* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download [pdf]
Human brain wikipedia , lookup
Convolutional neural network wikipedia , lookup
Neural oscillation wikipedia , lookup
Holonomic brain theory wikipedia , lookup
Neuroethology wikipedia , lookup
Clinical neurochemistry wikipedia , lookup
Neural coding wikipedia , lookup
Visual search wikipedia , lookup
Cortical cooling wikipedia , lookup
Brain Rules wikipedia , lookup
Artificial general intelligence wikipedia , lookup
Recurrent neural network wikipedia , lookup
Types of artificial neural networks wikipedia , lookup
Evolution of human intelligence wikipedia , lookup
Binding problem wikipedia , lookup
Neurophilosophy wikipedia , lookup
Optogenetics wikipedia , lookup
Synaptic gating wikipedia , lookup
Neuroplasticity wikipedia , lookup
Time perception wikipedia , lookup
Artificial intelligence for video surveillance wikipedia , lookup
Aging brain wikipedia , lookup
Cognitive neuroscience wikipedia , lookup
Neuroanatomy wikipedia , lookup
Channelrhodopsin wikipedia , lookup
C1 and P1 (neuroscience) wikipedia , lookup
Neuroeconomics wikipedia , lookup
Functional magnetic resonance imaging wikipedia , lookup
Neural engineering wikipedia , lookup
Affective neuroscience wikipedia , lookup
Embodied cognitive science wikipedia , lookup
Nervous system network models wikipedia , lookup
Neuropsychopharmacology wikipedia , lookup
Visual selective attention in dementia wikipedia , lookup
Neural correlates of consciousness wikipedia , lookup
Development of the nervous system wikipedia , lookup
Metastability in the brain wikipedia , lookup
Feature detection (nervous system) wikipedia , lookup
Efficient coding hypothesis wikipedia , lookup
Spotlights moral disgust is felt when people judge others to have moved downward on that vertical dimension. Moral disgust is different from anger. People associate cheating, stealing, and most matters of harm and fairness more closely with anger (and angry faces) than with disgust (and disgust faces) [8]. Secular westerners have gradually lost touch with the ethics of divinity, shrinking the moral domain mostly to what Shweder called ‘the ethics of autonomy’ [6], but disgust and divinity concerns still play a powerful role in many political controversies, from abortion and euthanasia to gay marriage and flag burning [7]. TKLD study only harm-fairness actions that elicit primarily anger. Their ‘three domains of disgust’ scale [9] asks about actions such as cheating on an exam or stealing a candy bar. TKLD seem to ignore entirely the domain that Shweder called ‘the ethics of divinity’ [6]. It is true that people will sometimes use the word ‘disgust’ in response to actions that are harmful or unfair, and it is surely the case that people vary in their willingness to apply the word ‘disgust’ to violations of the ethics of autonomy. However, it is not clear whether this variation reveals anything deep about the emotion of disgust or whether it reveals only variations in linguistic usage. After reducing the moral domain to issues of harm and fairness, TLKD proceed to propose a computational account of moral disgust that seems to miss most of what is distinctive about disgust. TLKD say that disgust intuitions ‘serve as input to systems that judge the strategic value of endorsing a rule’. Disgust helps people to navigate the ‘landscape of condemnation’ and to avoid blame. Many of the moral rules and boundaries of daily life have nothing to do with disgust; they are guarded much more closely by anger. It is unclear why TLKD focus on disgust rather than anger (and the fear of other people’s anger) as the emotion that was shaped by the need to navigate many of the complexities of everyday negotiations over fairness, rights, and harms. TLKD have done the field a great service by pushing biological evolution further than anyone before them. They Trends in Cognitive Sciences August 2013, Vol. 17, No. 8 are the first authors to seriously consider the computational mechanisms involved in the detection and evaluation of disgust-related risks. However, we can see no reason to ignore cultural evolution and symbolic processes. TLKD achieved greater parsimony, referring only to biological evolution, than RHM, but at the cost of surrendering much that is distinctive about disgust and about the culturally variable human experience of disgust. Cultural and biological evolution often work together and employ the same evolutionary principles (variation, transmission, natural selection). They should not be seen as providing mutually exclusive views. We urge an integration [10]. References 1 Rozin, P. et al. (2008) Disgust. In Handbook of Emotions (3rd edn) (Lewis, M. and Haviland, J., eds), pp. 757–776, Guilford 2 Tybur, J.M. et al. (2012) Disgust: evolved function and structure. Psychol. Rev. 120, 65–84 3 Elias, N. (1978) The Civilizing Process, Vol. I: The History of Manners. (Jephcott, E. trans.)Pantheon Books 4 Haidt, J. et al. (1997) Body, psyche, and culture: the relationship between disgust and morality. Psychol. Dev. Soc. 9, 107–131 5 Becker, E. (1973) The Denial of Death. The Free Press 6 Shweder, R.A. et al. (1997) The ‘big three’ of morality (autonomy, community, and divinity), and the ‘big three’ explanations of suffering. In Morality and Health (Brandt, A. and Rozin, P., eds), pp. 119–169, Routledge 7 Haidt, J. (2012) The Righteous Mind: Why Good People are Divided by Politics and Religion. Pantheon 8 Rozin, P. et al. (1999) The CAD triad hypothesis: a mapping between three moral emotions (contempt, anger, disgust) and three moral codes (community, autonomy, divinity). J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 76, 574–586 9 Tybur, J.M. et al. (2009) Microbes, mating, and morality: Individual differences in three functional domains of disgust. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 97, 103–122 10 Rozin, P. (2010) Evolutionary and cultural psychology: complementing each other in the study of culture and cultural evolution. In Evolution, Culture, and the Human Mind (Schaller, M. et al., eds), pp. 9–22, Psychology Press 1364-6613/$ – see front matter ß 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.001 Trends in Cognitive Sciences, August 2013, Vol. 17, No. 8 Attention flexibly alters tuning for object categories Jiye G. Kim1 and Sabine Kastner1,2 1 2 Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ 08544, USA Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ 08544, USA Using functional MRI (fMRI) and a sophisticated forward encoding and decoding approach across the cortical surface, a new study examines how attention alternates tuning functions across a large set of semantic categories. The results suggest a dynamic attention mechanism that allocates greater resources to the attended and related semantic categories at the expense of unattended ones. Natural environments contain many objects that cannot all be processed simultaneously due to capacity limitations of the visual system. Therefore, flexible mechanisms are Corresponding author: Kastner, S. ([email protected]). 368 needed to selectively prioritize information that is relevant to ongoing behavior at the expense of irrelevant distracting information. This selection process is often referred to as ‘attention’. A variety of attention-related modulatory effects on neural processing across the visual system have been demonstrated, such as increases in baseline activity [1], increases in response gain of neurons that selectively respond to an attended feature or location [2,3], as well as shifts of neuronal tuning curves, for example, changes in preferred feature selectivity [4]. Although these studies have shaped initial understanding of attention mechanisms, they have been limited to examining neural responses evoked predominantly by synthetic stimuli, such Spotlights as gratings or oriented bars, which have been shown to elicit different response patterns than those obtained during natural vision [4,5]. It is not yet clear whether attention mechanisms revealed using synthetic stimuli generalize to a more complex, but ecologically valid context that is characteristic for natural vision, such as looking for a car when crossing a street. Recent fMRI studies have begun to examine the neural basis of attentional selection used in naturalistic vision by probing subjects’ performance in tasks that involve categorical detection of objects embedded in scenes [6–8]. These studies have demonstrated that category-based attention biases the processing of an attended target stimulus category, even in the absence of visual stimulation [7], which facilitates and predicts behavioral search performance to the attended target category [7,8]. However, the category space probed thus far has been restricted to a small set of objects, which does not lend to the vast number of objects that people are able to recognize and discriminate in daily life. How do attentional mechanisms operate across the representations that constitute the large object space that people are able to perceive? A new fMRI study by Çukur and colleagues [9] utilizes a recently reported continuously mapped semantic space of a large (1000) set of object and action categories across the whole brain [10] to investigate how attention mechanisms operate on such a large object space during naturalistic visual conditions. First, using a forward modeling approach, a category tuning function was estimated for each individual voxel (a volumetric unit of fMRI measurement), while subjects passively viewed natural movies. A single voxel measures a small portion of cortex at the millimeter scale. As a voxel contains many thousands of neurons with varying response properties, there are likely several different subpopulations of neurons that respond selectively to different semantic categories within this unit. A voxel’s tuning function was estimated as the gradient of category selectivity across the large set of object categories for which the voxel’s response could be most reliably predicted. Second, the modulations in voxel tunings were probed while subjects covertly attended to ‘vehicles’ or ‘humans’ in a movie search task. Attending to a particular category, say humans, shifted the category representations across a large proportion of cortical voxels distributed across the brain towards the ‘human’ representation, even in the absence of the target object. This change in category representation was observed for voxels that were initially tuned to the semantically related categories (e.g., representations for ‘body parts’ and ‘animals’ became more ‘human’-like), as well as for those voxels initially tuned to the unattended and semantically unrelated categories (e.g., representations of ‘vehicles’ became less ‘vehicle’like). This finding strongly suggests that category-based attention alters the category selectivity of cortical voxels towards the behaviorally relevant category at the expense of unattended or behaviorally irrelevant categories. Thus category-based attention appears to orchestrate a large network of cortical representations and the results of this study highlight the flexible and adaptive nature of attentional modulations across the whole brain. Because a voxel is not a biological, but rather a technical unit, a critical unresolved issue is to reveal the neural Trends in Cognitive Sciences August 2013, Vol. 17, No. 8 mechanisms by which the changes in category selectivity across object space are mediated. The authors argue that the observed modulations are a consequence of shifts in categorical tuning functions that are not related to an additive or multiplicative change of neural responses within a voxel. However, a tuning change at the voxel-level could be mediated by selective response gain operating differentially on subpopulations of neurons contained in a voxel. For example, consider a voxel composed of a large subpopulation of neurons tuned to humans and a small subpopulation of neurons tuned to vehicles. This voxel would be labeled as a human-preferring voxel. When attention is directed towards humans, a response gain of only those neurons tuned to humans and suppression of those neurons tuned to vehicles could manifest without necessarily changing the proportion of individual neurons’ preference or selectivity for the human and vehicle categories. At the pooled-voxel level, however, these changes in response gain and suppression would be manifested as an expansion of the representation of the human (attended) category and compression of the vehicle (unattended) category, which could account for the observed findings without assuming a neural mechanism that does not necessarily alter the selectivity of neurons responding to a preferred object category. If however, the neural mechanisms that underlie voxel-wise tuning shifts were mediated by shifts at the neural level, this would imply that attention allows changeable neural selectivity that arises from varying behavioral goals. Çukur et al.’s findings provide evidence for a potential large-scale neural operation by which attention overcomes capacity limitations to prioritize processing of attended and conceptually-related categories. This study sets the stage for future research in several important ways. First, how does attention influence object category tunings when the object space itself is changing? Daily experience is such that visual representations are vastly adapted to contextual information both in terms of visuo-spatial context (e.g., the representation of vehicles when other semantically related objects are present vs when semantically unrelated objects are present) and temporal context (e.g., the representation of vehicles when attending to humans shortly after attending to vehicles vs after continually attending to humans). Second, what are the functional dissociations between different brain regions (e.g., sensory vs higher order cortex) that mediate the attentional modulations in category tuning functions? And finally, how do these changes relate to behavioral outcome? Given that a large proportion of the brain exhibits significant attentional tuning changes, it is highly unlikely that all of these areas have redundant functional roles. How distinct brain regions work collectively to achieve a common overarching goal and how this guides and relates to efficient behavioral performance, such as greater detection sensitivity or better memory for attended and semantically related objects, will be exciting questions for future research. References 1 Kastner, S. et al. (1999) Increased activity in human visual cortex during directed attention in the absence of visual stimulation. Neuron 22, 751–761 369 Spotlights 2 Desimone, R. and Duncan, J. (1995) Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 193–222 3 Maunsell, J.H.R. and Treue, S. (2006) Feature-based attention in visual cortex. Trends Neurosci. 29, 317–322 4 David, S.V. et al. (2008) Attention to stimulus features shifts spectral tuning of V4 neurons during natural vision. Neuron 59, 509–521 5 Hasson, U. et al. (2009) Reliability of cortical activity during natural stimulation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 40–48 6 O’Craven, K.M. et al. (1999) FMRI evidence for objects as the units of attentional selection. Nature 401, 584–587 7 Peelen, M.V. and Kastner, S. (2011) A neural basis for real-world visual search in human occipitotemporal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 12125–12130 370 Trends in Cognitive Sciences August 2013, Vol. 17, No. 8 8 Seidl, K.N. et al. (2012) Neural evidence for distracter suppression during visual search in real-world scenes. J. Neurosci. 32, 11812– 11819 9 Çukur, T. et al. (2013) Attention during natural vision warps semantic representation across the human brain. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 763–770 10 Huth, A.G. et al. (2013) A continuous semantic space describes the representation of thousands of object and action categories across the human brain. Neuron 76, 1210–1224 1364-6613/$ – see front matter ß 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.05.006 Trends in Cognitive Sciences, August 2013, Vol. 17, No. 8