Download Functional Properties of Corticotectal Neurons in the Monkey`s

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Brain–computer interface wikipedia , lookup

Types of artificial neural networks wikipedia , lookup

Environmental enrichment wikipedia , lookup

Functional magnetic resonance imaging wikipedia , lookup

Neurotransmitter wikipedia , lookup

Molecular neuroscience wikipedia , lookup

Axon guidance wikipedia , lookup

Activity-dependent plasticity wikipedia , lookup

Axon wikipedia , lookup

Convolutional neural network wikipedia , lookup

Nonsynaptic plasticity wikipedia , lookup

Clinical neurochemistry wikipedia , lookup

Multielectrode array wikipedia , lookup

Electrophysiology wikipedia , lookup

Caridoid escape reaction wikipedia , lookup

Single-unit recording wikipedia , lookup

Development of the nervous system wikipedia , lookup

Metastability in the brain wikipedia , lookup

Central pattern generator wikipedia , lookup

Neuroanatomy wikipedia , lookup

Stimulus (physiology) wikipedia , lookup

Biological neuron model wikipedia , lookup

Circumventricular organs wikipedia , lookup

Mirror neuron wikipedia , lookup

Neural oscillation wikipedia , lookup

Neural coding wikipedia , lookup

Process tracing wikipedia , lookup

Neuropsychopharmacology wikipedia , lookup

Nervous system network models wikipedia , lookup

Pre-Bötzinger complex wikipedia , lookup

Neural correlates of consciousness wikipedia , lookup

Optogenetics wikipedia , lookup

Synaptic gating wikipedia , lookup

Feature detection (nervous system) wikipedia , lookup

Channelrhodopsin wikipedia , lookup

Premovement neuronal activity wikipedia , lookup

Superior colliculus wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
JOURNALOFNEUROPHYSIOLOGY
Vol. 58, No. 6, December 1987. Printed
in U.S.A
Functional Properties of Corticotectal Neurons
in the Monkey’s Frontal Eye Field
MARK
A. SEGRAVES
AND
MICHAEL
E. GOLDBERG
Laboratory of SensorimotorResearch,National Eye Institute, National Institutes of Health,
Bethesda,Maryland 20892
SUMMARY
AND
CONCLUSIONS
1. We examined the activity of identified
corticotectal neurons in the frontal eye field
of awake behaving rhesus monkeys (Macaca
mulatta). Corticotectal neurons were antidromical y excited using biphasic cu rrent
pulsespassedthrough monopolar microelectrodes within the superior colliculus. The activity of single corticotectal neurons was
studied while the m.onkey performed behavioral tasks designed to test the relation of the
neuron’s discharge to visual and oculomotor
events.
2. Fifty-one frontal eye field corticotectal
neurons were examined in two monkeys.
Current thresholds for antidromic excitation
ranged from 6 to 1,200 PA, with a mean of
330 PA. Antidromic latencies ranged from
1.2 to 6.0 ms, with a mean of 2.25 ms.
3. Fifty-three percent of the identified
corticotectal neurons were classified as having movement-related activity. They had little or no responseto visual stimuli, but very
strong activity before both visually guided
and memory-guided saccades.An additional
6% of corticotectal n.eurons had v,isuomovement activity, combining both a visual- and
a saccade-related response. In each case, visuomovement neurons antidromically excited from the superior colliculus had movement-related activity, which was much
stronger than the visual component of their
response.
4. Twenty-two percent of the corticotectal
neurons were primarily responsive to visual
stimulation of the fovea. These included
both neurons responding to the onset and
neurons responding to the disappearance of a
light flashed on the fovea.
5. The remaining 20% of the corticotectal
neurons were a heterogeneous group whose
activity could not be classified as movement,
visuomovement or foveal. Their responses
included postsaccadic, anticipatory, and reward-related activity, as well as activity
modulated during certain directions of
smooth-pursuit eye movements. One neuron
was unresponsive during all of the behavioral
tasks used. There were no corticotectal
neurons that could be classified as primarily
responsive to peripheral visual stimuli.
6. Histological reconstructions of electrode penetrations localized corticotectal
neurons to layer V of the frontal eye field.
For 22 corticotectal neurons tested, each had
its minimum threshold for antidromic excitation within the superior colliculus, as
judged by either histological confirmation,
or surrounding neuronal responsesrecorded
through the stimulation microelectrode. The
majority of these neurons had minimum
threshold sites within the intermediate
layers; a few minimum threshold sites were
located within the superficial or deep collicular layers.
7. The lowest thresholds for antidromic
excitation were obtained when the optimal
saccade vectors associated with the frontal
eye field recording and collicular stimulation
sites were closely matched. There was a
strong correlation between a measure of the
difference between saccadesassociated with
recording and stimulation sitesand the log of
threshold for antidromic excitation. This relationship was such that small increases in
the vector difference between frontal eye
field and collicular saccadeswere accompanied by large increasesin threshold.
8. In comparison to the entire population
1387
1388
M.
A. SEGRAVES
AND
of frontal eye field neurons examined by
Bruce and Goldberg (7), we conclude that
there is a selective enrichment
within the
population
of corticotectal
projection
neurons for neurons with eye movement-related activity and neurons with fovea1 visual
activity, and a paucity of neurons with peripheral visual receptive fields and postsaccadic activity.
9. These data suggest that the visual activity prevalent within the frontal eye field is
likely to help generate the activity of movement-related neurons, but it is not a source
for visual activity within the superior colliculus.
10. The frontal eye field’s projection to
the superior colliculus provides several messages relevant for oculomotor
performance,
including information pertinent to the maintenance and release of fixation, and targeting
information regarding an intended saccade.
INTRODUCTION
Since Ferrier’s original demonstration
( 12)
that electrical stimulation
of the monkey’s
prearcuate frontal cortex produced conjugate eye movements, it has been postulated
that this region participates in the voluntary
control of gaze. However, a number of experiments have raised questions about the
exact role of the frontal eye field in eyemovement control. This uncertainty results,
in part, from the existence of parallel pathways that are likely to be involved in the
input of eye movement control signals to the
brain stem ocuomotor centers. The frontal
eye field can affect the oculomotor
system
via three pathways
(26, 33-35, 55). The
first is by a direct projection to perioculomotor regions in the midbrain and pons (33).
The second is by a projection to the caudate
nucleus, which might then affect the inhibitory pathway from the substantia nigra pars
reticulata to the superior colliculus (11, 24).
The final one is a direct projection to the
intermediate layers of the superior colliculus
(1, 30). The relative contribution
that each
pathway makes to eye-movement
control
has not been determined. Additional problems arise when one considers the number of
different neuron activity types that have
been described in the frontal eye field. Bizzi
(4) originally found that most frontal eye
M.
E. GOLDBERG
field neurons were not related to spontaneous saccades made in the dark. The only
neurons related to saccades were a small percentage (30/700) that became active only
after the initiation of a saccadic eye movement. More recent single-neuron
studies
have indicated a role for the frontal eye field
in the initiation of saccades. Mohler, Goldberg, and Wurtz (39) demonstrated that cells
in the frontal eye field discharge in response
to visual stimuli. This visual activity is enhanced when the monkey makes a saccade to
the stimulus in the receptive field (62), and
the enhancement
only occurs before saccades and not before arm movements
or
other activity in which saccades do not occur
(9). Bruce and Goldberg (7) recently reported
that only - 19% of frontal eye field neurons
have purely postsaccadic activity. The majority of neurons recorded from by Bruce
and Goldberg in awake behaving monkeys
had some form of activity that preceded
visually guided saccadic eye movements,
including a range of neuron activity from
purely visual to purely movement related.
It is clear there are signals in the frontal eye
field that could drive saccadic eye movements and neural pathways by which these
signals could reach brain stem oculomotor
centers. However, to understand how the cerebral cortex controls a specific behavior, it is
not sufficient to know the types of activity in
the cortex and the anatomical projections of
that region. One must also know what information is carried by the corticofugal signals.
These output signals are likely to be produced bv a subset of the activity types within
the entire neuronal population, and the output population will be enriched for some signal types and lacking others. To begin to answer this question, we identified corticotectal
projection neurons by antidromic excitation
from the superior colliculus and then characterized these neurons in awake behaving animals according to the scheme described by
Bruce and Goldberg (7). A preliminary
report of these experiments has been published
elsewhere (5 3).
METHODS
Preoperative training
Two adult rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta)
were trained preoperatively to do a simple visual
FRONTAL
EYE
FIELD
CORTICOTECTAL
fixation task using established techniques (60).
The monkey was seated in a primate chair and
began a trial by pressing a metal bar in front of
him. This resulted in the onset of a target light on
a screen in front of the monkey. The monkey was
required to detect the dimming of the light and
signal this detection by releasing the metal bar to
receive a liquid reward.
Surgery
Surgery was performed under aseptic conditions. The monkey was anesthetized
with ketamine hydrochloride
(10 mg/kg im) followed by
pentobarbital
sodium through an intravenous
catheter as needed. During the surgical procedure,
a subconjunctival
wire coil for the measurement
of eye position with the magnetic search-coil technique was implanted in one eye (27,44). Trephine
holes were made through the skull over the superior colliculi and over both left and right frontal
eye fields. Stainless steel bolts to strengthen the
bond of dental acrylic to the skull were fastened in
slots cut through the skull and extending away
from the edges of the trephine holes. Three recording cylinders, a steel receptacle to fix the
monkey’s head during recording sessions, and the
connector for the eye coil were fixed in place and
bonded to the skull with dental acrylic. Immediately after surgery, the monkey was given gentamicin sulfate (5 mg/kg im) as a prophylactic measure against infection. The monkey received daily
dosages of gentamicin sulfate for 1 wk postoperatively. Postoperative training was begun between
1.5 and 2 wk after surgery.
Postoperative training
After surgery, each monkey received additional
training in several visual and saccade tasks. The
computer hardware and software used to monitor
and control the monkey’s behavior has recently
been described in detail elsewhere (17). Briefly,
the monkey was seated in a primate chair with its
head fixed and centered within horizontal
and
vertical magnetic field coils. Visual stimuli presented to the monkey included stationary and
movable light stimuli originating
from yellow
light-emitting
diodes rear-projected
onto a tangent screen 57 cm in front of the monkey. Each
projected stimulus was -0.25’
in diameter with a
brightness of 0.4 log units above a background of
1 cd/m*. The movable stimulus was positioned by
a pair of servo-controlled
mirror galvanometers
(General Scanner, Watertown, MA) driven by analog signals synthesized by 12-bit digital-to-analog
converters under the control of the computer.
Since, after surgery, it was possible to accurately
measure eye position, the monkey was no longer
required to detect the dimming of the stimulus.
Instead, the monkey had to meet predetermined
NEURONS
criteria for eye position relative to target position
and for saccade amplitude and direction, in order
to receive a liquid reward. Eye position was measured by the magnetic search-coil system using the
C.N.C. Engineering phase-sensitive detector. Eye
position measurement was accurate to 15 min of
arc within a range of 20” from the center of gaze
and was not corrected for cosine error.
Each monkey was trained to do several behavioral tasks (Fig. 1). Each task began with the appearance of a central stationary light on the screen
(Fig. 1, FP). The computer monitored the monkey’s eye position, and when the monkey had
achieved fixation for 100 ms the computer then
began the various timing intervals of each task:
1) FIXATION
TASK (FIG. 1A).
The monkey was
required to hold fixation throughout
the trial.
During some trials the fixation light was turned
off at an unpredictable
moment for a brief period
of time. The interval during which the light was
turned off was the same in any given block of
trials, but was varied from 200 to 500 ms between
blocks of trials. The monkey was rewarded for
maintaining
eye position within a window surrounding the target such that the sum of the horizontal and vertical differences between eye and
target position was ~5” (54). This was a criterion
that the monkeys met easily, and usually surpassed. With practice each monkey was able to
maintain fixation of the target’s initial position
throughout the trial, including periods when the
light was turned off. This paradigm enabled us to
test the effects of visual stimulation
of the fovea
on neuronal activity.
2) VISUAL
NO-SACCADE
TASK
(FIG.
1B).
The
monkey fixated the stationary light spot in the
center of the tangent screen for the duration of the
trial. During the trial, at an unpredictable
time, a
second light spot (Fig. lB, S) was flashed for a
variable period of time. The position of the second light was controlled by mirror galvanometers
and could be placed anywhere within the monkey’s visual field. The monkey was rewarded for
maintaining
fixation of the center light throughout the trial. The peripheral stimulus had no behavioral significance, but its position was used to
map the visual receptive fields of frontal eye field
neurons.
3) VISUALLY
1 C).
After
GUIDED
SACCADE
TASK
(FIG.
a variable period of fixation, the
center light was extinguished at the same moment
that a peripheral target light (Fig. 1, C and D, T)
was turned on. When the peripheral target appeared, the monkey was required to make a saccade to it within 500 ms. After the saccade, the
monkey was rewarded either for maintaining
eye
position within a window with -3-6’
radius surrounding the new target position or for having
M.
1390
A. SEGRAVES
AND
M.
E. GOLDBERG
A
Fixation
VH
L
m-/
I
B
Visual
No-Saccade
VH
1
FP-I
s
I
1
C
Visually
Guided
Saccade
VH
I
IL
FPJ
T
\
/
D
Memory
Guided
Saccade
VH
10"
\‘I
/
I
FP-J
T
I
L
r
1
250 ms
FIG. 1. Behavioral
tasks. H, horizontal
eye position;
V, vertical eye position.
An upward
deflection
signifies
movement
to the right for H and up for V. FP, fixation point; S, peripheral
stimulus light; T, peripheral
target light.
For FP, S, and T, an upward deflection
signifies light on. Each trial began when the monkey
fixated a light in the
center of the tangent screen and maintained
fixation for at least 100 ms. A: fixation task. The sketch to the right of
the eye position traces shows that only the fixation point appears in this task. During the trial, the fixation point was
turned off for 200 ms. B: visual no-saccade task. Sketch at right indicates that both fixation point and stimulus flash
occur in this task, but no saccade is made. C: visually guided saccade task. Sketch at right indicates that a saccade is
made to the position of the target light after the fixation point is turned off. D: memory-guided
saccade task. Sketch
indicates that saccade is made to the remembered
position of the target light after the disappearance
of the fixation
point. See text for an additional
description
of each task.
made a saccade whose end point was within the
window. The movable light spot was again used
for the peripheral target and could be positioned
anywhere on the tangent screen. In this task the
simultaneous disappearance of the center fixation
light and onset of peripheral target light were the
cue for the monkey to make a saccade.
4) MEMORY-GUIDED
10).
In a second
SACCADE
TASK
(FIG.
more difficult form of saccade
task, the center light came on to start the trial, as
before, and the monkey was required to begin and
maintain fixation of the center light until it was
turned off. During the central fixation period, a
peripheral target was turned on for 50-300 ms.
When the center light was turned off the monkey
was required to make a saccade to the position of
the flashed peripheral target light. The duration
and time of occurrence of the target flash were
adjusted so that the target light was turned off
during the fixation period, requiring the monkey
to maintain fixation for up to 250 ms after the
target light’s disappearance and then make a sac-
cade to a remembered target location. This task
enabled us to distinguish
between visual and
movement associated neuronal activity during the
trial.
In this study, the location of visual stimuli and
targets are expressed in polar coordinates, radius
and angle, where radius is equal to the distance, in
degrees of arc, of the target or stimulus from the
central fixation point. An angle of 0” describes a
rightward horizontal
direction, and a 90” angle
describes an upward vertical direction.
Electrophysiology
A diagram of the stimulation
and recording
setup is provided in Fig. 2, illustrating the antidromic excitation of a single neuron in the frontal
eye field by a monopolar stimulating electrode in
the superior colliculus. In these experiments, we
defined the frontal eye field as the area of cortex,
located primarily on the rostra1 bank of the arcuate sulcus, where eye movements can be evoked
FRONTAL
EYE
FIELD
CORTICOTECTAL
NEURONS
1391
Stimulation
Superior
FIG.
frontal
Colliculus
2. Stim ulation and neuron
eye field were antidromically
recording
configuration
excited by a monopolar
with thresholds of 50 PA or less (7, 8). Initial
neuron recording in both the superior colliculus
and the frontal eye field was done with glasscoated platinum-iridium
electrodes (59) introduced through the intact dura. For the superior
colliculi, penetrations through the dura were used
to define the collicular topography relative to the
recording
cylinder. Once this topography
was
known, an M-gauge guide tube was introduced
through the dura with the monkey under ketamine anesthesia (5- 10 mg/kg). The portion of
the guide tube exposed above the dura was cemented to the wall of the recording cylinder with
dental acrylic. The tip of the guide tube was positioned a few millimeters above the surface of the
superior colliculus. This made it possible to make
repeated penetrations with fine tungsten microelectrodes through the same region of the superior
colliculus and reduced the amount of daily preparation time before the actual search for antidromically excitable neurons could begin. When the
guide tube was not in use, a wire matched to the
inside diameter of the 1&gauge tubing and coated
with antibiotic
(3% tetracycline
HCl ointment)
was inserted into it. In several instances, a single
guide tube in the superior colliculus remained
implanted for 1 mo without adverse effect. The
tangential area of the superior colliculus sampled
through the guide tube could be increased by
slightly bending the electrode - 1 cm from its tip,
resulting
in the area “seen” by penetrations
through the guide tube to be conical in shape.
Neuronal recording in the frontal eye field was
done both with platinum-iridium
electrodes introduced through the dura and with tungsten electrodes passed through guide tubes. The use of
guide tubes in the cortex made it easier to make
Frontal
.
Eye Fields
Single neurons isolated
stimulating
microelectrode
by a
in t
icroelectrode
in the
superior collicul us.
repeated penetrations through areas where antidromically
excitable neurons were found previously and improved
the recording
stability.
Twenty-seven (5 3%) of the antidromically
excited
and physiologically
characterized
neurons included in this report were recorded from electrodes passed through implanted guide tubes.
Stimulation
through the superior colliculus
electrode to excite frontal eye field neurons antidromically was done with biphasic negative first
pulses, with 0.2-ms pulse width. The output of the
stimulus generator was connected to the electrode
through constant-current
optical isolators. The
amount of stimulus current was determined from
the voltage measured across a l-k0 resistor in
series with the electrode. Searching for antidromitally excitable neurons was routinely done with
stimulus currents of 1 .O mA. Once an antidromitally excitable neuron was isolated, its excitation
threshold was determined and the stimulus current was then set to an amount -50% greater
than threshold. We defined threshold as the current intensity at which antidromic
spikes were
obtained in response to -50% of the stimulus
pulses. Single-shock stimulation
never appeared
to have any effect on the monkey; no eye or skeletal movement
were ever evoked, nor did the
monkey ever display a behavior that could be interpreted as resulting from an aversive stimulus.
In fact, the monkey frequently fell asleep during
the interval between behavioral trials when the
superior colliculus stimulus was administered.
Antidromically
excited neuronal responses were
identified by their fixed latency and by our ability
to collide the neuronal spike with spontaneous
spikes originating in the frontal eye field (3, 16).
The amplified electrode signal was sampled at 12 1
1392
M.
A. SEGRAVES
AND
kHz by an analog-to-digital
converter and saved
in computer memory. One sampling of the signal
from the electrode lasted for 8.5 ms and was triggered by either the onset of the collicular stimulus
or the beginning of a spontaneous neuronal spike
in the frontal eye field. The digitized electrode
signal trace was displayed on an oscilloscope and
could be averaged with successive traces following
the same trigger. Single and averaged traces were
saved by the computer for future analysis and display. Figure 3 shows examples of antidromic excitation (Fig. 3, A and C) and collision (Fig. 3, B
and 0). Two smaller neuronal spikes, driven by
the collicular stimulus but not collidable by the
larger neuron spike, are indicated by arrows in
Fig. 3, C and D.
Trains of pulses were occasionally applied at a
frequency of 330 Hz through the recording microelectrode, to establish that the electrode was located in the low-threshold
frontal eye field or the
intermediate
layer of the superior colliculus.
Trains of stimulation
in the deeper colliculus,
close to the central gray occasionally evoked stereotyped skeletal movements,
or, more rarely,
aversive reactions. In the latter cases train stimulation was stopped and the electrode elevated to a
nonaversive site. Train durations of 70 ms were
used to evoke saccades, and durations >300 ms
were used to evoke smooth pursuit (8).
We searched for frontal eye field corticotectal
neurons in two different ways. We obtained the
highest yield of antidromically
excitable neurons
by stimulating at 50- to loo-pm intervals along a
penetration through cortex, testing the activity of
a neuron during behavioral tasks only when it had
been identified as a corticotectal neuron. Alternatively, we made penetrations through the cortex
and tested the activity of isolated neurons during
the various behavioral tasks and then stimulated
the colliculus to see if the neuron could be excited
antidromically.
The latter method was used to
make sure that our electrode penetrations sampled all of the neuron types previously identified
in the frontal eye field (7).
Behavioral and neuronal activity were sampled
at 1 kHz. Rasters and histograms
were constructed with bin widths of 4 ms (250 Hz) on-line
from this I-kHz sampled data and were then
stored on magnetic disk for off-line analysis. Because the original I-kHz data was resampled at
250 Hz, minor quantization
differences occurred
between pairs of rasters and histograms constructed from the same original data set but with a
fixed relative offset, for example beginning and
end of a target flash. Since the precise positioning
of sampling bins would not necessarily be the
same for two different pairs of rasters and histograms, a given spike could fall in one bin on one
raster and in an adjacent bin in the other raster.
M.
E. GOLDBERG
c
A
B
C
*
U
A
1 ms
*
A
A
FIG. 3. Example
of antidromic
excitation,
collision.
Each trace begins with the occurrence
of a spontaneous
spike generated
by a neuron isolated in the frontal eye
field. Traces are aligned upon the superior
colliculus
stimulus artifact. A and B: single traces 8.5 ms in duration digitized by a high-speed analog-to-digital
converter
(see METHODS);
C and D: averages of 10 consecutive
traces. In A, 1 marks the spontaneous
neuronal
spike; 2,
the superior colliculus stimulus artifact; and 3, the antidromically
excited neuronal
spike. When the spontaneous spike and superior colliculus stimulation
were separated by 2.5 ms (A and C), the isolated neuron
was
excited antidromically
with a latency of 1.5 ms. However, when the separation
between
spontaneous
spike
and stimulus was reduced to 0.6 ms, the antidromically
evoked spike collided with the spontaneous
spike and
was not seen at the frontal eye field electrode (B and D).
Note that two smaller neuron spikes evoked at constant
latencies of 2.0 and 4.6 ms (arrow
in C and D) were not
affected. Time between ticks in lowest trace was 1 ms.
Stimulus
intensity,
50 PA.
Furthermore,
two spikes falling in one bin were
treated as one spike, and thus a pair of spikes with
frequency ~250 Hz could fall in one bin and
count as a single spike in one raster, or fall in
adjacent bins and count as two spikes in another
raster. This effect is evident in the histograms of
Figs. 8, B and C’, 10, -4 and B, and 11, B and C.
These minor differences had absolutely no influence upon the classification of the firing activity of
these units.
FRONTAL
EYE FIELD CORTICOTECTAL
Data analysis
NEURON CLASSIFICATION.
The behavioral
tasks described
above were used to classify
neurons according to the schema proposed by
Bruce and Goldberg
(7). The largest class of
neurons in the frontal eye field are presaccadic
neurons, which discharge before a visually guided
eye movement. Presaccadic neurons are classified
as visual, movement, or visuomovement.
VC2&
Y~~URVZSrespond to peripheral
visual stimuli
whether or not the monkey actually uses the stimulus as the target for a saccade. The response of a
visual neuron may be enhanced when the monkey
does make a saccade to the visual target, but the
neuron does not fire when the monkey makes the
identical eye movement in a learned saccade task
in which the target light is not presented. Movement neuronshave little or no response to visual
stimuli, but very strong activity before both visually guided and memory-guided
saccades.
Movement
neurons have definable movement
fields, but give little or no discharge before spontaneous saccades, of the appropriate
direction,
made in the dark. They do not discharge in response to visual stimulation
of the fovea. Visuomovementneuronsshow both visual and movement-related activity, but their optimal discharge
occurs before visually guided saccades, which distinguishes them from movement neurons whose
activity is similar before both visually and memory-guided
saccades.
The visuomovement
neurons occupy a continuum from strongly visual
neurons
with weak movement
discharge
to
strongly movement neurons with weak visual activity. All presaccadic neurons can have anticipatory activity, beginning their discharge before the
signal to make a saccade in paradigms where the
monkey expects to make a saccade of a certain
amplitude and direction. There are a number of
neurons that cannot be classified as presaccadic.
Two types are of interest to this study: fovea1
neurons
and postsaccadic
neurons.
Fovea/
neuronsrespond to visual stimulation of the fovea
and include neurons with on or off responses to
fovea1 stimulation. We found uncomplicated
foveal neurons that could only be driven by fovea1
stimuli and complex fovea1 neurons whose fovea1
responses were combined with visual, visuomovement, or movement anticipatory activity. Postsaccadicneuronsbegin to discharge during and
after saccades, but not before.
Histology
Marking lesions were made at the site of an
antidromically
excited neuron in each hemisphere and at the lowest threshold collicular sites
from which these neurons could be antidromitally excited. At the conclusion of the experiment,
the monkeys were given a lethal dose of pento-
NEURONS
1393
barbital sodium and were perfused transcardially
with saline followed by 10% formalin. The brains
were then removed, photographed,
and frozen
sectioned at 48 pm. Two planes of section were
used. Sections through the superior colliculus
were made in the coronal/frontal
plane. Sections
through the arcuate sulcus were made in a plane
running rostrocaudally,
parallel to the principal
sulcus (see Fig. 5). Every 10th section was
mounted and stained with cresyl violet. Additional sections were stained as needed.
RESULTS
Physiological
antidromically
classzj?cation of.
excited neurons
We physiologically
characterized
5 1 antidromically
excited
neurons.
Twenty-four
neurons were recorded
from the left frontal
eye field of monkey M30, and 22 neurons
from the right and 5 neurons from the left
frontal eye field of monkey IM50. Table 1
shows the number and percentage of corticotectal neurons in each category of neuronal activity.
LMOVEMENT
NEURONS.
Fifty-three percent of the corticotectal
neurons (27
neurons) identified and characterized in this
study belonged to the movement category.
Figure 4 shows the activity, recorded during
the memory-guided saccade task (Fig. 1D),
from a movement neuron antidromically
excited from the superior colliculus. The
neuron did not discharge in response to the
appearance of the target flash (Fig. 4A) but
discharged briskly before the saccade (Fig.
4B), with a burst starting - 160 ms before,
1. Distribution
neurons antidromically
the superior colliculus
TABLE
Monkey
Activity
Type
Visual
Visuomovement
Movement
Postsaccadic
Fovea1
Others
Nonresponsive
Totals
See METHODS
M30
ofactivity typesjtir
excited from
Monkey
hf.50
Total
Percent
0
0.0
12
3
27
2
4
9
11
5.9
52.9
2.0
21.6
15.7
2.0
24
27
3
1.5
for description
8
of neuron
51
activity
100.1
categories.
M.
1394
A. SEGRAVES
AND
A
M.
E. GOLDBERG
B
Frontal
Hvv
Eye Field
/
r loo
FP L
p1
f
T
H
vv
FP ‘-1
T1
1
.....
.--.
...
.
.
.
.
w
.
...
.
...
.....
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
. .
.
...
.
.s-
.
.
..
.
.
.....
.
,
-.
.....
.
...
.
.
.
W”
.
.....
....
.
.
”
...
.....
.
...
.
.
...
.
.
.
.
.
.“_
.
..e
.
.
..
...
.
.........
.”
.
.
I.
.
..
..-.
...............
.
.......
.I
.
. .
.
.
..
.
....
.......
.
"S.
-.a-
._.”
...
.
.
..-w.
.
.
....
.
.
.......
...
.
.
...
..”
.
.
w
.
.
...
..“.
...
.........
.
..-
.
.
.
.
.w
. ..s
.”
.
.
..“.
.”
.
.
...
.m
-.-
..
....
....
..-.
.--
.....
.
.
..”
......
.
.
..
...
“M.
.
.
.
....
..-.s
.
.
.......
.
...
.
..
WV
.
.-.-
.
.
.w
...
.-.
.
..
.
.
......
..
..e-
.
s
.
..-.s.
.I
...
.......
.
B-.-w
.
.
.
. ..q
..“-.
.
.
..
..-.
.
.....
m
.....
....
.-
......
w-..-
.
Activity
.
....
.
.
..
*-
..
-.
“W
.
....
,
.
...
I.
;
.
.
.
.
......
...
’
.:
.
200 ms/div
C
D
Superior
vv
FP
T
1
Activity
Hv\
loo
I
I1
Colliculus
1
11
I
FP
T1
1
I
I
I
I
I
I
~-1
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
200 ms/div
FIG. 4. Activity
of a frontal eye field movement
neuron antidromically
excited from the superior colliculus (A, B)
and multineuron
activity recorded at the collicular stimulation
site with lowest threshold
for antidromic
excitation
of
the frontal eye field neuron (C, D). Activity
was obtained
during performance
of the memory-guided
saccade task
(Fig. lD), and each pair of illustrations
compares activity synchronized
on the appearance
of the target (A, C) with
activity synchronized
on the beginning of the saccade (B, D). Each portion of this figure includes a raster, histogram,
and sample eye movement
traces for the paradigm
employed.
Each raster dot represents one neuron spike (sampled
at 250 Hz), every raster line includes the neuron activity from a 2-s interval
of a single trial. Each histogram
is the
summation
of the raster illustrated
above it with 4-ms bin width. The calibration
mark to the left of each histogram
FRONTAL
EYE
FIELD
CORTICOTECTAL
and peaking at the beginning of the saccade.
The center of the movement field for this
neuron was 5’ from the fixation point in the
lower right quadrant (5” radius, 308’ angle
in polar coordinates).
The latency for antidromic excitation from the superior colliculus was 1.4 ms. An antidromic
excitation
threshold of 110 PA was first recorded for
this neuron while stimulating from a depth
within the superior colliculus where purely
visual activity was recorded prior to stimulation. However, the minimum threshold for
this neuron was obtained when the stimulating electrode was lowered 1 mm deeper into
the superior colliculus. At this depth, the antidromic
excitation threshold was 70 PA.
The threshold for excitation increased as the
electrode was advanced further, reaching a
level of 115 PA at a depth of 1.5 mm below
the initial stimulation site. Multiunit activity
recorded at the superior colliculus stimulation site with lowest threshold showed a preponderance of activity surrounding
the beginning of the eye movement, with only a
slight visual response as is shown in Fig. 4, C
and D. The center of the movement field for
this multiunit activity was of greater amplitude, but in roughly the same direction into
the lower right quadrant ( 11 O radius, 328’
angle). The lesions marking the recording
site for this neuron in the frontal eye field, as
well as the stimulation
site with lowest
threshold in the superior colliculus are illustrated in Fig. 5. The lesion marking the location of the corticotectal
neuron was within
cortical layer V. The site of lowest threshold
for antidromic excitation was within the intermediate gray layer of the superior colliculus.
2. VISUOMOVEMENT
NEURONS.
we
anti-
dromically
excited three visuomovement
neurons (5.9%), one of which is shown in Fig.
6. In the memory-guided
saccade task, this
neuron had a weak visual response (Fig. 6A)
followed by much stronger activity associated with the eye movement
(Fig. 6B).
Note that for this task, the beginning of the
visual response and movement activity are
FIG.
4,
cont.
NEURONS
1395
separated in time by ~750 ms. The activity
of this neuron in the visually guided saccade
task is shown in Fig. 6, C and D. Note the
brisker response for the visually guided saccade, in which both the movement and the
visual components
occur simultaneously.
The latency for antidromic excitation of this
neuron was 1.2 ms. The minimum current
threshold for excitation was 160 PA. Figure 7
shows photomicrographs
of the recording
and stimulation sites for this neuron. As was
the case for the movement neuron described
above, the recording site was localized to
cortical layer V, and the stimulation site with
lowest current threshold was within the intermediate gray layer of the superior colliculus. Neurons with visuomovement
activity
occurred infrequently in our sample of frontal eye field corticotectal neurons (3 neurons,
5.9%). Each of the other identified visuomovement neurons had activity that resembled that shown in Fig. 6 with a weak visual
response overshadowed
by strong movement
activity.
The activity preceding saccades for both
visuomovement
and movement corticotectal
neurons occurred before both visually guided
and memory-guided
saccades.
3. FOVEAL
NEURONS.
Eleven neurons
(21.6%) responded to visual stimulation
of
the retinal fovea. Fovea1 neurons included
six neurons that exhibited a relatively uncomplicated response to either the onset or
disappearance of a fovea1 stimulus. The remaining five neurons exhibited fovea1 responses in combination
with activity that
was anticipatory, evoked by peripheral visual
stimuli, or related to some other behavioral
or environmental
parameter. The response
of a foveal-on neuron is illustrated in Fig. 8.
This neuron increased its firing rate immediately after the appearance of the central fixation light at the start of the trial (Fig. 8A) in
the fixation task. Its activity level decreased
during the time when the fixation light was
turned off during the trial (Fig. 8B) and was
reactivated following the reappearance of the
light (Fig. 8C). Figure 9 shows the activity of
represents a firing rate of 100 spikes/s. The vertical line passing through
both raster and histogram
is the point of
alignment
for the activity. The abbreviations
and conventions
for the eye movement
and stimulus traces at the top of
each raster are identical to those of Fig. 1.
1396
M. A. SEGRAVES
AND M. E. GOLDBERG
anterior
ran
B
.i-3 ‘i
a cm
FIG. 5. Recording and stimulation sites for the same corticotectal neuron illustrated in Fig. 4. A: recording site
for antidromically excited neuron marked by a lesion in cortical layer V in the fundus of the arcuate sulcus--blackened in drawing of section through arcuate sulcus, and indicated by white arrowhead
in photomicrograph of same
section. Cortical sections were made in a plane that was roughly parallel to that of the principal sulcus. The neuron
was located in the left frontal eye field, and the cortical sections are oriented so that anterior is to the left, posterior to
the right. In the section drawing, the shaded area on the anterior bank ofthe arcuate sulcus marks a region of necrotic
tissue, probably resulting from the effects of previous penetrations. P.S.,principal sulcus; AS, arcuate sulcus. B:
lesion marking stimulation site in the intermediate gray layer of the left superior colliculus. Coronal section. SGS,
stratum griseum superticiale; SGI, stratum griseum intermediale; SGP, stratum griseum profundum.
FRONTAL
FP L
T
I
.
w
.
,*
.
.
.
. ..s.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
,
.
.
.
.
.
..-m.
.
. .
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
s-.--w
*
.
.
.
_..L
.
IS
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
m--w.”
.
.
.
. . .
.
.
.
.
e
.
.
.
..
.
.
.
.
.. .
.
.
.
NEURONS
1397
.
.
.
. .
. .
.
e
.
..“.-...
.
.
.
.
.w-.-.m.
.
.-we-.
.
“.--....
..w.-.-.-
.
.
.
.
.
.MLI....
.
.
.
.
. .
.
.e...
m--m
.
. . ,
.
. .
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
-.
.M
.
CORTICOTECTAL
FP 1
T
.
.
FIELD
L
.
w
.
EYE
.
.
.
.
.*
200 ms/div
C
i
f
l
loo
FP
[
FP
-
200 ms/div
6. Corticotectal
visuomovement
neuron activity. A, B: activity during memory-guided
saccade task. C, D:
visually guided saccade task. Rasters and histograms
aligned on the onset of the peripheral
target in A and C and on
the beginning
of the saccadic eye movement
in I? and D. Note that the visuomovement
neuron has both visual
activity, seen in the first burst in A and B, and movement
activity, seen in the second burst in A and B. In Cand D the
visual activity burst and the movement
activity burst are superimposed,
and the activity is greater than either of the
bursts in A and II. The visual nature of the onset of the burst is illustrated
by its excellent
synchrony
with the
appearance
of the target in C. The burst continues through the movement
in each trial.
FIG.
M. A. SEGRAVES
1398
AND M. E. GOLDBERG
As
anterior
posterior
$jj
-
3? 1
FIG. 7. Recording and stimulation sites for the corticotectal neuron illustrated in Fig. 6. Same conventions as used
in Fig. 5. A: the lesion marking the recording site for this neuron was in layer V in the posterior bank of the arcuate
sulcus near the fundus of the sulcus. B: the stimulation site was in the intermediate gray layer of the superior
colliculus.
a neuron that at first appeared to be movement related (Fig. 94. However, this activity
occurred before saccades in every direction
in the memory-guided saccade task and was,
in fact, best interpreted as a visual response
to the disappearance of the fixation light
FRONTAL
A
EYE
CORTICOTECTAL
NEURONS
B
FP
H
V
[
H
V
loo
FP ,-I
f
II
1399
C
H
V
I
FIELD
1
III
I
III
I
I
FP
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
1
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
200 ms/div
IG. 8. Corticotectal
neuron with foveal-on
response. Fixation
the fixation
light at the beginning
of the trial. B: disappearance
fixation. C: reappearance
of the fixation light 500-ms later.
(Fig. 9B). In the fixation task, this neuron
became quiet when the fixation light was
turned on at the beginning of the trial (Fig.
9C). It fired during the period when the fixation point was off in the middle of the trial
(Fig. 90) and ceasedfiring -75 ms after the
reappearance of the fixation point (Fig. 9E).
It became active once more, 100 ms after the
disappearance of the fixation point at the end
of the trial (Fig. 9F). It should be noted that
the activity of this neuron in the saccadetask
is not fully explained by a foveal-off response, since the increase in firing rate actually occurs before the disappearance of the
fixation light (Fig. 9B). This may represent
activity that anticipates the impending eye
movement. In the fixation task, where saccades are suppressed,the neuron’s firing rate
did not increase until after the disappearance
of the fixation light (Fig. 90).
As mentioned above, about one-half
(5/ 11) of the fovea1 corticotectal neurons exhibited more than a simple on or off response.The activity of one of these complex
fovea1 neurons is illustrated in Fig. 10. In the
fixation task, this neuron stopped firing
when the fixation point was turned off during the trial (Fig. 1OA) and resumed firing in
response to the reappearance of the fixation
light (Fig. 1OB). However, its firing rate was
affected by more than fovea1 visual stimulation alone. Figure 10, C and D, shows this
neuron’s activity during a variation of the
task. Rasters and
of fixation
light
histograms aligned. A:
while
the monkey
onset of
maintained
visually guided saccade task where the peripheral target light was continually present.
The omnipresence of the peripheral target
prevented it from evoking a phasic response
from the neuron. The neuron was active
while the monkey fixated the central fixation
point, but it showed a suppressionof activity
when the fixation point disappeared. This
suppressionwas followed by a reactivation of
the neuron’s activity coincident with the
onset of the saccade that would attain the
target, that is the future fixation point, in the
periphery. Note that this increase in firing
occurs before the target light is foveated.
There did not appear to be a preferred target
location associated with this activity. The
neuron’s activity was linked to the fixation of
a peripheral target light, rather than the retinotopic location of that target.
Corticotectal fovea1 neurons did not appear to be localized to a particular portion of
the topographic map of the frontal eye field.
They were found in the vicinity of movement neurons with preferred saccadeamplitudes ranging from 6 to 30’. Likewise, the
fovea1 neurons were antidromically excited
from a wide range of points within the topographic representation in the superior colliculus.
NEURONS.
There was
one neuron (2.0%) in our sample of corticotectal neurons classified as postmovement.
4. POSTMOVEMENT
M. A. SEGRAVES
1400
FP;
T1
FP
FP 1
I
T1
.
.-.
.
.
-
..
......
....
...
..-
.m
..
..
.....
.
.....
.
...
....
.
*.*I
*
.
. . ..w
.
.
........
..”
. .
.
.
.
.
...
..
w
.
.
.
.....
.
.
. .
.
.
. -.
..
...
-.
.
.-.
. ..m
.
-
. ..
...
.
....
...
-.
......
......
-
...
...
.....
.
...
w.-
........
..
...........
...
.. . ,
. . . . . . .. . .
.. . . .
..
. -. . . . . . . . . -.
.-.
.
.
.-... . . .
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
. .. ..
. . .
.
...
....
“._.
...
....
.
.
.
..-
....
.
...........
......
SW.“..“.
-
.
..
-.-..
.
......
.
...........
.
s
.........
.....
......
....
.
...
....
.
.
.
.
.......
.....
...-.
.
.
...
C..
.
........
........
..
.
..w
.....
.
.....
.........
......
..
.
...
.s....
..
. ..-.
.........
..
.
.
....
w
.
._...“.
.....
.
.
.
. .-.
..-
....
....
.
.
.....
..
...
..........
......
.
.
......
....
.
.
.“.
...
.
....
.
...
.-.
...
....
.
AND M. E. GOLDBERG
-
..
. . -----**. --.....*
‘I”
...
.... ...
....
.
.
. .-, .
..
.
..-.
.-...- . .. . . .. . .. .
.. . .
.
.
*
‘ I
I
I
II
1
I
1
200 ms/div
D
E
H
V
FP
F
H
H
V
[
V
loo
I
1
FP
t
FP 1
I
.
.
.
.-.
. .
.
1.
.
. -
. .
.
.
.
. . ..^..
.
..“..
FIG. 9. Neuron responsive to disappearance of fixation light. A, B:
memory-guided saccade task. Data in A
aligned to beginning of eye movement, B aligned to disappearance of fixation point-the signal to make a saccade.
C-l? fixation task. Data in C aligned to when the fixation light appeared at the beginning of the trial. D,
disappearance of fixation light during the trial; E, reappearance of the fixation light; and F, disappearance of the
fixation light at the end of the trial.
Its activity was related to saccades, but began
after the beginning of the eye movement.
There were eight
antidromically excited neurons ( 15.7%), four
in each monkey, that could not be classified
as visual, visuomovement,
movement, postsaccadic, or foveal. They were a heterogeneous group, and included neurons with I)
high spontaneous activity that was suppressed 100 ms before and during saccades to
the contralateral hemifield (1 neuron), 2) activity that began after the eye movement in
5. OTHER NEURON TYPES.
the visually guided saccade task and anticipated the occurrence of the reward (1
neuron), 3) a burst of activity before an eye
movement followed by a high activity rate up
to the start of the next trial (1 neuron), 4) a
sustained response to visual stimuli in the
ipsilateral hemifield (1 neuron), 5) activity
that combined an enhanced visual response,
anticipatory, and postmovement activity (1
neuron), or anticipatory and postmovement
activity (1 neuron), and 6) neurons that discharged briskly during smooth pursuit in
certain directions (2 neurons). The activity of
FRONTAL
EYE
FIELD
...
.
.”
.
”
....
.
CORTICOTECTAL
.
.
..-
m.
.
.
.
1401
.
.
.”
...
.
.
......
.
.m”.
.
.
.
-
........"
.
NEURONS
.
.....
.......
.....
...
.
.
.w
..“.
....
.......
.
.
.
.
.“.
.
.
.
.
. .
.
.
.
.
.....
.
.
-a..
.
. .
.
.
. .
. .
.
“...““.
.
. .
.
.
. .
.
.”
.
.
.
. .
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
. .
. .
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.”
.
.
.
.
. .
.
.
.
. .
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.”
.
..”
.
,
.”
.
.
.
.
.
.
8
.
.
.
.
”
.
.
.
.
.W..”
m
.
.
.
a..
,
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
. .
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
. .
.
.“..
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
em.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
. .
..
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
. .
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
. . .
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
u..
. .
.
.
.
.
“m
.
. .
.
.”
.
.
.
. . .
.
. .
.
.
.
.
“.
.
”
200 ms, div
FP
T
FP
.
.
.““.
“.
-...-.
”
.“.
.
.
..
”
...
...
.
..
.
....
”
....
. .
“.
.”
.“.
...
..
”
......
.-.-.e...”
......
m-
..”
.
.w
....
.
.
.
...
..
..-
““.”
.
...
....
-
.
.“_.
.
w
.
m
.
. . .
.
.
.
m ...
......
.
...
._.““.
........
. .”
S..“.“.
.L
.
.
“.
......
.
...
....
..
..”
....
.
.
-
“.
....
..-..B”
.
.rn..”
“.
“I
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
...
.
.
.
...
.
.
.
.....
..
..”
..”
”
..
...
.
.....
--
.
.
.....
.
...
.
...
“.N.W”
..““.
.
...
..
.-
I
..
...
.”
.
.
“”
.
.
”
..
......
.....
......
.....
.
.“.
.
.”
......
.
...
...
...........
.
.
me
....
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
...
*..
.“..“.
...
“.
.
.
.“..
....
.
...
.
.
.
.
........
..”
.
..w.m.-
....
.
...
.
..““.o.
.
.
..
. .
.....
.
..w
.
.
.
.
.
.
......
....
.
...
. .
.
.
.
.w
.....
.
.-.
..-.
.
.......
.
.“.
...
...
...
.
.
....
“.
....
.“.”
.
.“-m.o.-.
.......
............
.-
.
““.
_....“.“.
....
.
-
..........
...
......
*
..
...........
..
.
...
.
-
..
.“.
.
.
200 ms/div
FIG. 10.
Response of a complex
fovea1 neuron in the fixation
(A-B) and saccade (C-D)
tasks. A: aligned on
disappearance
of fixation
light during fixation
task trial. B: reappearance
of fixation
light. C-D:
activity
in a
variation
of the visually guided saccade task where the peripheral
target is always on. C: aligned on disappearance
of
fixation light. D: aligned on the beginning
of the saccade. Note that in C and D, the target light status line begins and
ends in the “on” condition,
since the target light was never turned off in this task. The monkey
was trained to always
fixate the central light whenever
it was present and make a saccade to and fixate the peripheral
target light only when
the central light was turned off.
two of these neurons is described in more
detail below.
Figure 11 illustrates the activity of a
neuron whose greatest activity occurred after
the monkey attained fixation at the very beginning of the trial in the memory-guided
saccade task (Fig. 1 IA). This neuron did not
respond to either the onset or disappearance
of the stimulus light (Fig. 11, B and C). After
the initial large burst of firing at the beginning of fixation, the neuron’s activity remained at a high rate throughout the trial
and ended with a burst of activity that began
after the start of the eye movement (Fig.
11D). This postmovement component of the
activity was strongest after saccades to targets
located on the horizontal meridian in the
contralateral hemifield at - 12’ away from
1402
M.
A. SEGRAVES
AND
*+====
M.
E. GOLDBERG
B!y=E
FP J
T
I
-. .
-. . T”“”
FP
T
1
..-.w. . .
. . . . . . . . . . . . . am..
J
. .---w-
lp
..“..S. . .
. . . . . . . . . . . . . Wm..
200 ms,
7
FP
T
T.I”.
.
....
-M.-w.
.
...
....
S”.
.....
m..-m-.
.
....
.
.
.......
. .
.....
.........
.--
.
...............
..
-.
....
.
. .
.....
.
-*-
w.
.
6
.
--.m
.
.
...
.
--
.
..
.
..w
...
.
...
1
.
.
.
..w.-
...
..
.....
..
.
....
.”
....
.
I
.....
..
.
.
.
....
.
..
/
.
...
...
. ..I
.-..-.
.
.
....
s-.-.
--.
...
.
.
..
-..-.
-
.
.
.
..
..
B-w
....
.
.
.WW”
-w-
“-w-w-.
.
s
.
.-
..w
..
....................
.-
.
.
“.I,
....
.a.....--
.
....
...
..M
......
..-...a.-_.
.
.
.
.
.
-...w
.
..................
..-
..
.,
..-.
...
.
....
.
-.
....
-.
m..
...
W”.
.
.....
.
-...-.
...
..I
“.....-
...
-
.......
.
.
.
.
....
-..-
-
...
.
. .
..
.
.
..
..
.......
. .
.
.
..-
.
,
.
.......
.
-.
....
“.w”...-....
....
......
.
...
.-
....
.
........
....
...
*
.
....
. .
..-
.-.
.
M”..“.
-
..M”..
........
/
..
-”
..
.-S.W”.
...
..-.
.
......
........
m...
.
..
I.
....
.......
-
,
.
...
......
....
.
s-s
.
........
.-
..a--
.
......
...
....
.
a..
..-e-
,
“...“.
..“.
..-..
“I..
.
.
..-
.
w..
....
.
......
.
....
...
.s-
.....
.
..
-w..#.
.......
....
S.-s.
SW.
em
.....
...
..-.
........
L
1
.
.
.
...
.
...
...
.
*-
200 ms/div
FIG. 11.
Activity
of a corticotectal
neuron
with combined
anticipatory
and postmovement
activity,
during a
memory-guided
saccade task. A: activity
aligned on the achievement
of fixation,
at the beginning
of the trial. B:
activity aligned on the appearance
of the target. C: activity aligned on the disappearance
of the target. D: activity
aligned on the beginning
of the saccade.
the center of gaze. It also occurred after saccades made in the dark. Although
this
neuron responded weakly to the reappearance of the fixation light in the fixation task,
the magnitude of the activity occurring before the eye movement in the task illustrated
is best described as a form of anticipatory
activity. In general, anticipatory neuron activity in the frontal eye field is strongest when
the monkey is required to make eye movements to the same target location in consecutive trials. Under these conditions, the events
of the trial become predictable,
and the
monkey anticipates their occurrence (7). For
the neuron shown in Fig. I 1, the early activity was not a response to a visual stimulus.
The activity preceded the beginning of the
eye movement by - 1,200 ms, so it could not
FRONTAL
EYE
FIELD
CORTICOTECTAL
be considered strictly movement related. Instead, it appears to have anticipated events
that would occur near the end of the trial, in
particular, the saccade and its associated reward.
Another neuron showed a modulation of
response
during different
directions
of
smooth-pursuit
eye movements. It did not
respond to either peripheral or fovea1 visual
stimuli, and it did not respond in saccade
tasks. However, it was active while the monkey tracked~ slowly moving targets with
smooth-pursuit
eye movements. The neuron
preferred tracking movements directed up to
the left, a direction
contralateral
to the
neuron’s location in cortex. It was also active, to a lesser extent, during smooth-pursuit
eye movements
down to the left, but was
nearly silent during tracking movements directed ipsilaterally. The neuron was best activated by slow stimulus velocity in the preferred direction. Using a target with sinusoidally varied velocity, and a peak-to-peak
excursion of 20”, the preferred frequency
was 0.1 Hz, producing a mean eye speed of
4”/s. This neuron had a high rate of activity
during the inter-trial interval, and its best activity during smooth pursuit tasks did not
exceed its activity during the intertrial interval. Electrical
stimulation
(0.2-ms pulse
width, 330 Hz, 400-ms train, 75PA pulses) at
the recording site of this neuron in the frontal eye field produced
upward
slow eye
movements that began -45 ms after stimulus onset, and lasted until the stimulus was
turned off.
6. NONRESPONSIVE
NEURONS.
one neuron
(2.0%), antidromically
excited from the su-
NEURONS
1403
perior colliculus, could not be activated during any of the behavioral tasks.
Neurons adjacent to ant idrom ically
excitable neurons
Most antidromically
excitable neurons
were isolated during electrode penetrations
where only responses to electrical stimulation of the superior colliculus were routinely
examined, and neuron activity during visual
and eye movement tasks was studied only
after an antidromically
excitable neuron had
been isolated. However,
we frequently
recorded the activity
characteristics
of all
neurons encountered in a single penetration
to ascertain
the overall distribution
of
neuron types that were being sampled by the
electrode. As an example, Fig. 12 illustrates
the activity of neurons encountered during a
penetration through 6 mm of cortex made
with a platinum-iridium
electrode. Because
of the length of this penetration, it is likely
that the electrode traveled obliquely through
the cortical layers of the frontal eye field, beginning at layer I and ending in white matter
after passing through layer VI. The thickness
of the frontal eye field cortex from surface to
white matter is normally ~2 mm. Two antidromically excitable neurons were isolated
during this penetration.
Neuronal activity
recorded at the beginning of the penetration
contained only visual responses, including
tonic multineuron visual responses recorded
at a depth of 1,502 pm (Fig. 12A). Movement-related activity was first encountered at
3,300 pm, and at this depth the movement
component of the multineuron
activity was
stronger than the visual component. The first
FIG. 12. Neurons
encountered
during penetration
through 6 mm of frontal eye field cortex. A-G shows neuronal
activity. In each truce H and V are horizontal
and vertical eye position,
respectively,
with 10” calibration
mark shown
for each pair. For each example of neural activity (A-G) two rasters and histograms
are shown. In the left of each pair
activity is synchronized
on the appearance
of the target. In the right of each pair activity is synchronized
on the
tonic visual activity recorded at 1,502 pm. Memory-guided
saccade task.
beginning of the saccade. A: multineuron
excited neuron with movementNote that activity starts with stimulus and ends at movement.
B: antidromically
related presaccadic activity, 3,75 1 pm. Memory-guided
saccade task. Note absence of response to target onset (left). C
and D: neurons with postmovement
activity recorded at 3,742 and 3,841 pm. Memory-guided
saccade task with
zero delay between target light and fixation
point disappearance.
Note lack of response to target onset (left) and
excited neuron isolated, premoveresponse beginning
with beginning
of saccade (right).E: second antidromically
ment activity combined
with strong anticipatory
activity. Memory-guided
saccade task with zero delay. Note activity
neurons. Note
preceding target onset (left) and burst occurring
before movement
(right).F and G: postmovement
no activity synchronized
on target onset. (left) and activity beginning
after the beginning
of the saccade (right).H
indicates the locations
of the recording
sites for the neurons illustrated
in A-G. The vertical rule shows distance
traversed by electrode. The asterisks mark the sites at which antidromically
excited neurons were found. The activity
of these antidromically
excited neurons is shown in B and E. See text for additional
details.
110°
r
FP 1
T
H
v
FP
T
-
f
I
‘I
I
200 ms/div
_.
FP
T-
FP
T1
_____
.
FP
T
1loo
I
......
......
...
-.
-.....
_
......
.............
....
t_.
..
-.
.
..
.-.
-.--.
....
.
.........
_.
_
...
.......
.___
-__._
__
.
.....
.
-_.....
.
........
_
...........
....
.._.
-._.
_
....
......
_
.....
.......
..
_.
..
”
..
.._.
I..
..
....
-_-.
-.
.......
.
......
__...
.....
I,
............
...........
...... - _
....
.....
..........
. -.....
.....
........
7
1
200 ms/div
FP
T-
1
...
..
FRONTAL
7
*. .
*...
.
:
. . . .
* .
,
. .
. .
*
*.
*.
: : . *-.
. .
.
.
. . . .
‘1.:
*.
.- * . . ’ . .
..‘i*
a.......
;;:
.:I::
:;,*
.i .: . .
* :
,l;‘fl;!.~~;“i
. .
.
.:
*
*.
.
:‘.
. . .
Iiq!WI$/i
!ii~!!!!li:l.:/
$iI;;]]i[ji;j
.:I
i’I;‘!:
0
0
LJ
:
-, : ;;!ii;i!i;.
a*
-.,I’
: . *I. a..;..,. .
r-:
:-!..
:.
:..
. . . :
:
. .:i
.
.
.
..*
.
.
;
.
.
.
Ii’t
;:i:
I
;’
. .
. .
. .
;
.
:
EYE
FIELD
CORTICOTECTAL
NEURONS
1405
M.
1406
A. SEGRAVES
AND
antidromically
excitable neuron was isolated
at 3,75 1 pm (Fig. 12B). This neuron was
classified as a movement
neuron. It increased its firing rate - 100 ms before the
start of the eye movement.
However,
the
bulk of its activity occurred after the beginning of the eye movement. A neuron isolated
adjacent to the corticotectal
neuron at a
depth of 3,742 pm (Fig. 12C) had entirely
postmovement
activity; it did not begin to
fire until after the beginning of the eye movement. A second postmovement
neuron was
encountered
at 3,841 pm (Fig. 120). At
4,050 pm, the second antidromically
excitable neuron was encountered
(Fig. 12E).
This was also a movement neuron, however,
its activity was very different from that of the
first corticotectal neuron. It exhibited strong
anticipatory activity, beginning immediately
after the target flash onset in the memoryguided saccade task, and included a premovement burst of activity that peaked before the start of the eye movement. Two additional
postmovement
neurons
were
isolated at 4,176 and 4,70 1 pm (Fig. 12, F
and G). The penetration was ended at 6,000
pm when the electrode entered white matter.
One of the most impressive components of
the activity of the corticotectal neuron iso-
M.
E. GOLDBERG
lated at 4,050 pm (Fig. 12E) was the suppression
of its activity
surrounding
eye
movements in the off direction. When the
monkey was required to make a saccade directed away from the optimal direction for
the movement activity of the neuron, there
was a strong suppression of the neuron’s activity beginning 100 ms after the onset of the
target light in the visually guided saccade
task, and continuing through the eye movement (Fig. 13). The optimal presaccadic activity for this neuron was obtained when the
target light was presented on the horizontal
meridian, 10’ to the right. The suppression
of activity surrounding
the eye movement
was optimal when the target light was presented at 10’ to the left of the center of gaze,
near the horizontal meridian. It is interesting
to note that the optimal target location for
the postmovement
neurons illustrated in Fig.
12 was in the lower left (Fig. 12, C, D, and F)
and lower right (Fig. 12G) quadrants. The
optimal target location for the visual neurons
recorded at the beginning of this penetration
were in the upper right quadrant. Together
these results suggest that frontal eye field visual neurons have an excitatory effect upon
movement
neurons,
and postmovement
neurons have an inhibitory effect. However,
B
A
H
V
loo
FP
T
1
t
FP
I
T
1
t
.
.
. .
.
.
.
. .
. .
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
.
..s.
.
.
.
200 ms/div
FIG. 13. Suppression
surrounding
eye movements
in the off direction
for antidromically
excited neuron isolated
at 4,050 pm in the penetration
illustrated
in Fig. 12 (this neuron’s activity is shown in Fig. 12~9. Suppression
begins
100 ms after the onset of target light ( 12,176”) in visually guided saccade task (A) and continues until after the end of
the eye movement
(B).
FRONTAL
EYE
FIELD
CORTICOTECTAL
NEURONS
1407
n
n = 45
Mean
= 330 pamps
SD = 324 pamps
O-
~0 :B
$1
2 o=
n = 24
SD = 265
n = 26
Mean = 1.97 ms
SD = 0.89 ms
0
5
10
n
0
1000
1500
0
1000
1500
0
0
0
d0
m
0
cl
0
-
Threshold
5
Latency
10
(ms)
FIG. 14. Percent distribution
of latencies for antidrofor all corticotectal
mic excitation.
A: distribution
neurons
isolated
in this study. B: distribution
for
neurons classified as movement.
C: neurons classified
as foveal. Bin width is 0.5 ms.
these effects do not fully account for the activity of the corticotectal neuron illustrated
in Figs. 12E and 13, since the suppressionof
its activity begins 100 ms before the start of
the eye movement, and thus precedes the activity in the postmovement neurons.
One of the striking results of this study was
our failure to excite antidromically neurons
that had nerinheral visual recebtive fields but
pamps
FIG. 15.
dromic
(pamps)
Percent distributions
of thresholds
excitation.
Bin width is 50 PA.
for anti-
not movement activity. We isolated more
than 100 of these neurons during the course
of our exploration of the frontal eye field,
and we were never able to excite one antidromically. We made a number of penetrations specifically looking for visual neurons,
both through guide tubes from which we
successfully isolated corticotectal neurons,
and from simple searching penetrations
made after passing through the dura with
glass-coated platinum-iridium
electrodes.
Because visual neurons were common, and
antidromic excitation technically feasible in
our hands. we feel it is unlikelv that the fron-
M.
TABLE
A. SEGRAVES
AND
2. Mean latencies jbr antidromic
All neurons
Movement
neurons
Fovea1 neurons
Compare
movement
SD
SE
n
0.16
0.17
0.42
50
26
11
P
latenciesjor ail neurons
2.25
1.97
3.19
1.12
0.89
1.41
with fovea1
B.
All neurons
Movement
neurons
Fovea1 neurons
Compare
movement
E. GOLDBERG
excitation
Mean
A. AlI
M.
NS
<o.o 1
co.002
A4ean latencies with latencies G.0 excluded
2.04
1.83
2.62
with fovea1
0.74
0.58
0.68
0.11
0.12
0.26
47
25
NS
<0.02
<0.002
Entries include all corticotectal
neurons isolated in the frontal eye field, neurons whose activity was classified as
movement,
and those classified as foveal. Significance
levels are for comparison
of movement
or fovea1 to all
neurons,
and comparison
of movement
to foveal. A, includes all latencies for all neurons in each category;
B,
latencies >5.0 ms have been excluded.
tal eye field sends a significant visual signal
to the superior colliculus.
a standard deviation equal to 324 PA, and
ranged from 6 to 1,200 ,uA (Fig. 15).
To ensure that high stimulation currents
Antidromic Latencies
were not exciting extracollicular
frontal
The latencies for antidromic excitation
fibers, we studied the threshold for antidrowere unimodally distributed (Fig. 14). The mic stimulation as a function of location of
mean latency for all corticotectal neurons the collicular electrode for 22 of the 5 1 isowas 2.25 ms (range 1.2-6.0 ms). Movement
lated antidromically excitable neurons. To
neurons had a mean latency of 1.97 ms that do this we moved the stimulating electrode
was slightly shorter than that for all corticothrough the superior colliculus and recorded
tectal neurons, but this difference was not the threshold current for antidromic excitasignificant. Fovea1 neurons had a mean la- tion at 250-pm intervals in depth. In a few
tency of 3.19 ms, which was 0.9 ms longer cases,an electrolytic lesion was made at the
than the mean for all corticotectal neurons, minimum threshold site so that its position
and 1.2 ms longer than the mean for move- could be determined histologically. Two
ment neurons. This difference was signifisuch lesions have been reconstructed, and
cant in both instances (Table 2A). Three each was located within the intermediate
neurons had latencies >5 ms, and appear gray layers of the superior colliculus (see
separate from the main distribution in each Figs. 5 and 7). However, it was difficult to
graph. However, when these longer latencies recover electrolytic lesionsmade more than a
were excluded from the calculation and com- few weeks before the time at which the monparison of means, the same relationships de- key was perfused, and the number of lesions
scribed above were maintained (Table 2B).
made in each monkey was limited so that
The estimated conduction velocity of single lesion sites could be identified with a
frontal eye field corticotectal neurons based high degree of certainty. As an alternative
upon their mean antidromic excitation la- means for identifying the anatomical sites
tency is 18 m/s (range 7-34 m/s). This as- with lowest threshold, the locations in depth
sumesan estimated distance from fundus of where minimum thresholds were obtained
the arcuate sulcus (AP+22.0), through interwere correlated with the neuronal activity
nal capsule and thalamus, to midcolliculus
properties recorded at those depths. For 14 of
(AP- 1.5) of 40.5 mm.
the 22 antidromic neurons examined, such
comparisons could be made directly with
Antidromic thresholds
collicular neuronal activity recorded during
The current thresholds for antidromic ex- the threshold determination experiment. For
the other eight antidromic neurons neuronal
citation had a mean of 330 PA (n = 45), with
FRONTAL
EYE
FIELD
CORTICOTECTAL
Multiunit
NEURONS
Responses:
0 Surface
Visual
Visual
Visual
1 Visual
Saccade
> Saccade
> Visual
Saccade
> Visual
2
Saccade
> Visual
Deep layer
t”“l”“l”“l”“l”“I”“;
0
100
Threshold
200
for
300
Antidromic
400
Activation
500
600
(ramps)
FIG. 16.
Threshold
for antidromic
excitation
compared
with depth from collicular
surface. Based upon multineuron activity indicated on the right side of the figure, the transition
between superficial
and intermediate
layers was
at a depth of 1 mm from the collicular
surface. The border between intermediate
and deep layers was at 2.5 mm. The
minimum
threshold for antidromic
excitation
occurred at the transition
point between intermediate
and deep layers.
Antidromic
excitation
thresholds
increased both above and below this depth.
activity could not be recorded by the stimulation electrode, but the stimulating electrode depths could be determined by comparison with other penetrations. By these criteria each of the 22 neurons tested had their
minimum thresholds within the superior colliculus. The minimum thresholds for these
neurons ranged from 7 to 500 PA.
For all but one neuron, the minimum
threshold site in the colliculus was a reversal
point, with increasing thresholds above and
below the minimum threshold depth. For
one neuron, thresholds were only tested
above the minimum threshold site, so we
cannot rule out the possibility that its threshold would have decreased at lower depth.
Figure 16 shows a comparison of antidromic
threshold and collicular neuronal activity for
a single neuron stimulated at various sites on
a colliculus penetration. Note that the
threshold reached a minimum at a depth in
the colliculus of 2.5 mm. This depth corresponded to the transition between intermediate and deep layer activity. Just above the
reversal point, at 2.250 mm, the threshold
for electrically evoked eye movements began
to increase with increasing depth, and the
movement component of the multiunit ac-
tivity was diminished in strength relative to
that recorded at 1.750 mm. At 2.500 mm,
the phasic movement burst associated with
intermediate layer neuron activity was replaced by activity that increased before the
monkey achieved fixation and continued
until after the end of the eye movement in
the memory-guided saccade task. Of the 13
neurons where accurate correlations could
be made between threshold reversals and
collicular neuronal activity, the reversal
points of three neurons were located at
depths where superficial layerlike properties
were present, including a prominent visual
response,little or no movement activity, and
a relatively high threshold (generally >50
PA) for electrically evoked eye movements.
Nine neurons had reversal points at depths
where movement activity was recorded and
thresholds for electrically evoked eye movements were low ~50 PA). These include the
following. I) Two neurons with reversal
points at the transition between superficial
and intermediate layerlike activity-identified by the presenceof both visual and movement activity components, with a prominence of visual over movement activity. 2)
Five neurons with reversal points located at
200
ms/dlv
200
ms/dlv
FIG. 17. Comparison of frontal eye field and superior colliculus premovement activity for low-threshold corticotectal neuron. A: activity in visually guided saccade task
for a frontal eye field neuron excited from the superior colliculus with a threshold of 7 PA. B: single-neuron activity recorded at low-threshold site within the superior
colliculus. Rasters are aligned upon target onset. In both A and B the amplitude of the target’s displacement from the fixation point was 10”.
ii
8~~
B
FRONTAL
EYE
FIELD
CORTICOTECTAL
1411
NEURONS
-2
4
f4
.\
/
\
/
4
)
zq 001
1412
M.
A. SEGRAVES
AND
sites where activity characteristic
of the intermediate layers was recorded. These characteristics include strong movement activity,
weak or no visual response, and the lowest
threshold for electrically evoked eye movements. 3) Two neurons with reversal points
at the transition between intermediate and
deep collicular layers. At this depth, there
was a decrease in the strength of the movement activity, a return of visual activity, and
an increased threshold for electrically evoked
eye movements. One neuron’s reversal point
was localized to a depth with deep layerlike
activity where, in addition to increased visual
and decreased movement activity, the electrical stimulus train used to evoke eye movements appeared to be mildly aversive to the
monkey at currents of 75 PA, suggesting that
the electrode was in the vicinity of the central
gray matter.
In general, the lowest thresholds occurred
when the movement fields of the collicular
multiunit activity and the frontal eye field
neuron were alike. An example of this relationship is provided in Figs. 17 and 18. The
corticotectal
neuron illustrated in Fig. 17A
had movement activity that was strongest
preceding saccades to target positions on the
vertical meridian, 10’ below the horizontal
meridian. This neuron was antidromically
excited from the superior colliculus with a
minimum threshold of 7 PA. Premovement
single neuron activity recorded at the collicular stimulation
site (Fig. 17B) had a preferred target location that was similar to the
best target location for the frontal eye field
neuron. In contrast, the optimal target locations for the corticotectal neuron illustrated
in Fig. 18A and multineuron
activity recorded at its collicular stimulation site (Fig.
18B) differed in both amplitude and direction from the center of gaze. The center of
the movement field of the frontal eye field
neuron was at 15” radius and 45” angle, and
at 10’ radius and 270” angle for the collicular activity. The minimum threshold for antidromic excitation of this visuomovement
neuron was 160 PA. The ideal situation exemplified by the electrode pair illustrated in
Fig. 17 was rarely realized, however, because
of the different topographical organizations
of the superior colliculus
and frontal eye
field.
M.
E. GOLDBERG
We have con rpared antidromic excitation
thresholds with a value expressing the vector
difference between frontal eye field and collicular saccade vectors for data obtained for
25 frontal eye field visuomovement
and
movement neurons (Fig. 19). The “index of
vector difference” was equal to the amplitude of the vector difference between optimal frontal eye field and collicular saccade
vectors divided by the sum of the amplitudes
of the two saccade vectors. This method was
chosen to enable us to compare data from
both large and small eye movement sites.
Our hypothesis
was that stimulation
and
recording sites with similar optimal saccade
vectors would have low thresholds.
Since
equal saccade vectors result in an index of
vector difference of 0, and opposite saccade
vectors result in an index value of 1, it was
expected that low thresholds would be associated with index values near 0, and high
thresholds with index values approaching 1.
In fact, there was a significant linear correlation (Y = 0.62) between the regression of
index of vector difference upon log of threshold for antidromic
excitation (P < 0.001).
These data suggest that small increases in the
vector difference between the stimulation
0
n = 25
r = 0.62
p < 0.001
1
o-o,,,
I
I
rI11..,
10
Threshold
l
I
100
l
8
l
1
a
l
I
I .““7
1000
(ramps)
FIG. 19. Effect of topography
of recording and stimulation sites upon threshold
for antidromic
excitation.
Index of vector difference
is equal to the amplitude
of
the vector difference between optimal saccade vectors at
frontal eye field and collicular
sites divided by the sum of
the amplitudes
of the two vectors. The index of vector
difference
would equal 0 for identical
saccade vectors
and 1 for saccade vectors that are in opposite directions.
The data were taken from 25 antidromically
excited visuomovement
and movement
neurons.
Threshold
is ‘.
nlotted on a log scale.
FRONTAL
EYE
FIELD
CORTICOTECTAL
NEURONS
1413
and recording sites result in large increases in
threshold. For example, a 23% increase in
difference between saccade vector angles,
that is for saccades of equal loo amplitude
but increase in angular difference from 44 to
54”, would be accompanied by a doubling of
threshold for antidromic excitation from 50
to 100 ,uA.
frontal eye field and superior colliculus, as
well as several technical aspects concerning
these experiments.
Nonhomogeneous projection of frontal eye
jield activity to the colliculus
Figure 20 compares the distributions
of
our population
of identified corticotectal
neurons (solid bars) with the population of
all
frontal eye field neurons described by
DISCUSSION
Bruce and Goldberg (hatched bars) (7).
In this study, we have demonstrated that Fifty-three
percent of the corticotectal
the projection from the frontal eye field to neurons were classified as having movement
the superior colliculus, first described using activity, compared with 11% in the general
anatomical techniques, can also be shown sample. The second largest single group of
using electrophysiological
techniques in corticotectal neurons were those with fovea1
awake monkeys. The most striking result is responses. They included 20% of the corticothat there seems to be a selective enrichment
tectal neurons, but only 7% of all frontal eye
for two neuron types, those in which movefield neurons. There were no neurons antiment activity predominates, and those with dromically excited from the superior collicfovea1 visual activity. There is also a selection
ulus that could be classified as preferring
against two types of neurons, those with pe- purely peripheral visual stimuli. Neurons
ripheral visual receptive fields and those whose predominant
activity occurs during
whose discharge only occurs during and after and after the saccade only rarely project to
saccades. We will discuss the implications of the superior colliculus.
these results for our understanding
of the
Given such a striking difference between
generation of saccadic eye movements by the all the neurons in the area and the collicular
0
GO
q
q
All frontal
Neurons
eye
activated
field
neurons
antidromically
(Bruce
& Goldberg,
from
superior
1985),
colliculus,
n = 752
n = 51
Movement
Visual
movement
FIG. 20. Comparison
of distribution
among activity categories of frontal eye field neurons
from the superior
colliculus
(solid bars) to the distribution
of all frontal eye field neurons
Goldberg
(7) (hatched bars).
excited
reported
antidromically
by Bruce and
M.
A. SEGRAVES
AND
projection
neurons,
we must consider
whether or not this difference could be an
experimental artifact. The first possibility is
always sampling error. However, in order to
gather our modest population of antidromic
neurons we had to record from a much larger
population of frontal eye field neurons. We
made a special effort to find neurons of the
types that we could not drive antidromically.
Therefore probabilistic sampling error is unlikely. The second possibility is that there is a
physical difficulty that makes it possible for
antidromic
spikes to invade the somata of
some corticotectal neurons, but not others.
This is unlikely because anatomical studies
showing the somata of frontotectal
projection neurons fail to distinguish
multiple
morphologies for such neurons. They are all
large layer V pyramidal neurons (15, 34).
Therefore it is likely that the population of
antidromically
excited neurons was not terribly biased by a systematic error.
The next problem is whether or not an
electrode in the superior colliculus excites
frontal eye field fibers other than frontotectal
ones. This might seem especially likely in
view of the large currents needed to excite
some neurons. Threshold currents for antidromic excitation ranged from 6 to 1,200
PA, with a mean of 330 PA and standard
deviation of 324 PA. The administration
of
these currents within the superior colliculus
undoubtedly
resulted in both passive and
transsynaptic
spread of electrical activity for
some distance away from the tip of the stimulating electrode (2, 38, 42). An estimate
based upon Ranck’s review of several studies
of mammalian
CNS stimulation
suggests
that the spread of passive current from a
330-PA source could excite axons l,OOO1,500 pm from the electrode tip. A ~-PA
source (one standard deviation below the
mean threshold in this study) could excite
axons from 70 to 140 pm away, and a 654-PA
stimulus has a potential range of 1,3001,800 pm. Our stimulation
sites were presumably in the vicinity of unmyelinated axonal endings and terminals of frontal eye
field neurons. The experiments reviewed by
Ranck involved stimulation
of myelinated
axons only. Unfortunately
there have been
no reports of the relative sensitivities of axon
terminals versus axons en passage to applied
extracellular current. Thus our estimates of
M.
E. GOLDBERG
current spread are only rough approximations.
Frontal eye field axons projecting to the
midbrain and pons follow both transthalamic and pedunculopontine
pathways (26,
32) (Stanton,
personal communication).
Transthalamic
fibers travel within and adjacent to the internal medullary lamina. At the
level of the posterior thalamus, the transthalamic pathway bifurcates, with corticotectal
axons passing dorsally through the pretecturn to terminate in the superior colliculus,
and the remaining axons passing ventrolaterally toward the mesencephalic
reticular
formation and pontine tegmentum. Frontal
eye field axons within the pedunculopontine
pathway travel within the cerebral peduncle
and project to the pontine and reticularis
tegmentis pontis nuclei. Recent evidence
suggests that this pathway does not include
corticotectal axons (Stanton, personal communication).
Although the potential range of our stimulating currents were often large, there was no
indication that neurons antidromically
excited in the frontal eye field were being excited by current spread outside of the superior colliculus. In every case tested (4 1% of
the antidromically
excited neurons), the reversal point in depth where the minimum
threshold for antidromic excitation was obtained was located within the superior colliculus. Thus it is unlikely that we were stimulating frontal axons projecting to the mesencephalic reticular formation or the region of
the oculomotor nuclei instead of frontotectal
axons.
It is most likely that high thresholds for
antidromic
excitation were the result of a
lack of overlap between the movement fields
at the site of the collicular electrode and the
movement field of the antidromically excited
neuron. The frontal eye field-collicular pathway connects equivalent points within each
topographic representation (29, 55), and our
efforts to stimulate and record from sites
with similar movement fields were meant to
take advantage of this relationship. However,
it was often difficult to position our electrodes within overlapping movement fields
because of the different topological representations of the intermediate layers of the superior colliculus and the frontal eye field. The
colliculus contains a simple visuotopic map
FRONTAL
EYE
FIELD
CORTICOTECTAL
of desired movement (45), but the frontal eye
field combines a dorsoventral representation
of saccade amplitude superimposed upon repetitive representations
of saccade direction,
such that a single saccade vector is represented at multiple nonadjacent locations (8,
46). This produced frequent changes in saccade direction during a single frontal eye
field penetration and resultant mismatches
with the fixed collicular saccade vector. In
the cases where the saccade vectors overlapped, the threshold was usually quite low;
when they did not overlap, the threshold was
higher.
Functional aspects offrontal
neuron types
eye field
NEURONS.
We have demonstrated that the principal component of the
frontal eye field’s input to the superior colliculus consists of increased neuronal activity
preceding saccadic eye movements, both visually and memory guided. This input originates from neurons with presaccadic activity
in cortical layer V that terminate in the superior colliculus in a topographic manner. Our
results complement
the findings of recent
stimulation and neuron recording studies (7,
8) showing that minimum threshold stimulation sites (as low as 10 PA) within the frontal
eye field are likely to be located near neurons
with movement activity. Low-threshold
sites
were generally located within cortical layers
V and VI, and 63% of the movement and
visuomovement
neurons were located within
this region. In addition, Bruce and colleagues
found a close correspondence
between the
optimal direction
within
the movement
fields of adjacent neurons and the saccade
vector generated by electrical stimulation.
These present results suggest, therefore, that
one pathway by which electrical stimulation
or natural neuronal activity in the frontal eye
field can initiate saccades is through this direct projection to the intermediate layers of
the superior colliculus.
This contribution
will be better understood when we are able to
determine the information
content of the
signal carried by the axons of corticotectal
movement neurons. It is possible that the
frontal eye field’s movement output is specifically related to motor parameters for the impending saccade, since lesions of the frontal
eye field and superior colliculus both have an
MOVEMENT
NEURONS
effect upon saccade dynamics,
cadic velocity ( 10, 25).
1415
including
sac-
NEURONS.
An unexpected finding
in these experiments is the strength within
the frontotectal
projection
of foveally responsive neurons. Fovea1 neurons comprised
a small fraction of the total population of
frontal eye field neurons examined by Bruce
and Goldberg (7), but formed the second
largest population of frontal eye field corticotectal neurons. Twenty-two
percent of antidromically
excited neurons were classified
as fovea1 neurons versus 7% in the total population of frontal eye field neurons. These
neurons were typically suppressed or excited
by fovea1 light stimuli and discharged most
strongly in our task at the signal to make a
saccade or during attentive fixation, respectively. There were equal numbers of fovealon and foveal-off neurons. The observation
that frontal eye field neurons discharge during fixation was first made by Bizzi (4). The
neurons were studied most extensively by
Suzuki and his co-workers
(56, 57), who
showed that the fovea1 responsiveness
of
these neurons explained much but not all of
their activity.
Fovea1 neurons may play a role in the act
of attentive fixation, which is known to be
physiologically different from merely maintaining the eye in a given orbital position (14,
19, 40). They send a message complementary to that of the movement neurons: a signal either to suppress or initiate a saccade. It
is possible that the artificiality of our task, in
which a visual stimulus bears the signal to
suppress or initiate a saccade, may be responsible for the fovea1 responsiveness
of these
neurons, which are otherwise more closely
associated with a fixation or end-of-fixation
role in the untrained monkey. Presumably
neurons active during attentive
fixation
would provide a suppressive effect upon superior colliculus
movement
neurons and
thereby assist in the maintenance of fixation.
This effect may be direct or indirect. Layer V
pyramidal neurons in the frontal eye field do
not appear to contain the inhibitory transmitter y-aminobutyric
acid (52), although
they may use another inhibitory neurotransmitter. It is more likely that they act indirectly via collicular interneurons.
This potential inhibitory effect of frontal eye field
FOVEAL
M.
1416
A. SEGRAVES
AND
neurons is similar to that demonstrated
for
substantia nigra neurons that project to the
colliculus (24). The frontotectal suppressive
effect would be stronger specifically during
attentive fixation, unlike the nigra signal,
which is always maximal, except around a
saccade.
OTHER
CORTICOTECTAL
NEURON
TYPES.
Two neurons in our sample of corticotectal
neurons seemed to have activity related to
the performance
of smooth-pursuit
eye
movements.
This finding is of interest in
light of recent reports of deficits in smoothpursuit eye movements
in monkeys
with
frontal eye field lesions (28, 36). Neurons active during smooth pursuit have been described previously in the frontal eye field (5),
where they were found at the same sites associated with the evocation of smooth pursuit by electrical
stimulation
(8). These
neurons have not been exhaustively characterized to determine if they could be movement neurons for pursuit or if they are instead discharging in response to target movement or the retinal slip induced by the
stationary environment
on the moving retina. The superior colliculus has not been
shown to have a role in smooth pursuit, and
it is possible that these neurons are, in fact,
suppressing
small saccades, similar to the
role postulated for the fovea1 neurons.
A few other neurons discharged in relation
to anticipation of the reward or the next trial,
were suppressed around saccades, or combined anticipatory,
postmovement
activity,
and/or visual activity. These neurons were
unusual in these experiments and unusual in
previous ones, and it is difficult at this point
to understand their function or significance.
NEURONS
FIELDS.
WITH
PERJPHER
+t
VISUAL
None of the corGcotecta1 neurons
in our sample could be classified as peripheral visual neurons. Thus one can assume
that the visual activity present in the superior
colliculus does not originate from the frontal
eye field. Only 10% of the visual neurons
characterized in the work of Bruce and colleagues (8) were located within a region
where thresholds for evoked eye movements
were ~50 PA. Although they do not project
to the superior colliculus, visual neurons are
still likelv to be involved in the generation of
M.
E. GQLDBERG
saccades. Fifty percent of frontal eye field visual neurons are enhanced before saccades to
the stimulus in their receptive field (62), and,
unlike other types of attentive movements,
this enhancement is spatially selective and
specific to saccades (18). Our results suggest
that these neurons participate in the intrinsic
generation of a movement signal within the
frontal eye field, rather than project directly
to the oculomotor
system. Of the visuomovement neurons, some with strong movement activity project to the tectum, but those
with visual predominance do not. This result
reaffirms the likelihood that visuomovement
neurons do not comprise a unique class of
neurons by themselves, but rather belong to
a continuum extending from visual to movement-related activity.
Comparison
to other corticotectal neurons
Corticotectal
neurons within striate cortex have been examined in both cats (41) and monkeys (13).
In both species these neurons belong to a
subset of complex
visual receptive-field
types. In cats, removal of visual cortical
input to the colliculus results in a loss of direction selectivity as well as input from the
ipsilateral eye in superficial layer neurons
and also affects receptive-field properties of
deep layer neurons (47, 58). However, in the
monkey, ablation or cooling of visual cortex
only affects the visual responses of neurons
within the intermediate and deep layers of
the superior colliculus
(50). The average
conduction
latencies
for corticotectal
neurons in monkey striate cortex were longer
(4.6 ms) (13) than those in our sample of
frontal eye field neurons (2.25 ms). The conduction velocity reported for striate corticotectal neurons was an average of 8 m/s (range
3- 19 m/s) versus an estimated 18 m/s for
frontal eye field corticotectal neurons.
Although prestriate and parietal neurons
have been shown by anatomical methods to
project to the intermediate layers of the superior colliculus (3 1, 37) antidromic
studies
have not been done to characterize
them
physiologically.
Other prefrontal
neurons,
especially those more anterior to the frontal
eye field, have also been shown by anatomical methods to project to the colliculus (20,
34), but the nature- of this projection is also
STRIATE
CORTICOTECTAL.
FRONTAL
EYE
FIELD
CORTICOTECTAL
not known. We did not attempt antidromic
excitation in frontal areas that we had not
characterized as being in the low-threshold
arcuate frontal eve
4 field.
The role of the frontal eyefield in the
generation of eye movements
These results contribute to the growing evidence that the arcuate frontal eye field is
important in the initiation of purposive saccadic eye movements. We have now established that the oculomotor region of the superior colliculus, the intermediate layers, receives a distinct oculomotor
message from
the frontal eye field. The message is twofold:
it tells about the state of fixation and fovea1
stimulation and sends a command to make a
movement of certain dimensions.
The superior colliculus and the frontal eye
field can function independently in the generation of saccades. Lesions of the superior
colliculus do not affect the generation of saccades by electrical stimulation of the frontal
eye field (48), and monkeys can make visually guided saccades in the absence of the
frontal eye field or the superior colliculus,
although they cannot make visually guided
saccades at all when both are ablated (5 1).
Recent results suggest that some types of saccades do, in fact, require the frontal eye field.
We have shown that monkeys with unilateral
frontal eye field lesions have difficulty learning to make saccades to remembered targets,
and when they do learn, the motor performance of memory-guided
but not visually
guided saccades is impaired ( 10). Bruce has
shown that although normal monkeys can
make predictive
saccades, monkeys
with
frontal lesions cannot (6). Guitton and colleagues have shown that humans with frontal
lesions have difficulty making saccades away
from a visual target, and instead make inappropriate saccades toward the target (2 1).
Thus there is a repertory of saccades, characterized by behavioral complexity,
that require the presence of the frontal eye field.
Our results suggest that in normal monkeys the signal for these as well as visually
guided saccades progress from the frontal eye
field to the intermediate layers of the superior colliculus, which in normal monkeys
then serve as a final common path for saccades (49). Neurons in the superior colliculus
NEURONS
1417
discharge before all saccades (49, 6 1) and the
region has a monosynaptic
projection to the
long-lead bursters in the brain stem reticular
formation (43). The arcuate frontal eye field
can affect this presaccadic
final common
path in three ways. The first is through the
direct targeting signal to the colliculus that
we have demonstrated
here. The second is
through the fovea1 signals, which could directly trigger or suppress a collicular saccadic
signal. The third is by affecting the substantia
nigra. The frontal eye field projects to the
head of the caudate, which contains a presaccadic signal similar in quality but reversed
in sign from that of the substantia nigra. This
frontal-caudate
signal could inhibit the subtantia nigra and result in a presaccadic release of the nigral suppression of the superior
colliculus. Thus the frontal command to the
colliculus would be exceedingly powerful because it combines an excitatory signal with
the release of a suppressive
one. When a
monkey is actively fixating, thresholds for
evoking saccades from the superior colliculus
(22) are elevated. Presumably this is true because saccades that occur without changes in
frontal and nigral activitv must overcome the
suppression
which inhibits
them. Since
monkeys and humans can make normal saccades after lesions of the frontal eye field, the
colliculus must be able to function on its
own, and under certain circumstances,
for
example spontaneous saccades made in total
darkness, there is little or no frontal signal
(4, 7). We submit, however, that when the
normal monkey makes purposive saccades,
these saccades are organizaed by the frontotectal pathways discussed here.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors are grateful to the technical
staff of the
Laboratory
of Sensorimotor
Research for their invaluable help: A. Ziminsky
for electronic
support,
C. Crist
and T. Ruffner for machining,
G. Creswell and L. Cooper for histology,
G. Snodgrass and J. Pellegrini
for animal care and surgical assistance, A. Hayes for computer
hardware
support, J. Steinberg
for manuscript
preparation, and N. Hight for facilitating
everything.
Dr. Lance
M. Optican wrote the graphics package with which the
illustrations
were produced.
We thank the photographic
staff of the National
Eye Institute
for preparation
of
figures.
Received
July 1987.
29 March
1987; accepted
in final
form
30
1418
M.
A. SEGRAVES
AND
M.
E. GOLDBERG
REFERENCES
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
8.
9.
10.
ASTRUC, J. Corticofugal
connections
of area 8
(frontal
eye field) in Macaca mulattta. Brain Rex
33: 241-256,
1971.
BAGSHAW, E. V. AND EVANS, M. H. Measurement
of current spread from microelectrodes
when stimulating within the central nervous system. Exp. Brain
Res. 25: 39 l-400, 1976.
BISHOP, P. O., BURKE, W., AND DAVIS, R. Singleunit recording
from antidromically
activated
optic
radiation
neurones. J. Physiol. Lond. 162: 432-450,
1962.
BIZZI, E. Discharge of frontal eye field neurons during saccadic and following
eye movements
in unanesthetized
monkeys.
Exp. Brain Rex 6: 69-80,
1968.
BIZZI, E. AND SCHILLER, P. H. Single unit activity in
the frontal
eye fields of unanesthetized
monkeys
during head and eye movement.
Exp. Brain Res. 10:
151-158,197O.
BRUCE, C. J. AND BORDEN, J. A. The primate frontal eye fields are necessary for predictive
saccadic
tracking. Sot. Neurosci. Abstr. 12: 1086, 1986.
BRUCE,~.
J. AND GOLDBERG,
M.E.Primate
frontal eye fields. I. Single neurons discharging
before
saccades. J. Neurophysiol. 53: 603-635, 1985.
BRUCE,~. J., GOLDBERG, M. E., STANTON,G. B.,
AND BUSHNELL, M. C. Primate frontal eye fields. II.
Physiological
and anatomical
correlates
of electrically evoked eye movements.
J. Neurophysiol. 54:
714-734,
1985.
BUSHNELL, M.C., GOLDBERG, M.E., ANDROBINSON, D. L. Behavioral
enhancement
of visual responses in monkey
cerebral cortex. I. Modulation
in
posterior
partietal
cortex related to selective visual
attention.
J. Neurophysiol. 46: 755-772, 198 1.
DENG,S.-Y.,GOLDBERG, M.E., SEGRAVES, M.A.,
UNGERLEIDER, L.G., ANDMISHKIN, M.The effect
of unilateral
ablation
of the frontal eye fields on
saccadic performance
in the monkey.
In: Adaptive
17.
18
19.
20.
21
22
23.
24.
Processes in the Visual and Oculomotor Systems,
11.
12.
13.
14.
15.
16.
edited by E. Keller and D. Zee. New York: Elsevier
North-Holland,
1986, p. 20 l-208.
EVARTS, E., KIMURA, M., WURTZ, R. H., ANDHIKOSAKA, 0. Behavioral correlates of activity in basal
ganglia
neurons.
Trends Neurosci. 7: 447-453,
1984.
FERRIER, D. The localization
of function
in the
brain. Proc. R. Sot. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 22: 229-232,
1874.
FINLAY, B.L., SCHILLER, P.H., ANDVOLMAN,S. F.
Quantitative
studies of single-cell
properties
in
monkey
striate cortex.
IV. Corticotectal
cells. J.
Neurophysiol. 39: 1352- 136 1, 1976.
FISCHER, B. AND BOCH, R. Peripheral
attention
versus central fixation: modulation
of the visual activity of prelunate
cortical
neurons
of the rhesus
monkey.
Brain Res. 345: 111-123, 1985.
FRIES,W. Cortical projections to the superior colliculus in the macaque
monkey:
A retrograde
study
using horseradish
peroxidase.
J. Comp. Neurol. 230:
55-76, 1984.
FULLER.
J. H. AND SCHLAG. J. D. Determination
of
hd.
26.
27.
28.
29.
30.
antidromic
excitation
by the collision test: Problems
of interpretation.
Brain Res. 112; 283-298,
1976.
GOLDBERG, M. E. Studying the neurophysiology of
behavior:
Methods
for recording
single neurons in
awake behaving
monkeys.
In: Methods in Cellular
Neurobiology, edited by J. L. Barker and J. F.
McKelvy.
New York: Wiley, 1983, vol. 3, chapt. 8,
p. 225-248.
GOLDBERG, M. E. AND BUSHNELL, M. C. Behavioral enhancement
of visual responses in monkey
cerebral cortex. II. Modulation
in frontal eye fields
specifically
related to saccades. J. Neurophysiol. 46:
773-787,
1981.
GOLDBERG, M.E., BUSHNELL, M.C., ANDBRUCE,
C. J. The effect of attentive
fixation
on eye movements evoked by electrical stimulation
of the frontal
eye fields. Exp. Brain Res. 6 1: 579-584, 1986.
GOLDMAN, P. S. AND NAUTA, W.J. H.Autoradiographic
demonstration
of a projection
from prefrontal association
cortex to the superior colliculus
in the rhesus monkey.
Brain Res. 116: 145- 149,
1976.
GUITTON, D., BUCHTEL, H. A., AND DOUGLAS,
R. M. Disturbances
of voluntary
saccadic eye movement mechanisms
following
discrete
unilateral
frontal lobe removals.
In: Functional Basis of Ocular Motility Disorders, edited by G. Lennerstrand,
D. S. Zee, and E. L. Keller. Oxford,
UK: Pergamon
1982, p. 497-500.
GUTHRIE, B.L., PORTER, J.D., ANDSPARKS, D.L.
Corollary
discharge provides
accurate eye position
information
to the oculomotor
system.
Science
Wash. DC221:
1193-l 195, 1983.
HIKOSAKA,~. AND SAKAMOTO, M.Cellactivityin
monkey
caudate
nucleus
preceding
saccadic eye
movements.
Exp. Brain Res. 63: 659-662, 1986.
HIKOSAKA,O.ANDWURTZ, R.H.Visualandoculomotor
functions
of monkey
substantia
nigra pars
reticulata.
IV. Relation
of substantia
nigra to superior colliculus.
J. Neurophysiol. 49: 1285- 130 1,
1983.
HIKOSAKA, 0. AND WURTZ, R. H. Saccadic eye
movements
following
injection
of lidocaine into the
superior
colliculus.
Exp. Brain Res. 6 1: 53 l-539,
1986.
HUERTA, M. F., KRUBITZER, L. A., AND KAAS,
J. H. Frontal
eye field as defined by intracortical
microstimulation
in squirrel
monkeys,
owl monkeys, and macaque monkeys:
I. Subcortical
connections. J. Comp. Neurol. 253:4 15-439,
1986.
JUDGE, S. J., RICHMOND, B. J., AND CHU, F. C.
Implantation
of magnetic search coils for measurement of eye position:
an improved
method.
Vision
Res. 20: 535-538, 1980.
KEATING, E. G., GOOLEY, S. G., AND KENNEY,
D. V. Impaired
tracking
and loss of predictive
eye
movements
after removal
of the frontal eye fields.
Sot. Neurosci. Abstr. 11: 472, 1985.
KOMATSU, H. AND SUZUKI, H. Projections
from
the functional
subdivisions
of the frontal eye field to
the superior
colliculus
in the monkey.
Brain Res.
327: 324-327,
1985.
KUNZLE. H. AND AKERT. K. Efferent connections
FRONTAL
31.
32.
33.
34.
35.
36
37.
38.
EYE
FIELD
CORTICOTECTAL
of cortical area 8 (frontal eye field) in Macaca fascicularis. A reinvestigation
using the autoradiographic
technique.
J. Comp. Neural. 173: 147-164, 1977.
KUYPERS, H. G. J. M. AND LAWRENCE, D. G. Cortical projections
to the red nucleus and the brain
stem in the rhesus monkey.
Brain Res. 4: 15 1- 188,
1967.
LEICHNETZ,
G. R. The prefrontal
cortico-oculomotor trajectories
in the monkey.
J. Neuro. Sci. 49:
387-396,
1981.
LEICHNETZ,
G., SMITH, D. J., AND SPENCER, R. F.
Cortical
projections
to the paramedian
tegmental
and basilar pons in the monkey.
J. Comp. Neural.
228: 388-408,
1984.
LEICHNETZ,
G. R., SPENCER,
R. F., HARDY,
S. G. P., AND ASTRUC, J. The prefrontal
corticotectal projection
in the monkey:
An anterograde
and
retrograde
horseradish
peroxidase
study. Neuroscience 6: 1023-1041,
1981.
LEICHNETZ,
G. R., SPENCER, R. F., AND SMITH,
D. J. Cortical projections
to nuclei adjacent to the
oculomotor
complex
in the medial dien-mesencephalic tegmentum
in the monkey.
J. Comp. Neural.
228: 359-387,
1984.
LYNCH, J. C. AND ALLISON,
J. C. A quantitative
study of visual pursuit deficits following
lesions of
the frontal
eye fields in Rhesus monkeys.
Sot.
Neurosci. Abstr. 1 1: 473, 1985.
LYNCH, J. C., GRAYBIEL, A. M., AND LOBECK, L. J.
The differential
projection
of two cytoarchitectonic
subregions of the inferior parietal lobule of macaque
upon the deep layers of the superior
colliculus.
J.
Comp. Neural. 235: 241-254,
1985.
MCILWAIN,
J. T. Lateral spread of neural excitation
during microstimulation
in intermediate
gray layer
of cat’s superior
colliculus.
J. Neurophysiol. 47:
167-178,
1982.
39. MOHLER,
C. W., GOLDBERG,
M. E., AND WURTZ,
R. H. Visual receptive
fields of frontal eye neurons.
Brain Res. 61: 385-389, 1973.
40. MOUNTCASTLE,
V. B., ANDERSON,
R. A., AND
MOTTER, B. C. The influence
of attentive
fixation
upon the excitability
of the light-sensitive
neurons
of the posterior
parietal
cortex.
J. Neurosci. 1:
1218-1235,
1981.
41. PALMER, L. A. AND ROSENQUIST,
A. C. Visual receptive fields of single striate cortical units projecting to the superior colliculus
in the cat. Brain Res.
67: 27-42,
1974.
42. RANCK, J. B., JR. Which elements are excited in
electrical
stimulation
of mammalian
central nervous system: A review. Brain Res. 98: 417-440,
1975.
43. RAYBOURN,
M. S. AND KELLER, E. L. Colliculoretitular organization
in primate oculomotor
system.
J. Neurophysiol. 40: 86 l-878, 1977.
44. ROBINSON, D. A. A method of measuring eye movement using a scleral search coil in a magnetic field.
IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 10: 137- 145, 1963.
45. ROBINSON, D. A. Eye movements
evoked by collicular stimulation
in the alert monkey.
Vision Res. 12:
1795-1808,
1972.
46. ROBINSON,
D. A. AND FUCHS, A. F. Eye move-
ments evoked
1419
NEURONS
by stimulation
of frontal
eye fields.
J.
Neurophysiol. 32: 637-648, 1969.
47. ROSENQUIST,
A. C. AND PALMER, L. A. Visual receptive field properties
of cells of the superior colliculus after cortical lesions in the cat. Exp. Neural. 33:
629-652,
1971.
48. SCHILLER,
P. H. The effect of superior
colliculus
ablation on saccades elicted by cortical stimulation.
Brain Res. 122: 154- 156, 1977.
49. SCHILLER, P. H. AND KOERNER, F. Discharge characteristics of single units in superior colliculus of the
alert rhesus monkey.
J. Neurophysiol. 34: 920-936,
1971.
50. SCHILLER, P. H., STRYKER, M. P., CYNADER,
M.,
AND BERMAN, N. Response characteristics
of single
cells in the monkey
superior
colliculus
following
ablation or cooling of visual cortex. J. Neurophysiol.
37: 181-194,
1974.
51. SCHILLER, P. H., TRUE, S. D., AND CONWAY, J. L.
Deficits
in eye movements
following
frontal
eye
field and superior
colliculus
ablations.
J. Neurophysiol. 44: 1175- 1189, 1980.
52. SCHWARTZ, M. L., ZHENG, D. S., AND GOLDMANRAKIC, P. S. Laminar
and tangential variation
in the
morphology
and distribution
of GABA-containing
neurons in rhesus monkey
prefrontal
cortex. Sot.
Neurosci. Abstr. 11: 503, 1985.
M. E. Func53. SEGRAVES, M. A. AND GOLDBERG,
tional properties
of tectal projection
neurons in the
monkey
frontal eye field. Sot. Neurosci. Abstr. 11:
472, 1985.
54. SPARKS, D. L. AND HOLLAND,
R. Computer
control of eye position
and velocity.
Behav. Res.
Methods Instrum. 7: 115- 119, 1975.
G. B., BRUCE, C. J., AND GOLDBERG,
55. STANTON,
M. E. Organization
of subcortical
projections
from
saccadic eye movement
sites in the macaque’s frontal eye fields. Sot. Neurosci. Abstr. 8: 293, 1982.
H. AND AZUMA,
M. Prefrontal
neuronal
56. SUZUKI,
activity during gazing at a light spot in the monkey.
Brain Res. 126: 497-508,
1977.
H. Stimu57. SUZUKI, H., AZUMA, M., AND YUMIYA,
lus and behavioral
factors contributing
to the activation of monkey prefrontal
neurons during gazing.
Jpn. J. Physiol. 29: 47 l-489, 1979.
58. WICKELGREN,
B. G. AND STERLING, P. Influence of
visual cortex on receptive fields in the superior colliculus of the cat. J. Neurophysiol. 32: 16-23, 1969.
59. WOLBARSHT,
M. L., MACNICHOL,
E. F., JR, AND
WAGNER,
H. G. Glass insulated
platinum
microelectrode. Science Wash. DC 132: 1309- 13 10, 1960.
60. WURTZ, R. H. Visual receptive fields of striate cortex neurons in awake monkeys.
J. Neurophysiol. 32:
727-742,
1969.
M. E. Activity
of
61. WURTZ, R. H. AND GOLDBERG,
superior
colliculus
in behaving
monkey.
III. Cells
discharging
before eye movements.
J. Neurophysiol.
35: 575-586,
1972.
62. WURTZ, R. H. AND MOHLER,
C. W. Enhancement
of visual response
in monkey
striate cortex and
frontal
eye fields. J. Neurophysiol. 39: 766-772,
1976.