* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download In cognitive neuroscience, the prefrontal cortex represents a kind of
Subventricular zone wikipedia , lookup
Environmental enrichment wikipedia , lookup
Central pattern generator wikipedia , lookup
Eyeblink conditioning wikipedia , lookup
Cognitive neuroscience wikipedia , lookup
Nervous system network models wikipedia , lookup
Multielectrode array wikipedia , lookup
Clinical neurochemistry wikipedia , lookup
Metastability in the brain wikipedia , lookup
Binding problem wikipedia , lookup
Holonomic brain theory wikipedia , lookup
Embodied cognitive science wikipedia , lookup
Emotional lateralization wikipedia , lookup
Premovement neuronal activity wikipedia , lookup
Cognitive neuroscience of music wikipedia , lookup
Development of the nervous system wikipedia , lookup
Neuroanatomy wikipedia , lookup
Neuroeconomics wikipedia , lookup
Neural coding wikipedia , lookup
Neuroesthetics wikipedia , lookup
Visual selective attention in dementia wikipedia , lookup
Stimulus (physiology) wikipedia , lookup
C1 and P1 (neuroscience) wikipedia , lookup
Neuropsychopharmacology wikipedia , lookup
Time perception wikipedia , lookup
Optogenetics wikipedia , lookup
Executive functions wikipedia , lookup
Neural correlates of consciousness wikipedia , lookup
Efficient coding hypothesis wikipedia , lookup
Synaptic gating wikipedia , lookup
Prefrontal cortex wikipedia , lookup
Channelrhodopsin wikipedia , lookup
Bart Moore Cognitive Neuroscience Essay 2 Integration and Segregation in Prefrontal Cortex In cognitive neuroscience, the prefrontal cortex (PFC) represents a kind of final frontier as debate about the anatomical and functional organization is played out. Current developments are reminiscent of questions raised by Gall’s phrenology: to what degree is function segregated in the brain, and what functions are distributed where? We can imagine that if there is widespread segregation of function in PFC, it could be along lines drawn by the nature of the information being processed, or the function being subserved, or some mix of the two. Fortunately, modern scientists posses a plethora of advanced investigative anatomical and physiological techniques that have been brought to bear on the issue of segregation of function in the prefrontal cortices, and this essay will examine two primary articles: one supporting the theory that PFC possesses anatomical and functional subdivisions consistent with a segregation of information streams, and another paper supporting the theory that PFC primarily serves to integrate information from other cortical areas. Support for Segregation of Function in PFC In extrastriate cortex, it is widely recognized that there are two separate processing streams, loosely described as the ‘what’ and ‘where’ processing streams. While regions in the superior parietal cortices selectively respond to visuo-spatial stimuli, the inferior parietal and temporal regions are more selective for objects. O’Scalaidhe, et al. (1997) report a study involving electrophysiological recordings collected from macaque monkey PFC and propose that the ventral ‘what’ stream of the visual cortex extends forward to PFC. The authors found 46 ‘face selective’ cells in the inferior PFC in a highly circumscribed region supporting theories proposing a segregation of prefrontal function by the type of sensory information. The authors examined hundreds of cells in all areas of PFC and found face selective cells in only in ventral areas during a passive viewing condition. Two types of faceselective cells were observed: onset and offset. Onset cells began firing when face stimuli were presented and persisted in firing for some time thereafter. Offset cells began firing when the face stimuli were removed and continued firing for several seconds. The authors found that the vast majority of cells were found in areas of PFC which receive input from the temporal lobe visual areas, certainly supporting the idea of a PFC extension to the ventral ‘what’ stream, and the idea of compartmentalization of function in PFC. To their credit, the authors injected dyes at the termination of cellular recordings in two monkeys and found projections to that site originating in the superior temporal sulcus and inferior temporal gyrus. These are both regions which have previously been implicated in visual face processing. Support for Integration in the PFC Perhaps, though, simply using the types of stimuli suitable for probing response properties of neurons in sensory areas is not adequate for investigation in PFC. Indeed, if one is to examine PFC for evidence of information integration, shouldn’t a suitable experimental paradigm be used? Fortunately, Rao et al. have conducted a similar experiment in macaque PFC which engaged both ‘what’ and ‘where’ working memory. They recorded from 195 neurons in ‘lateral’ PFC, but unfortunately do not provide much more information regarding the specific location of recording sites. The monkey was instructed to fixate on a fixation spot while a pictorial stimulus was presented. After a delay, two objects were presented at two of four positions flanking the test spot. One of the objects matched the initial object, and after they were removed, the monkey was instructed to make a saccade to the remembered location of the object matching the initial stimulus. Thus there were two periods in which memory was required: after the initial presentation of the primary stimulus, and after the presentation of the flanking ‘test’ stimuli. Furthermore, the type of information required to be retained during those two periods differed: during the first period, the monkey had to remember what picture was presented, while during the second delay the monkey was required to remember where the object was presented. Of the 195 neurons recorded, 123 showed significant activity during the ‘what’ delay, the ‘where’ delay, or both. Of those 123 PFC neurons, 7% showed delay activity that was significantly tuned to the ‘what’ delay period, while 41% showed activity that was selectively tuned to the ‘where’ delay period. These results alone would not be surprising, and could support a segregation of what and where visual processing streams even if their location distribution sets overlapped. What is remarkable is that over half (64/123) neurons showed significantly increased activity during both the ‘what’ delay and the ‘where’ delay, implicating them in both object and spatial working memory. The authors claim that these ‘what/where’ cells may contribute to the integration of information regarding visual object identity and spatial location. Interestingly, the ‘what/where’ cells were found in both ventral and dorsal frontal areas. The ‘what’ and ‘where’ cells likewise showed no anatomical clustering or segregation. These results are in contrast to those of O’Scalaidhe, et al. (1997), who report a strong clustering of face-specific cells in the inferior PFC. These two studies provide some fodder for each camp in the debate about localization of function in the prefrontal lobe. The quality of the data presented is hard to contest, and thus some reconciliation between the strong stances of the two sets of authors must take place. In truth, the prefrontal lobe is comprised of billions of neurons and seems somewhat different from the other lobes in that it is involved in executive control and the genesis of action, both of which require convergent input from different sensory streams to be useful. Attempting to wholly carve the frontal lobe into discreet functional areas analogous to those in the occipital lobe might prove futile, but there do appear to be some specialized areas such as Brocha’s area and the frontal eye fields, and even perhaps an area specialized for complex objects like faces, as suggested by the data presented by O’Scalaidhe et al. Future Directions Using fMRI to investigate the distribution of information processing in prefrontal cortex would appear to be a viable way to carry the debate to a higher level, and no doubt can address some questions about the gross architecture. fMRI lacks the resolution, however to detect subtle concentrations of neurons such as those presented by O’Scalaidhe, et al. The same is true for extracranial electrodes, and probably local field electrodes as well. Are we doomed to fish with our electrode poles forever in the prefrontal cortex in order to map its functional organization? Let us hope not! One of the most promising avenues of research with regard to this debate is anatomical. Indeed, one of the strongest pieces of evidence presented by O’Scalaidhe et al. is that dyes injected into the site of face-specific frontal cells outlined (presumably feedforward) projections from temporal lobe. Unraveling the major anatomical projections into the PFC will be highly informative for physiologists and should help guide the ‘poles’ of single-cell electrophysiologists. In specific one possible avenue to be explored would be to conduct an experiment complimentary to that of O’Scalaidhe. Instead of looking for neurons that responded to complex objects like faces, however, one could look for neurons selective for visuo-spatial information. It would be interesting if such a distribution of neurons were found clustered in a ventral prefrontal area, and would suggest that the theory of continuation of the ‘what’ and ‘where’ visual processing streams is indeed present in the posterior frontal lobe. This notion could possibly be confirmed by again injecting dyes into the sites of putative spatially selective cells and examining the brain for traces of the dye in ventral parietal areas.