Download Loading “EBSCOhost”

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Nonsynaptic plasticity wikipedia , lookup

Biochemistry of Alzheimer's disease wikipedia , lookup

Cognitive neuroscience wikipedia , lookup

Neuroplasticity wikipedia , lookup

Convolutional neural network wikipedia , lookup

Brain wikipedia , lookup

Types of artificial neural networks wikipedia , lookup

Stimulus (physiology) wikipedia , lookup

Environmental enrichment wikipedia , lookup

Activity-dependent plasticity wikipedia , lookup

Bird vocalization wikipedia , lookup

Molecular neuroscience wikipedia , lookup

Axon wikipedia , lookup

Axon guidance wikipedia , lookup

Brain Rules wikipedia , lookup

Animal consciousness wikipedia , lookup

Connectome wikipedia , lookup

Single-unit recording wikipedia , lookup

Embodied language processing wikipedia , lookup

Multielectrode array wikipedia , lookup

Caridoid escape reaction wikipedia , lookup

Neuroeconomics wikipedia , lookup

Artificial general intelligence wikipedia , lookup

Vilayanur S. Ramachandran wikipedia , lookup

Neural oscillation wikipedia , lookup

Metastability in the brain wikipedia , lookup

Neural coding wikipedia , lookup

Neural correlates of consciousness wikipedia , lookup

Development of the nervous system wikipedia , lookup

Clinical neurochemistry wikipedia , lookup

Central pattern generator wikipedia , lookup

Circumventricular organs wikipedia , lookup

Nervous system network models wikipedia , lookup

Neuroanatomy wikipedia , lookup

Synaptic gating wikipedia , lookup

Neuropsychopharmacology wikipedia , lookup

Premovement neuronal activity wikipedia , lookup

Feature detection (nervous system) wikipedia , lookup

Pre-Bötzinger complex wikipedia , lookup

Optogenetics wikipedia , lookup

Channelrhodopsin wikipedia , lookup

Mirror neuron wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Loading “EBSCOhost”
03/22/2007 12:04 PM
Back
5 page(s) will be printed.
Record: 1
Title:
A Revealing Reflection.
Authors:
Dobbs, David
Source:
Scientific American Mind; 2006, Vol. 17 Issue 2, p22-22, 6p, 6c
Document Type:
Article
Subject Terms:
*BRAIN
*CEREBRAL cortex
*EMPATHY
*HUMAN behavior
*HUMAN mechanics
*NEURONS
Abstract:
The article discusses the role of mirror neurons in movement and other
functions of human beings. These neurons are scattered throughout key
parts of the brain, the premotor cortex and centers for language,
empathy and pain and fire not only as human beings perform a certain
action but also when they watch someone else perform that action.
Research revealed that mirror neurons help to share other people's
experiences as reflected in their expressions, providing a biological basis
for empathy and for the well-known contagiousness of yawns, laughter
and good or bad moods.
Full Text Word
Count:
2380
ISSN:
1555-2284
Accession Number:
20691889
Database:
Academic Search Premier
A Revealing Reflection
MIRROR NEURONS ARE PROVIDING STUNNIGN INSIGHTS INTO EVERYTHING FROM
HOW WE LEARN TO WALK TO HOW WE EMPATHIZE WITH OTHERS
Sometime just before my second child was born, I read that if you stick your tongue out at
a newborn, he will do the same. So in young Nicholas's first hours, even as my wife was
still in the recovery room after 40 hours of labor and a C-section, I tried it. Holding the
gooing, alert lad before me in my hands, I stuck my tongue out at him. He immediately
returned the gesture, opening his mouth and subtly but distinctly moving his tongue. I
hadn't slept in two days. I laughed till I cried.
I did not know it then, but Nick was showing off what some consider one of the greatest
drivers of human progress and one of the prime discoveries in recent neuroscience: mirror
neurons. These neurons are scattered throughout key parts of our brain — the premotor
cortex and centers for language, empathy and pain — and fire not only as we perform a
certain action but also when we watch someone else perform that action.
http://web.ebscohost.com/ehost/delivery?vid=8&hid=104&sid=7d847e1c-c834-4ceb-a47b-7bcf28470435%40sessionmgr104
Page 1 of 6
Loading “EBSCOhost”
03/22/2007 12:04 PM
These neurons have been studied in the past for their roles in movement and other
functions. Now, however, researchers are examining them intensely for what seems to be
an additional function — the way they fire in response to something observed. The
discovery of this mechanism, made about a decade ago, suggests that everything we watch
someone else do, we do as well — in our minds. At its most basic, this finding means we
mentally rehearse or imitate every action we witness, whether it is a somersault or a subtle
smile. It explains much about how we learn to smile, talk, walk, dance or play tennis. At a
deeper level, it suggests a biological dynamic for our understanding of others, the complex
exchange of ideas we call culture, and psychosocial dysfunctions ranging from lack of
empathy to autism. Comprehending mirror neurons helps us make sense of everything from
why yawns are contagious to why, watching Lawrence Olivier fall to his knees, we share
Hamlet's grief for Ophelia.
For some, this explanatory power makes mirror neurons the biggest neuroscientific
discovery in recent times. "This completely changes the way we think about how the brain
works," says Marco Iacoboni, a mirror-neuron researcher at the University of California, Los
Angeles. Cognitive neuroscientist Vilayanur S. Ramachandran of the University of California,
San Diego, even ventures that "mirror neurons will do for psychology what DNA did for
biology: they will provide a unifying framework and help explain a host of mental abilities
that have hitherto remained mysterious and inaccessible to experiments." In
Ramachandran's view, mirror neurons may clarify not only how we come to learn and to
understand others but how humans took a "great leap forward" about 50,000 years ago,
acquiring new skills in social organization, tool use and language that made human culture
possible
The Raisin Incident
Big-picture speculation is not needed to see the wonder of mirror neurons, however. Even
their basics astonish.
The neuroscientists who discovered the cells found them by happenstance. Giacomo
Rizzolatti, Vittorio Gallese and Leonardo Fogassi of the University of Parma in Italy had run
electrodes to individual neurons in a monkey's premotor cortex, to study neural activity as
the monkey reached for different objects. The eureka moment came when Fogassi walked
into the room where the macaque was and casually reached out and picked up a raisin. As
the monkey watched him, its premotor neurons fired just as they had earlier, when the
monkey had picked up the raisin itself. The men could hardly believe what they had
witnessed. But after replicating that experiment and similar ones many times, they realized
they had discovered something new, and in a series of 1996 papers they gave the "mirror
neurons" their name.
Since then, the Parma team, working often with Iacoboni, Michael A. Arbib of the University
of Southern California and Christian Keysers of the University of Groningen in the
Netherlands, has significantly expanded those findings. The researchers have learned, for
instance, that mirror neurons do not just fire when an animal is watching someone else
perform an action. Mirror neurons also fire if a monkey hears the sound of someone doing
something it has experienced — say, tearing a piece of paper. And as the scientists began
studying humans (using brain imaging rather than electrodes), they found groups of mirror
neurons in higher numbers and in more places than occur in monkeys. Mirror neurons
revealed themselves in the premotor cortex and the inferior parietal areas — associated
with movement and perception — as well as in the posterior parietal lobe, the superior
temporal sulcus and the insula, regions that correspond to our abilities to comprehend
someone else's feelings, understand intention and use language.
http://web.ebscohost.com/ehost/delivery?vid=8&hid=104&sid=7d847e1c-c834-4ceb-a47b-7bcf28470435%40sessionmgr104
Page 2 of 6
Loading “EBSCOhost”
03/22/2007 12:04 PM
From Action to Understanding
Unlike monkeys, humans also use mirror neurons to directly imitate actions and to
understand their meanings. It appears we use mirror neurons to learn everything from our
first smiles and steps to our most suave expressions and graceful dance moves. We also
use them to appreciate these things, to feel the meaning behind a smile or to enjoy — in a
sense by doing it at a premotor neural level — the thrill of hitting a tennis ball as we watch
a Pete Sampras backhand.
These functions became evident in the first round of mirror-neuron studies in the late
1990s. Since then, imaging studies have shown that mirror neurons in humans encompass
many more areas and functions. In 1998 Rizzolatti and Arbib discovered that one of the
regions particularly rich in mirror neurons is the famous Broca's area, which Paul Broca
found in the 1850s to be critical for language processing. Mirror-neuron theory began to
mesh with existing language theory, which held that actions have a syntax similar to that of
spoken or signed language. For mirror neurons, "hand grasps ball" is the same whether it is
an action or expressed in sign language or a spoken sentence. Thus, language arises from
the syntactic understanding generated by our mirror neurons. This idea gained credence in
2005: an international team that included Gallese and Rizzolatti found that when people
listened to sentences describing actions, the same mirror neurons fired as would have had
the subjects performed the actions themselves or witnessed them being performed.
Remarkably, the cells responded to an abstract representation of a process that would
seem to be quite visual and visceral.
Another major insight relates to our understanding of other people's intentions and
emotions. Several studies have demonstrated the dynamics of empathy, two with particular
elegance.
One, described by Iacoboni in 2005, shows that our mirror neurons work in elaborate sets.
We possess a basic set of mirror neurons corresponding to an action's most essential form
— reaching, for instance — that is supplemented by other groups of mirror neurons that
selectively fire according to the action's perceived purpose. Iacoboni had volunteers watch
films of people reaching for various objects within a teatime setting — a teapot, a mug, a
pitcher of cream, a plate of pastries, napkins — in different contexts. In every instance, a
basic collection of "reaching" mirror neurons fired. But different additional mirror neurons
would also fire depending on what expected action was suggested by various details in the
scene. If the viewer saw a neatly set table and expected the hand to pick up a teacup to
drink, one array fired; if the viewer saw a messy table and expected the hand to pick up a
cup to clear it away, another group fired. Thus, mirror neurons seem to play a key role in
perceiving intentions — the first step in understanding others and also in building social
relations and feeling empathy.
A number of experiments, meanwhile, have shown that mirror neurons help us share other
people's experiences as reflected in their expressions, providing a biological basis for
empathy and for the well-known contagiousness of yawns, laughter, and good or bad
moods. One of the most convincing (and certainly the most memorably titled) observations
appeared in a 2003 paper, "Both of Us Disgusted in My Insula: The Common Neural Basis of
Seeing and Feeling Disgust," published by Bruno Wicker of the University of the
Mediterranean in Marseilles, France. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI),
Wicker's team found that feeling disgust and seeing a look of revulsion on someone's face
caused the same set of mirror neurons to fire in the insula, a part of the cortex active in
synthesizing convergent information.
When the Mirror Fogs
http://web.ebscohost.com/ehost/delivery?vid=8&hid=104&sid=7d847e1c-c834-4ceb-a47b-7bcf28470435%40sessionmgr104
Page 3 of 6
Loading “EBSCOhost”
03/22/2007 12:04 PM
Given that mirror neurons are so fundamental to understanding, it makes sense that faults
among them might create profound problems. Indeed, it appears that deficits may help
account for difficulties ranging from excessive reserve to autism. The possible failure of
mirror neurons in autism is particularly intriguing. The cause and even the nature of this
strange condition have eluded researchers for decades, leaving sufferers and their families
and caregivers with little knowledge about why the behaviors are happening, let alone how
to treat them. But recent research suggests that an inactive mirror-neuron system may
explain the deep troubles with language, learning and empathy that do so much to isolate
the autistic person.
The findings indicate breakdowns in both basic and complex mirror-neuron activity. One
study at Harvard Medical School, for instance, found that mirror neurons that fired in
nonautistic people when they watched someone else make meaningless finger movements
fired much less often in autistic children. This lack of response could reflect a failure of
mirror neurons' most basic function, that of recognizing others' actions. In another study,
researchers showed pictures of people with distinctive facial expressions to autistic and
nonautistic adolescents. Both sets of subjects could imitate the expressions and say what
emotions they expressed. But whereas the nonautistic teens showed robust activity in
mirror neurons corresponding to the emotions expressed, the autistic teens showed no such
activity. They understood the expressions cognitively but felt no empathy.
How these discoveries might lead to treatments is not clear. Yet identifying this apparent
deficit, if the findings hold up, could be a major advance in pinning down the neural roots
of autism.
Reflections Deep and Dark
Mirror neurons' role in understanding others lies at the heart of the deeper claims about
them. Some, such as Ramachandran, believe that mirror neurons were crucial in the
development of the elaborate social skills, social networks and knowledge infrastructure we
call culture — from tool use to reveling in Shakespeare, from collaborative hunting to hiphop. The archaeological record suggests that this "great leap forward" — the beginnings of
human culture — began about 50,000 years ago. But human brains underwent no growth
spurt then; indeed, they have been roughly their present size for about 200,000 years. So
what changed? Ramachandran and others speculate that the change was a genetic
adaptation that gave key neurons the mirroring capacity they now hold, paving the way for
accelerating advances in understanding, communication and learning. For the first time,
information could be spread, built on and modified to create the intellectual and social
dynamic of culture.
Mirror neurons do not always reflect on us kindly, of course. For example, they may be
implicated in the influence of violent video games. Initial studies by Iacoboni suggest that
such games reinforce, at a basic neuronal level, an association of pleasure and
accomplishment with inflicting harm — an impetus that society would not want to
encourage. Iacoboni speculates that the strength of mirror neurons may be such that
imitative violence, if reinforced, may be harder to resist than we would like to think. The
power of mirror-neuron systems, Iacoboni says, "suggests that imitative violence may not
always be a consciously mediated process" — that is, not so easily subject to our control.
Work on mirror neurons has greatly accelerated in the past five years and seems sure to
up-shift even more. Whether Rizzolatti, Fogassi and Gallese's breakthrough of 1996 will
turn out to be as big as James D. Watson and Francis Crick's 1953 discovery of DNA
remains to be seen. Yet mirror neurons already constitute one of the richest areas in
neuroscience, both intellectually and experimentally. If their enormous explanatory power is
http://web.ebscohost.com/ehost/delivery?vid=8&hid=104&sid=7d847e1c-c834-4ceb-a47b-7bcf28470435%40sessionmgr104
Page 4 of 6
Loading “EBSCOhost”
backed by more robust results, they
neuroscience. In the meantime, mirror
Nicholas is now four years old, which is
out at me of his own accord. I have no
know how.
03/22/2007 12:04 PM
might indeed become regarded as the DNA of
neurons explain some intriguing wonders. My son
advanced enough to enable him to stick his tongue
idea where he learned such a thing. But at least I
We mentally imitate every action we witness, prompting us to dance, grieve and yawn with
others.
More complex mirror-neuron systems may have sparked the advance of human cultures
50,000 years ago.
PHOTO (COLOR): MIMICRY Mirror neurons enable infants and toddlers to learn facial
expressions and physical maneuvers through imitation.
PHOTO (COLOR): CONTAGION When we see another person yawn, mirror neurons in primal
brain regions tell us to do the same. Group laughter can be catching, too.
PHOTO (COLOR): EMPATHY Theatergoers feel the pain of an actress spurned because their
mirror neurons fire as if they were experiencing their own rejection firsthand.
PHOTO (COLOR): Neurons inside the red outline fired when one test individual experienced
disgust because of a foul odor. Neurons inside the yellow outlines fired later when the same
subject watched another person experience disgust. The areas overlap strongly, indicating
mirror neurons. (Blue and green outlines represent the region and method of testing.)
PHOTO (COLOR): SOCIALIZATON Using mirror neurons, we develop elaborate forms of
social interaction that constitute human culture.
PHOTO (COLOR)
Further Reading
• Action Recognition in the Premotor Cortex. V. Gallese, L. Fadiga, L. Fogassi and G.
Rizzolatti
in Brain, Vol.
119, No.
2, pages 593-609; 1996. Available at
http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/abstract/119/2/593
• How Mimicry Begat Culture. Beth Azar in Monitor on Psychology, Vol. 36, No. 9, pages
54-57; October, 2005. Available at www.apa.org/monitor/oct05/mimicry.html
• From Monkey-Like Action Recognition to Human Language: An Evolutionary Framework
for Neurolinguistics. Michael Arbib in Behavioral and Brain Sciences, Vol. 28, pages 105167; 2005.
~~~~~~~~
By David Dobbs
DAVID DOBBS (www.daviddobbs.net) profiled Joseph LeDoux and his work on fear in the
February/March issue of Scientific American Mind.
http://web.ebscohost.com/ehost/delivery?vid=8&hid=104&sid=7d847e1c-c834-4ceb-a47b-7bcf28470435%40sessionmgr104
Page 5 of 6
Loading “EBSCOhost”
03/22/2007 12:04 PM
Copyright of Scientific American Mind is the property of Scientific American Inc. and its
content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the
copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or
email articles for individual use.
Back
http://web.ebscohost.com/ehost/delivery?vid=8&hid=104&sid=7d847e1c-c834-4ceb-a47b-7bcf28470435%40sessionmgr104
Page 6 of 6