Download Evolution of the Size and Functional Areas of the Human Brain

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Emotional lateralization wikipedia , lookup

Cortical cooling wikipedia , lookup

Clinical neurochemistry wikipedia , lookup

Limbic system wikipedia , lookup

Dual consciousness wikipedia , lookup

Nervous system network models wikipedia , lookup

Cognitive neuroscience of music wikipedia , lookup

Neuromarketing wikipedia , lookup

Causes of transsexuality wikipedia , lookup

Functional magnetic resonance imaging wikipedia , lookup

Activity-dependent plasticity wikipedia , lookup

Lateralization of brain function wikipedia , lookup

Time perception wikipedia , lookup

Donald O. Hebb wikipedia , lookup

Neurogenomics wikipedia , lookup

Blood–brain barrier wikipedia , lookup

Embodied cognitive science wikipedia , lookup

Craniometry wikipedia , lookup

Brain wikipedia , lookup

Artificial general intelligence wikipedia , lookup

Human multitasking wikipedia , lookup

Haemodynamic response wikipedia , lookup

Neuroesthetics wikipedia , lookup

Neuroscience and intelligence wikipedia , lookup

History of anthropometry wikipedia , lookup

Neurotechnology wikipedia , lookup

Neuroinformatics wikipedia , lookup

Neurophilosophy wikipedia , lookup

Sports-related traumatic brain injury wikipedia , lookup

Impact of health on intelligence wikipedia , lookup

Neuropsychopharmacology wikipedia , lookup

Selfish brain theory wikipedia , lookup

Connectome wikipedia , lookup

Neuroanatomy wikipedia , lookup

Holonomic brain theory wikipedia , lookup

Neurolinguistics wikipedia , lookup

Human brain wikipedia , lookup

Brain morphometry wikipedia , lookup

Cognitive neuroscience wikipedia , lookup

Neuroeconomics wikipedia , lookup

Neuroplasticity wikipedia , lookup

Aging brain wikipedia , lookup

Brain Rules wikipedia , lookup

History of neuroimaging wikipedia , lookup

Metastability in the brain wikipedia , lookup

Neuropsychology wikipedia , lookup

Evolution of human intelligence wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
ANRV287-AN35-20
ARI
9 September 2006
8:42
Evolution of the Size
and Functional Areas
of the Human Brain
P. Thomas Schoenemann
Department of Behavioral Sciences, University of Michigan–Dearborn,
Dearborn, Michigan 48128; email: [email protected]
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006. 35:379–406
Key Words
First published online as a Review in
Advance on June 16, 2006
neuroanatomy, encephalization, behavior, adaptation, selection
The Annual Review of Anthropology is online
at anthro.annualreviews.org
Abstract
This article’s doi:
10.1146/annurev.anthro.35.081705.123210
c 2006 by Annual Reviews.
Copyright All rights reserved
0084-6570/06/1021-0379$20.00
The human brain is one of the most intricate, complicated, and
impressive organs ever to have evolved. Understanding its evolution requires integrating knowledge from a variety of disciplines in
the natural and social sciences. Four areas of research are particularly important to this endeavor. First, we need to understand basic
principles of brain evolution that appear to operate across broad
classes of organisms. Second, we need to understand the ways in
which human brains differ from the brains of our closest living relatives. Third, clues from the fossil record may allow us to outline the
manner in which these differences evolved. Finally, studies of brain
structure/function relationships are critical for us to make behavioral sense of the evolutionary changes that occurred. This review
highlights important questions and work in each of these areas.
379
ANRV287-AN35-20
ARI
9 September 2006
8:42
INTRODUCTION
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
Encephalization
quotient (EQ):
calculated as the
ratio of a species’
actual brain size to
the size expected
given its body weight
380
The evolution of the human brain has been
one of the most significant events in the
evolution of life. Although the outline of
how and why this happened is being filled
in, many fundamental questions remain to
be answered. The fossil record, in concert
with a comparative neuroanatomical analysis of closely related species, shows that the
hominid brain increased in size more than
threefold over a period of approximately 2.5
million years. However, it has become increasingly clear that the human brain is not
simply a large ape brain: Important qualitative and quantitative changes occurred as
well. Some of these changes are a result of
broad patterns of brain evolution that appear
across species, either for developmental reasons or because of patterns of adaptation that
are inherent in the nature of life. Some are
presumably a result of direct selection for
specific behavioral abilities of various kinds.
Unraveling which adaptational explanations
are possible or likely requires understanding
as much as we can about how brain structure relates to behavioral variation. Unraveling the story of human evolution requires
research in each of these areas: (a) general
patterns of brain evolution, (b) comparative assessment of brain anatomy across
species, (c) the fossil history of human-brain
evolution, and (d ) brain structure/function
relationships.
This review focuses on trying to understand evolutionary changes in brain size as
well as the proportions of different brain
areas. It highlights conceptual areas and
questions that are prone to misunderstanding, need particularly careful assessment, are
of current controversy, or present important avenues for future research. It necessarily leaves out some research areas that
are both important and interesting, such as
deep cortical and brainstem nuclei, the evolution of specific neuron types and their
Schoenemann
specializations, and possible gene-expression
changes.1
PATTERNS OF BRAIN
EVOLUTION
Brain/Body Scaling
How should species be compared with respect
to brain size? It has long been known that
brain size scales with body size across broad
groups of animals (Dubois 1913). For this reason, some measure of relative brain size has
usually been favored in comparative studies.
Empirically the relationship between brain
and body size can be estimated using a function of the form [brain] = c[body]a , where c
and a are empirically derived constants. Because the brain/body relationship is nonlinear, a simple ratio of brain to body is problematic (e.g., small animals have larger ratios
than larger animals on average). Encephalization quotients (EQs) are widely used measures
of relative brain size that take this empirical relationship between brain and body size
into account. They are simply the ratio of a
species’ actual brain size to the brain size expected for an animal of its body size ( Jerison
1973). The expected brain size for a given
species is usually derived using a regression
(or other method, e.g., reduced major axis) of
log brain to log body size for the comparison group of species, resulting in a formula of
the type [log brain] = log c + a[log body].
Jerison (1973) used mammals as the comparison group, but one can estimate EQs, for
example, on the basis of primates only. One
can also extend the concept to subcomponents
of the brain and scale them either against
1
I encourage those interested in extended reviews of human
brain evolution to consult Allman 1999, Deacon 1997, Falk
1992, Geary 2005, Holloway et al. 2004a, and Striedter
2005.
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
ANRV287-AN35-20
ARI
9 September 2006
8:42
body size or brain size (see, e.g., Schoenemann
1997).
Because EQs are calculated on the basis
of empirical estimates of brain/body-scaling
relationships, they are sensitive to the particular sample used to derive a and c parameters. Jerison (1973) originally estimated the
scaling parameter a (i.e., slope) for mammals
as ∼0.67, but Martin (1981) estimated it to
be 0.76 using a larger sample. Using Jerison’s
(1973) equation, human EQs are ∼7, whereas
Martin’s (1981) equation gives the values at
∼5. Regardless of the slope estimate used,
however, humans consistently have the highest values among mammals.
Figure 1 shows brain/body-size relationships for a sample of 52 primate species and
illustrates different scaling estimates for separate primate subtaxa. Primates as a group
tend to have larger brains than the average mammal, with EQs for anthropoids
(all primates excluding prosimians) averaging ∼2 (i.e., anthropoids have brains approximately twice the size of the average
mammal of their body size). Even though
absolute brain size is significantly larger
in pongids (chimpanzee, bonobo, gorilla,
orangutan) than in all other anthropoids except humans, they do not have substantially
larger EQs, indicating their brains are scaling approximately similar to other anthropoids. Human brain sizes, by contrast, are
not explained by brain/body scaling in either
mammals or primates.
Although EQs are superficially appealing as a measure of comparative brain size,
it is unclear exactly how to interpret EQ
differences between species. The behavioral
relevance of EQ has long been questioned
(Holloway 1966), yet it is often incorrectly
assumed that it must be the most behaviorally relevant variable of brain size—as if
it were some coarse estimate of intelligence
or other behavioral ability (e.g., Kappelman
1996). Others have uncritically assumed that
increases in absolute brain size may not be
meaningful if EQs remain the same (e.g.,
Wood & Collard 1999, Wynn 2002). Unfor-
tunately, EQs are not so easily interpreted.
If EQ really tells us something about intelligence or general behavioral complexity, what
are we to make of the large whales, who have
the lowest EQs of all mammals [e.g., humpback whales (Megaptera nodosa) have EQs
of 0.18 (Schoenemann 1997)]? Humpback
whales display a variety of complex behaviors,
including structured vocal sequences (songs?)
that last 5–25 min before repeating, and complex feeding techniques, including the use of
bubble clouds to encircle prey (Rendell et al.
2001).
Other species comparisons further highlight the problem: Guinea pigs (Cavia cutler) have significantly higher EQs (0.95) than
do elephants (Loxodonta africana, 0.63). This
is true even though guinea pig brains weigh
only ∼3.3 g, whereas elephant brains can
weigh over 5700 g (Schoenemann 1997). If
EQ really tells us something important about
behavior, this should be evident in a comparison of guinea pig versus elephant behavior, yet this does not appear to be the
case. If anything, given the variety of complex social behaviors known in elephants (e.g.,
McComb et al. 2001), absolute brain size
appears more behaviorally relevant in this
comparison.
In fact, a number of studies suggest that—
for some behavioral domains—absolute brain
size is more relevant than EQ. This is true for
measures of the ease of learning abstract rules
as opposed to simple associations (Rumbaugh
et al. 1996), as well as the speed of learning
object-discrimination tasks (Riddell & Corl
1977). In addition, although relative brain size
is often emphasized in brain/behavior studies,
associations are invariably also significant if
absolute brain volumes are used (e.g., Dunbar
1995, Reader & Laland 2002). Jerison (1973)
recognized this when he suggested an alternative to EQ: an estimate of extra neurons,
which is the absolute amount of neural tissue beyond that predicted empirically by body
mass (e.g., two species with identical EQs but
different body masses also differ in number
of extra neurons). This measure is, however,
www.annualreviews.org • Evolution of the Human Brain
Pongids:
larger-bodied apes,
part of the taxonomic
superfamily
Hominoidea, which
include common and
bonobo
chimpanzees,
gorillas, and
orangutans
381
ARI
9 September 2006
8:42
not commonly used in discussions of hominid
brain evolution.
The incorrect assumption that absolute
brain size is not particularly behaviorally relevant may stem from the fact that, because
brain size scales with body size, it is assumed
growth of the two must be tightly constrained
together developmentally (e.g., Finlay et al.
2001). Such developmental constraints would
require that a larger-bodied species have a
larger brain, so simply comparing species in
absolute brain size would improperly conflate brain size with body-size differences.
The problem with this view is that brain/body
scaling does not, in fact, necessarily imply developmental constraint. Although the correlation between brain and body size is high
(typically r > 0.95), at least a tenfold range
of variation in absolute brain size exists at
a given body size in mammals (Finlay et al.
2001, Schoenemann 1997). Thus, any developmental constraint would not appear to be
strong. In addition, it is not often recognized
that brain/body scaling could be the result
of body size constraining brain size, rather
than the brain and body being tightly developmentally linked. Jerison’s (1973) explanation
for the brain/body relationship is that larger
bodies need larger brains both to control
greater muscle mass and to process greater
amounts of sensory information. Other explanations emphasize the limiting role of
metabolic resources in brain growth across
species (Armstrong 1983, Martin 1981). In
either case, absolutely larger brains might
always be adaptive if they can be paid for
metabolically (and hence ecologically). Because brains are metabolically expensive [i.e.,
having high metabolic rates per gram of tissue
(Aiello & Wheeler 1995, Hofman 1983b)],
brain sizes tend to vary at a given body size
as a function of the usefulness of brains for a
particular species niche. Larger animals generally have larger brains because they have
more metabolic resources available to put toward brain development and maintenance,
not because of developmental constraints
(Schoenemann 2004).
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
ANRV287-AN35-20
382
Schoenemann
Under this model, body size varies according to a variety of ecological constraints
and tends to put upper limits on possible
brain size, which in turn tend toward the
high end, particularly in social species (see below). The human condition is explained by behavioral adaptations that lead to a relaxation
of ecological constraints. This behavioralselection/body-size-limiting model of brainsize evolution is more consistent with the wide
range of brain sizes at a given body size in
mammals, and it also explicitly does not assume that EQ is the only behaviorally relevant
comparative measure. In addition, this model
is consistent with the finding that two genetic
loci known to be important to brain size development, ASPM and microcephalin, both
show signatures of strong selection specifically at the evolutionary divergence of the line
leading to pongids and hominids (away from
all other primates) even though pongids do
not show an increase in relative brain size over
other primates (as noted above) (see Evans
et al. 2004, Wang & Su 2004).
In summary, brain size does scale with body
size, and EQ differences do suggest something
about the relative importance brains have had
during a species’ evolutionary history, but they
should not be used uncritically as proxies of
species’ behavioral capacities. That brain size
scales with body size across mammals does not
constitute strong evidence of developmental
constraints tying the two together. Absolute
brain size itself appears to be behaviorally
relevant.
Patterns of Internal Brain Allometry
Larger brains have more neurons (Haug
1987), but for these neurons to remain equally
well connected to each other (in the sense
of a signal having the same average number of synapses to traverse to travel between
any two neurons), the number of connections
(axons) must increase much faster than the
number of neurons (Ringo 1991). Hofman’s
(1985) data show that white matter increases
faster than gray matter with increasing brain
size. (White matter contains longer-distance
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
ANRV287-AN35-20
ARI
9 September 2006
8:42
axonal connections between cortical areas,
whereas gray matter contains most neuronal
cell bodies and dendritic connections.) However, the increase is not fast enough to maintain equal degrees of connectivity between
neurons (Ringo 1991). As brain size increases,
therefore, there is a concomitant increase in
the separation between existing areas, leading to a strong correlation between brain
volume and the number of distinguishable
cortical areas across mammals (Changizi &
Shimojo 2005). These scaling relationships
predict ∼150 distinct cortical areas in humans. Although the human cortex is not yet
completely mapped, this predicted number is
broadly consistent with what is thought to be
the actual number (Van Essen et al. 1998).
HUMAN BRAINS IN
COMPARATIVE PERSPECTIVE
Understanding human brain evolution requires the assessment of exactly how human
brains differ with respect to the size of various
components from those of other living animals, particularly our closest evolutionary relatives: the pongids. Empirically mapping all
the possible differences across many species is
an extraordinarily time-consuming task, and
at present we are nowhere near a complete
understanding of all the differences that may
exist. Below I review the brain components
that have been studied in enough detail to
give at least preliminary assessments about
the relative status of human brains. The neuroanatomical variables studied to date are either relatively easy to measure or have been
thought to be particularly interesting behaviorally. Because conscious awareness is localized to areas of the cortex (the outermost
layer of gray matter of the cerebral hemispheres), much comparative research has focused on cortical subdivisions. In some areas, only the size of an entire cortical lobe
has been estimated in enough species to allow
any comparison; in other areas, studies exist of
more-localized (and presumably functionally
specific) areas.
Because humans differ from other species
on a number of interesting behavioral dimensions (e.g., communication, ability to harness
technology, problem solving, complexity of
social relationships; see below), and because
the neural processing underlying these is
often located in different brain regions,
there is no general agreement about what
components are most important to study a
priori. For this reason, this review covers
most of the components for which information is available. These include overall
brain size, olfactory bulb, cerebellum, visual
cortex, temporal lobe, and the overall frontal
cortex and its components (primary motor,
premotor, and prefrontal cortices). Because
of space limitations, noncortical areas other
than the cerebellum (e.g., deep cortical and
brainstem nuclei) are not discussed here.
Behavioral implications of anatomical differences are reviewed in the subsequent sections.
Our knowledge of anatomical differences is
further advanced than our knowledge of what
these differences might mean behaviorally. In
general, however, it is generally assumed, implicitly or explicitly, that more tissue translates
into greater sophistication in neural processing in some way, which in turn suggests increased complexity of the behaviors mediated
by that particular area (or areas). Actual direct
tests of this assumption are relatively rare,
however.
Brain Size
The most obvious evolutionary change during human evolution, as noted above, has been
an increase in both absolute and relative brain
size (Holloway 1995). Estimated brain sizes of
our closest living relatives, the pongids (largebodied apes), are as follows: common chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes), 337 (±16) cc; pygmy
chimpanzee (P. paniscus), 311 (±11) cc; gorilla
(Gorilla gorilla), 397 (±67) cc; and orangutan
(Pongo pygmaeus), 407 (±29) cc (Rilling &
Insel 1999). Modern human brain sizes vary
widely, but average ∼1330 cc (Dekaban 1978,
Garby et al. 1993, Ho et al. 1980a, Pakkenberg
www.annualreviews.org • Evolution of the Human Brain
383
ANRV287-AN35-20
ARI
9 September 2006
8:42
& Voigt 1964).2 Modern human brains are
3.1 times larger than predicted on the basis
of primate brain/body-size allometric scaling
(Schoenemann 1997).
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
Olfactory bulb. The olfactory bulb is the
first major processing area for the sense of
smell. Stephan et al.’s (1981) data show that
the olfactory bulb is only ∼30% as large
as predicted for primate brains of our size
(Schoenemann 1997). Because overall brain
size is approximately three times larger in
modern humans, this suggests the olfactory
bulb has lagged behind overall brain-size evolution. This finding is consistent with the
belief that olfaction is relatively poor in humans, comparatively. However, the human olfactory bulb is ∼1.6 times larger than expected
for a primate of our body size (Schoenemann
1997). Exactly how one should interpret differences relative to body size versus differences relative to brain size is a major unresolved issue. It would appear that olfaction is
not unimportant.
Cerebellum. The cerebellum plays a key
role in modulating patterns of muscle movements and appears to play a role in timing
generally. It may be involved in aspects of
language processing as well (Gazzaniga et al.
1998). The human cerebellum is ∼2.9 times as
large as expected for a primate of our body size
(calculated from data in Stephan et al. 1981)
and as such has increased only slightly more
slowly than the brain as a whole. MacLeod
et al. (2003) have shown that there is a grade
shift in hominoids with respect to the size of
the cerebellar hemispheres, with hominoids
as a group (humans included) showing greatly
enlarged cerebella compared with monkeys.
The cerebellum’s participation in language
presumably explains why it has not lagged behind as has the olfactory bulb, for example.
2
Original data in grams was converted to estimated cc using
the formula (brain volume in cc) = (brain mass in grams)/
1.036. European-derived samples only.
384
Schoenemann
Visual cortex. The visual cortex is so named
because it is the site of the initial conscious
processing of visual information. It is located
in the occipital lobe, which is the most posterior portion of the cortex. The human primary visual cortex (the initial cortical area
devoted to processing visual information) is
only ∼60% the size it should be for a primate brain that size (Holloway 1992), but it
is ∼1.5 times larger in absolute terms than
it is in chimpanzees (Stephan et al. 1981).
The human primary visual cortex is 5% larger
than expected given a primate of our body size
(Deacon 1997, Schoenemann 1997). It is not
clear whether this says anything about relative
visual processing abilities in humans. However, given that the human primary visual cortex is smaller in relative terms, but larger in
absolute terms, it could be used to test which
is more behaviorally relevant: If humans have
behavioral advantages over apes in the visual
domain (specifically in those known to be mediated by the primary visual cortex), it would
suggest that absolute amounts of neural tissue would be more important than relative
amounts, at least for visual processing. Such a
study has not been done.
Preuss & Coleman (2002) have documented a variety of changes of the neurons in
the primary visual cortex in humans, particularly a population of interneuronal connections in layer 4a that appears to have expanded
significantly in human evolution. The exact
behavioral significance is not known, but these
changes emphasize that human-brain evolution involved more than simple changes in size
(relative or absolute) of brain regions.
Temporal lobe. The temporal lobe plays a
critical role in auditory information, as well as
memory (through the hippocampal formation
and associated areas), emotion (amygdala),
and conceptual understanding (Carpenter &
Sutin 1983). As a result, it also plays an important role in language processing. Rilling &
Seligman (2002) report that humans have significantly larger overall volumes, white matter
volumes, and surface areas of their temporal
ANRV287-AN35-20
ARI
9 September 2006
8:42
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
lobes than predicted on the basis of ape scaling relationships. This suggests an elaboration
in humans of the behaviors mediated in this
lobe.
Frontal lobe. The frontal lobe consists of all
cortical areas anterior to the central sulcus
(which angles inferoanteriorly to superoposteriorly along the midlateral convexity of the
cortex on both hemispheres).3 It contains a
number of different functional areas, including the primary motor area (also known as
Brodmann’s area 4 to neuroanatomists, located immediately anterior and adjacent to the
central sulcus), which directly controls conscious muscle movements; the premotor area
(known as Brodmann’s area 6, located immediately anterior to the primary motor area),
which plans complex muscle-movement sequences; and the prefrontal cortex (everything
anterior to the premotor area), which mediates a number of higher cortical functions
important for planning, language, and social
interactions, as well as having a general executive oversight of other brain regions (see below). A number of studies have quantified the
entire frontal cortex (which is relatively easy
to delineate across species), without subdividing it into its functional subdivisions. Overall,
the frontal lobe in humans appears to be as
large as expected, given a primate brain of our
size (Bush & Allman 2004, Semendeferi et al.
2002, von Bonin 1963). Semendeferi et al.
(2002) report that human frontal cortex averaged 37.7 (±0.9)% of the entire brain, compared with 35.4 (±1.9)% for common chimpanzees, 34.7 (±0.6)% for bonobo, 36.0% for
gorilla, and 37.6 (±1.1)% for orangutan.
However, recent data suggest a difference
with respect to gray matter/white matter proportions of the frontal lobe, which suggest potentially important behavioral implications.
Schenker et al. (2005) report that humans have
3
The surface of the cortex is not smooth but is folded back
and forth upon itself, resulting in patterns of indentations
referred to as sulci (singular, sulcus) separated by ridges
referred to as gyri (singular, gyrus).
significantly more white matter volume in areas close to the cortical surface than hominoid (pongids plus the smaller-bodied apes
such as gibbons and siamangs) data predict.
Calculating from their data (Schenker et al.
2005), human frontal cortical gray matter is
3.6 times larger than the average for their
pongid sample, but human frontal gyral white
matter is 4.7 times larger. This suggests a bias
toward white matter expansion in humans, although the extent to which this is explained
statistically by allometric-frontal/nonfrontal
(or some other) scaling cannot be determined (e.g., total brain sizes are not reported). Because allometric explanations have
neither straightforward behavioral implications nor developmental-constraint implications, as discussed above, it is not clear what
an allometric explanation would mean in any
case. However, it may reflect increased functional distinctions of areas within the frontal
lobe. In any case, the frontal lobe is, at a minimum, more than three times larger than it is
in pongids, and this likely has important behavioral implications of some kind. To understand what these might be, it is useful to look
at specific subdivisions of the frontal lobe.
Primary motor and premotor areas. Although the sample sizes are small (N = 7), the
primary motor area in humans appears to be
only ∼33% as large as predicted for a primate
brain our size (Blinkov & Glezer 1968). This
suggests our primary motor cortex has scaled
approximately with absolute body size during
human evolution (Deacon 1997). Given that
this area mediates the direct conscious control
of muscle movements, and given that humans
do not seem particularly gifted or particularly
poor comparatively with respect to muscle
control, this approximate scaling with body
size (but not brain size) argues against the importance of relative amounts of neural tissue
for behavioral ability, at least for this area.
The human premotor area, just anterior to
the primary motor area, also appears smaller
than predicted given absolute brain size, although not to the same extent. The premotor
www.annualreviews.org • Evolution of the Human Brain
385
ANRV287-AN35-20
ARI
9 September 2006
8:42
area is ∼60% as large as predicted for a
primate brain our size (Blinkov & Glezer
1968).4 This suggests the premotor area has
not lagged as far behind as the primary motor
area as brain size increased. Taken together,
these two findings suggest an elaboration of
motor planning but not an increase in motor
control per se. No direct tests of these suggestions have been reported, however.
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
Prefrontal. If the entire frontal lobe is approximately as large as expected given overall human brain size, yet the two portions
of the frontal lobe reviewed above (the primary motor and premotor areas) are significantly smaller, then the rest of the frontal
lobe (i.e., the prefrontal) must necessarily be
larger than expected (Preuss 2000). There
is nevertheless currently some controversy
over this point. Brodmann’s original cytoarchitectural studies, which form the basis for
much of our knowledge of cortical areas,
strongly point to significantly larger prefrontal cortices in humans compared with
other primates (Brodmann 1909). Allometrically, Brodmann’s data suggest the human
prefrontal is ∼2 times larger than predicted
on the basis of the size of the rest of the
brain (Deacon 1997). The suggestion of biased expansion is also supported by research
quantifying the degree of folding in different
parts of the cortex: Humans appear to have
substantially more convoluted (and hence, a
greater volume of) cortex in prefrontal regions (Armstrong et al. 1991, Rilling & Insel
1999).
Studies using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) to quantify prefrontal cortex also
support this contention. Because the posterior boundary of the prefrontal cortex does
4
Semendeferi et al. (2002) report that human precentral
gyrus volumes (expressed as percent of total cortex; i.e.,
not allometrically) fall within the range of their hominoid
sample. However, because the precentral gyrus does not
contain all of the motor cortex, and only contains a small
portion of the premotor cortex, it is not clear exactly what
this suggests about either motor or premotor cortex size in
humans.
386
Schoenemann
not follow obvious sulcal/gyral gross morphological features, it is not possible to exactly
delineate it using structural MRI. However,
a proxy for prefrontal cortex volume can be
used: cortical volume anterior to the corpus
callosum (the major tract of white matter connecting the two hemispheres). This proxy is
commonly applied to both human (e.g., Raz
et al. 2005, Sax et al. 1999) and nonhuman primate (Lyons et al. 2002; variant in McBride
et al. 1999) studies. Estimated in this way, human prefrontal cortex was shown to be significantly larger than in pongids (Schoenemann
et al. 2005b). Specifically, human values averaged 12.7% of total brain volume, compared with an average of 10.3% for the four
pongid species. Nonprefrontal cerebral volume in humans averaged 3.7 times larger than
the average of P. paniscus and P. troglodytes,
but the prefrontal portion averaged 4.9 times
larger.
Assessing both Semendeferi et al.’s (2002)
data on the total frontal lobe and our own
data, it appears that if the analysis is restricted
to increasingly anterior regions of the frontal
(keeping in mind that the prefrontal occupies the most-anterior portions of the frontal
lobe), humans appear increasingly disproportionate. With respect to allometric scaling,
total human frontal cortex is slightly smaller
than predicted (Semendeferi et al. 2002), but
the prefrontal (using our proxy) is slightly
larger than predicted (Schoenemann et al.
2005b). Given that the prefrontal proxy used
appears to underestimate human values much
more so than other primates (Schoenemann
et al. 2005a), this strongly suggests that human
prefrontal is in fact larger, both as a percentage
of total brain volume, as well as allometrically.
Our data do not allow a clear confirmation of
whether human prefrontal cortex is actually
twice the predicted size, however.
In addition, the human difference appeared biased toward white matter rather
than gray matter (Schoenemann et al. 2005b).
Comparing humans with chimpanzees, prefrontal gray volumes averaged 4.8 times
larger in humans, whereas nonprefrontal gray
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
ANRV287-AN35-20
ARI
9 September 2006
8:42
volumes averaged 4.2 times larger. By contrast, prefrontal white volumes averaged 5.0
times larger in humans, whereas nonprefrontal white volumes averaged only 3.3
times larger. Furthermore, the human average
value was significantly allometrically larger
as well, although not to the extent found in
Brodmann’s original data. Sherwood et al.
(2005) point out that prefrontal white volume
is predicted by prefrontal gray volume in our
dataset, suggesting that the difference appears
to be in proportions of prefrontal versus nonprefrontal. Given that a critically important
role of the prefrontal is to moderate activity
in posterior cortical areas, this apparent shift
in proportions likely has important behavioral
implications, as discussed below.
Additional support for the biased expansion of the prefrontal comes from work morphing primate brains into human brains.
Deformation maps describing the necessary
transformations allow for detailed, global
assessments of morphological differences.
Comparisons between humans and bonobo
(P. paniscus) (Zilles 2005), common chimpanzees (P. troglodytes) (Avants et al. 2005),
and macaque monkeys (Van Essen 2005) have
been reported. In all three cases, substantial
increases in the prefrontal region were reported. Our own group found that the average common chimp–human difference was
approximately twofold for some prefrontal areas (Avants et al. 2005). To date, these studies involve comparisons between only two
species, so scaling trends across primates cannot be estimated. However, it should be possible to extend these analyses to multiple species
comparisons, resulting in separate allometricscaling estimates—and extent of human divergence, if any—for individual areas at high
resolution. Furthermore, the morphing algorithms can be applied to collections of cellstained brain sections, thereby combining the
resolution of detailed cytoarchitectural analyses (studies of patterns of neurons in the cortex) with the ability of morphing algorithms
to quantify changes in shape.
Within the prefrontal itself, studies suggest a mosaic of evolutionary changes. A cytoarchitectural study of Brodmann’s area 13,
a subdivision of the prefrontal that mediates aspects of social behavior (particularly
emotional dimensions), suggests that this area
lagged behind the expansion of the brain as a
whole: It is only 1.5 times larger than the average pongid value (Semendeferi et al. 1998).
By contrast, another subdivision of the prefrontal, Brodmann’s area 10, which is known
to mediate tasks involving planning and organization of thought and future behavior
(Carpenter & Sutin 1983), is 6.6 times larger
in humans than in pongids (Semendeferi et al.
2001). Holloway (2002) notes this is actually only slightly more than one would predict given how area 10 scales with brain size.
This is because the relationship is strongly
positively allometric (i.e., as brain size increases, area 10 seems to increase much
faster).
How should we interpret these findings
in subareas of the prefrontal? First, as discussed above, even if allometry statistically
predicts some increase in humans, this does
not license us to conclude that the increase
is behaviorally irrelevant (Schoenemann et al.
2005a). Nor does the existence of allometric
scaling constitute a demonstration of the existence of inherent developmental constraints.
Larger brains may have larger prefrontal cortices (or subregions therein) because selection places greater demands on the oversight
role of prefrontal areas as posterior regions
become more complex. As discussed above,
absolute amounts of cortical tissue have been
shown to be correlated with a variety of behavioral dimensions. Thus, even though area
13 is relatively smaller than one would predict, it might nevertheless indicate an important behavioral change, particularly given
the enhanced interactive sociality that has increasingly characterized the human condition
(see below). Increases in area 10 almost surely
suggest increased importance in the various
dimensions of behavioral planning.
www.annualreviews.org • Evolution of the Human Brain
387
ANRV287-AN35-20
ARI
9 September 2006
8:42
FOSSIL EVIDENCE OF HUMAN
BRAIN EVOLUTION
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
What is known about the evolutionary history of these apparent differences in brain
anatomy? The goal of research in this area
is to determine exactly what can be inferred
about the behavior of fossil hominids from
imprints on the inside surface of their braincases. This is of central importance to understanding human brain evolution. Because
of this intrinsic interest in the behavior of
fossil hominids, a great deal of effort has
been spent trying to extract maximal inference
out of minimal data. Because brains do not
fossilize, the richness of comparative data is
always greater than the fossil data. The following subsections highlight these limitations
and serve to illustrate the inherent difficulty of
the task. At present, the fossil record is clearest for overall brain size as indexed by cranial
capacity, although other suggestive clues have
been found regarding possible early changes
in different brain regions related to visual processing and language.
Cranial Capacity
Cranial capacity is by far the most wellattested change in human brain evolution
in the fossil record. Figure 2 plots cranial
capacity estimates against estimated specimen age for published hominid fossils dating from 15 KYA to 4 MYA, a total
of 145 individual specimens. It is apparent
from Figure 2 that changes in hominid brain
size began sometime between 3 and 2 Mya.
Although it has been argued that brain
size has undergone punctuational (i.e., not
continuous and gradual) change at various points during hominid evolution (e.g.,
Hofman 1983a), Figure 2 suggests the range
of variation at any particular point in time
(best exemplified in modern species) combined with the sparse sampling evident for
particular time periods of our evolutionary
history (e.g., between 1.0 and 0.5 Mya) indicate that it may be premature to assess
388
Schoenemann
the likelihood of punctuational events. Recent empirical analyses of cranial capacity
changes over time support this notion (De
Miguel & Henneberg 2001, Lee & Wolpoff
2003). Furthermore, it is not clear how important species designations (which are inherently problematic for fossils) are for understanding brain-size evolution. De Miguel &
Henneberg (2001) showed that 90% of the
variation in fossil hominid brain size can be
explained simply by the age of fossils, leaving
only 10% to be explained by species differences and/or measurement error. This suggests that most of the brain size increase during hominid evolution was not closely tied to
speciation events.
Lunate Sulcus
The portion of the cortex that has received
perhaps the greatest amount of attention in
fossil specimens is the visual cortex. A sulcus known as the lunate marks the anterior
boundary of the primary visual cortex in nonhuman primates, although it is often missing in humans (Allen et al. 2005a, Connolly
1950). In relative terms, an anterior position
of the lunate (and therefore anterior extent of
the primary visual cortex) is characteristic of
the general pongid condition, whereas a posterior placement is characteristic of modern
humans. As discussed above, the primary visual cortex is a relatively smaller portion of
the human brain in comparison with chimpanzee brains, even though in absolute terms
it is ∼1.5 times larger than that of either chimpanzees or gorillas (Stephan et al. 1981).
The evolutionary history of this change has
been the source of considerable controversy
over the years: Did it occur early in hominid
evolution (e.g., in early Australopithecines),
thereby signaling some form of early reapportionment (Holloway prefers the term reorganization) of neural resources? Or was it simply
the result of the primary visual cortex lagging
behind as brain size increased during human
evolution? Holloway and Falk have, over the
years, disagreed on the possible positioning of
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
ANRV287-AN35-20
ARI
9 September 2006
8:42
the lunate sulcus on brain endocasts attributed
to both Australopithecus africanus (Taung) and
A. afarensis (AL 162–28, 3.2 Mya) (e.g., Falk
1987, Holloway 1995). Holloway believes the
lunate either cannot be seen (Taung) or is
likely in a modern human posterior position
(AL 162–28) in early Australopithecines, long
before substantial changes in absolute brain
size occurred. Falk and others have argued
that it likely occurred later, as a result of brain
size increases in other cortical regions. Debates over the position of the lunate have
not been resolved, in part because it is difficult to assess its location unequivocally on
endocast specimens and because of disagreements about the proper orientation of the AL
162–28 specimen. Holloway and colleagues
(Holloway et al. 2004b) have recently reported on another A. africanus specimen,
STW 505, which they show appears to have a
relatively posterior lunate.
In addition, Holloway et al. (2003) discuss the brains of two apparently normal
chimpanzees that nevertheless have lunates
in human-like posterior locations. They note
this finding demonstrates the possibility of a
posterior lunate in the absence of increased
brain size, which means Holloway’s long-held
contention of a posterior shift in early smallbrained hominids is certainly possible. However, these specimens also complicate behavioral interpretations of the apparent change.
Holloway et al. (2003) argue that the reduction of the primary visual cortex has no behavioral implications. However, if a chimpanzee
can have a human lunate pattern (presumably
indicating a reduced primary visual cortex),
this pattern is not a clear indicator of humanlike behavior, and therefore the location of
the lunate in fossil endocasts may not be behaviorally interpretable, rendering the lunate
sulcus debate moot.
However, Holloway and colleagues
(Holloway et al. 2004b, p. 6) argue that a
posterior lunate in Australopithecines “indicates an expanded posterior parietal cerebral
cortex [anterior to the primary visual cortex],
and was most likely associated with enhanced
social behavior including communication.”
Thus cortical expansion anterior to the lunate
is argued to have behavioral implications, but
reduction posterior to the lunate is argued to
be behaviorally meaningless. If the size of the
primary visual cortex really is irrelevant, why
do humans have 1.5 times as much primary
visual cortex, given the evolutionary costs of
brain tissue (see below)? If Holloway and colleagues are right about the location of the lunate in AL 162–28 and STW 505 (which they
seem to be), it is either behaviorally meaningless, or Australopithecines had reduced visual
processing capabilities of some kind. Both
these alternatives are problematic. Thus, at
this point it is not clear how to interpret the
position of the lunate in fossil specimens.
Broca’s Area
Evidence relevant to the origin of language is
intrinsically of great interest. There is some
suggestion of the elaboration of Broca’s area
(a key cortical region that plays a central role
in language processing, located in the left prefrontal portion of the inferior frontal lobe) in
fossil hominids. Holloway (1983) noted that a
Broca’s cap (an endocranial bump over what
would be Broca’s area) is present—although
inconsistently—in pongids. Thus, the presence of a Broca’s cap does not definitively indicate the existence of language, unfortunately.
Tobias (1975) and Holloway (1983) argue
that Broca’s cap becomes increasingly present
in early Homo, however (see also Broadfield
et al. 2001). Tobias (1983) argues this indicates
early Homo had language. Both Falk (1983)
and Holloway (1995) agree that the endocast
of the early Homo specimen KNM-ER 1470
(1.8 Mya) looks more modern-human-like in
the inferior frontal region. Although this does
not prove that KNM-ER 1470 had a Broca’s
area or that it had language, it nevertheless
suggests something important is occurring in
this region. Broadfield et al. (2001) report that
the specimen Sambungmacan 3 has asymmetrical Broca’s caps, which they speculate may
indicate a level of language ability beyond
www.annualreviews.org • Evolution of the Human Brain
389
ANRV287-AN35-20
ARI
9 September 2006
Petalias: areas
where the brain
extends farther in
some direction for
one hemisphere over
another
8:42
that found in earlier hominids. This specimen is assumed to be Middle Pleistocene
Homo erectus, although it is unfortunately
not well provenienced (Marquez et al. 2001).
Thus, suggestive (but equivocal) evidence exists of possible language-related changes in
the brains of early Homo, dating back to
almost 2 Mya.
Asymmetry
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
Because important aspects of behavior are
asymmetrically organized (e.g., key components of language are usually processed in
the left hemisphere, which is usually also the
dominant hemisphere for hand movements),
it is of interest to determine whether cortical
asymmetries can be found that predict these
behavioral asymmetries, thereby possibly
allowing us to infer behavior from fossil specimens. Research to date has centered on particular anatomical asymmetries known as petalias. Holloway & de la Coste-Lareymondie
(1982) report that only modern and fossil hominids (including australopithecines, H. erectus, and Neanderthals) show a consistent,
distinct right-frontal and left-occipital petalial pattern. Pongids also show petalias, but
they did not display the same degree of consistency, particularly in the combination of
right-frontal and left-occipital petalias. Only
25% of pongids assessed displayed this pattern, compared with 82% of hominids. Falk
et al. (1990) demonstrated that Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) show a significant tendency toward right-frontal petalias (although
not to the same consistency as found in modern and fossil hominids) but not for leftoccipital petalias. Thus it appears that petalias in general are common in anthropoids,
but the particular pattern of petalias and
the degree of consistency appear unique to
hominids.
Holloway & de la Coste-Lareymondie
(1982) speculate this right-frontal/leftoccipital petalial pattern may be related to
right-handedness, language-related symbol
manipulation, and spatio-visual integration.
390
Schoenemann
However, because the hominid pattern
occurs in a fair number of pongid specimens,
the functional significance of this finding is
obviously not clear. It cannot be considered
definitive evidence of any particular behavior
in individual specimens.
Summary of the Fossil Evidence
It is clear that brain evolution started in
earnest sometime between 2 and 3 Mya. Although punctuated models of brain size increase can be fit to the data, there is no compelling reason to assume anything other than
a reasonably constant trend toward increasing brain size over time. Apart from cranial
capacity, only suggestive, equivocal clues of
possible behavioral patterns are evident in
the fossil record of hominid brain evolution,
mostly relating to the question of language
evolution. Although definitive statements are
not currently warranted, we do not presently
know the limits of possible inferences about
the behavior of fossil hominids from their endocranial remains. The intrinsic interest in
reconstructing hominid behavior ensures that
every possible avenue of inference will be explored in the future. It is certainly possible that
additional associations between cranial form
and brain anatomy (and ultimately behavior)
will eventually be uncovered using modern
morphometric methods.
EVOLUTION OF BRAIN AND
BEHAVIOR
Explaining the anatomical changes in the human brain reviewed in the sections above remains a central question in human evolutionary studies. Exactly why these changes
happened, and what they might mean behaviorally, is a question of fundamental importance. As with any evolutionary change, there
are two basic kinds of explanation: adaptive
change (the result of selection) and nonadaptive change (the result of genetic drift). Adaptive explanations are probably the strongest
for questions of brain evolution as compared
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
ANRV287-AN35-20
ARI
9 September 2006
8:42
with almost any other biological characteristic. This is partly because of the pattern of
changes and partly because of the apparent
evolutionary costs involved in the changes,
both of which strongly argue for adaptive
explanations.
Although genetic drift can lead to the
fixation of particular alleles at a single locus by chance, the odds that an evolutionary succession of alleles at a variety of loci
(e.g., see Gilbert et al. 2005) leads progressively in a particular direction (e.g., larger
brain size) is extraordinarily unlikely. In addition, these changes have occurred in the
face of apparently strong evolutionary costs
(Smith 1990). Costs in this context refer to
correlated effects that—everything else being equal—ultimately translate into decreased
numbers of offspring produced per unit time.
These costs have to be paid every generation.
First, as noted above, the brain is among the
most metabolically expensive organs in the
body [i.e., neural tissue has among the highest
metabolic rates per gram (Aiello & Wheeler
1995, Hofman 1983b)]. Second, increasing
brain size in primates is strongly correlated
with longer gestation periods, an increased
period of infant dependency, and delayed reproduction (Harvey & Clutton-Brock 1985),
which all decrease the number of offspring an
individual can produce per unit time. Third,
there is an apparent trade-off in bipedal hominids between locomotor efficiency and ease
of childbirth (Lovejoy 1975). Fourth, there
is potentially a problem of cooling a larger
brain (Falk 1990). If there were no counterbalancing advantages to larger amounts of brain
tissue, individuals with smaller brains would
necessarily have a selective advantage.
For adaptive evolutionary change to occur, reproductive benefits must have consistently (or at least on average) accrued
to individuals—within successive populations connecting ancestral and descendent
populations—who varied anatomically from
the average of their populations. Of course,
brain size may not have uniformly and gradually increased in every successive popula-
tion. If so, there would have been some ancestral populations in which individuals with
larger-than-average brain sizes did not have
reproductive advantages. [This might explain
the possible decrease in brain size from Neanderthal to modern humans (Schoenemann
2004).] However, on average over our evolutionary history, reproductive benefits needed
to have been at least marginally greater for
larger-brained individuals.
Brain Evolution Through Behavioral
Selection
As the various mental faculties gradually
developed themselves the brain would almost
certainly become larger. No one, I presume,
doubts that the large proportion which the size of
man’s brain bears to his body, compared to the
same proportion in the gorilla or orang, is closely
connected with his higher mental powers.
Darwin 1871, p. 145
Given that larger brain sizes must have
provided individuals with reproductive advantages during significant portions of our
evolutionary history, it is reasonable to ask
both (a) what exactly the advantages were,
and (b) whether these advantages still exist
within modern humans (and/or other living
primates). Although Gould (1981) suggested
that variation in brain size is behaviorally
meaningless in modern populations, it is not
at all clear why we should expect brain size
above some threshold to have absolutely no
advantage. The simplest a priori model would
posit a behavioral benefit (or usefulness), on
average, to larger brains that would be present
regardless of whether the particular benefit
translated into reproductive advantages in any
given environment. The behavioral benefit
might be, for example, greater memory ability,
planning ability, or linguistic ability. Whether
such benefits were evolutionarily meaningful depends on the degree to which they
helped pay (reproductively) for the evolutionary costs associated with larger brains. This,
in turn, depends on the specific ecological
and social conditions characteristic of a given
www.annualreviews.org • Evolution of the Human Brain
391
ANRV287-AN35-20
ARI
9 September 2006
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
Genetic
correlation: occurs
when changes in the
genetic effects on
one trait have the
effect of changing
the other trait
Heritability: the
proportion of
phenotypic
(observable) variance
of a characteristic in
a population that is
explained by genetic
variance in that
population
8:42
population. An increasingly powerful linguistic processor is likely not worth the costs for a
species that is not highly socially interactive,
for example. More complex models regarding costs and benefits of increasing brain size
are of course possible (e.g., wildly nonlinear
relationships between behavioral abilities and
brain size). However, parsimony requires investigating simpler explanations first.
For selection on some behavioral ability to cause evolutionary changes in brain
anatomy, there must necessarily have been
a genetic correlation between them. Failing
this, selection on behavior would have no evolutionary effect on brain anatomy, and we
would lack an explanation for evolutionary
changes in the brain. Attempts to assess such
brain/behavior correlations are therefore central for any model that posits the evolutionary importance of behavior in human brain
evolution.
It is also not generally appreciated, however, that the genetic correlation between behavior and brain size does not actually have to
be large to explain human brain-size evolution
(Schoenemann et al. 2000). This is partly because the rate of change in brain size per generation is only ∼8 mm3 (assuming a 1000-cc
increase in brain size over 2.5 million years
and a 20-year average generation length) (see
Figure 2), or approximately 0.0002 standard
deviations of chimpanzee-brain size (see also
Holloway et al. 2004a). The size of the genetic correlation needed to account for this
amount of change per generation is partly a
function of the strength of selection operating on the hypothesized correlated (directly
selected) variable (e.g., some behavioral ability), as well as of the extent to which both brain
size and the selected variable are genetically
influenced, or heritable (Falconer 1981).5 If
5
The strength of selection and the size of the genetic correlation interact to determine the likely amount of change per
generation. Assuming reasonable heritabilities, the same
amount of change can occur if either selection is strong
and the genetic correlation is weak, or if selection is weak
but the genetic correlation is strong.
392
Schoenemann
we posit a small genetic correlation of only
r = 0.05 and heritabilities (i.e., the proportion of phenotypic variation in a population
explained by genetic variation) of 0.3, the selected individuals (i.e., those who contribute
to the subsequent generation) only have to
differ from their overall population average
by less than 0.006 standard deviations on the
hypothesized directly selected characteristic
(Schoenemann et al. 2000). This is so small
that it would be extremely difficult to demonstrate empirically. Although the strength of
selection could have varied over time during our evolutionary history, these estimates
clearly demonstrate that we do not need to
hypothesize more than a weak genetic correlation to explain the dramatic change in brain
size via selection on correlated behavioral
characteristics.
This does not mean, however, that the
genetic correlation must therefore be small
and that there is no justification for assessing
genetic correlations within humans without
massive sample sizes. It simply shows that it
need not be large. Because of the evolutionary
costs described above, it is highly unlikely that
the correlation was in fact zero (or negative),
as that would require some unknown nonbehavioral explanation for increasing brain
size.
Evidence of Genetic Correlations
Between Brain and Behavior
There is substantial evidence that many features of brain anatomy are influenced by genetic factors. In a number of studies, researchers have reported heritability of brain
size to be significant, with estimates ranging from 0.66 to 0.94 (reviewed in Winterer
& Goldman 2003). Heritability estimates of
the patterns of sulci and gyri on the cortical
surface are generally significantly lower, although this may be an artifact of the difficulty
of quantifying them (Winterer & Goldman
2003). Thompson et al. (2001) estimated independent heritabilities for each voxel (the
smallest unit of volume in an MRI image) of
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
ANRV287-AN35-20
ARI
9 September 2006
8:42
cortical gray matter in a sample of monozygotic and dizygotic twins and found evidence
for widespread genetic influence. Volumes
of the left and right frontal lobes have estimated heritabilities of between 0.52 and 0.66
(Geschwind et al. 2002), and a number of
Brodmann areas (including some prefrontal
regions) appear to have at least moderate
heritabilities (Wright et al. 2002). Thus,
brain anatomy appears to be under genetic
influence.
Similarly, cognitive abilities have been
shown to have genetic influence. Although intelligence is a controversial concept in some
areas of the social sciences, the consensus
among those who research individual differences in cognitive ability is that genetic
influences on general cognitive ability (g)6
are substantial, although environmental influences are also clearly evident (Neisser et al.
1996). Genetic influences have also been
demonstrated for other cognitive domains,
such as verbal and spatial factors, although the
degree to which these genetic factors are independent of g is not always clear (Plomin et al.
1997).
Although there is evidence of genetic influences on both brain and behavior, to what
extent are these influences genetically correlated? A large number of studies reported
finding phenotypic (not genetic) correlations
between brain size and behavioral ability (usually g, or other IQ-related abilities). Obviously, our ancestors were not selected to do
well on modern IQ tests, but some of the
abilities tapped by these tests possibly were
selected for. Studies using MRI to estimate
actual brain size report correlations averaging
r = ∼0.45 (Rushton & Ankney 1996).
However, these are phenotypic correlations, not genetic correlations, and are therefore not necessarily evolutionarily relevant.
Phenotypic correlations could be caused by a
6
Performance on a wide variety of cognitive tests is correlated. g is a measure that statistically explains these correlations and as such is typically interpreted as a measure of
general cognitive ability. IQ tests are good measures of g.
third variable affecting both the brain and behavior (or other hypothesized evolutionarily
relevant correlate) in similar directions. Socioeconomic status, for example, is correlated
with both IQ scores (Herrnstein & Murray
1994) and growth of the body (Bogin 1999),
and one might therefore predict a spurious
correlation between brain size and IQ on this
basis. In addition, cross-assortative mating
can result in phenotypic correlations between
characteristics that actually have completely
independent genetic influences ( Jensen &
Sinha 1990). If the correlation between brain
size and some behavior is spurious, selection
on that behavior cannot explain evolutionary
changes in brain size. It is therefore critically
important to assess the strength of the genetic
correlation, not just the phenotypic correlation, between the brain and behavior.
One way to control for possible confounds is by assessing the strength of the
association between brain size and behavior among siblings within families. Family
members share essentially the same socioeconomic status (this can be directly assessed),
and meiosis ensures siblings contain random
associations of alleles at different loci, thereby
eliminating cross-assortative mating effects.
To date, only a few studies have used this
methodology. Two within-family studies using head circumference as a proxy for brain
size have reported equivocal results ( Jensen
1994, Jensen & Johnson 1994; see discussion in Schoenemann et al. 2000). Only a few
MRI studies have been reported to date. Our
own group found essentially no correlation
(r = −0.05, NS) within families between
brain size and an estimate of g in 36 pairs of
adult sisters (Schoenemann et al. 2000). Another study of male-sibling pairs reported a
nonsignificant but positive within-family correlation of r = 0.23 between brain size and
g (Gignac et al. 2003). In addition, a recent
study using a genetically informative cohort
of 24 monozygotic pairs, 31 dizygotic pairs,
and 25 additional nontwin siblings estimated
a genetic correlation between g and both gray
and white matter volumes of r = 0.29 and
www.annualreviews.org • Evolution of the Human Brain
393
ARI
9 September 2006
8:42
r = 0.24, respectively (both p < 0.05)
(Posthuma et al. 2002). The genetic correlation for overall brain size was not reported,
unfortunately, but was presumably similar
(given that gray + white = total brain).
One unresolved issue is the direction of
causality. It has long been known that environmental influences can affect brain volume,
specifically cortical gray matter volume in rats
(Diamond 1988). This means that genes with
higher g could be causing individuals to experience more-stimulating and complex environmental situations, which may in turn
cause developmental (not genetic) increases
in their brain sizes and cortical gray volumes
(Posthuma et al. 2003). If this were the sole
reason for the brain size/g correlation, then
selection for g is not likely the explanation for
our increased brain sizes during our evolutionary history because it implies brain-size
variation is solely a developmental response.
However, some of the brain size/g correlation
is likely explained through g influencing brain
size, which means that the evolutionarily critical association—in which brain size causes
(or allows for) changes in g—is likely smaller
than the r = 0.24 to r = 0.29 genetic correlations suggested by Posthuma et al.’s (2002)
study.
Taken together, these studies suggest the
genetic correlation between brain size and g
is not zero but is not as large as the phenotypic
correlations typically reported for MRI studies of brain volume and g mentioned above.
The proportion of genetic variance in g explained by genetic variance in brain volume
appears substantially less than 8.5%. This is
still large enough to explain the evolution of
brain size in hominids, however.
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
ANRV287-AN35-20
Other behavioral measures. Although
most research has focused on correlations
with behaviors associated with general cognitive ability, natural selection may have acted
on other unrelated behavioral dimensions as
well as (or instead of ) general cognitive ability. Associations between neuroanatomical
variation and other behavioral dimensions
394
Schoenemann
that are conceivably evolutionarily relevant
have also been found, including spatial ability,
working memory, and the ability to extract
relevant information from a distracting
environment.
Spatial ability has been the focus of research partly because it was proposed to help
explain sex differences in brain size, which average ∼1 standard deviation (Ho et al. 1980b).
Some but not all of this difference is explained
by body weight (Ankney 1992, Falk et al.
1999) and/or fat-free weight (Schoenemann
2004). Because males and females differ on
average in spatial ability, in Western populations at least (Halpern 1987), if spatial ability
correlates strongly with brain size, the residual sex difference in brain size might therefore
be explained. However, many studies, including the more recent genetically informative
MRI-based ones, have failed to find a significant association between brain size and spatial
abilities (Posthuma et al. 2003, Schoenemann
et al. 2000, Wickett et al. 2000).
Spatial-ability differences still may explain differences in the shape of the corpus callosum, which in women appears to be
larger in the posterior region (e.g., Davatzikos
& Resnick 1998, de Lacoste-Utamsing &
Holloway 1982). This portion of the corpus
callosum, known as the splenium, connects
areas of the parietal lobes known to mediate spatial tasks. Thus, Holloway and colleagues (Holloway et al. 1993) have suggested
the anatomical difference in corpus callosum
morphology may therefore be explained by
sex differences in spatial abilities. Consistent
with this suggestion, our own group has recently found that women with smaller (more
male-like) splenia score better on a test of
mental rotation–spatial ability (P.T. Schoenemann, A. Dubb, J. Hu, J. Lewis & J. Gee, unpublished manuscript).
Working-memory abilities (i.e., the ability to manipulate information in short-term
memory to solve particular problems or goals)
appear to be associated with dimensions of
brain size (r = 0.40, p < 0.05 for gray matter; r = 0.33, p < 0.05 for white matter)
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
ANRV287-AN35-20
ARI
9 September 2006
8:42
(Posthuma et al. 2003). Because the prefrontal
cortex is known to be particularly important to
the mediation of working memory (GoldmanRakic 1996), Posthuma et al.’s finding is of
particular interest in light of the possible biased increase in the prefrontal cortex during
human evolution discussed above. Working
memory abilities have been shown to correlate with the length of the main prefrontal sulcus (principal sulcus) across a variety of Old
and New World monkeys, and this correlation completely explains the association between working-memory ability and cranial capacity across these species (Redmond 1999).
This raises the question of whether the association found by Posthuma et al. (2003) in
humans is actually more properly localized
to the prefrontal cortex (which unfortunately was not separately delineated in their
study).
The size of the prefrontal cortex in humans has been shown to correlate with the
Stroop task within families (Schoenemann
et al. 2000). This test measures the extent of
linguistic interference in naming colors, when
ink color and word name are mismatched
(e.g., the word red written in blue ink). It is
generally considered a test of the ability to extract (and focus on) the relevant information
from an environment and is known to be mediated by prefrontal areas.
The prefrontal cortex also mediates a variety of additional particularly interesting behaviors, including planning (Damasio 1985),
memory for serial order and temporal information (Fuster 1985), aspects of language
(Deacon 1997), and social information processing (de Bruin 1990). The importance of
serial-order memory is generally not appreciated in discussions of human behavioral evolution. One of the clearest behavioral advantages humans have over other organisms is
the ability to reconstruct, understand, and utilize causal information. All human technological sophistication is dependent on this ability.
In turn, causality is dependent on the ability
to remember the serial order of past events.
Without this, it is impossible to reconstruct
(remember) what actions or behaviors lead
to exactly which outcomes. Thus serial-order
memory likely played a key role in human behavioral evolution. Although no studies have
addressed whether serial-order memory is associated with prefrontal size (or size of some
other region), this would seem to be a fruitful
direction to pursue.
Brain Size and Conceptual
Complexity
A general argument can be made that increasing brain size brought with it an increase in
conceptual or semantic complexity (Gibson
2002). Jerison (1985, p. 30) suggested that
“[g]rades of encephalization presumably correspond to grades of complexity of information processing. These, in turn, correspond in
some way to the complexity of the reality created by the brain, which may be another way
to describe intelligence.”
A number of observations support this
view. First, concepts are instantiated in the
brain as webs or networks of activation between different areas (see Pulvermuller 2001).
Most of our subjectively experienced concepts
are actually complex combinations of sensory
information processed in various ways by the
different cortical centers. Taste, for example,
is actually a complex interaction of olfactory
(smell) and gustatory (taste) inputs (e.g., the
flavor of a banana is largely olfactory). Similarly, the auditory perception of a phoneme
can be altered if it is paired with a mismatched
visual input (McGurk & MacDonald 1976).
This means there must be networks connecting differing regions as well as areas that mediate the integration of this information.
To what extent is brain size relevant to conceptual complexity? Larger-brained species
have more complicated networks of interconnection, thereby leading to greater potential
conceptual complexity (Lieberman 2002). It
has long been known that certain areas of the
body (e.g., the lips and hands) are disproportionately represented in the somatosensory and primary motor areas of the brain
www.annualreviews.org • Evolution of the Human Brain
395
ARI
9 September 2006
8:42
and that these match differences in the degree
of sensitivity and/or motor control for different parts of our body (Penfield & Rasmussen
1950). Thus, even within species, we have
a clear association between the amount of
cortical tissue and behavioral dimensions
(Gibson 2002). Animals with specific behavioral specializations (e.g., bat echolocation)
have correlated increases in areas of the brain
known to mediate those behaviors (Krubitzer
1995).
We can then add to this the tendency for
larger-brained animals to display greater degrees of cortical specialization: Individual areas tend to be more specific in function and
less directly connected to other areas. This
is critical to conceptual complexity because
it increases the brain’s potential to differentiate complex sensory information into diverse constituent parts, thereby helping to
magnify subtle differences between different
streams of sensory input. The argument can
be summarized as follows: Increasing brain
size leads to increasingly complex processing within areas, greater degrees of autonomy between areas, and greater complexity
of the possible interactions between areas.
This leads to a greater complexity of possible
network-activation states, which is equivalent
to a greater degree of conceptual subtlety and
sophistication possible in the organism’s representations of reality. Whatever else increasing brain size led to in hominid evolution, it is
difficult to escape the conclusion that conceptual complexity increased substantially during
this time. Furthermore, given the fundamentally socially interactive nature of humans, as
well as the general association between degree
of sociality and brain size (discussed below),
this increase in conceptual complexity is likely
directly relevant to the evolution of language.
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
ANRV287-AN35-20
Brain Evolution and Language
Although the exact evolutionary changes in
the brain necessary to allow for language are
not known, language clearly relies on a large
number of neural resources. The importance
396
Schoenemann
for language of Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas has been known for more than a century, but it has become increasingly clear
that language requires the cooperation of a
wide range of cortical areas, including the
cerebellum (Gazzaniga et al. 1998), righthemisphere areas [important for processing logical inferences encoded in language
(Beeman et al. 2000)], prefrontal cortex [important for higher-level language functioning (Novoa & Ardila 1987)], and areas of the
frontal lobe outside Broca’s area (Alexander
et al. 1989). Functional brain imaging studies suggest the prefrontal cortex also plays a
critical role in conceptual/semantic processing (Gabrieli et al. 1998). All of this indicates
that language draws on a wide array of neural
resources, which suggests that important features of brain evolution may be explained by
the coevolution of language (Deacon 1997).
That language evolution is specifically relevant to brain-size evolution has been suggested many times (e.g., Dunbar 1996, Gibson
2002, Wang 1991, Washburn 1960). Darwin
(1871, p. 57) himself argued that, although
language use during human evolution likely
had effects on the elaboration of the vocal
organs, “the relation between the continued
use of language and the development of the
brain has no doubt been far more important.”
Brain size, in this view, is itself an index of language evolution. This suggests language has
origins that are substantially older than the
appearance of anatomically modern H. sapiens, which date to perhaps ∼160,000 years
ago (White et al. 2003). As reviewed above,
the trend toward increasing brain size began
sometime before 2 Mya (Figure 2). Although
there is widespread disagreement about how
far back language extends in human evolution
(for a review, see Schoenemann 2005), it is difficult to escape the conclusion that language
likely played a major role in the evolution of
the human brain. The evidence of the relationship between brain size and conceptual
complexity at a minimum suggests that fundamental changes in human cognition critical to
language evolution began prior to ∼2 Mya.
ANRV287-AN35-20
ARI
9 September 2006
8:42
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
Sociality and Brain Evolution
Primates in general, and humans in particular, are socially interactive animals. Our ability
to survive and reproduce is at least as dependent on successfully navigating social arrangements as it is navigating the physical environment (Holloway 1975, Humphrey 1984).
Social interactions are intrinsically complicated, and the complexity increases with increasing social group size. Humphrey (1984)
pointed out that the increasing complexity of the social world selects for increasing
cognitive sophistication (social intelligence)
in individuals, which in turn creates even
more complex social interactions. This creates
a cycle of ever-increasing social complexity,
leading to ever-increasing intellect among
individuals—what Humphrey refers to as
an evolutionary ratchet. Because of the
apparent benefits of being skilled at social manipulation in such an increasingly
complex social existence, this has become
known as the Machiavellian intelligence
hypothesis.
It appears likely that selection for social
abilities was an important influence on brain
evolution. A number of comparative studies
of primates have confirmed an association between measures of brain and/or neocortex
size (both absolute and relative) and a variety of measures of social complexity, including mean social group size, social clique size,
frequency of reported acts of deceptive behavior, amount of social play (but not nonsocial types of play), and the degree to which
male-dominance rank fails to accurately predict mating success (see review in Dunbar
2003, and references therein). All of this is
consistent with the idea that brain size is a factor in social ability, broadly defined, although
there are glaring exceptions: Orangutans are
relatively large-brained but relatively asocial. Clearly, brain size is not a perfect function of social complexity. However, there
is no other known behavioral variable that
correlates as highly with brain size across
species.
Dunbar (1996) has further argued that
human language represents a form of social
grooming that allowed the increase in group
size beyond that otherwise possible. Although
there are a number of extrapolations needed
to arrive at this conclusion, language clearly
serves a highly social function in humans.
Although the comparative evidence that
social complexity correlates with brain size is
strong, the specific abilities crucial to social
ability within humans (or any other species)
are not clearly defined or understood. Some
people are more social than others, and some
social people are better at understanding
and/or manipulating social interactions than
others. However, there does not appear to
have been much research into this question
from a neurocognitive and/or neuroanatomical standpoint. Presumably social competence
depends on a wide variety of abilities, including language, nonverbal-cue processing,
memory (particularly of past interactions and
the order of past events), and probably many
other basic cognitive abilities. Interestingly,
intact prefrontal cortex (unlike other cortical areas) appears to be crucial for the maintenance of high position in dominance hierarchies in monkeys (de Bruin 1990, Myers
et al. 1973). Given that this area seems to have
undergone disproportionate increase, as discussed above, this appears to be a promising
avenue of investigation with respect to brain
evolution. Other parts of the brain that appear to be important for social behavior include the amygdaloid nuclei and overlying
temporal pole (tip of the temporal lobe) and
the posterior medial orbital cortex (including
Brodmann’s area 13, located in the inferior
prefrontal cortex) (Kling 1986). As reviewed
above, Brodmann’s area 13 shows a moderate increase in absolute terms, although it
does lag behind the increase of the brain as
a whole. Piecing together the effects the social environment had on modifying the brain
during human evolution will likely continue
to be the focus of significant research in the
future.
www.annualreviews.org • Evolution of the Human Brain
397
ANRV287-AN35-20
ARI
9 September 2006
8:42
Ecological Hypotheses
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
An alternative (but not mutually exclusive)
hypothesis is that adaptation for ecological
challenges influences brain evolution. Milton
(1981) pointed out that different types of diet
vary with respect to the cognitive demands
they place on individuals. Fruit is patchily
distributed in both time and space, whereas
leaves are much less cognitively demanding
to obtain. This suggests that species that
specialize in fruit (or, more generally, any food
source that is cognitively demanding to obtain) are expected to have larger brains than
species that do not. Dunbar (1995) did not find
a significant association between a measure of
relative brain size (ratio of the neocortex to the
rest of the brain) and percent of fruit in diet in
anthropoid primates. However, Barton (1996)
did find a significant association within diurnal haplorhines (specifically, diurnal monkeys
and apes) between absolute and relative brain
size and percent of fruit in the diet. This discrepancy is possibly a result of a difference in
neuroanatomical variables used.
It is also possible that the causality runs
the other way: Because larger brains are more
metabolically expensive, some sort of dietary
accommodation may be necessary to pay for it
nutritionally. The expensive-tissue hypothesis
(Aiello & Wheeler 1995) argues for a tradeoff between brain and gut size. If larger brains
mean smaller guts, then one would predict a
higher quality diet. This hypothesis is supported in primates (Fish & Lockwood 2003).
It also fits the human case, in which brain size
started increasing at about the same time as
meat became increasingly important in hominid diets (as indexed by the initial appearance of stone tools).
Tools and Brain Evolution
Is it possible that the cognitive demands of
tool making itself spurred brain evolution?
Reader & Laland (2002) showed that frequency of tool use in primates is positively correlated with both absolute and relative brain
398
Schoenemann
volume. Although early hominid stone-tool
industries are not highly complex technologically, they may have required cognitive abilities beyond that shown by apes, for example,
in the sequencing of required actions (Toth &
Schick 1993). A preliminary functional brain–
imaging study of stone-tool manufacturing
suggests the activation of cortical areas mediating spatial cognition, as well as motor, somatosensory, and cerebellar areas (as might
be expected given the nature of the task), although prefrontal areas known to be relevant to planning were not significantly activated (Stout et al. 2000). If this finding can
be replicated, given that spatial ability does
not appear significantly correlated with brain
size in modern humans (as discussed above),
it may argue against early stone-tool manufacturing specifically spurring brain-size evolution (although the spatial abilities tested
involved paper-and-pencil tests, rather than
hands-on, three-dimensional manipulation as
in the stone-tool study). Research on the possible importance of stone-tool manufacturing
is clearly in its infancy, and future functionalimaging studies are needed to clarify the
issue.
It has also been suggested that the development of accurate throwing might have spurred
brain evolution. Calvin (1983) pointed out
that human throwing accuracy requires timing abilities (for the release of the thrown
object) that far exceed the probable timing
accuracies of neurons (judging from measurements of the intrinsic variability in neuronal
signals). He pointed out that increasingly accurate timers could be built by putting greater
and greater numbers of even inaccurate neurons in parallel in a timing circuit. He also suggested that, given the cortical areas in the primary motor cortex controlling the mouth and
tongue (involved in language) and the hand
(involved in throwing) were reasonably close
to each other, selection for throwing ability may have led to changes that preadapted
the brain for language. This hypothesis has
proven difficult to test, but it may be consistent with the tentative finding that the
ANRV287-AN35-20
ARI
9 September 2006
8:42
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
premotor cortex in humans may not have
lagged as far behind as the primary motor
cortex during human brain evolution. It is
also consistent with the finding that sequential finger tapping is disrupted by concurrent
speech (which depends on the left hemisphere
in most individuals) only if the tapping is done
with the right hand (which is also controlled
by the left hemisphere) and not the left hand
(which is controlled by the right hemisphere)
(Ikeda 1987).
The Cognitive Reserve Hypothesis
An additional explanation for the increase
in brain size in human evolution is that it
may have allowed for an increase in longevity
(Allen et al. 2005b, Humphrey 1999). The argument is that larger brains would buffer individuals against a variety of inevitable brain
insults as individuals age, thereby increasing
the useful cognitive lifespan. The results of a
variety of clinical studies are consistent with
the idea that larger brain size has a protective
effect for a number of brain diseases and types
of injury (reviewed in Allen et al. 2005b). Why
(and whether) longevity would be evolutionarily adaptive in humans is unclear, although
the survival of older, postreproductive individuals has been argued to be important (e.g.,
the grandmother hypothesis). The cognitive
reserve hypothesis and the idea that more neural resources translate into better cognitive
functioning of some kind are not mutually exclusive, of course.
Summary of the Evolution of Brain
and Behavior
Explaining why the human brain changed as
it did requires determining the behavioral implications of changing brain size and/or the
proportions of various brain components. To
be evolutionarily relevant, associations between the brain and behavior must be genetic
correlations. These correlations can be quite
small, however. Although both brain morphology and behavioral dimensions have been
shown to be genetically influenced, genetic
correlations between brain anatomy and behavior appear to be quite modest. The genetic
correlation between overall brain size and
general cognitive ability appears to be substantially smaller than overall phenotypic correlation. Associations between specific functional areas and specific behavioral abilities
appear to be somewhat more robust.
A number of general behavioral models
of brain evolution have been proposed that
have theoretical and/or cross-species empirical support. These include the idea that
brain size is associated with increased conceptual complexity, language ability, social ability, ecological challenges, the development of
tools, and the need for a cognitive reserve.
Direct tests of these hypotheses await future
research.
CONCLUSION
Over the past 2 to 3 million years, our brain
has changed in dramatic and behaviorally interesting ways. Although brain size and body
size are correlated, absolute increases in neural tissue are likely behaviorally relevant, and
the overemphasis on EQ needs to be tempered. There is substantial evidence that the
human brain is also not simply a larger version
of a generic primate brain, with some areas
showing evidence of lagging behind (such as
the olfactory bulb, primary visual cortex, primary motor and premotor areas) and some accounting for disproportionate increases (such
as the prefrontal). We should expect to find
clues about the details of the behavioral evolution of our species from these patterns.
Given the evolutionary costs of neural tissue, disproportional increases (even in absolute terms) would not likely have occurred
unless they conferred some sort of adaptive
(reproductive) advantages, on average, to individuals in the successive populations. The
advantages could be slight, however, making
our task as scientists potentially difficult. General cognitive ability appears to show weak
associations with brain size, and a number
www.annualreviews.org • Evolution of the Human Brain
399
ANRV287-AN35-20
ARI
9 September 2006
8:42
of behavioral dimensions appear to be associated with specific brain areas. Hypotheses
involving conceptual complexity, social abilities, language, ecological challenges, tool use,
and the cognitive reserve hypothesis all appear to have merit for explaining human brain
evolution
Filling out the history of human brain evolution will continue to utilize an intensively
interdisciplinary approach in which informa-
tion, methods, and resources from a wide variety of fields will increasingly be marshaled
to the task of squeezing every last possible
bit of valid inference out of the data. Fundamentally, our arguments will always necessarily be statistical judgments. At present,
we do not even know the limits of what we
can and cannot know about this history. Discovering these limits is a central task for the
future.
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
I wish to thank Vincent Sarich, John Allen, Ralph Holloway, Dean Falk, Tim White, Thomas
Budinger, Arthur Jensen, Bruce Lahn, Karen Schmidt, Janet Monge, Dan Glotzer, Michael
Sheehan, and Reina Wong for stimulating discussions on the topics discussed in this review.
Any errors that remain are my own, of course.
LITERATURE CITED
Aiello LC, Wheeler P. 1995. The expensive tissue hypothesis: the brain and the digestive
system in human and primate evolution. Curr. Anthropol. 36:199–221
Alexander MP, Benson DF, Stuss DT. 1989. Frontal lobes and language. Brain Lang. 37:656–91
Allen JS, Bruss J, Damasio H. 2005a. MRI analysis of the calcarine and lunate sulci in modern
humans. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 126(S40):67
Allen JS, Bruss J, Damasio H. 2005b. The aging brain: the cognitive reserve hypothesis and
hominid evolution. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 17:673–89
Allman JM. 1999. Evolving Brains. New York: W.H. Freeman
Ankney CD. 1992. Sex differences in relative brain size: the mismeasure of woman, too? Intelligence 16:329–36
Armstrong E. 1983. Relative brain size and metabolism in mammals. Science 220:1302–4
Armstrong E, Zilles K, Curtis M, Schleicher A. 1991. Cortical folding, the lunate sulcus and
the evolution of the human brain. J. Hum. Evol. 20:341–48
Avants BB, Schoenemann PT, Gee JC. 2005. Lagrangian frame diffeomorphic image registration: morphometric comparison of human and chimpanzee cortex. Med. Image Anal.
10:397–412
Barton RA. 1996. Neocortex size and behavioral ecology in primates. Proc. R. Soc. London Ser.
B 263:173–77
Beeman MJ, Bowden EM, Gernsbacher MA. 2000. Right and left hemisphere cooperation for
drawing predictive and coherence inferences during normal story comprehension. Brain
Lang. 71:310–36
Blinkov SM, Glezer II. 1968. The Human Brain in Figures and Tables. New York: Plenum.
482 pp.
Bogin B. 1999. Patterns of Human Growth. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press. 2nd ed.
Broadfield DC, Holloway RL, Mowbray K, Silvers A, Yuan MS, Márquez S. 2001. Endocast
of Sambungmacan 3 (Sm 3): A new Homo erectus from Indonesia. Anat. Rec. 262:369–79
Brodmann K. 1909. Vergleichende Lokalisationsiehre der Grosshirnrinde in ihren Prinzipien
Dargestellt auf Grund des Zellenbaues. Leipzig: Johann Ambrosius Barth Verlag
400
Schoenemann
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
ANRV287-AN35-20
ARI
9 September 2006
8:42
Bush EC, Allman JM. 2004. The scaling of frontal cortex in primates and carnivores. Proc.
Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 101:3962–66
Calvin WH. 1983. A stone’s throw and its launch window: timing precision and its implications
for language and hominid brains. J. Theor. Biol. 104:121–35
Carpenter MB, Sutin J. 1983. Human Neuroanatomy. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins
Changizi MA, Shimojo S. 2005. Parcellation and area-area connectivity as a function of neocortex size. Brain Behav. Evol. 66:88–98
Connolly CJ. 1950. External Morphology of the Primate Brain. Springfield, IL: C.C. Thomas
Damasio AR. 1985. The frontal lobes. In Clinical Neuropsychology, ed. K Heilman, E Valenstein,
p. 339–75. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press
Darwin C. 1871. The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex. Vol. 1. London: John Murray
Davatzikos C, Resnick SM. 1998. Sex differences in anatomic measures of interhemispheric
connectivity: correlations with cognition in women but not men. Cereb. Cortex 8:635–40
Deacon TW. 1997. The Symbolic Species: The Coevolution of Language and the Brain. New York:
W.W. Norton
de Bruin JPC. 1990. Social behavior and the prefrontal cortex. In Progress in Brain Research, ed.
HBM Uylings, CG Van Eden, JPC de Bruin, MA Corner, MGP Feenstra, pp. 485–97.
New York: Elsevier Science
Dekaban AS. 1978. Changes in brain weights during the span of human life: relation of brain
weights to body heights and body weights. Ann. Neurol. 4:345–56
de Lacoste-Utamsing C, Holloway RL. 1982. Sexual dimorphism in the human corpus callosum. Science 216:1431–32
De Miguel C, Henneberg M. 2001. Variation in hominid brain size: How much is due to
method? Homo 52:3–58
Diamond MC. 1988. Enriching Heredity: The Impact of the Environment on the Anatomy of the
Brain. New York: Free Press
Dubois E. 1913. On the relation between quantity of brain and the size of the body in vertebrates. Verh. Kon. Akad. Wetenschappen Amsterdam 16:647
Dunbar R. 1996. Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language. London: Faber & Faber
Dunbar RIM. 1995. Neocortex size and group size in primates: a test of the hypothesis. J.
Hum. Evol. 28:287–96
Dunbar RIM. 2003. The social brain: mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective.
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 32:163–81
Evans PD, Anderson JR, Vallender EJ, Gilbert SL, Malcom CM, et al. 2004. Adaptive evolution
of ASPM, a major determinant of cerebral cortical size in humans. Hum. Mol. Genet.
13:489–94
Falconer DS. 1981. Introduction to Quantitative Genetics. New York: Longman. 2nd ed.
Falk D. 1983. Cerebral cortices of East African early hominids. Science 221:1072–74
Falk D. 1987. Hominid paleoneurology. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 16:13–30
Falk D. 1990. Brain evolution in Homo: the “radiator” theory. Behav. Brain Sci. 13:333–81
Falk D. 1992. Braindance: New Discoveries About Human Origins and Brain Evolution. New York:
Henry Holt
Falk D, Froese N, Sade DS, Dudek BC. 1999. Sex differences in brain/body relationships of
Rhesus monkeys and humans. J. Hum. Evol. 36:233–38
Falk D, Hildebolt C, Cheverud J, Vannier M, Helmkamp RC, Konigsberg L. 1990. Cortical
asymmetries in frontal lobes of Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Brain Res. 512:40–45
Finlay BL, Darlington RB, Nicastro N. 2001. Developmental structure in brain evolution.
Behav. Brain Sci. 24:263–308
www.annualreviews.org • Evolution of the Human Brain
401
ARI
9 September 2006
8:42
Fish JL, Lockwood CA. 2003. Dietary constraints on encephalization in primates. Am. J. Phys.
Anthropol. 120:171–81
Fuster JM. 1985. The prefrontal cortex, mediator of cross-temporal contingencies. Hum. Neurobiol. 4:169–79
Gabrieli JD, Poldrack RA, Desmond JE. 1998. The role of left prefrontal cortex in language
and memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 95:906–13
Garby L, Lammert O, Kock KF, Thobo-Carlsen B. 1993. Weights of brain, liver, kidneys, and
spleen in healthy and apparently healthy adult Danish subjects. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 5:291–96
Gazzaniga MS, Ivry RB, Mangun GR. 1998. Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind.
New York: W.W. Norton
Geary DC. 2005. The Origin of Mind: Evolution of Brain, Cognition, and General Intelligence.
Washington, DC: Am. Psychol. Assoc.
Geschwind DH, Miller BL, DeCarli C, Carmelli D. 2002. Heritability of lobar brain volumes
in twins supports genetic models of cerebral laterality and handedness. Proc. Natl. Acad.
Sci. USA 99:3176–81
Gibson KR. 2002. Evolution of human intelligence: the roles of brain size and mental construction. Brain Behav. Evol. 59:10–20
Gignac G, Vernon PA, Wickett JC. 2003. Factors influencing the relationship between brain
size and intelligence. In The Scientific Study of General Intelligence: Tribute to Arthur R.
Jensen, ed. H Nyborg, pp. 93–106. London: Elsevier
Gilbert SL, Dobyns WB, Lahn BT. 2005. Genetic links between brain development and brain
evolution. Nat. Rev. Genet. 6:581–90
Goldman-Rakic PS. 1996. The prefrontal landscape: implications of functional architecture
for understanding human mentation and the central executive. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London
Ser. B 351:1445–53
Gould SJ. 1981. The Mismeasure of Man. New York: Norton. 352 pp.
Halpern DF. 1987. Sex Differences in Cognitive Abilities. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Harvey PH, Clutton-Brock TH. 1985. Life history variation in primates. Evolution 39:559–81
Haug H. 1987. Brain sizes, surfaces, and neuronal sizes of the cortex cerebri: a stereological
investigation of man and his variability and a comparison with some mammals (primates,
whales, marsupials, insectivores, and one elephant). Am. J. Anat. 180:126–42
Herrnstein RJ, Murray C. 1994. The Bell Curve. New York: Free Press
Ho K, Roessmann U, Straumfjord JV, Monroe G. 1980a. Analysis of brain weight. I. Adult
brain weight in relation to sex, race, and age. Arch. Pathol. Lab. Med. 104:635–39
Ho K, Roessmann U, Straumfjord JV, Monroe G. 1980b. Analysis of brain weight. II. Adult
brain weight in relation to body height, weight, and surface area. Arch. Pathol. Lab. Med.
104:640–45
Hofman MA. 1983a. Encephalization in hominids: evidence for the model of punctuationalism.
Brain Behav. Evol. 22:102–17
Hofman MA. 1983b. Energy metabolism, brain size, and longevity in mammals. Q. Rev. Biol.
58:495–512
Hofman MA. 1985. Size and shape of the cerebral cortex in mammals. I. The cortical surface.
Brain Behav. Evol. 27:28–40
Holloway. 1966. Cranial capacity and neuron number: a critique and proposal. Am. J. Phys.
Anthropol. 25:305–14
Holloway RL. 1975. The Role of Human Social Behavior in the Evolution of the Brain. New York:
Am. Mus. Nat. Hist.
Holloway RL. 1983. Human paleontological evidence relevant to language behavior. Hum.
Neurobiol. 2:105–14
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
ANRV287-AN35-20
402
Schoenemann
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
ANRV287-AN35-20
ARI
9 September 2006
8:42
Holloway RL. 1992. The failure of the gyrification index (GI) to account for volumetric reorganization in the evolution of the human brain. J. Hum. Evol. 22:163–70
Holloway RL. 1995. Toward a synthetic theory of human brain evolution. In Origins of the
Human Brain, ed. J-P Changeux, J Chavaillon, pp. 42–54. Oxford: Clarendon Press
Holloway RL. 2002. Brief communication: How much larger is the relative volume of area 10
of the prefrontal cortex in humans? Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 118:399–401
Holloway RL, Anderson PJ, Defendini R, Harper C. 1993. Sexual dimorphism of the human
corpus callosum from three independent samples: relative size of the corpus callosum. Am.
J. Phys. Anthropol. 92:481–98
Holloway RL, Broadfield DC, Yuan MS. 2003. Morphology and histology of chimpanzee
primary visual striate cortex indicate that brain reorganization predated brain expansion
in early hominid evolution. Anat. Rec. A Discov. Mol. Cell Evol. Biol. 273:594–602
Holloway RL, Broadfield DC, Yuan MS. 2004a. The Human Fossil Record, Volume 3. Brain
Endocasts: The Paleoneurological Evidence. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley & Sons
Holloway RL, Clark RJ, Tobias PV. 2004b. Posterior lunate sulcus in Australopithecus africanus:
Was Dart right? C. R. Palevol. 3:287–93
Holloway RL, de la Coste-Lareymondie MC. 1982. Brain endocast asymmetry in pongids and
hominids: some preliminary findings on the paleontology of cerebral dominance. Am. J.
Phys. Anthropol. 58:101–10
Humphrey N. 1984. The social function of intellect. In Consciousness Regained, ed. N.
Humphrey, pp. 14–28. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press
Humphrey N. 1999. Why human grandmothers may need large brains. Psycholoquy 10:24.
http://psycprints.ecs.soton.ac.uk/archive/00000659/
Ikeda K. 1987. Lateralized interference effects of concurrent verbal tasks on sequential finger
tapping. Neuropsychologia 25:453–56
Jensen AR. 1994. Psychometric g related to differences in head size. Pers. Individ. Dif. 17:597–
606
Jensen AR, Johnson FW. 1994. Race and sex differences in head size and IQ. Intelligence
18:309–33
Jensen AR, Sinha SN. 1990. Physical correlates of human intelligence. In Biological Approaches
to the Study of Human Intelligence, ed. PA Vernon. Norwood, NJ: Ablex
Jerison HJ. 1973. Evolution of the Brain and Intelligence. New York: Academic
Jerison HJ. 1985. Animal intelligence as encephalization. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London Ser. B
308:21–35
Kappelman J. 1996. The evolution of body mass and relative brain size in fossil hominids. J.
Hum. Evol. 30:243–76
Kling AS. 1986. Neurological correlates of social behavior. Ethol. Sociobiol. 7:175–86
Krubitzer L. 1995. The organization of neocortex in mammals: Are species differences really
so different? Trends Neurosci. 18:408–17
Lee SH, Wolpoff MH. 2003. The pattern of evolution in Pleistocene human brain size. Paleobiology 29:186–96
Lieberman P. 2002. On the nature and evolution of the neural bases of human language. Yearb.
Phys. Anthropol. 45:36–62
Lovejoy CO. 1975. Biomechanical perspectives on the lower limb of early hominids. In Primate
Functional Morphology and Evolution, ed. RH Tuttle, pp. 291–326. The Hague: Mouton
Lyons DM, Afarian H, Schatzberg AF, Sawyer-Glover A, Moseley ME. 2002. Experiencedependent asymmetric variation in primate prefrontal morphology. Behav. Brain Res.
136:51–59
www.annualreviews.org • Evolution of the Human Brain
403
ARI
9 September 2006
8:42
MacLeod CE, Zilles K, Schleicher A, Rilling JK, Gibson KR. 2003. Expansion of the neocerebellum in Hominoidea. J. Hum. Evol. 44:401–29
Marquez S, Mowbray K, Sawyer GJ, Jacob T, Silvers A. 2001. New fossil hominid calvaria
from Indonesia—Sambungmacan 3. Anat. Rec. 262:344–68
Martin RD. 1981. Relative brain size and basal metabolic rate in terrestrial vertebrates. Nature
293:57–60
McBride T, Arnold SE, Gur RC. 1999. A comparative volumetric analysis of the prefrontal
cortex in human and baboon MRI. Brain Behav. Evol. 54:159–66
McComb K, Moss C, Durant SM, Baker L, Sayialel S. 2001. Matriarchs as repositories of
social knowledge in African elephants. Science 292:491–94
McGurk H, MacDonald J. 1976. Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature 264:746–48
Milton K. 1981. Distribution patterns of tropical plant foods as an evolutionary stimulus to
primate mental development. Am. Anthropol. 83:534–48
Myers RE, Swett C, Miller M. 1973. Loss of social group affinity following prefrontal lesions
in free-ranging macaques. Brain Res. 64:257–69
Neisser U, Boodoo G, Bouchard TJJ, Boykin AW, Brody N, et al. 1996. Intelligence: knowns
and unknowns. Am. Psychol. 51:77–101
Novoa OP, Ardila A. 1987. Linguistic abilities in patients with prefrontal damage. Brain Lang.
30:206–25
Pakkenberg H, Voigt J. 1964. Brain weight of the Danes. Acta Anatom. 56:297–307
Penfield W, Rasmussen T. 1950. Cerebral Cortex of Man: A Clinical Study of Localization of
Function. New York: Macmillan
Plomin R, DeFries JC, McClearn GE, Rutter M. 1997. Behavioral Genetics. New York: W.H.
Freeman. 3rd ed.
Posthuma D, Baare WF, Pol HEH, Kahn RS, Boomsma DI, De Geus EJ. 2003. Genetic correlations between brain volumes and the WAIS-III dimensions of verbal comprehension,
working memory, perceptual organization, and processing speed. Twin Res. 6:131–39
Posthuma D, De Geus EJ, Baare WF, Pol HEH, Kahn RS, Boomsma DI. 2002. The association
between brain volume and intelligence is of genetic origin. Nat. Neurosci. 5:83–84
Preuss TM. 2000. What’s human about the human brain? In The New Cognitive Neurosciences,
ed. MS Gazzaniga, pp. 1219–34. 2nd ed. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press
Preuss TM, Coleman GQ. 2002. Human-specific organization of primary visual cortex: alternating compartments of dense Cat-301 and calbindin immunoreactivity in layer 4A.
Cereb. Cortex 12:671–91
Pulvermuller F. 2001. Brain reflections of words and their meaning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 5:517–24
Raz N, Lindenberger U, Rodrigue KM, Kennedy KM, Head D, et al. 2005. Regional brain
changes in aging healthy adults: general trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cereb.
Cortex 15:1676–89
Reader SM, Laland KN. 2002. Social intelligence, innovation, and enhanced brain size in
primates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 99:4436–41
Redmond JCJ. 1999. Cranial capacity and performance on delay-response task correlated with
principal sulcus length in monkeys. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 109:33–40
Rendell L, Whitehead H, Rendell L, Whitehead H. 2001. Culture in whales and dolphins.
Behav. Brain Sci. 24:309–24; discussion 324–82
Riddell WI, Corl KG. 1977. Comparative investigation of the relationship between cerebral
indices and learning abilities. Brain Behav. Evol. 14:385–98
Rilling JK, Insel TR. 1999. The primate neocortex in comparative perspective using magnetic
resonance imaging. J. Hum. Evol. 37:191–223
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
ANRV287-AN35-20
404
Schoenemann
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
ANRV287-AN35-20
ARI
9 September 2006
8:42
Rilling JK, Seligman RA. 2002. A quantitative morphometric comparative analysis of the primate temporal lobe. J. Hum. Evol. 42:505–33
Ringo JL. 1991. Neuronal interconnection as a function of brain size. Brain Behav. Evol. 38:1–6
Rumbaugh DM, Savage-Rumbaugh ES, Wasburn DA. 1996. Toward a new outlook on primate learning and behavior: complex learning and emergent processes in comparative
perspective. Jpn. Psychol. Res. 38:113–25
Rushton JP, Ankney CD. 1996. Brain size and cognitive ability: correlations with age, sex,
social class, and race. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 3:21–36
Sax KW, Strakowski SM, Zimmerman ME, DelBello MP, Keck PEJ, Hawkins JM. 1999.
Frontosubcortical neuroanatomy and the continuous performance test in mania. Am. J.
Psychiatry 156:139–41
Schenker NM, Desgouttes AM, Semendeferi K. 2005. Neural connectivity and cortical substrates of cognition in hominoids. J. Hum. Evol. 49:547–69
Schoenemann PT. 1997. An MRI study of the relationship between human neuroanatomy and
behavioral ability. PhD diss. Univ. of Calif., Berkeley
Schoenemann PT. 2004. Brain size scaling and body composition in mammals. Brain Behav.
Evol. 63:47–60
Schoenemann PT. 2005. Conceptual complexity and the brain: understanding language origins. In Language Acquisition, Change and Emergence: Essays in Evolutionary Linguistics, ed.
WS-Y Wang, JW Minett, pp. 47–94. Hong Kong: City Univ. of Hong Kong Press
Schoenemann PT, Budinger TF, Sarich VM, Wang WS. 2000. Brain size does not predict
general cognitive ability within families. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 97:4932–37
Schoenemann PT, Glotzer LD, Sheehan MJ. 2005a. Reply to “Is prefrontal white matter
enlargement a human evolutionary specialization?” Nat. Neurosci. 8:538
Schoenemann PT, Sheehan MJ, Glotzer LD. 2005b. Prefrontal white matter volume is disproportionately larger in humans than in other primates. Nat. Neurosci. 8:242–52
Semendeferi K, Armstrong E, Schleicher A, Zilles K, Van Hoesen GW. 1998. Limbic frontal
cortex in hominoids: a comparative study of area 13. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 106:129–55
Semendeferi K, Armstrong E, Schleicher A, Zilles K, Van Hoesen GW. 2001. Prefrontal cortex
in humans and apes: a comparative study of area 10. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 114:224–41
Semendeferi K, Lu A, Schenker N, Damasio H. 2002. Humans and great apes share a large
frontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 5:272–76
Sherwood CC, Holloway RL, Semendeferi K, Hof PR. 2005. Is prefrontal white matter enlargement a human evolutionary specialization? Nat. Neurosci. 8:537–38; author reply 538
Smith BH. 1990. The cost of a large brain. Behav. Brain Sci. 13:365–66
Stephan H, Frahm H, Baron G. 1981. New and revised data on volumes of brain structures in
insectivores and primates. Folia Primatologica 35:1–29
Stout D, Toth N, Schick K. 2000. Stone tool-making and brain activation: position emission
tomography (PET) studies. J. Archaeol. Sci. 27:1215–23
Striedter GF. 2005. Principles of Brain Evolution. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates
Thompson PM, Cannon TD, Narr KL, van Erp T, Poutanen VP, et al. 2001. Genetic influences on brain structure. Nat. Neurosci. 4:1253–58
Tobias PV. 1975. Brain evolution in the Hominoidea. In Primate Functional Morphology and
Evolution, ed. RH Tuttle, p. 353–92. The Hague: Mouton
Tobias PV. 1983. Recent advances in the evolution of the hominids with especial reference to
brain and speech. In Recent Advances in the Evolution of Primates, ed. C Chagas, p. 85–140.
Vatican City: Pontificia Acad. Sci.
www.annualreviews.org • Evolution of the Human Brain
405
ARI
9 September 2006
8:42
Toth N, Schick K. 1993. Early stone industries and inferences regarding language and cognition. In Tools, Language and Cognition in Human Evolution, ed. KR Gibson, T Ingold,
pp. 346–62. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press
Van Essen DC. 2005. Surface-based comparisons of macaque and human cortical organization.
In From Monkey Brain to Human Brain, ed. S Dehaene, J-R Duhamel, MD Hauser, G
Rizzolatti, pp. 3–19. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Van Essen DC, Drury HA, Joshi S, Miller MI. 1998. Functional and structural mapping of
human cerebral cortex: Solutions are in the surfaces. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 95:788–95
von Bonin G. 1963. The Evolution of the Human Brain. Chicago: Univ. Chicago Press
Wang WSY. 1991. Explorations in language evolution. In Explorations in Language, pp. 105–31.
Taipei, Taiwan: Pyramid Press
Wang YQ, Su B. 2004. Molecular evolution of microcephalin, a gene determining human brain
size. Hum. Mol. Genet. 13:1131–37
Washburn SL. 1960. Tools and evolution. Sci. Am. 203:63–75
White TD, Asfaw B, DeGusta D, Gilbert H, Richards GD, et al. 2003. Pleistocene Homo
sapiens from Middle Awash, Ethiopia. Nature 423:742–47
Wickett JC, Vernon PA, Lee DH. 2000. Relationships between factors of intelligence and brain
volume. Pers. Individ. Dif. 29:1095–22
Winterer G, Goldman D. 2003. Genetics of human prefrontal function. Brain Res. Brain Res.
Rev. 43:134–63
Wood B, Collard M. 1999. The human genus. Science 284:65–71
Wright IC, Sham P, Murray RM, Weinberger DR, Bullmore ET. 2002. Genetic contributions to regional variability in human brain structure: methods and preliminary results.
Neuroimage 17:256–71
Wynn T. 2002. Archaeology and cognitive function. Behav. Brain Sci. 25:389–438
Zilles K. 2005. Evolution of the human brain and comparative cyto- and receptor architecture.
In From Monkey Brain to Human Brain, ed. S Dehaene, J-R Duhamel, MD Hauser, G
Rizzolatti, pp. 41–56. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
ANRV287-AN35-20
406
Schoenemann
AN35-20-Schoene.qxd
9/9/06
10:02 AM
Page C-1
10,000
a
Brain mass (g)
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
1000
100
10
Average mammal
Prosimii
Ceboidea
1
Cercopithecoidea
Hominoidea
Homo sapiens
0.1
0.01
0.1
1
10
100
1000
Body mass (kg)
Figure 1
Brain/body relationships among primates. (a) Individual species plotted with Martin’s (1981)
estimate for the average mammal. (b) Least squares–regression estimates for various primate subtaxa.
All primates (excluding humans): [log brain g] 1.24 0.761 [log body kg], r2 0.92, N 51;
Hominoidea (excluding humans): [log brain g] 1.548 0.553 [log body kg], r2 0.99,
N 6; Cercopithecoidea: [log brain g] 1.54 0.477 [log body kg], r2 0.87, N 14; Ceboidea:
[log brain g] 1.35 0.765 [log body kg], r2 0.94, N 13; Prosimii: [log brain g] 1.111 0.659
[log body kg], r2 0.92, N 18. Data extracted from literature sources (see Schoenemann 1997 for
details); pongid data estimated from cranial capacities using [brain weight g] [cranial capacity cc]/
1.14 (following Kappelman 1996).
www.annualreviews.org
●
Evolution of the Human Brain
C-1
AN35-20-Schoene.qxd
9/9/06
10:02 AM
Page C-2
10,000
b
Brain mass (g)
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
1000
100
10
Average mammal
All primates
Prosimii
Ceboidea
1
Cercopithecoidea
Hominoidea
Homo sapiens
0.1
0.01
0.1
1
10
Body mass (kg)
Figure 1
(Continued)
C-2
Schoenemann
100
1000
AN35-20-Schoene.qxd
9/9/06
10:02 AM
Page C-3
2000
1800
1600
Pan troglodytes
H. habilis
H. ergaster
Extant H. sapiens sapiens
H. rudolfensis
H. georgicus
H. erectus
H. antecessor
H. soloensis
H. heidelbergensis
H. sapiens neanderthalensis
H. sapiens idaltu
H. sapiens sapiens
A. afarensis
A. aethiopicus
A. boisei
A. robustus
A. africanus
A. garhi
Cranial capacity (cc)
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
1400
1200
1000
800
600
400
200
0
4.0
3.5
3.0
2.5
2.0
1.5
1.0
0.5
Ma
Figure 2
Cranial capacity in fossil hominids over time. Extant chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and humans
(Homo sapiens sapiens) are included for comparison. Fossil data and species designations from Holloway
et al. 2004a. Pan and Homo species data compiled from literature sources listed in Schoenemann (1997).
www.annualreviews.org
●
Evolution of the Human Brain
C-3
0
Contents
ARI
13 August 2006
13:30
Annual Review of
Anthropology
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
Contents
Volume 35, 2006
Prefatory Chapter
On the Resilience of Anthropological Archaeology
Kent V. Flannery p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 1
Archaeology
Archaeology of Overshoot and Collapse
Joseph A. Tainter p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p59
Archaeology and Texts: Subservience or Enlightenment
John Moreland p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 135
Alcohol: Anthropological/Archaeological Perspectives
Michael Dietler p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 229
Early Mainland Southeast Asian Landscapes in the First
Millennium a.d.
Miriam T. Stark p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 407
The Maya Codices
Gabrielle Vail p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 497
Biological Anthropology
What Cultural Primatology Can Tell Anthropologists about the
Evolution of Culture
Susan E. Perry p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 171
Diet in Early Homo: A Review of the Evidence and a New Model of
Adaptive Versatility
Peter S. Ungar, Frederick E. Grine, and Mark F. Teaford p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 209
Obesity in Biocultural Perspective
Stanley J. Ulijaszek and Hayley Lofink p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 337
ix
Contents
ARI
13 August 2006
13:30
Evolution of the Size and Functional Areas of the Human Brain
P. Thomas Schoenemann p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 379
Linguistics and Communicative Practices
Mayan Historical Linguistics and Epigraphy: A New Synthesis
Søren Wichmann p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 279
Environmental Discourses
Peter Mühlhäusler and Adrian Peace p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 457
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
Old Wine, New Ethnographic Lexicography
Michael Silverstein p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 481
International Anthropology and Regional Studies
The Ethnography of Finland
Jukka Siikala p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 153
Sociocultural Anthropology
The Anthropology of Money
Bill Maurer p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p15
Food and Globalization
Lynne Phillips p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p37
The Research Program of Historical Ecology
William Balée p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p75
Anthropology and International Law
Sally Engle Merry p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p99
Institutional Failure in Resource Management
James M. Acheson p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 117
Indigenous People and Environmental Politics
Michael R. Dove p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 191
Parks and Peoples: The Social Impact of Protected Areas
Paige West, James Igoe, and Dan Brockington p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 251
Sovereignty Revisited
Thomas Blom Hansen and Finn Stepputat p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 295
Local Knowledge and Memory in Biodiversity Conservation
Virginia D. Nazarea p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 317
x
Contents
Contents
ARI
13 August 2006
13:30
Food and Memory
Jon D. Holtzman p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 361
Creolization and Its Discontents
Stephan Palmié p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 433
Persistent Hunger: Perspectives on Vulnerability, Famine, and Food
Security in Sub-Saharan Africa
Mamadou Baro and Tara F. Deubel p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 521
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
Theme 1: Environmental Conservation
Archaeology of Overshoot and Collapse
Joseph A. Tainter p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p59
The Research Program of Historical Ecology
William Balée p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p75
Institutional Failure in Resource Management
James M. Acheson p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 117
Indigenous People and Environmental Politics
Michael R. Dove p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 191
Parks and Peoples: The Social Impact of Protected Areas
Paige West, James Igoe, and Dan Brockington p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 251
Local Knowledge and Memory in Biodiversity Conservation
Virginia D. Nazarea p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 317
Environmental Discourses
Peter Mühlhäusler and Adrian Peace p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 457
Theme 2: Food
Food and Globalization
Lynne Phillips p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p37
Diet in Early Homo: A Review of the Evidence and a New Model of
Adaptive Versatility
Peter S. Ungar, Frederick E. Grine, and Mark F. Teaford p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 209
Alcohol: Anthropological/Archaeological Perspectives
Michael Dietler p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 229
Obesity in Biocultural Perspective
Stanley J. Ulijaszek and Hayley Lofink p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 337
Food and Memory
Jon D. Holtzman p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 361
Contents
xi
Contents
ARI
13 August 2006
13:30
Old Wine, New Ethnographic Lexicography
Michael Silverstein p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 481
Persistent Hunger: Perspectives on Vulnerability, Famine, and Food
Security in Sub-Saharan Africa
Mamadou Baro and Tara F. Deubel p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 521
Indexes
Subject Index p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 539
Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2006.35:379-406. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org
by UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA LIBRARY on 09/21/06. For personal use only.
Cumulative Index of Contributing Authors, Volumes 27–35 p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 553
Cumulative Index of Chapter Titles, Volumes 27–35 p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p p 556
Errata
An online log of corrections to Annual Review of Anthropology chapters (if any, 1997 to
the present) may be found at http://anthro.annualreviews.org/errata.shtml
xii
Contents