Download Comparison of Quantities: Core and Format

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Emotion perception wikipedia , lookup

Binding problem wikipedia , lookup

History of neuroimaging wikipedia , lookup

Clinical neurochemistry wikipedia , lookup

Development of the nervous system wikipedia , lookup

Aging brain wikipedia , lookup

Neuroplasticity wikipedia , lookup

Allochiria wikipedia , lookup

Synaptic gating wikipedia , lookup

Dual consciousness wikipedia , lookup

Human brain wikipedia , lookup

Premovement neuronal activity wikipedia , lookup

Nervous system network models wikipedia , lookup

Neuropsychopharmacology wikipedia , lookup

Cortical cooling wikipedia , lookup

Emotion and memory wikipedia , lookup

Visual search wikipedia , lookup

Neural coding wikipedia , lookup

Connectome wikipedia , lookup

Neurolinguistics wikipedia , lookup

Visual selective attention in dementia wikipedia , lookup

Optogenetics wikipedia , lookup

Functional magnetic resonance imaging wikipedia , lookup

Neuroanatomy of memory wikipedia , lookup

Psychophysics wikipedia , lookup

Stimulus (physiology) wikipedia , lookup

Channelrhodopsin wikipedia , lookup

Metastability in the brain wikipedia , lookup

Affective neuroscience wikipedia , lookup

Neural correlates of consciousness wikipedia , lookup

Neurophilosophy wikipedia , lookup

Time perception wikipedia , lookup

Neuroeconomics wikipedia , lookup

Visual N1 wikipedia , lookup

Embodied language processing wikipedia , lookup

Visual extinction wikipedia , lookup

Cognitive neuroscience of music wikipedia , lookup

P200 wikipedia , lookup

Feature detection (nervous system) wikipedia , lookup

Inferior temporal gyrus wikipedia , lookup

C1 and P1 (neuroscience) wikipedia , lookup

Neuroesthetics wikipedia , lookup

Emotional lateralization wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Cerebral Cortex June 2012;22:1420–1
1430
doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr219
Advance Access publication August 1, 2011
Comparison of Quantities: Core and Format-Dependent Regions as Revealed by fMRI
Philippe Chassy1 and Wolfgang Grodd2
1
Institute of Medical Psychology and Behavioral Neurobiology, Faculty of Medicine, University Hospital Tübingen, 72076 Tübingen,
Germany and 2Klinik für Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie, Universitätsklinikum Aachen, D-52074 Aachen, Germany
Address correspondence to Philippe Chassy, Institute for Medical Psychology and Behavioral Neurobiology, University Hospital Tübingen, 29
Gartenstrasse, Office 309, 72076 Tübingen, Germany. Email: [email protected].
The perception and handling of numbers is central to education.
Numerous imaging studies have focused on how quantities are
encoded in the brain. Yet, only a few studies have touched upon
number mining: the ability to extract the magnitude encoded in
a visual stimulus. This article aims to characterize how analogue
(i.e., disks and dots) and symbolic (i.e., positive and negative
integers) formats influence number mining and the representation
of quantities. Sixteen adult volunteers completed a comparison task
while we recorded the blood oxygen level--dependent response
using functional magnetic resonance imaging. The results revealed
that a restricted set of specific subdivisions in the right intraparietal
sulcus is activated in all conditions. With respect to magnitude
assessment, the results show that 1) analogue stimuli are
predominantly processed in the right hemisphere and that 2)
symbolic stimuli encompass the analogue system and further
recruit areas in the left hemisphere. Crucially, we found that
polarity is encoded independently from magnitude. We refine the
triple-code model by integrating our findings.
Keywords: IPS, mathematical cognition, number, visual perception
Introduction
Due to its central role in the development of mathematical
abilities, the number sense has generated much research.
Overall, the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) in both hemispheres is
believed to play a key role in coding quantities (Nieder and
Dehaene 2009). Numerous functional magnetic resonance
imaging (fMRI) studies have consistently shown that the IPS
is involved in magnitude processing regardless of the type of
task (McNeil and Warrington 1994; Van Harskamp and
Cipolotti 2001; Zago et al. 2001; Zhou et al. 2007; Ischebeck
et al. 2009) and irrespective of format (Eger et al. 2003; Fias
et al. 2003). Furthermore, in a study on nonhuman primates,
Nieder and Miller (2004) have demonstrated that single
neurons in the IPS display magnitude-dependent activity. The
question of magnitude representation has thus been intensely
investigated during the past decades. However, much less is
known about how the quantity is extracted from various types
of visual displays. The process of quantity extraction, which we
term number mining, is a necessary step prior to any further
mathematical processing. Several research groups primarily
interested in how a quantity is encoded in the IPS have touched
upon the number mining problem (Fias et al. 2003; Lammertyn
et al. 2003; Venkatraman et al. 2005; Cohen Kadosh et al. 2007;
Piazza et al. 2007). These studies have yielded a complex, and
sometimes conflicting, pattern of results that is difficult to
integrate into a unique theory. The present article aims at
qualifying the different routes that serve number mining and at
Ó The Author 2011. Published by Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.
For permissions, please e-mail: [email protected]
determining whether magnitude representation depends up on
number mining.
Previous literature has introduced a distinction between the
numerical processing of analogue and symbolic stimuli
(Dehaene and Cohen 1997; Fias et al. 2003; Lammertyn et al.
2003; Venkatraman et al. 2005; Piazza et al. 2007; Holloway
et al. 2010). Whereas in an analogue stimulus the magnitude is
a perceptually accessible aspect of the stimulus, in a symbolic
stimulus the magnitude being coded is independent from
physical characteristics. For example, 3 dots and the Arabic
digit ‘‘3’’ both implement the notion of 3 but the visual codes
differ drastically. Although analogue and symbolic visual signals
may utilize the same brain areas for representing magnitudes,
they most likely will utilize different routes for number mining.
To identify regions that are responsive to both analogue and
symbolic stimuli, Fias et al. (2003) requested participants to
compare either numbers or nondiscrete quantities. Their
analysis identified the left IPS as the center of a network
responsive to magnitude. Rather than focusing on common
regions, Venkatraman et al. (2005) used an arithmetical task to
reveal the areas that respond differentially when participants
process digits or dots. They found that the processing of digits
activated areas that are more lateralized to the left. Recently,
Holloway et al. (2010) asked participants to compare digits or
dots in arrays and found that the posterior region of the right
superior parietal lobule (SPL) was more responsive for dots.
Since these studies involved different tasks and types of stimuli,
it is difficult to draw a definite conclusion on how the brain
extracts magnitudes from various types of visual display. Yet, by
revealing format-dependent processing, these studies opened
the door for number mining considerations.
Within the analogue category, we distinguished the stimuli
coding for approximate magnitudes (e.g., object size) from
those coding for exact quantities (e.g., dots). This distinction
between an approximate and an exact number sense is
grounded on clinical studies (Dehaene and Cohen 1991; Lemer
et al. 2003) and was confirmed by neuroimaging experiments
(Piazza et al. 2006; Kucian et al. 2008). For example, Piazza et al.
(2006) asked adult participants to perform an estimation and
a calculation task. The results corroborated the distinction by
showing that approximate processing was represented by
a right lateralized network, while exact processing predominantly recruited areas in the left hemisphere. These results
were echoed by developmental studies showing that the
acquisition of notion of exact quantities modifies the activity
pattern of number processing (see Halberda and Feigenson
2008 for behavioral data; Cantlon et al. 2010 for an fMRI
demonstration). Finally, research convincingly shows that the
processing of quantities in the IPS is inherited from animals
(see Nieder 2005 for a review). Hence, one purpose of the
present study was to explore how the inherited number system
handles quantities that are coded in approximate and exact
formats.
With respect to the processing of symbolic stimuli, we
distinguished between the symbolic representation of positive
(e.g., 3) and negative integers (e.g., –3). The latter were
introduced as a more abstract level of numerical conceptualization. Even though negative integers have not attracted as
much attention as positive integers, they are a valid option to
study abstract quantities. Based on behavioral evidence, Fischer
(2003) and Shaki and Petrusic (2005) have convincingly argued
in favor of negative numbers as being mapped in a spatial code.
Based on this evidence, we expected negative numbers to
activate the IPS. Also, that negative integers are rooted in the
same system than positive integers will enable exploring the
influence of the notion of negativity. To address this issue, 2
behavioral studies compared the processing of negative and
positive numbers (Ganor-Stern and Tzelgov 2008; Tzelgov et al.
2009). Their results provide evidence that polarity is independently coded from magnitude, which suggests that
additional neural networks might be in charge of encoding
the notion of negativity. As negative integers do not visually
differ substantially from positive integers, we do not expect
differential activations in primary visual areas. But as negative
integers represent a more abstract numerical concept, we
hypothesized that processing would recruit the IPS and an
additional region coding for negativity. To the best of our
knowledge, until now no imaging study has compared the
neural correlates of positive and negative integers.
In line with the literature, the experiment was designed with
4 classes of visual stimuli: 2 classes belonging to the analogue
class (i.e., approximate and exact) and 2 classes belonging to
the symbolic class (i.e., positive integers and negative integers).
Each type of stimuli implemented a different way to code
a magnitude.
screen was viewed by the subjects via a mirror mounted on the head
coil. All participants were asked to use their right thumb. The 4
comparison conditions were as follows: 1) disk size (disk), 2) number
of dots (dots), 3) positive integers (positive) and 4) negative integers
(negative). The tasks were purposefully designed to be easy so that no
speed--accuracy trade-off could limit the interpretation of results. In the
resting condition, the participants were asked to attend passively to
a fixation cross at the center of a square. No response was required.
The purpose of this was to minimize the interference with activation in
comparison tasks so as to have as complete a mapping as possible.
Materials and Methods
Apparatus and fMRI Data Acquisition
All participants were scanned in the Section of Experimental MRI of the
Central Nervous System at the Department of Neuroradiology of the
University Hospital Tübingen, Germany. Structural and functional
images were acquired using a whole-body 3-T Siemens Trio scanner
equipped with a 32-channel head coil (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany).
Participants were positioned head first and supine in the bore.
Anatomical data were collected with a magnetization prepared rapid
gradient echo sequence (time echo [TE] = 2.92 ms, time repetition
[TR]= 2300 ms, flip angle = 8°, field of view [FoV] = 256 3 256 mm, 176
slices, 1-mm thickness). After the anatomical map, a magnetic field map
was acquired to correct for inhomogeneities of the static magnetic
field. Functional images were acquired using a gradient echo planar
imaging T2* sequence sensitive to blood oxygen level--dependent
(BOLD) signal with the following parameters: TE = 30 ms, TR = 2000
ms, flip angle = 90°, FoV = 192 3 192 mm, matrix size = 64 3 64. Thirtytwo slices were acquired with a thickness of 3.2 + 0.8 mm gap resulting
in voxels with the resolution of 3 3 3 3 4 mm3.
Participants
To minimize the potential number of confounding variables, several
factors have been controlled during the selection of the participants. As
handedness may affect the lateralization of functional brain areas
(Szaflarski et al. 2002), we selected only right-handed individuals. As
evidence exists that males and females might differ in their approach to
mathematical processing (Keller and Menon 2009) and that numerical
processing differs between children and adults (Kucian et al. 2008), we
consequently limited the study to male adults.
fMRI data were acquired from 16 right-handed male volunteers (age:
mean = 28.62 years-old, standard deviation = 5.75 years-old) while they
were performing a comparison task. All participants had normal or
corrected-to-normal vision and reported no history of neurological or
psychiatric illness. The participants were paid V15 per hour for their
participation. Before giving written informed consent, all participants
were informed of safety issues and of the aim of the study. The study
was approved by the ethics committee of the University Hospital of
Tübingen.
Task
The comparison task has been used to elicit magnitude-specific
activation in the IPS (Zorzi et al. 2011). Because of its robustness,
and since it includes no arithmetical processing, the comparison task
constitutes a good candidate to examine number mining and the
retention of magnitudes. The participants had to perform a comparison
task and to indicate by a left or right button press which of the stimuli
(left or right) implemented the larger magnitude. The translucent
Stimuli and Procedure
Four types of stimuli were used (see Fig. 1, left panel): disks (9 different
radii), number of dots (from 1 to 9), positive integers (range from 1 to
9), and negative integers (range from –1 to –9). The dots were randomly
assigned to a 3 3 3 canonically arranged matrix (similar to Piazza et al.
2002). For each condition, 24 pairs of stimuli were randomly generated.
Laterality was controlled by presenting the bigger magnitude half of the
time (50%) in the right and half of the time (50%) in the left visual
hemifield. The stimuli pairs were then distributed randomly in separate
blocks with the constraint that a block was made of only 1 type of
stimuli (see Fig. 1, right panel).
A trial consisted of a 1.8-s display of a pair of stimuli. The stimuli were
presented at equal eccentricities from a central fixation point (invisible
to the participant). The stimuli were followed by an intertrial interval of
0.2 s in which a fixation cross was presented. A sequence of 8 trials
with 1 stimulus type constituted a block. There were 3 blocks for each
experimental condition. A complete run was made up of the 12 blocks
with interleaved resting periods of 14 s. The run started and terminated
on a resting period of 14 s. The 12 blocks were randomly distributed
within a run with the restriction that 2 blocks of the same category
could not be presented successively.
Prior to the start of a block, a cueword displayed for 2 s informed the
participant whether to compare disk size, the number of dots, positive
integers, or negative integers. Before the participants entered the scanner,
they practiced with 2 blocks for each category of stimuli. This procedure
enabled the experimenter to check that the participant understood the
task and helped the participant familiarizing with the task.
fMRI Data Analysis
Standard preprocessing procedures were performed using Statistical
Parametric Mapping (SPM, version 8, Wellcome Trust for Neuroimaging, University College London). The first 5 volumes of each run
were discarded to allow for T1 equilibration. By using the magnetic field
map, each volume was corrected for magnetic field inhomogeneities.
All functional volumes were then realigned to the first volume of the
session and corrected for head motion. The functional mean image was
coregistered to the anatomical image. Functional images were
additionally normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI)
Cerebral Cortex June 2012, V 22 N 6 1421
Figure 1. (Left) Examples of stimuli used for the 2 analogue and the 2 symbolic conditions. (Right) Time course of events in a run: blocs of 16 s (8 trials) were alternating with
resting periods of 14 s.
standard template. During normalization, the functional volumes were
resampled to a 3 3 3 3 3--mm3 resolution. Last, functional images were
smoothed using an isotropic Gaussian kernel (full-width at halfmaximum = 8 mm).
Functional data were subjected to a general linear model analysis
as implemented in SPM8. To model the response of each subject, we
used 4 conditions: disk sizes, arrays of dots, positive integers, and
negative integers. For each condition, the canonical hemodynamic
response function (cHRF), the time derivative, and dispersion derivative were calculated. The cHRF models the most commonly
observed response of the BOLD impulse. The time derivative function
accounts for signal responses that are up to 1 s earlier (or later) than
the cHRF. Similarly, the dispersion derivative indicates if the peak lasts
less or longer than usual. The calculation of these additional parameters
allows a better modeling of the BOLD response (Henson et al. 2001). To
control for motion, the 6 motion parameters were entered as regressors
of no interest.
To generate the basic activation maps, baseline activity was subtracted
from task conditions. Hence, the first step of the analysis yielded 4 basic
activation maps: 1) disk size versus baseline (disk), 2) arrays of dots
versus baseline (dots), 3) positive integers versus baseline (positive),
and 4) negative integers versus baseline (negative). These contrasts
reflect the brain areas in charge of dealing with number mining, number
comparison, planning, and execution of the response. The basic
activation maps were then used to examine how stimulus format
influences neural processing.
To qualify the components common to all routes, first a conjunction
analysis of all 4 basic activation maps (disk \ dots \ positive \ negative)
was performed. To identify regions that are commonly activated by
analogue stimuli, a disk \ dots conjunction analysis was applied. To
establish which regions are differentially active, we computed the
contrast between the 2 classes of analogue stimuli (dots > disk). Both
the conjunction and contrast activity maps were then used to identify
regions that are in charge of computing approximate (disk) and exact
quantities (dots).
A similar procedure was applied for the 2 types of symbolic stimuli:
we generated a conjunction (negative \ positive) and a contrast
activation map (negative > positive). These maps were used to identify
the symbolic pathway and, more specifically, to assess how the level of
abstraction of the mathematical quantity influences number mining. In
a last step, the analogue stimuli on the one hand and symbolic stimuli
on the other hand were merged. Both categories were compared [(disk
+ dots) > (positive + negative)] to detect the areas that are differentially
activated during the processing of analogue and symbolic stimuli.
Unless stated otherwise, both first- and second-level analysis of
significant clusters of activation were determined using a voxel-level
statistical threshold of P < 0.0001 uncorrected with a spatial extent
threshold of k = 5. For conjunction analysis, only clusters of a size larger
than 10 voxels exhibiting a corrected familywise error at a < 0.05 are
reported. The activated brain regions were assigned according to the
anatomical toolbox (Eickhoff et al. 2005, version 1.7b, anatomical
update 2010). Cerebellar activations were not considered due to
incomplete coverage of the cerebellum in most of the subjects.
1422 Number Mining
d
Chassy and Grodd
Results
Behavioral
Mean performance was calculated by dividing the number of
correct responses by the total of responses. Figure 2 shows the
mean response time (RT) and performance of the participants
for each experimental condition. As expected, all subjects
performed with high accuracy (mean = 97.22%, standard error
[SE] = 1.58%).
Mean performance and RTs for each participant were
computed for each condition (disk, dots, positive, and negative)
and laterality of the correct responses (right vs. left). A repeatedmeasures analysis of variance performed with format and laterality
as within-subjects factors and average performances as the
dependent variable yielded no significant main effect (format:
F3,45 = 1.24, P = 0.31, mean squared error [MSE] < 0.01; laterality:
F1,15 = 0.25, P = 0.62, MSE = 0.02) and no interaction (F3,45 = 1.00,
P = 0.40, MSE < 0.01). For the RT analysis, all error trials were
discarded. Response latencies larger than 1800 ms or smaller than
200 ms were also excluded from the analysis (0.1% of the total
sample). Due to equipment failure, behavioral data of 1 participant
were not available for the last run. The average RT was calculated
for each participant and entered into a repeated analysis of
variance with format (disk, dots, positive, and negative) and
laterality (left vs. right) as within-subjects factors (see Fig. 2).
Laterality did not significantly influence the RTs (F1,15 = 0.39, P =
0.54, MSE = 0.01). Visual format significantly affected the RTs of
the participants (F3,45 = 78.11, P < 0.01, MSE < 0.01), with
significant shorter RTs for dots compared with all other
conditions. The mean RTs were as follows: disk, 0.68 s (SE = 0.02
s); dots, 0.58 s (SE = 0.02 s); positive, 0.65 s (SE = 0.02 s); and
negative, 0.69 s (SE = 0.02 s). The Format 3 Laterality interaction
was not significant (F3,45 = 1.47, P = 0.24, MSE < 0.01).
fMRI Results
Figure 3 presents the results of the 4 basic activation maps:
disk, dots, positive and negative maps. In all 4 conditions,
various visual areas along the dorsal (including the bilateral
SPL) and ventral streams were activated, showing that an
appreciable number of cortical areas are involved in number
mining (Supplementary Table 1). More specifically, disk
comparison yielded a pattern with increased BOLD signal in
the right middle occipital gyrus, left V5, and bilaterally in the
superior parietal lobules and precentral gyri. A decrease in the
BOLD signal was found in the right precentral gyrus, left
Figure 2. Behavioral results as a function of experimental conditions. ( A) Mean performance of the participants and (B) mean response time of the participants. The bars
indicate standard errors of the mean. PI, positive integers; NI, negative integers.
Figure 3. Basic contrast of each condition against the baseline. PI, positive integers; NI, negative integers. Note. The sites marked in red are activated and the sites marked in
blue are deactivated.
angular gyrus, middle cingulate cortex, and the cuneus. The
dots contrast revealed increased activation in the right and left
superior parietal lobules together with the right insula and
precentral gyrus. Deactivation occurred in the right angular
gyrus, Rolandic operculum, middle cingulated cortex, and the
cuneus. Positive integers generated a bilateral activation of the
superior parietal lobules and a deactivation in the left angular
gyrus and the anterior part of the medial temporal gyri. The
Cerebral Cortex June 2012, V 22 N 6 1423
comparison of negative integers yielded a similar pattern of
activation except for an additional activation in the left V5 and
a deactivation in the anterior parts of the temporal lobe.
The cortical areas that exhibit a significant BOLD signal change
in all task conditions are revealed by the conjunction analysis of
the 4 basic contrasts (see Fig. 4A). The analysis confirms that
comparing magnitudes involves a large network comprising lowlevel visual areas (right BA17), high-level visual areas (left occipital
gyrus), and numerical centers in the right IPS. Two peak activation
sites were located in the horizontal segment: one in hIP3 (MNI:
30, –51, 48) and one in hIP2 (MNI: 45, –39, 45). The execution of
the behavioral response has generated a left-dominant motor
activation cluster that peaked in BA6.
To examine the regions that are active during the processing
of analogue stimuli, a conjunction analysis was performed with
the disk and dots conditions (Fig. 4B). The analysis revealed
a network involving the Rolandic operculum, rectal gyrus, and
middle occipital gyrus in the left hemisphere, and the middle
orbital and supramarginal gyrus in the right hemisphere (see
Fig. 4B). Paralleling the activation map yielded by the
conjunction of all 4 conditions, the conjunction of analogue
conditions revealed a bilateral activation of visual cortices. We
also note the activation of a huge, motor-related activation
cluster that peaks in the left precentral gyrus and extends to
the supplementary motor area. Finally, localized sites of
activation were spotted in the postcentral gyrus (i.e., somatosensory areas). These cortical areas are known to process
somatosensory or motor information and as such are not of
central interest in examining number mining. The dots > disk
contrast presented in Figure 4C revealed clusters of activation
Figure 4. ( A) Activation map resulting from the conjunction analysis including the 4 experimental basic contrasts. This contrast reveals the areas that were active in all
conditions and thus putatively play a central role in any task involving a comparison between quantities. (B) Activation map of the conjunction analysis of the 2 analogue
conditions (i.e., disk and dots). This contrast reveals the core areas that constitute the inherited number system. (C) Dots [ disk contrast. It reveals the elements that are the
most fundamental to number mining and processing. (D) The conjunction of negative integers (NI) and positive integers (PI). It reveals the areas that are engaged in symbolic
processing regardless of the level of abstraction of the mathematical entity being processed. (E) The NI [ PI contrast reveals the area in charge of coding the notion of negativity.
(F) The analogue [ symbolic contrast shows the regions that are involved in extraction of visually coded quantities.
1424 Number Mining
d
Chassy and Grodd
in the bilateral primary visual cortices (BA17), the right
superior parietal lobule, and the bilateral middle occipital gyri.
Crucially, the ventral part of left visual area 3 (i.e., left hOC3v)
was sensitive to both the cHRF and its time derivative,
supporting the view that this region was more responsive in
the dots condition than in the disk condition and peaks earlier
than predicted by the canonical response.
The conjunction analysis of both symbolic conditions yielded
an interesting pattern of activation (see Fig. 4D): The left motorrelated BA6 areas, the right hIP3, and superior parietal lobule
were more active when the participants were performing
numerical comparison than at rest. Contrasting the 2 symbolic
conditions (negative > positive) highlighted significant activation in the right superior orbital gyrus. The time derivative
contrast revealed significant clusters of early activation in the left
SPL (Fig. 4E).
To distinguish the regions that belonged to the symbolic
path from those belonging to the analogue path, the 2 analogue
conditions were contrasted with the 2 symbolic conditions
(disk \ dots) -- (positive \ negative). The contrast (Fig. 4F)
reveals that the bilateral BA17 was more active during the
processing of analogue stimuli. The right inferior parietal
cortex and the right inferior temporal gyrus also show higher
activation for the analogue stimulus. It is worth noting that the
opposite contrast (i.e., symbolic > analogue conditions) does
not yield significant results.
The contrast estimates at the sites of peak activation provide
further information on number mining and retention. The disk
\ dots conjunction peak is located in the right superior parietal
lobule (MNI: 24, –63, 51). Noticeably, according to the
probability map (Eickhoff et al. 2005, software update 2010),
this site has a 20% chance of belonging to hIP3. The contrast
estimates at this location (Fig. 5A) show that the site is more
active in both conditions than at rest. As shown in Figure 5B,
the contrast estimates of dots > disk at this location (MNI: 24,
–63, 51) revealed that the BOLD signal is adequately modeled
by the cHRF and its time derivative (see the bar graph in
Fig. 5B). A similar procedure for the activity peak in the left
superior lobule (MNI: –24, –60, 51) reveals that this spot is
active in both symbolic conditions (Fig. 5C) and does activate
faster when participants had to compare negative integers
(Fig. 5D; statistics at a = 0.001, k > 5). With respect to the
conjunction analysis of all conditions, the contrast estimates at
the peak site (MNI: 30, –51, 48) of the cluster indicate that the
hIP3 region (at MNI: 30, –51, 48) is significantly more active is
all conditions compared with baseline (see bar graph in
Fig. 5E). As shown in Figure 5F, the activity in this site is
appropriately modeled by the cHRF.
Discussion
The aims of our study were to characterize the various routes
that extract the magnitude in visual displays and to understand
how this process influences the encoding in the IPS. A
comparison task was used to assess how participants compare
magnitudes encoded in 4 different formats. In line with
previous studies, we used disks and dots as analogue stimuli,
and positive integers as symbolic stimuli. We introduced
negative integers to examine the coding of abstract mathematical concepts (i.e., negativity). It was expected that the right
IPS, as the repository of the number sense, should be activated
in all conditions. With respect to semantic assignment, the
activation foci for each category should be located in distinct
regions: we expected right IPS activation for analogue stimuli
and a bilateral IPS activation for symbolic stimuli.
The discussion is divided in 4 sections. First, we discuss the
comparison of analogue stimuli as the findings show how exact
and approximate magnitudes are extracted. We discuss in the
second section the comparison of symbolic stimuli. It reveals
the neural networks that process numbers in symbolic format,
including abstract mathematical notions such as negativity. In
the third section, the commonalities of and differences
between analogue and symbolic stimuli will be synthesized.
In the last section, we integrate our findings into the triplecode model from Dehaene et al. (2003).
Analogue Routes
The analogue route is primarily characterized by the brain
structures that were active in both analogue conditions. The
conjunction analysis showed that several subparts (hIP2 at MNI:
45, –39, 45; hIP3 at MNI: 30, –51, 48) of the right IPS are
consistently active in both conditions. This result is in line with
the study by Piazza et al. (2002) that reported a similar rightdominant pattern of activation in the parietal cortex when
participants compared arrays of dots. Our finding supports the
idea that the neural signals coding magnitude are held in
a specific subpart of the IPS. The activation of the left Rolandic
operculum indicates verbal processing and thus suggests that
human agents automatically access a verbal code. There is,
however, a possibility that activation of the left Rolandic
operculum may merely reflect subvocalization. Our results are
consistent with the triple-code model (Dehaene et al. 2003)
and locate with higher precision where the information about
magnitude is encoded.
The contrast dots > disk highlights the areas that are specific
to code exact quantities. The contrast shows that not only the
left middle occipital gyrus but also the ventral part of left area
V3 (hOC3v) is more active in the dots than in the disk
condition. hOC3v is a retinotopic region belonging to the
ventral stream (Rottschy et al. 2007), and as such, one might
suggest that the analogue route relies partly on verbal coding.
However, the ventral V3 region is usually not thought to be
part of the network that is connected to the visual word area
(McCandliss et al. 2003). Furthermore, in monkeys this region
projects onto the middle temporal area (Lyon and Kaas 2001)
showing that it can modulate spatial processing. As hOC3v
activation has been shown to merely reflect top-down
controlled attention (Lauritzen et al. 2009), we suggest that
the differential activation is accounted for by the increase in
attentional resources needed to estimate the number of dots.
The differential activity of right V1 (BA17) also reflects spatially
driven attention (Proverbio et al. 2010). hOC3v and right V1
are part of the number mining system as their activation is
dependent upon the format of the visual input.
The central finding of the contrast between the 2 analogue
conditions is that the right superior parietal lobule is more
active in the dots than in the disk condition. This finding
suggests that this area is more involved in the comparison of
exact quantities (i.e., number of dots). Our results are
consistent with the idea that the route taken by approximate
and exact signals will eventually terminate in the right IPS. A
neurophysiological explanation is that simple accumulator
neurons are sufficient to explain comparison of approximate
Cerebral Cortex June 2012, V 22 N 6 1425
Figure 5. Location and parameter estimates for each site of peak activity. ( A) Right superior parietal lobule, (B) right superior parietal lobule, (C) left superior parietal lobule, (D)
left superior parietal lobule, (E) right IPS, and (F ) right superior occipital gyrus. Note. The sites marked in red are activated and the sites marked in blue are deactivated. Note. PI,
positive integers; NI, negative integers.
quantities, while accumulator-tuned neurons are necessary for
the comparison of exact numbers. Our conclusions are
consistent with previous research about the distribution of
numerical-sensitive neurons in the right parietal cortex and
with current models of number processing (Dehaene and
Changeux 1993; Piazza and Izard 2009).
1426 Number Mining
d
Chassy and Grodd
Symbolic Routes
The main finding yielded by the conjunction analysis is that the right
IPS (hIP3) and superior parietal lobule were significantly active. The
time derivatives further specify that these 2 regions were fast in
responding to stimulus presence. These results again highlight the
central role that the right hIPS plays in the number sense.
When we contrasted the activity maps generated by the
comparison of negative and positive digits, we found a significant difference in the right BA17. Contrary to our prediction,
the early visual areas were significantly more activated by the
additional presence of a minus sign. This difference in
activation may be accounted for by enhanced spatial attention
(Proverbio et al. 2010). In addition, the contrast revealed that
the left SPL was more active in the negative condition (see Fig.
5E). This finding is crucial regarding the debate about the brain
structures implicated in the processing of abstract mathematical knowledge. Our results suggest that the notion of
negativity is implemented in a restricted zone that lies close
to a cortical site where positive numbers (left IPS) are mapped
onto spatial codes (right IPS). This interpretation is supported
by various strands of research. First, our results are consistent
with developmental studies showing that the left IPS is
progressively recruited during the learning of mathematical
symbols (Cantlon et al. 2006, 2010). Second, adults performing
a subtraction task, which obviously includes the notion of
negativity, activate the left superior parietal lobule (Simon et al.
2004). In addition, when adults react to potential money losses,
their left superior parietal module is particularly responsive
(De Pascalis et al. 2010). Finally, our view is in line with the
cultural recycling hypothesis (Dehaene and Cohen 2007),
which states that cultural acquisition of mathematical abilities
needs a neural niche close to language processing areas. Our
findings are consistent with the pool of available data
suggesting that the left superior parietal lobule is a core neural
module in representing negativity. In short, both symbolic
stimuli include the right hIP3 as a terminal representation of
magnitudes, but they make a differential use of the neural
networks in the left superior parietal lobule.
The Common System
The conjunction analysis of all conditions (see Fig. 5A) revealed
a right lateralized network including key elements in the right
IPS and in the right superior parietal lobule (MNI: 30, –60, 54).
These results basically replicate what has been previously
found in quantity comparison tasks: Regardless the format of
the stimuli, the IPS will eventually code the quantities being
compared (Pinel et al. 2001; Piazza et al. 2002; Cohen Kadosh
et al. 2005; Holloway et al. 2010). In addition to this predictable
result, the conjunction analysis revealed further specific
information about the localization of the core parts of number
processing. Due to advances in the precision of the anatomical
atlases (Eickhoff et al. 2005, software update 2010), we were
able to identify localized activation within 2 subregions of the
right IPS: namely, hIP2 (MNI: 45, –39, 45) and hIP3 (MNI: 30,
–51, 48). The central module of magnitude representation is in
our view a neural network recruiting neurons in the hIP2 and
hIP3 regions. In contrast with Wu et al. (2009), we did not find
activation in hIP1. Since we also used probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps, it is unlikely that the difference in pattern of
activation is due to the type of analysis. Rather, considering the
fact that Wu et al. (2009) used arithmetic operations, we would
suggest that hIP1 is somehow involved in the mixing of the
magnitude values. For example, it might hold the result of an
addition in a spatial format. We note that hIP2 was active in the
conjunction of all 4 conditions (MNI: 45, –39, 45), but that it did
not constitute a zone of peak activity when the conjunction
was limited to the symbolic conditions (i.e., positive \
negative). These results suggest that the mapping of the
analogue representation of quantities recruits the right hIP3
but not the right hIP2. We further suggest that hIP2 houses
accumulator cells (similar to the accumulator cells found in
monkey; see Roitman et al. 2007); the cells in hIP3 would be
accumulator cells tuned to respond to specific values. Our
theoretical conclusion is that symbols are mapped directly into
these hIP3 cells.
The left angular gyrus was deactivated in all 4 conditions.
This finding is consistent with previous studies addressing
related issues (see Ischebeck et al. 2009 for an example and
a discussion). According to the triple-code model, the angular
gyrus is implicated in the retrieval of mathematical facts
(Dehaene et al. 2003). In the present experiment, there was no
need to retrieve mathematical facts so that this region was not
required. Basically, our results suggest that the core system
used to process quantities does not involve the angular gyrus.
The verbal-related regions that were active were not restricted
to the right Rolandic operculum. We also found sporadic
activations in pars orbitalis and pars opercularis, regions known
to be involved in language processing (Saur et al. 2008). Such
findings lend support to the view that verbal areas are
automatically engaged when adults manipulate numbers. The
exact role played by each of the 3 structures should be
addressed in further research.
Interestingly, the contrast between the analogue and
symbolic systems did not reveal differences in verbal-related
areas. This result could suggest that the participants used
subvocalization to compare not only symbolic but also analogue
stimuli. This point might be of importance as it suggests that
adults automatically rely on verbal cues to facilitate the
decision even though the task might not necessarily require
a verbal response. The finding that the analogue path mobilizes
more brain areas, as evidenced by the analogue > symbolic
contrast (see Fig. 5F), may reflect that extracting the
magnitude from a physical stimulus is more resource demanding for the visual areas than retrieving the value from
a symbolic stimulus.
Conversely, since symbolic processing implies verbal
components, one could have expected that the symbolic >
analogue contrast would highlight some verbal areas. However, as discussed in the previous paragraph, the comparison
of analogue quantities does not preclude the activation of
verbal knowledge. This view is supported by the conjunction
analysis of analogue conditions (Fig. 4B), which shows that
verbal knowledge is called upon to complete the task. That no
statistically significant difference was detected does imply
that activation levels were similar between the analogue and
symbolic conditions but does not rule out the hypothesis that
the verbal content was different. For example, whereas the
comparison of positive integers activates symbols such as ‘‘3,’’
the approximate condition might have activated verbal
quantifiers such as ‘‘larger’’ (disk). Further research should
evaluate, separately for analogue and symbolic processing, to
which extent verbal activation is attributable to number
processing and to the task (i.e., comparing 2 items along
a determined dimension).
A Functional Framework for Number Mining
We propose a theoretical attempt to systematize the knowledge
about number mining and the pattern of activity concerned with
Cerebral Cortex June 2012, V 22 N 6 1427
magnitude coding (see Fig. 6). In line with the approach
suggested by Arsalidou and Taylor (2011), we recommend
updating the triple-code model (Dehaene et al. 2003). Our use
of cytoprobabilistic maps, together with the cHRF and its time
derivative, enables us to propose a finer-grained map of the
anatomical structures involved in quantity processing.
The flowchart in Figure 6 depicts the information flow
underpinning the number sense. The system is composed of 3
functional sets: the spatial core of the system, the culturally
recycled plug-ins, and the number mining modules. The spatial
core regroups the anatomical structures that code magnitudes
as spatial quantities. This system has been inherited from
primates as indicated by the similar performance of animals
(Nieder et al. 2002) and non-educated humans (Pica et al.
2004) in completing basic numerical tasks. The ‘‘inherited’’
system represents magnitudes in a logarithmic format until it
receives proper training to improve its ability to represent
quantities. In line with the distinction between approximate
and exact spatial mapping of magnitudes, we distinguished 2
subparts: While neurons in hIP2 are encoding approximate
quantities such as bar length or disk size, neurons in hIP3
respond to exact values (e.g., as the number of dots in an array).
The culturally recycled regions regroup the anatomical
structures that were recruited to serve new, evolutionaryrelevant functions (Dehaene and Cohen 2007). Since these
regions add specific capabilities to the core system, we term
them plug-ins. The verbal code plug-in refers to the area in
charge of storing the visual representations of Arabic digits.
The visual patterns of Arabic digits are associated to the
neurons in hIP3 (which code exact quantities). Whenever an
Arabic digit is recognized, the neurons coding for the given
magnitude are activated. The verbal plug-in constitutes the last
stage of a symbolic recognition process. As such, there is no
quantity extraction made along the path, rather the quantity is
associated to the symbol and automatically activated in case of
recognition. Another plug-in enables the retrieval of simple
facts to perform arithmetical operations (such as multiplication). This plug-in has been localized in the left angular gyrus
(see Dehaene et al. 2003 for the theory and Grabner et al. 2009
for empirical evidence). The last plug-in we identified codes for
the notion of negativity. The present study shows that
negativity is coded in the left superior parietal lobule. This
plug-in connects to the core system to integrate the notion of
negativity into an independent representation of a magnitude.
Some components of the system are highly sensitive to
format. For example, the left ventral V3 and the right V1 have
been shown to modulate their levels of activity in both the
dots > disk and negative > positive contrasts. In addition, the
system depicted in Figure 6 could be expanded by adding
a decision-supporting module in charge of holding information online (which would account for the frontal activations)
and a motor module in charge of computing the behavioral
responses (with a center of gravity in BA6).
The experiment presented in this article has deepened our
understanding of numerical cognition. Similar to Santens et al.
(2010), we reached the conclusion that number-sensitive neurons
can be accessed by various pathways. For each format, we further
specified the network supporting number mining. A limit in
mapping the possible areas sensitive to format is that we could not
test all the possible visual formats. Further research can complete
our work by testing the comparison of various other formats (e.g.,
roman format). A second limit is that the role of the verbal regions
remains unclear when the stimuli are in analogue format. This is
another question that is to be addressed in further research.
Evidently, each tenet underlying the proposed mapping of
the triple-code model (Dehaene et al. 2003) can be tested.
Since a major conclusion of our research is the putative
functional difference between the neurons in hIP2, coding
approximate magnitudes, and the neurons in hIP3, coding
exact quantities, it is crucial to replicate our results with
Figure 6. Proposed functional neuroanatomy for the extraction and comparison of quantities (mapped into the triple-code model; Dehaene et al. 2003).
1428 Number Mining
d
Chassy and Grodd
equivalent material. For example, to yield a functional map of
the hIPS that can be compared with our own, we suggest
replicating our experiment with the use of other material to
implement exact (e.g., roman numeral) and approximate
quantities (e.g., bar length). Central to our model is the
prediction that comparing roman numerals will activate hIP3
neurons, while comparing bars will yield a peak of activity in
hIP2 neurons. Furthermore, contrasting the activation sites
obtained during the comparison of bars with our activation
map of the disk condition could reveal a difference in peak
site in hIP2 region. Such a difference would show that the
coding of approximate quantities is dependent on format and
thus would suggest that number mining influences how
quantities are ultimately coded in the hIPS. In addition, it
would be of great interest to manipulate the visual features of
targets and their spatial distribution in the display so as to
disentangle number mining processes from attentional processes. The proposed approach will not only test whether the
areas we identified as number mining are active when
processing other types of material but it also might enable
the identification of other number mining areas. Finally, using
the material we used in various numerical tasks will challenge
the robustness of our mapping by possibly revealing that some
areas are task dependent rather than format sensitive.
As understanding negativity is a crucial step in the
development of mathematical abilities, would it be only for
its role in physics, it seems essential to conduct further
research in this direction. The model posits that hIP3 neurons
are the actual recipients coding exact quantities and that left
SPL neurons code for negativity. In line with our model, an
experiment requiring the participants to learn an association
of new symbols with integers would lead to 2 predictable
results. First, neurons in hIP3 should not be active during the
passive viewing of the symbols before learning has taken
place, but these neurons should be active afterward. Second,
an association of symbols with negative integers will activate
not only hIP3 neurons but also left SPL neurons. Should not
both results be found, then the proposed model would
require revision.
What our key findings and theoretical framework highlight is
a crucial distinction between the processing of symbolic
stimuli and the processing of analogue stimuli. For analogue
stimuli, number mining requires much visual processing within
early visual areas, the neural signal being ultimately processed
in the parietal cortex. The neurons that eventually code the
magnitude are lateralized to the right. If the quantity is
approximate, then only hIP2 neurons respond. If the quantity
is exact, then hIP2 and hIP3 neurons respond. Number mining
with respect to symbolic stimuli is a process of an altogether
different nature. The signal is processed along the ventral
pathway until recognition of the Arabic digit is completed.
Thereafter, the activation spreads to the associated neurons in
hIP3. Supplementary plug-ins, such as negativity or fact
retrieval, can enter the game whenever required. We conclude
that, in a restricted sense, the concept of number mining
applies to analogue but not symbolic stimuli, the processing of
which is closer to pattern recognition.
Supplementary Material
Supplementary material can be found at: http://www.cercor.
oxfordjournals.org/
Funding
Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG), Germany (GR833/81, GR833/9-1).
Notes
The authors are grateful to Prof. Andreas Nieder, Tübingen; Prof.
Brigitte Stemmer, Montreal; Prof. Klaus Willmes, Aachen; and Prof.
Simon Eickhoff, Aachen, for their useful comments and critical insights.
Preliminary results were presented in a poster at the HBM conference,
Barcelona, 2010. Conflict of Interest : None declared.
References
Arsalidou M, Taylor MJ. 2011. Is 2+2=4? Meta analysis of brain
areas needed for numbers and calculations. Neuroimage. 54:
2382--2393.
Cantlon JF, Brannon EM, Carter EJ, Pelphrey KA. 2006. Functional
imaging of numerical processing in adults and four-y-old children.
PLoS Biol. 4:e125.
Cantlon JF, Libertus ME, Pinel P, Dehaene S, Brannon EM, Pelphrey KA.
2010. The neural development of an abstract concept of number.
J Cogn Neurosci. 21:2217--2229.
Cohen Kadosh R, Cohen Kadosh K, Kaas A, Henik A, Goebel R. 2007.
Notation-dependent and -independent representations of numbers
in the parietal lobes. Neuron. 53:307--314.
Cohen Kadosh R, Henik A, Rubinsten O, Mohr H, Dori H, Van de Ven V,
Zorzi M, Hendler T, Goebel R, Linden DEJ. 2005. Are numbers
special? The comparison systems of the human brain investigated by
fMRI. Neuropsychologia. 43:1238--1248.
Dehaene S, Changeux JP. 1993. Development of elementary numerical
abilities: a neural model. J Cogn Neurosci. 5:390--407.
Dehaene S, Cohen L. 1991. Two mental calculation systems. A case
study of severe acalculia with preserved approximation. Neuropsychologia. 29:1045--1074.
Dehaene S, Cohen L. 1997. Cerebral pathways for calculation: double
dissociation between rote verbal and quantitative knowledge of
arithmetic. Cortex. 33:219--250.
Dehaene S, Cohen L. 2007. Cultural recycling of cortical maps. Neuron.
56:384--398.
Dehaene S, Piazza M, Pinel P, Cohen L. 2003. Three parietal circuits for
number processing. Cogn Neuropsychol. 20:487--506.
De Pascalis V, Varriale V, D’Antuono L. 2010. Event-related components of
the punishment and reward sensitivity. Clin Neurophysiol. 121:60--76.
Eger E, Sterzer P, Russ MO, Giraud AL, Kleinschmidt A. 2003.
A supramodal number representation in human intraparietal cortex.
Neuron. 37:719--725.
Eickhoff SB, Stephan KE, Mohlberg H, Grefkes C, Fink GR, Amunts K,
Zilles K. 2005. A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic
cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. Neuroimage.
25:1325--1335.
Fias W, Lammertyn J, Reynvoet B, Dupont P, Orban GA. 2003. Parietal
representation of symbolic and nonsymbolic magnitude. J Cogn
Neurosci. 15:47--56.
Fischer M. 2003. Cognitive representation of negative numbers. Psychol
Sci. 14:278--282.
Ganor-Stern D, Tzelgov J. 2008. Negative numbers are generated in the
mind. Exp Psychol. 55:157--163.
Grabner RH, Ansari D, Koschutnig K, Reishofer G, Ebner F, Neuper C.
2009. To retrieve or to calculate? Left angular gyrus mediates the
retrieval of arithmetic facts during problem solving. Neuropsychologia. 47:604--608.
Halberda J, Feigenson L. 2008. Developmental change in the acuity of
the ‘‘number sense’’: the approximate number system in 3-, 4-, 5-,
and 6-year-olds and adults. Dev Psychol. 44:1457--1465.
Henson RNA, Rugg MD, Friston KJ. 2001. The choice of basis functions
in event-related fMRI. Neuroimage. 13:149.
Holloway ID, Price GR, Ansari D. 2010. Common and segregated neural
pathways for the processing of symbolic and nonsymbolic
numerical magnitude: an fMRI study. Neuroimage. 49:1006--1017.
Cerebral Cortex June 2012, V 22 N 6 1429
Ischebeck A, Schocke M, Delazer M. 2009. The processing and
representation of fractions within the brain: an fMRI investigation.
Neuroimage. 47:403--413.
Keller K, Menon V. 2009. Gender differences in the functional and
structural neuroanatomy of mathematical cognition. Neuroimage.
47:342--352.
Kucian K, von Aster M, Loenneker T, Dietrich T, Martin E. 2008.
Development of neural networks for exact and approximate
calculation: a fMRI study. Dev Neuropsychol. 33:447--473.
Lammertyn J, Reynvoet B, Dupont P, Orban GA. 2003. Parietal
representation of symbolic and nonsymbolic magnitude. J Cogn
Neurosci. 15:1--11.
Lauritzen TZ, D’esposito M, Heeger DJ, Silver MA. 2009. Top-down flow
of visual spatial attention signals from parietal to occipital cortex.
J Vision. 9:1--14.
Lemer C, Dehaene S, Spelke E, Cohen L. 2003. Approximate quantities
and exact number words: dissociable systems. Neuropsychologia.
41:1942--1958.
Lyon DC, Kaas JH. 2001. Connectional and architectonic evidence for
dorsal and ventral V3, and dorsomedial area in marmoset monkeys.
J Neurosci. 21:249--261.
McCandliss BD, Cohen L, Dehaene S. 2003. The visual word form area:
expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus. Trends Cogn Sci.
7:293--299.
McNeil JW, Warrington EK. 1994. A dissociation between addition and
subtraction
with
written
calculation.
Neuropsychologia.
32:717--728.
Nieder A. 2005. Counting on neurons: the neurobiology of numerical
competence. Nat Rev Neurosci. 6:177--190.
Nieder A, Dehaene S. 2009. Representation of number in the brain.
Annu Rev Neurosci. 32:185--208.
Nieder A, Freedman DJ, Miller EK. 2002. Representation of quantity of
visual items in the primate prefrontal cortex. Science.
297:1708--1711.
Nieder A, Miller EK. 2004. A parieto-frontal network for visual
numerical information in the monkey. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A.
101:7457--7462.
Piazza M, Izard C. 2009. How humans count: numerosity and the
parietal cortex. Neuroscientist. 15:261--273.
Piazza M, Mechelli A, Butterworth B, Price CJ. 2002. Are subitizing and
counting implemented as separate or functionally overlapping
processes? Neuroimage. 15:435--446.
Piazza M, Mechelli A, Price CJ, Butterworth B. 2006. Exact and
approximate judgements of visual and auditory numerosity: an
fMRI study. Brain Res. 1106:177--188.
Piazza M, Pinel P, Le Bihan D, Dehaene S. 2007. A magnitude code
common to numerosities and number symbols in human intraparietal cortex. Neuron. 53:293--305.
Pica P, Lemer C, Izard V, Dehaene S. 2004. Exact and approximate
arithmetic in an Amazonian indigene group. Science. 306:499--503.
Cerebral Cortex June 2012, V 22 N 6 1430
Pinel P, Dehaene S, Rivière D, LeBihan D. 2001. Modulation of parietal
activation by semantic distance in a number comparison task.
Neuroimage. 14:1013--1026.
Proverbio AM, Del Zotto M, Zani A. 2010. Electrical neuroimaging
evidence that spatial frequency based selective attention affects V1
activity as early as 40-60 ms in humans. BMC Neurosci. 11:59.
Roitman JD, Brannon EM, Platt ML. 2007. Monotonic coding of
numerosity in macaque lateral intraparietal area. PLoS Biol. 5:e2008.
Rottschy C, Eickhoff SB, Schleicher A, Mohlberg H, Kujovic M, Zilles K,
Amunts K. 2007. Ventral visual cortex in humans: cytoarchitectonic
mapping of two extrastriate areas. Hum Brain Mapp. 28:1045--1059.
Santens S, Roggeman C, Fias W, Verguts T. 2010. Number processing
pathways in human parietal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 20:77--88.
Saur D, Kreher BW, Schnell S, Kümmerer D, Kellmeyer P, Vry MS,
Umarova R, Musso M, Glauche V, Abel S, et al. 2008. Ventral and
dorsal pathways for language. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A.
150:18035--18040.
Shaki S, Petrusic WM. 2005. On the mental representation of negative
numbers: context-dependent SNARC effects with comparative
judgments. Psychon Bull Rev. 12:931--937.
Simon O, Kherif F, Flandin G, Poline J-B, Rivière D, Mangin J-F, Le
Bihan D, Dehaene S. 2004. Automatized clustering and functional
geometry of human parietofrontal networks for language, space, and
number. Neuroimage. 23:1192--1202.
Szaflarski JP, Binder JR, Possing ET, McKiernan KA, Ward BD,
Hammeke TA. 2002. Language lateralization in left-handed and
ambidextrous people. Neurology. 59:238--344.
Tzelgov J, Ganor-Stern D, Maymon-Schreiber K. 2009. The representation of negative numbers: exploring the effects of mode of
processing and notation. Q J Exp Psychol. 62:605--624.
Van Harskamp NJ, Cipolotti L. 2001. Selective impairment for addition,
subtraction and multiplication implication for the organisation of
arithmetical facts. Cortex. 37:363--388.
Venkatraman V, Ansari D, Chee MWL. 2005. Neural correlates of
symbolic and non-symbolic arithmetic. Neuropsychologia.
43:744--753.
Wu SS, Chang TT, Majid A, Caspers S, Eickhoff SD, Menon V. 2009.
Functional heterogeneity of inferior parietal cortex during mathematical cognition assessed with cytoarchitectonic probability maps.
Cereb Cortex. 19:2930--2945.
Zago L, Pesenti M, Mellet E, Crivello F, Mazoyer B, Tzourio-Mazoyer N.
2001. Neural correlates of simple and complex mental calculation.
Neuroimage. 13:314--327.
Zhou X, Chen C, Zang Y, Dong Q, Chen C, Qiao S, Gong Q. 2007.
Dissociated brain organization for single-digit addition and multiplication. Neuroimage. 35:871--880.
Zorzi M, Di Bono MG, Fias W. 2011. Distinct representations of
numerical and non-numerical order in the human intraparietal
sulcus revealed by multivariate pattern recognition. Neuroimage.
56:674--680.