hall 1972). Thus projection neurons would be more likely to receive synaptic inputs from a particular source than inhibitory interneurons. Therefore low intensity stimuli could produce excitation of projection neurons while still subthreshold for evoking firing of interneurons. With higher stimulus intensities, interneurons would fire, and because of their strategically located synapses onto the soma and proximal dendrites (Carlsen 1988), inhibit the projection neurons, despite their receiving increasingly strong excitatory input. Thus we propose that information processing in the LAT nucleus may be linked to quantitative, rather than qualitative differences in activation of intra-LAT circuitry, with the balance of excitation and inhibition being more relevant than the absolute magnitude of the evoked responses, or the particular afferent stimulated, for determining the response of a LAT neuron. Thus we predict that large-scale generalized activity in LAT afferents should not be as effective in activating the LAT nucleus as would the activity of relatively small cortical neuronal ensembles. Simultaneous recordings from the PRH cortex and LAT nucleus should be performed to address these issues. The authors thank P. Giguère and G. Oakson for excellent technical and programming support, D. Drolet for help with the preparation of the figures, and S. Charpak for stimulating discussions. E. J. Lang was supported by a National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Grant 1 F32 NS-10006-01. This study was supported by the Medical Research Council of Canada (Grant MT-11562). Address reprint requests to D. Paré. Received 7 May 1996; accepted in final form 27 September 1996. REFERENCES ADOLPHS, R., TRANEL, D., DAMASIO, H., AND DAMASIO, A. R. Fear and the human amygdala. J. Neurosci. 15: 5879–5891, 1995. AMARAL, D. G., PRICE, J. L., PITK Ä NEN, A., AND CARMICHEAL, S. T. Anatomical organization of the primate amygdaloid complex. In: The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction, edited by J. P. Aggleton. New York: Wiley-Liss, 1992, p. 1–66. BARKER, J. L. AND MC BURNEY, R. M. Phenobarbitone modulation of postsynaptic GABA receptor function on cultured mammalian neurones. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 206: 319–327, 1979. 08-13-97 18:02:07 neupal LP-Neurophys Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.1 on May 2, 2017 down regulation of the GABAB IPSP and the presence of a synaptically activated Ca 2/ -dependent K / conductance in vivo. These will be treated in detail in the companion paper (Lang and Paré 1997). Here we will discuss the role of the extensive intra-amygdaloid and intracortical connectivity, which is largely lost with in vitro preparations, in shaping the temporal characteristics of the evoked responses in vivo. In our experiments, the extensive convergence of cortical inputs on LAT neurons was demonstrated by the possibility of evoking synaptic responses in LAT neurons from all levels of the PRH cortex as well as from the ENT cortex. A previous in vivo study performed in rats also demonstrated that LAT neurons receive convergent inputs from multiple sources (Mello et al. 1992). The variations in amplitudes and onset latencies of PRH-evoked IPSPs as a function of the stimulation site were consistent with the anatomic data on PRH projections to the LAT nucleus. Thus the shorter latency of responses to rostral PRH stimuli is consistent with the shorter distance separating the LAT nucleus from rostral PRH levels as compared with caudal PRH sites (Fig. 9E). In addition, the larger size of the responses evoked from rostral sites fits with the denser projection of rostral PRH levels to the LAT nucleus (Russchen 1982a; Witter and Groenewegen 1986; Witter et al. 1986). Given the extensive corticocortical connections within the PRH cortex and considering the fact that the entire PRH cortex projects to the LAT nucleus (Russchen 1982a; Witter and Groenewegen 1986; Witter et al. 1986), the PRHevoked IPSPs must have resulted from the direct activation of corticoamygdaloid neurons at the stimulation site and from the activation, via corticocortical circuits, of such neurons throughout a great extent of the PRH cortex (Fig. 9E). Consequently, PRH inputs arising from different rostrocaudal levels reach the LAT nucleus asynchronously, producing a temporally distributed activation of LAT inhibitory interneurons that might cause the ‘‘early’’ and ‘‘late’’ inhibitory responses seen in vitro to overlap, thus explaining the lack of a clear break between the GABAA and GABAB responses in vivo. In parallel with the spread of activity at the cortical level, intrinsic connections within the LAT nucleus (Krettek and Price 1978; Pitkänen et al. 1995; Russchen 1982b; Smith and Paré 1994) also must contribute to distribute PRH influences in time and space. In addition, it is likely that this spread of activity across the cortex is partly responsible for the longer duration of IPSPs evoked from rostral PRH sites as compared with caudal ones, since stimuli applied at rostral levels would recruit the shortest corticoamygdaloid pathways first and the longest pathways last (Fig. 9E1), resulting in a relatively longer activation of LAT interneurons. In contrast, IPSPs evoked by stimulation of caudal PRH sites would be relatively compressed in time because the longer corticoamygdalar fibers would be activated before the shorter ones (Fig. 9E2). However, given the differences in IPSP latencies from these sites, which reflect the differing conduction times, only 10–20% of the duration difference can be accounted for in this manner. The remaining difference must be due to other mechanisms that lead to a more sustained activation of LAT interneurons and/or to greater activation of the inhibitory intrinsic membrane conductances present in LAT projection neurons. INHIBITION IN THE LATERAL AMYGDALOID NUCLEUS / 9k0b$$ja43 MILLHOUSE, O. E. AND DEOLMOS, J. Neuronal configurations in lateral and basolateral amygdala. Neuroscience 10: 1269–1300, 1983. NITECK A, L. AND BEN-ARI, Y. Distribution of GABA-like immunoreactivity in the rat amygdaloid complex. J. Comp. Neurol. 266: 45–55, 1987. PAPE, H.-C. AND PARÉ, D. Rhythmic electrical activity in the amygdala: observations in vitro and in vivo. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 21: 1666, 1995. PARÉ, D., CURRÓ DOSSI, R., AND STERIADE, M. Three types of inhibitory post-synaptic potentials generated by interneurons in the anterior thalamic complex of the cat. J. Neurophysiol. 66: 1190–1205, 1991. PARÉ, D., DONG, J., AND GAUDREAU, H. Amygdalo-entorhinal relations and their reflection in the hippocampal formation: generation of sharp sleep potentials. J. Neurosci. 15: 2482–2503, 1995a. PARÉ, D. AND GAUDREAU, H. Projection cells and interneurons of the lateral and basolateral amygdala: distinct firing patterns and differential relation to theta and delta rhythms in conscious cats. J. Neurosci. 16: 3334– 3350, 1996. PARÉ, D., PAPE, H.-C., AND DONG, J. Bursting and oscillating neurons of the cat basolateral amygdaloid complex in vivo: electrophysiological properties and morphological features. J. Neurophysiol. 74: 1179–1191, 1995b. PARÉ, D. AND SMITH, Y. Distribution of GABA immunoreactivity in the amygdaloid complex of the cat. Neuroscience 57: 1061–1076, 1993. PARÉ, D., SMITH, Y., AND PARÉ, J.-F. Intra-amygdaloid projections of the basolateral and basomedial nuclei in the cat: phaseolus vulgaris-leucoagglutinin anterograde tracing at the light and electron microscopic level. Neuroscience 69: 567–583, 1995c. PITK Ä NEN, A., STEFANACCI, L., FARB, C. R., GO, G.-G., LEDOUX, J. E., AND AMARAL, D. G. Intrinsic connections of the rat amygdaloid complex: projections originating in the lateral nucleus. J. Comp. Neurol. 356: 288– 310, 1995. RAINNIE, D. G., ASPRODINI, E. K., AND SHINNICK-GALLAGHER, P. Excitatory transmission in the basolateral amygdala. J. Neurophysiol. 66: 986–998, 1991a. RAINNIE, D. G., ASPRODINI, E. K., AND SHINNICK-GALLAGHER, P. Inhibitory transmission in the basolateral amygdala. J. Neurophysiol. 66: 999–1009, 1991b. ROMANSKI, L. M. AND LEDOUX, J. E. Equipotentiality of thalamo-amygdala and thalamo-cortico-amygdala circuits in auditory fear conditioning. J. Neurosci. 12: 4501–4509, 1992. ROOM, P. AND GROENEWEGEN, H. J. Connections of the parahippocampal cortex in the cat. II. Subcortical afferents. J. Comp. Neurol. 251: 451– 473, 1986. RUSSCHEN, F. T. Amygdalopetal projections in the cat. I. Cortical afferent connections. A study with retrograde and anterograde tracing techniques. J. Comp. Neurol. 206: 159–179, 1982a. RUSSCHEN, F. T. Amygdalopetal projections in the cat. II. Subcortical afferent connections. A study with retrograde tracing techniques. J. Comp. Neurol. 207: 157–176, 1982b. SMITH, Y. AND PARÉ, D. Intra-amygdaloid projections of the lateral nucleus in the cat: PHA-L anterograde labeling combined with postembedding GABA and glutamate immunocytochemistry. J. Comp. Neurol. 342: 232–248, 1994. STEFANACCI, L., FARB, C. R., PITK Ä NEN, A., GO, G., LEDOUX, J. E., AND AMARAL, D. G. Projections from the lateral nucleus to the basal nucleus of the amygdala: a light and electron microscopic PHA-L study in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 323: 586–601, 1992. SUGITA, S., JOHNSON, S. W., AND NORTH, R. A. Synaptic inputs to GABAA and GABAB receptors originate from discrete afferent neurons. Neurosci. Lett. 134: 207–211, 1992. SUGITA, S., TANAK A, E., AND NORTH, R. A. Membrane properties and synaptic potentials of three types of neurone in rat lateral amygdala. J. Physiol. Lond. 460: 705–718, 1993. TAK AGI, M. AND YAMAMOTO, C. The long-lasting inhibition recorded in vitro from the lateral nucleus of the amygdala. Brain Res. 206: 474–478, 1981. THOMPSON, S. M. AND GÄHWILER, B. H. Effects of the GABA uptake inhibitor tiagabine on inhibitory synaptic potentials in rat hippocampal slice cultures. J. Neurophysiol. 67: 1698–1701, 1992. TURNER, B. H., MISHKIN, M., AND KNAPP, M. Organization of the amygdalopetal projections from modality-specific cortical association areas in the monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 191: 515–543, 1980. WAKEFIELD, C. The intrinsic connections of the basolateral amygdaloid nuclei as visualized with the HRP method. Neurosci. Lett. 12: 17–21, 1979. 08-13-97 18:02:07 neupal LP-Neurophys Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.1 on May 2, 2017 BECHARA, A., TRANEL, D., DAMASIO, H., ADOLPHS, R., ROCKLAND, C., AND DAMASIO, A. R. Double dissociation of conditioning and declarative knowledge relative to the amygdala and hippocampus in humans. Science Wash. DC 269: 1115–1118, 1995. BEN-ARI, Y., LE GAL LA SALLE, G., AND CHAMPAGNAT, J. Lateral amygdala unit activity. I. Relationship between spontaneous and evoked activity. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 37: 449–461, 1974. BERMAN, A. L. AND JONES, E. G. The Thalamus and Basal Telencephalon of the Cat. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Press, 1982. BORDI, F. AND LEDOUX, J. Sensory tuning beyond the sensory system: an initial analysis of auditory response properties of neurons in the lateral amygdaloid nucleus and overlying areas of the striatum. J. Neurosci. 12: 2493–2503, 1992. BORDI, F., LEDOUX, J., CLUGNET, M. C., AND PAVLIDES, C. Single-unit activity in the lateral nucleus of the amygdala and overlying areas of the striatum in freely behaving rats: rates, discharge patterns, and responses to acoustic stimuli. Behav. Neurosci. 107: 757–769, 1993 CALLAHAN, P. M., PARIS, J. M., CUNNINGHAM, K. A., AND SHINNICK-GALLAGHER, P. Decrease of GABA-immunoreactive neurons in the amygdala after electrical kindling in the rat. Brain Res. 555: 335–339, 1991. CARLSEN, J. Immunocytochemical localization of glutamate decarboxylase in the rat basolateral amygdaloid nucleus, with special reference to GABAergic innervation of amygdalostriatal projection neurons. J. Comp. Neurol. 273: 513–526, 1988. CLUGNET, M. C., LEDOUX, J. E., AND MORRISON, S. F. Unit responses evoked in the amygdala and striatum by electrical stimulation of the medial geniculate body. J. Neurosci. 10: 1055–1061, 1990. CONTRERAS, D., TIMOFEEV, I., AND STERIADE, M. Mechanisms of longlasting hyperpolarizations underlying slow sleep oscillations in cat corticothalamic networks. J. Physiol. Lond. 494: 251–264, 1996. DANOBER, L. AND PAPE, H.-C. Slow inhibitory synaptic responses in amygdaloid nuclei. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 21: 1666, 1995. DANOBER, L. AND PAPE, H.-C. Contribution of a slow membrane hyperpolarization to the prevention of epileptic activity in the lateral amygdala. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 22: 822.14, 1996. GAUDREAU, H. AND PARÉ, D. Projection neurons of the lateral amygdaloid nucleus are virtually silent throughout the sleep-waking cycle. J. Neurophysiol. 75: 1301–1305, 1996. HALL, E. The amygdala of the cat: a golgi study. Z. Zellforsch. 134: 439– 458, 1972. HORIK AWA, K. AND ARMSTRONG, W. E. A versatile means of intracellular labeling: injection of biocytin and its detection with avidin conjugates. J. Neurosci. Methods 25: 1–11, 1988. JOHNSTON, D. AND WU, S. M.-S. Foundations of Cellular Neurophysiology. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Book/MIT Press, 1995. KRETTEK, J. E. AND PRICE, J. L. A description of the amygdaloid complex in the rat and cat with observations on intra-amygdaloid axonal connections. J. Comp. Neurol. 178: 255–280, 1978. LANG, E. J. AND PARÉ, D. Synaptic and synaptically activated intrinsic conductances underlie inhibitory potentials in cat lateral amygdaloid projection neurons in vivo. J. Neurophysiol. 77: 353–363, 1997. LE GAL LA SALLE, G., PAXINOS, G., EMSON, P., AND BEN-ARI, Y. Neurochemical mapping of GABAergic systems in the amygdaloid complex and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis. Brain Res. 155: 397–403, 1978. LEDOUX, J. E., CICCHETTI, P., XAGORARIS, A., AND ROMANSKI, L. M. The lateral amygdaloid nucleus: sensory interface of the amygdala in fear conditioning. J. Neurosci. 10: 1062–1069, 1990. LEDOUX, J. E., RUGGIERO, D. A., AND REIS, D. J. Projections to the subcortical forebrain from anatomically defined regions of the medial geniculate body in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 242: 182–213, 1985. LEDOUX, J. E., SAK AGUCHI, A., IWATA, J., AND REIS, D. J. Interruption of projections from the medial geniculate body to an archi-neostriatal field disrupts the classical conditioning of emotional responses to acoustic stimuli. Neuroscience 17: 615–627, 1986 MC DONALD, A. J. Immunohistochemical identification of gamma-aminobutyric acid containing neurons in the rat basolateral amygdala. Neurosci. Lett. 53: 203–207, 1985. MC DONALD, A. J. Cell types and intrinsic connections of the amygdala. In: The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction, edited by J. P. Aggleton. New York: Wiley-Liss, 1992, p. 67–96. MELLO, L.E.A.M., TAN, A. M., AND FINCH, D. M. Convergence of projections from the rat hippocampal formation, medial geniculate and basal forebrain onto single amygdaloid neurons: an in vivo extra- and intracellular electrophysiological study. Brain Res. 587: 24–40, 1992. 351 352 E. J. LANG AND D. PARÉ WASHBURN, M. S. AND MOISES, H. C. Inhibitory responses of rat basolateral amygdaloid neurons recorded in vitro. Neuroscience 50: 811–830, 1992. WITTER, M. P. AND GROENEWEGEN, H. J. Connections of the parahippocampal cortex in the cat. IV. Subcortical efferents. J. Comp. Neurol. 252: 51–77, 1986. WITTER, M. P., R OOM, P., G ROENEWEGEN, H. J., AND LOHMAN, A.H.M. Connections of the parahippocampal cortex in the cat. V. Intrinsic connections; comments on input / output connections with the hippocampus. J. Comp. Neurol. 252: 78 – 94, 1986. Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.1 on May 2, 2017 / 9k0b$$ja43 08-13-97 18:02:07 neupal LP-Neurophys