* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Functional circuitry underlying visual neglect
Activity-dependent plasticity wikipedia , lookup
Neuropsychology wikipedia , lookup
History of neuroimaging wikipedia , lookup
Visual search wikipedia , lookup
Synaptic gating wikipedia , lookup
Affective neuroscience wikipedia , lookup
Brain Rules wikipedia , lookup
Cognitive neuroscience wikipedia , lookup
Holonomic brain theory wikipedia , lookup
Premovement neuronal activity wikipedia , lookup
Visual selective attention in dementia wikipedia , lookup
Environmental enrichment wikipedia , lookup
Neuropsychopharmacology wikipedia , lookup
Emotional lateralization wikipedia , lookup
Metastability in the brain wikipedia , lookup
Cognitive neuroscience of music wikipedia , lookup
Neuroanatomy of memory wikipedia , lookup
Neuroeconomics wikipedia , lookup
Human brain wikipedia , lookup
Time perception wikipedia , lookup
Neuroplasticity wikipedia , lookup
Aging brain wikipedia , lookup
Eyeblink conditioning wikipedia , lookup
Visual extinction wikipedia , lookup
C1 and P1 (neuroscience) wikipedia , lookup
Neural correlates of consciousness wikipedia , lookup
Neuroesthetics wikipedia , lookup
Feature detection (nervous system) wikipedia , lookup
Inferior temporal gyrus wikipedia , lookup
Cerebral cortex wikipedia , lookup
doi:10.1093/brain/awl140 Brain (2006), 129, 1803–1821 Functional circuitry underlying visual neglect R. Jarrett Rushmore,1 Antoni Valero-Cabre,1 Stephen G. Lomber,2 Claus C. Hilgetag3 and , Bertram R. Payne1 y 1 Laboratory of Cerebral Dynamics, Plasticity and Rehabilitation Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology, Boston University School of Medicine, Boston, MA, 2Centre for Brain and Mind, Departments of Physiology and Psychology, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada and 3School of Engineering and Science, International University Bremen, Bremen, Germany Correspondence to: R. Jarrett Rushmore, PhD, Laboratory of Cerebral Dynamics, Plasticity and Rehabilitation, Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology, Boston University School of Medicine, 700 Albany Street, W702, Boston, MA 02118, USA E-mail: [email protected] y The death of Dr Bertram Payne occurred during the preparation of this manuscript. Visuospatial neglect is a common neurological syndrome caused by unilateral brain damage to the posterior and inferior parietal cerebral cortex, and is characterized by an inability to respond or orient to stimuli presented in the contralesional hemifield. Neglect has been elicited in experimental models of the rat, cat and monkey, and is thought to result in part from a pathological state of inhibition exerted on the damaged hemisphere by the hyperexcited intact hemisphere. We sought to test this theory by assessing neural activity levels in multiple brain structures during neglect using 2-deoxyglucose (2DG) as a metabolic marker of neural activity. Neglect was induced in two ways: (i) by cooling deactivation of posterior parietal cortex or (ii) in conjunction with broader cortical blindness produced by unilateral lesion of all contiguous visual cortical areas spanning occipital, parietal and temporal regions. The direction and magnitude of changes in 2DG uptake were measured in cerebral cortex and midbrain structures. Finally, the 2DG uptake was assessed in a group of cats in which the lesion-induced neglect component of blindness was cancelled by cooling of either the contralateral posterior parietal cortex or the contralateral superior colliculus (SC). Overall, we found that (i) both lesion- and cooling-induced neglect are associated with decreases in 2DG uptake in specific ipsilateral cortical and midbrain regions; (ii) levels of 2DG uptake in the intermediate and deep layers of the SC contralateral to both cooling and lesion deactivations are increased; (iii) changes in 2DG uptake were not identified in the contralateral cortex; and (iv) reversal of the lesion-induced neglect component of blindness is associated with a reduction of contralesional 2DG uptake to normal or subnormal levels. These data are in accord with theories of neglect that include mutually suppressive mechanisms between the two hemispheres, and we show that these mechanisms operate at the level of the SC, but are not apparent at the level of cortex. These results suggest that the most effective therapies for visual neglect will be those that act to decrease neural activity in the intermediate layers of the SC contralateral to the brain damage. Keywords: visuospatial neglect; superior colliculus; animal models; parietal cortex; visual perception Abbreviations: 2DG = 2-deoxyglucose; MSs = middle suprasylvian sulcus; pMS = posterior middle suprasylvian; SC = superior colliculus; SGI = stratum griseum intermediale; SGP = stratum griseum profundum; SGS = stratum griseum superficiale; SO = stratum opticum Received March 7, 2006. Revised April 24, 2006. Accepted April 26, 2006. Advance Access publication May 26, 2006 Introduction Visual neglect is a neurological syndrome most commonly caused by unilateral brain damage to the posterior and inferior parietal cerebral cortex and is characterized by an inability to detect or orient to stimuli or objects presented in the contralesional visual hemifield (Heilman and Valenstein, 1993; Mesulam, 1999; Driver and Vuilleumier, 2001; Kerkhoff, 2001; Mort et al., 2003). Patients with neglect behave as if one half of visual space does not exist and typically deny the presence of a defect. However, neglect is usually not a sensorial problem of absent or impoverished visual signals from the neglected field # The Author (2006). Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Guarantors of Brain. All rights reserved. For Permissions, please email: [email protected] 1804 Brain (2006), 129, 1803–1821 because a variety of treatments such as the application of cold water to the ear canal ipsilateral to the neglected field (‘caloric stimulation’) or vibration of the dorsal neck musculature causes the patient to once again become aware of stimuli in the neglected hemifield (Pierce and Buxbaum, 2002; Schindler et al., 2002; Kerkhoff, 2003). Such treatments for neglect have only transitory or incomplete efficacy. These phenomena inform us that sufficient neural circuitry exists both for detection of stimuli in the neglected hemifield and for producing orienting responses to the stimuli, but that the circuits are prevented from acting. As a consequence, a potential cure may be effected if the secondary circuits were identified and reactivated through interventional therapy or training. However, precise dissection of circuits in humans with neglect has proven difficult owing to lesion variations and the emergence of neural and behavioural compensations following neglect-inducing damage to posterior parietal cortex. The detailed study of neglect requires a stable experimental model of the syndrome. To that end, we have studied the cat model of neglect. Just as in humans, damage to the posterior and inferior portion of the cerebral cortex in the cat results in a syndrome with the same features as neglect (Hardy and Stein, 1988; Lomber and Payne, 1996; Payne et al., 1996a). Stable versions of neglect are induced either through unilateral cooling deactivation of posterior parietal cortex (Payne et al., 1996a) or in conjunction with the generation of a broader cortical hemianopia in which all contiguous visually responsive cortex is removed (Lomber et al., 2002). In the cat model, lesion and reversible deactivation studies have allowed for the generation of a hypothesis of neglect (Payne and Rushmore, 2004). This hypothesis suggests that neglect arises because of an imposed interhemispheric imbalance of the orienting system, which works to bias the system towards orienting to stimuli in the intact visual hemifield, and skews the system against stimuli presented in the neglected field. The neglect-inducing lesion or deactivation of posterior– inferior parietal cortex has been hypothesized to cause an imbalance in activity between brain hemispheres through imposed disruption of two main circuits. (i) Cortical circuits: The neglect-inducing injury or deactivation of posterior parietal cortex silences or denervates transcortical commissural projections to the homologous cerebral region. In a functional sense, the injury is thought to release the contralateral homologous parietal cortex from inhibition and result in an elevated level of activity—an excitability that then is translated along efferent projections to overactivate the cerebral and subcortical orienting circuitry in the intact hemisphere (Payne and Rushmore, 2004). (ii) Corticocollicular circuits: The neglect-inducing manipulation of parietal cortex removes a major source of input to the superficial visual layers of the superior colliculus (SC; Harting et al., 1992), an action that feeds forward and decreases excitability in efferent collicular circuits. Most notably, efferent and inhibitory projections to the contralateral SC become deactivated, resulting in a disinhibition of R. J. Rushmore et al. the contralateral SC, a heightened state of activity that feeds back to the ipsilateral SC as a higher inhibitory tone (Kinsbourne, 1974, 1987, 1993; Hilgetag et al., 1999; Payne and Rushmore, 2004). The end result is that the ipsilateral SC is deactivated more than would be expected than on the basis of simple denervation of parietal inputs, and the contralateral SC becomes more active than normal. Accordingly, our hypothesis is that the neglect-inducing lesion of parietal cortex induces interhemispheric asymmetry both in the cerebral cortex and in the SC. To address this hypothesis, we prepared three groups of cats. In one group (Group A), we induced neglect by unilateral cooling deactivation of parietal cortex. Cooling does not induce neural compensations, and a stable neglect state is generated for the duration of cooling (Lomber and Payne, 1996; Payne et al., 1996a). In another group (Group B), we made large unilateral visual cortex lesions. While small lesions of posterior parietal cortex produce only transient deficits in visual spatial attention (Payne et al., 1996b), lesions of all contiguous visual areas prevent intrinsic neuroplastic mechanisms from mediating recovery (Wallace et al., 1989). These cats displayed permanent visual neglect in conjunction with a broader cortical blindness. Finally, in a third group of cats with large unilateral visual cortical lesions (Group C), we reversed the neglect by cooling contralateral parietal cortex or the SC (Lomber et al., 2002). In all cats, we assessed the functional state of the brain by injecting radiolabelled 2deoxyglucose (2DG) as a measure of neural activity during the cooling-induced neglect (Group A), during the lesioninduced neglect (Group B) or during cooling-induced cancellation of the lesion-induced neglect (Group C). Our results have implications for understanding the syndrome of neglect, and for its treatment. Material and methods Animals and animal care All cats were purchased from a licensed cat breeder and were grouphoused in an enriched environment with a 12 h daylight cycle. Cats were provided with water ad libitum and were fed dry Purina Cat Chow for 1 h per day. Animals were treated according to the National Research Council’s Guidelines for the Care and Use of Mammals in Neuroscience and Behavioral Research (2003), and with the approval of the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the Boston University School of Medicine. Sixteen cats were used in this study. Ten cats were divided into three experimental groups: Group A: Unilateral cooling loops within the posterior middle suprasylvian (pMS) sulcus (n = 4). Group B: Unilateral lesion of all contiguous visual areas (n = 3). Group C: Unilateral lesion of all contiguous visual areas with (i) contralateral pMS cooling loop (n = 2); (ii) contralateral SC cooling loop (n = 1). Behavioural results for Group C have been reported previously (Lomber et al., 2002), and will be repeated here in textual form. Six additional cats were used as controls: four were unoperated and two additional cats underwent cooling of temporal cortex. This cooling Functional circuitry of neglect did not have an impact on contralateral structures, and the contracooled hemispheres were included as control data. [Control group same as in Rushmore et al. (2005)]. Cooling loop construction Cooling loops were constructed from 23 gauge hypodermic tubing (Small Parts, Inc., Miami Lakes, FL, USA) and shaped to conform to the contour of the pMS sulcal cortex or the surface of the SC (Lomber et al., 1999, 2001). A microthermocouple was constructed by soldering the bared ends of twisted copper/constantine wire (30 gauge, Teflon insulated) to the union of the loop using silver solder. The hypodermic tubing was trimmed at the ends and led through a custom-made outside-threaded stainless steel length of tubing. The microthermocouple wire was passed to a thermocouple miniature connector. Dental acrylic was used to hold the hypodermic tubing and the thermocouple wires together, and a threaded cap was screwed on the outside-threaded tubing for protection against inadvertent damage to the hypodermic tubing. Each loop was tested for adequate flow rates and for proper microthermocouple function before sterilization with ethylene oxide. Brain (2006), 129, 1803–1821 1805 was used to remove the marginal and suprasylvian gyri to A-P 14, the posterolateral and posterior suprasylvian gyri in their entirety and the cortex in the middle and posterior limbs of the suprasylvian sulcus. Gelatin surgical sponges (Ethicon, Somerville, NJ, USA) were placed in the cortical defect to assist haemostasis and the bone piece was replaced. Closing and postoperative care Muscle and skin planes were re-approximated and sutured with chrome gut and silk, respectively. Lidocaine (1%) was injected locally into skin and muscle before suturing. Cats were placed in a recovery cage with heating pads and were closely monitored. Animals were given fluids (2.5% dextrose in half-strength lactated Ringer solution, s.c.) and antibiotic (Ambipen, 300 000 units, i.m.) was administered prophylactically. Buprenorphine (0.01 mg/kg, s.c.) was injected to reduce potential postoperative pain, and soft food and water was provided. Cats were monitored several times per day and sutures were removed 10 days after surgery. For cats with cooling loops, the margin of the implant was cleaned several times a week with saline and hydrogen peroxide, and topical triple antibiotic ointment was applied to guard against infection. Surgical procedures Surgery was performed to implant cooling loops, for unilateral removal of all contiguous visual cortical areas, or both. Surgery employed sterile surgical technique and was carried out in an aseptic environment. General procedures On the day before surgery, a catheter was inserted into the cephalic vein under ketamine sedation (10 mg/kg, i.m.). The next day, the cat was anaesthetized with sodium pentobarbital (25 mg/kg i.v., and to effect). Atropine sulphate (0.03 mg/kg, s.c.) was injected to decrease potential alimentary secretions, and dexamethasone sulphate (1.0 mg/kg, i.m.) was given to reduce potential swelling. Antibiotic ophthalmic ointment was applied to prevent corneal drying. The cat’s head was secured in a stereotaxic apparatus and centred in Horsley–Clarke coordinates (ReinosoSuarez, 1961). Core temperature, pulse rate, presence of reflexes and respiratory rate were monitored and anaesthesia adjusted as needed. Cooling loop implantation Stainless steel screws (Small Parts, Inc., Miami Lakes, FA, USA.) were tapped into the skull and covered with dental acrylic to provide a base to secure and attach the loop. A cooling loop was selected that best conformed to the individual brain contour and was held in place by dental acrylic. The dura was substituted with Gelfilm (Pharmacia & Upjohn, Kalamazoo, MI, USA), and the bone piece was replaced and covered with dental acrylic. Removal of contiguous visual areas Mannitol (1.5 g/kg in 25% aqueous solution; i.v.) was administered to harden the brain and make it more amenable to manipulation. A large unilateral craniotomy was made to expose the marginal, posterolateral, suprasylvian and ectosylvian gyri. Subpial aspiration Perimetry testing Perimetry testing was performed as described previously (Lomber and Payne, 1996; Payne et al., 1996a; Lomber et al., 2002). The perimetry testing apparatus consisted of a semicircular table (88 cm diameter) with a 30-cm-high wall enclosing the semicircle and holes in the floor at 15 intervals from left 90 to right 90 (Fig. 2A). All testing occurred at photopic illumination levels (85 cd/m2). Cats were initially trained to fixate a target introduced through the 0 hole (cynosure). The lateral canthi of the eyes were aligned with a line connecting the left 90 and right 90 holes. Upon correct fixation as deemed by the experimenter presenting the fixation target, the cat was allowed to proceed forward and claim a dry food reward at the cynosure. The cat was required to complete 50 consecutive trials to proceed with the next phase of the task. Any non-fixation behaviour in response to target presentation was not rewarded. Following completion of the fixation task, the full perimetry task was performed. The cat was required to fixate a target at the cynosure. Subsequently, a high-contrast stimulus was introduced through a peripheral opening. A correct response was scored if the cat broke fixation and quickly oriented to the peripheral stimulus. The cat was then allowed to approach the stimulus and was given a high-incentive wet food reward. A failure to detect the peripheral stimulus was signalled by movement of the cat forward to the cynosure, and the cat received a dry, low-incentive food reward. Stimuli were introduced throughout the visual field in a pseudorandom sequence equally balanced for eccentricity and visual hemispace. Slow orienting responses or scanning behaviours were scored as mistrials and not rewarded. Cats with lesions were tested 3 weeks postoperatively, and at 3-week intervals. No signs of spontaneous recovery were ever noted in any of the three cats. Lesion cats survived for 3 months after surgery to allow degenerative effects of the lesion to run to completion. Cats with lesion and cooling were tested as detailed in Lomber et al. (2002). 1806 Brain (2006), 129, 1803–1821 Cooling Following successful training, cats with cooling loops were acclimatized to wearing a harness connected via a tether to a stage over the perimetry arena. The stage held the cooling pump, a methanol bath and a methanol reservoir. For a cooling experiment, the protective cap was removed from the cooling loop and Teflon tubing was attached to the inlet and outlet of the cooling loop. The tubes were attached to the tether and connected to the cooling circuit. Cats were tested for freedom of movement in the arena, and a precooling set of trials was performed to twice sample every visual field location. Cooling was effected by circulating chilled methanol through the implanted cooling loop. Room temperature methanol was drawn from the methanol reservoir by a peristaltic pump, circulated through a cold methanol bath and pumped through the cooling loop. The outlet of the cooling loop led to the methanol reservoir. The temperature of the loop depended on the temperature of methanol in the cooling loop, the rate of flow of the methanol and the ability of the brain vasculature to counteract the cooling. Loop temperature was monitored and was changed to the desired temperature by adjusting flow rate. Typically, trials began 5 min after the start of cooling and every visual field position was sampled at least twice. After the cooling trials were over, the cooling pump was turned off, the coolant tubes were disconnected from the implant, the protective cap reapplied and 5 min allowed before post-cool (re-warm) control data were collected. 2DG administration and euthanasia For 5–8 days before final procedures, the cat was trained to remain calm for 30–45 min in a nylon veterinary cat sack. On the penultimate day, a venous catheter was inserted into the cephalic vein. On the day of the 2DG experiment, the cat was put in the nylon sack and the loop was cooled for 5 min to obtain a stable temperature of 1 6 3 C. The cat was then injected with four boluses of 2-deoxy-[14C]-glucose separated by 5 min (i.v., total 2DG dosage = 100 mCi/kg; specific activity = 310 mCi/mmol) (Vanduffel et al., 1995, 1997, 1998; Rushmore and Payne, 2003; Rushmore et al., 2005). Fifteen minutes later, cats were injected with the anticoagulant heparin (1000 units, i.v.) and the vasodilator sodium nitrite (1% w/v, i.v.), and finally deeply anaesthetized with sodium pentobarbital (65 mg/kg, i.v.). Intact cats and cats with lesions underwent the same procedure without the presence of cooling. The vascular system of the cat was perfused through the heart with a flush solution [15% sucrose in 0.1 M phosphate buffer (PB), pH = 7.4] for 1 min, followed by a fixative solution (2% paraformaldehyde and 15% sucrose in 0.1 M PB, pH = 7.4) for 5 min. Cooling was discontinued when fixative had entered the brain. The brain was quickly removed, blocked and photographed. It was covered with albumin and submerged in a 30 C bath of 2-methylbutane for 30 min. The brain was then transferred to a 80 C freezer until cutting. Histological procedures Brains were cut into 23-mm thick coronal sections using a Bright OTF cryostat (Hacker Instruments, Inc., Fairfield, NJ, USA) that was set at 23 C. Every fifth section was picked up with a 0.1% gelatin-chrome alum subbed coverslip, heated to 60 C and glued to Bristol board. Bristol boards with subbed coverslips applied to them were apposed to high-resolution X-ray film (Structurix D7, Agfa, R. J. Rushmore et al. Mortsel, Belgium) along with 14C calibration strips (Amersham, Piscataway, NJ, USA) and exposed at 80 C for 9 days. Films were developed for 10 min (Kodak D19 Developer at 5 C) and fixed (Kodak Fixer) in red light. Sections were transilluminated and digitized (MCID, Imaging Research, Ste. Catherines, Ontario). For each film, points in the calibration curve were joined by linear interpolation and extended with linear extrapolation. Care was taken not to over-expose or approach the saturation level of the film. Distance was calibrated to each image by measuring the number of pixels per millimetre. Adjacent sections were collected with subbed slides and processed for cytochome oxidase (Wong-Riley, 1979), acetyl thiocholinesterase (Geneser-Jensen and Blackstad, 1971) myelin (Schmued, 1990) or stained for Nissl substance. These sections were used to demarcate extent of primary and secondary degenerative effects of lesion. Analysis Lesion/cooling reconstruction The extent of the cortical defect or of the cooling deactivation was charted onto stereotyped coronal sections through the cat visual cortex and transferred to a whole brain image (Rosenquist, 1985). Secondary independent measures of the lesion were obtained by an assessment of the retrograde degeneration of the lateral geniculate nucleus and of the lateral posterior thalamic nucleus. Degeneration was straightforward to assess because successful lesions induce a retrograde degeneration with profound gliosis and subsequent collapse of the lateral geniculate nucleus, lateral posterior and pulvinar nuclei. 2DG analysis Two main regions were sampled: the SC and the contralateral pMS cortex. Analysis of the SC was similar to previous approaches (Rushmore and Payne, 2003; Rushmore et al., 2005) and consisted of a mediolateral sampling of 2DG uptake in the following major collicular sublaminae: stratum griseum superficiale (SGS), the stratum opticum (SO), the stratum griseum intermediale (SGI) and the stratum griseum profundum (SGP) (Fig. 1A; Kanaseki and Sprague, 1974). Profiles for each collicular sublamina were obtained by sampling at the midline outside the SC at a point overlying the collicular commissure and then sequentially progressing into the particular sublamina. This approach confers the advantage that each 2DG profile is spatially calibrated and collicular profiles can be overlaid and compared regardless of hemisphere or animal. Also, an impact on specific visual field representation in the SC can be assessed, rather than be diluted by using a gross structural sampling approach. Moreover, this sampling strategy provided embedded control measures of 2DG uptake in extra-collicular sites, which did not change with condition. 2DG profiles from the middle SC were generated from sampling from the middle 10 coronal sections of the SC. For each section, the 2DG profiles were normalized to sectional white matter values (Sharp et al., 1983; Rushmore et al., 2005) and averaged across the 10 sections. This ratio approach controls for variations in glucose uptake between animals, and allowed for a control of intersectional variability. Uptake of 2DG in the contracooled pMS sulcus was measured in sections from approximately AP0 to A1 by sequential sampling (250 mm diameter circular samples) from the lateral crest of the posterior aspect of the middle suprasylvian sulcus (MSs) via the Functional circuitry of neglect Brain (2006), 129, 1803–1821 1807 Fig. 1 (A) Dorsocaudal view of the SC showing sampling strategy. The left SC shows the retinotopic map and anatomical arrangement: Lat: lateral, Med: medial; the dashed line represents the horizontal meridian and the dotted line represents the vertical meridian. The right SC has been cut away in the coronal plane to show the sampling strategy. For each section, sampling (sampling circle: 300 mm diameter) began on the midline, and outside of the SC. Sequential sampling from this midline point took the sampling into the different sublaminae and maintained spatial calibration. SGS: stratum griseum superficiale; SO: stratum opticum; SGI: stratum griseum intermediale; SGP: stratum griseum profundum. Figure modified from Rushmore et al. (2005). (B) A dorsolateral view of the cat cerebrum (left) showing the demarcation of visual cortical areas (17: Area 17; 18: Area 18; 19: Area 18; 21: Area 21; 20: Area 20) and of select anatomical landmarks (MSg: middle suprasylvian gyrus; MSs: middle suprasylvian sulcus; pESg: posterior ectosylvian gyrus). Also shown (middle) is a higher magnification and unfolded view of the posterior terminus of the MSs (middle), illustrating the posterior aspect of the middle suprasylvian region (pMS). The right portion shows a further inset showing the sampling strategy in which 250 mm samples were sequentially collected from the crest of the lateral bank (#1) to the crest of the medial bank (#29). Other abbreviations: M: Medial; A: anterior; P: posterior; L: lateral; D: dorsal. fundus to the medial crest, in superficial, middle and deep layers (Fig. 1B). Data from each section were normalized to the white matter average. The average 2DG profiles for each animal were generated from 10 sections. Significant results between relative 2DG uptake in each neglect group and the normative group were qualified at a P < 0.05 level using non-parametric Mann–Whitney U-tests. Upon comparison of two cases with pMS cooling opposite lesion, we assigned significance at a P < 0.1 level based on the limited sample size. In one instance, we compared the 2DG uptake profile of normal cats with a single case of unilateral SC deactivation opposite a cortical lesion; since comparison of groups was not possible here, we assigned significance when the 2DG uptake values from the deactivation/ lesion case exceeded 0.0001 confidence intervals of the control mean. Results Group A: neglect induced by cooling Behaviour All four cats demonstrated proficient behaviour in the perimetry task when the cortex was warm. During unilateral cooling of the pMS loop to 0 6 3 C, cats neglected stimuli moved into the contracooled visual hemispace. Performance in the ipsicooled visual hemispace was unaffected by cooling (e.g. Fig. 2B). These results were comparable with earlier studies using identical techniques (Lomber and Payne, 1996; Payne et al., 1996a). Deactivation Coronal 2DG radiographs exhibited a decrease in 2DG uptake in the cooled pMS cortex in the vicinity of the loop (Fig. 2C). The extent of deactivation varied from cat to cat but always included the medial bank of the pMS sulcus. In most cats, the effects of cooling was observed to flow dorsally and medially and resulted in diminished 2DG uptake in various portions of the middle suprasylvian gyrus (MSg) (Fig. 3). This has been previously reported following pMS cooling (Lomber et al., 1999; Lomber and Payne, 2001) and represents an export of the cooling by vascular elements. In two of the cats, the loop caused damage to the pMS cortex (Fig. 3B, iii, iv, black). However, in both instances, the cats were able to perform the orienting task perfectly in the absence of cooling, and cooling deactivation selectively induced deficits in the visual field contralateral to the loop. The patterns of 2DG uptake, therefore, represent the neglect state independent of local damage to the cortex. 1808 Brain (2006), 129, 1803–1821 R. J. Rushmore et al. Fig. 2 The impact of unilateral cooling deactivation of pMS cortex on the ability to detect and orient to moving stimuli in the contracooled hemifield. (A) The perimetry task. Cats were trained to fixate (A ii) a stimulus (A i) appearing at the 0 position. Upon successful fixation, a high-contrast stimulus (B i) appeared at a position in either the left or the right visual hemifield. A correct response was recorded if the cat oriented to the second stimulus (B ii). A soft food reward was given for a correct orienting response. An incorrect response was recorded if the cat did not orient to the stimulus and instead moved ahead to the fixation point (A i)—in this case, the cat received a dry, low-incentive food reward. Figure modified from Lomber and Payne (1996). (B) Representative performance of one cat on the perimetry task with cooling loop at normal brain temperature (left) and during cooling of the left pMS loop (right). The brain icons beneath the radial graphs represent the state of the pMS cooling loop. During normal brain temperature, the cat was highly proficient and successfully responded to all secondary stimuli in both visual hemifields. During cooling deactivation of the left pMS cooling loop (grey), the cat ceased to respond to introduction of the high-contrast stimulus to positions in the right, but not left, visual hemifield. (C) A 2DG radiograph showing the position of the cooling loop (black circles) in the left pMS sulcus (pMSs) and the associated deactivation of primarily the medial pMS bank. Notice the cooling was exported to the surface of the MSg. Functional circuitry of neglect Brain (2006), 129, 1803–1821 1809 Fig. 3 Reconstruction of deactivation extents in cats with cooling of pMS cortex. (A) A dorsolateral view of the cat cerebrum (left) and inset of cropped area with sulci opened to show the location of the pMS cortex. Medial: medial bank of the MSs; Lateral: lateral bank of the MSs. Scale bar represents 1 mm. (B) Deactivation extents from the four cats with unilateral cooling loops (i–iv). The extents of deactivation (grey) were taken from coronal 2DG radiographs and represent a 50% or greater decrease in 2DG uptake. Estimates from damage from the cooling loop are derived from Nissl-stained sections and are shown as black regions. Abbreviations as in Fig. 1. Functional impact of deactivation 1. Ipsilateral SC. The SGS and the SO of the SC receive projections from layer 5 neurons of the pMS sulcus (Segal and Beckstead, 1984; Harting et al., 1992). Accordingly, cooling silenced neurons in pMS sulcal cortex and resulted in a decrease in 2DG uptake in the ipsilateral SGS and SO layers (Fig. 4). This decrease was relatively localized and on the basis of the visual maps of the SC (Feldon and Kruger, 1970) roughly corresponded to the representations of the area centralis and horizontal meridian, a finding concordant with the retinotopy of cooled pMS cortex (Palmer et al., 1978; Rosenquist, 1985). Thus, as in previous studies (Rushmore and Payne, 2003; Rushmore et al., 2005) the focal decrease in 2DG uptake in the SC was probably communicated via corticotectal projections from the deactivated cortex. The pMS cortex sends more limited projections to the ipsilateral intermediate grey level (SGI) of the SC, and no projections to the deep grey lamina (SGP; Harting et al., 1992). Correspondingly, these layers were largely unaffected by cooling (Fig. 4). 2. Contralateral SC. The contralateral SGS was unaffected by cooling of the pMS sulcus, whereas the SO exhibited an increase in 2DG uptake. 2DG uptake levels in the contralateral SGI and SGP were also significantly higher than normal levels (Fig. 4). 3. Contralateral pMS cortex. Cooling of pMS cortex did not consistently alter 2DG uptake within any layer of the contralateral pMS cortex (Fig. 5). In the middle layers, two points of significant (P < 0.05) deviation from the intact mean were noted, but consistent statistically significant differences were not observed. Group B: neglect induced by large cortical lesions Behaviour All cats neglected stimuli in the contralesional visual field (Fig. 6A). Orienting to stimuli moved into the 1810 Brain (2006), 129, 1803–1821 R. J. Rushmore et al. Fig. 4 The impact of unilateral cooling deactivation of pMS cortex (grey) on 2DG uptake in the ipsilateral and contralateral superior colliculi. 2DG profiles for each collicular layer in the cooling group (filled symbols, n = 4) are compared with 2DG profiles of intact cats (open diamonds, n = 6). Measures began at a midline point (M) outside of the colliculus and extended laterally in 300 mm increments to maintain spatial calibration from the midline point. The approximate point at which sampling entered the SC laminae is represented by vertical lines. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean. Statistical significance is set at a P < 0.05 level and is denoted by asterisks. Abbreviations: SGS: stratum griseum superficiale; SO: stratum opticum; SGI: stratum griseum intermediale; SGP: stratum griseum profundum. Functional circuitry of neglect Brain (2006), 129, 1803–1821 1811 A. B. C. D. Fig. 6 (A) Representative perimetry behaviour following lesion. Behavioural performance was identical for all three subjects and cats did not respond to stimuli in the contralesional visual hemifield. (B) Lesion reconstructions for cats L1, L2 and L3—black represents the visual cortex that was removed by the ablation. Grey represents portions of visual cortex that were spared by the lesion. The star and coronal line in L1 represent approximate position of the radiograph in C. (C) A pseudocolour 2DG radiograph showing the midbrain and cerebral hemispheres from cat L1. The star represents the location of the lesion. Notice the difference between the two SC. Scale bar: 1 mm. ipsilesional visual hemifield was highly proficient. No signs of spontaneous recovery were noted in any of the three cats. Lesion Fig. 5 The impact of pMS cooling on 2DG uptake in the contralateral pMS cortex. (A) Representation of the sampling strategy applied to cortex (see Fig. 1): 250 mm samples were sequentially collected from the crest of the lateral bank (#1) to the crest of the medial bank (#29). Samples were collected from the superficial (B), middle (C) and deep (D) layers in cats with pMS cooling (filled squares, n = 4) and compared with values from intact cats (open circles, n = 6). Error bars represent the standard error of the mean. Asterisks denote significance level of P < 0.05. The completeness of each cortical lesion was estimated at the time of brain removal and the lesion borders were subsequently reconstructed from brain sections stained for Nissl substance (Fig. 6B). Also, secondary measures of the lesion size were assessed by examining retrograde degeneration in the thalamus. In all three cats, the vast majority of intended cortex was judged to be removed and the assessment was confirmed by massive retrograde degeneration in connected thalamic 1812 Brain (2006), 129, 1803–1821 nuclei leading to gliotic infiltration and nuclear collapse. The retrograde degeneration was also evident in 2DG radiographs, which showed a profound reduction in 2DG uptake within the LP/pulvinar complex. A variable amount of spared cortex was present in the most ventral visuotemporal region, which corresponded to neuronal sparing and preserved 2DG signal in the most medial fringe of the lateral posterior nucleus (Fig. 6B, grey). Overall, any cortical sparing was minimal, and is probably irrelevant for the study because none of the cats responded to stimuli presented in the visual hemispace contralateral to the lesion. Functional impact of lesion 1. Ipsilateral SC. The unilateral ablation of visual cortex resulted in a profound decrease in 2DG uptake in the superficial layers (SGS and SO) of the ipsilateral SC (Fig. 7, left). 2DG uptake values in the SGI were at normal levels. In the deep SGP layer, 2DG uptake was increased at mid-collicular levels. 2. Contralateral SC. The contralateral SO, SGI and SGP layers displayed heightened 2DG uptake values relative to control measures, but 2DG uptake values in the contralesional SGS were not different from control measures (Fig. 7, right). 3. Contralateral pMS cortex. 2DG profiles from the contralateral pMS cortex showed that the pMS cortex contralateral to a large lesion did not differ reliably from normal 2DG uptake profiles (Fig. 8). In the middle layers, two points on the lateral bank were significantly decreased relative to normal values. Group C: reversal of neglect by pMS deactivation opposite a large cortical lesion To this point, we have shown that the presence of neglect for contralateral stimuli, either through unilateral cooling deactivation of pMS cortex (Group A) or from a large unilateral cortical lesion (Group B), corresponds to a specific and bilateral pattern of changes in 2DG uptake in the layers of the superior colliculi (Fig. 9). The pattern was similar in sign and distribution, but not magnitude; the pattern after lesion produced a larger impact in both the ipsilateral and contralateral SC. Regions of the SC in receipt of projections from the lesioned cortex or the deactivated cortex show a decrease in 2DG uptake; these decreases represent the functional removal of efferent projections from the lesioned or deactivated cortex. However, changes observed in the contralateral SC were inconsistent with known direct projections of the resected or deactivated cortex. These changes included heightened 2DG uptake in the contralateral R. J. Rushmore et al. intermediate and deep layers of the SC. Since these alterations cannot result from direct synaptic influence of the deactivated or lesioned cortex, they must represent secondary impacts promulgated by the initial lesion or deactivation. These results are in general agreement with theoretical predictions that suggest that the induction of neglect influences crossed circuitry to produce a heightened activation of the contralateral SC (Hilgetag et al., 1999). To determine whether the heightened activation of the contralateral SC co-varies with the presence of neglect, large unilateral cortical lesions were made to induce the heightened activation of the contralesional SC and induce enduring hemineglect; in the same animals, cooling loops were implanted either in the contralateral pMS cortex (n = 2) or in contact with the contralateral SC (n = 1). The purpose of these loops was to reduce the activity of the SC contralateral to lesion, either directly in the case of the SC loop or indirectly by deactivating the large corticotectal projection from the pMS cortex. The behavioural results and lesion analysis have been reported previously (Lomber et al., 2002), and will be reproduced in textual form below. Behaviourally, cooling of the pMS loop to 8 C or the SC loop to a temperature of 12 C produced a restoration of orienting responses to stimuli in the formerly blind hemifield (Lomber et al., 2002), which lasted only for the duration of the cooling. The restoration of visual responses in the formerly neglected field disappeared and the neglect became reinstated upon the cessation of cooling (Lomber et al., 2002). For the final section of the results, we report on the patterns of activity in the SC of the three cats who had an initial lesion-induced neglect for stimuli that was cancelled by subsequent deactivation of either the contralateral pMS cortex or the contralateral SC. Analysis of 2DG uptake patterns in the SC of cats with restored orienting ability 1. Cooling of pMS cortex opposite a large visual cortex lesion. Deactivation of the pMS cortex opposite a large lesion of visual cortex resulted in a reinstatement of the ability to respond to stimuli in the formerly blind visual field. Analysis of the pattern of 2DG uptake in the SC during the reinstatement indicated that the intermediate and deep layers of the ipsilesional or the ipsicooled SC were not different from 2DG uptake in normal cats (Fig. 10). Thus, the addition of pMS cooling opposite lesion eliminated the lesion-induced heightened activations in the contralesional SO, SGI and SGP, and in the ipsilesional SGP. A further comparison between 2DG uptake levels in the SC of lesioned cats and lesioned cats with pMS cooling showed that the SO on the side of the lesion, but not the SGS or SGI, displayed a larger decrease in Functional circuitry of neglect Brain (2006), 129, 1803–1821 1813 Fig. 7 The impact of unilateral lesion (black) of all contiguous visual cortical areas on 2DG uptake in the ipsilateral and contralateral superior colliculi. 2DG profiles for each collicular layer in the lesion group (filled symbols, n = 3) are compared with 2DG profiles of intact cats (open diamonds, n = 6). Abbreviations and conventions as in Fig. 4. 2DG uptake than could be accounted for strictly by the impact of the lesion. This difference was not a result of different lesion extents, but a result of the interaction of the pMS cooling with the extant lesion effects. Thus, we conclude that the addition of pMS cooling caused a further decrease in 2DG uptake in the ipsilesional SO, a decrease that was superimposed on the lesion-induced decrease. In these cases, 2DG uptake in the SGI and the SGP on both sides were comparable both with each other and with levels measured in intact cats. Activity patterns in the SO and SGS were asymmetrical. 1814 Brain (2006), 129, 1803–1821 2. Cooling of SC opposite a large visual cortex lesion. Similar to the results of adding the contralateral deactivation of pMS cortex to lesion, cooling the SC opposite lesion enabled the animals to respond to stimuli in the contralesional hemifield. Also similar to the effects of the pMS cooling opposite lesion, the addition of the SC cooling eliminated any evidence of heightened activity levels in the contralesional SO, SGI and SGP, and in the ipsilesional SGP (Fig. 11). In addition, direct collicular cooling also induced changes in 2DG uptake throughout the colliculus that were superimposed on the effects of lesion per se. For instance, the decrease in 2DG uptake within the ipsilesional SGS and the SO was clearly accentuated when the SC was cooled opposite lesion. In addition, the ipsilesional SGI measured in lesion cats exhibited no difference from control cats, but displayed a decrease in 2DG uptake when the contralesional SC was cooled (Fig. 12). Finally, analysis of relative activity levels showed that SC cooling opposite lesion induced symmetry in the SO lamina, but symmetry was not present, and was even more accentuated in the intermediate (SGI) and deep (SGP) laminae. However, it should be noted that in these layers the asymmetry was between activity levels that were both below normal. In no cases with SC cooling opposite lesion was the activity higher than normal. Thus, it appears that symmetry between the intermediate and deep layers is not necessary for the re-establishment of visual orienting responses. R. J. Rushmore et al. A. B. C. Discussion Our results show that the presence of neglect, induced either through unilateral cooling of pMS cortex or in conjunction with cortical blindness, is associated with (i) a diminished uptake of 2DG within the superficial laminae of the ipsilateral SC and (ii) a heightened level of 2DG uptake in the intermediate and deeper layers of the contralateral SC. The concordance of collicular 2DG uptake patterns across the two separate groups of animals with similar visual defects lends credence to our claim that neglect is associated with alterations in activity at the level of the SC. Furthermore, when the heightened level of activity in the contralesional SC is diminished, either through deactivation of the colliculus itself or via deactivation of contralateral parietal cortex, the neglect was eliminated. These data indicate that alterations in circuitry in the SC is the major mechanism through which neglect is manifested and reversed and suggest a re-evaluation of current theories of hemispheric asymmetry in the pathogenesis of neglect and in the design and application of therapeutic strategies Neglect has been understood to be intricately tied to the function of the human cerebral cortex, and in particular, the function of the posterior parietal cortex. However, neglect is not exclusive to the human condition, nor exclusive to insult of the parietal cortical area. Multiple animal models of neglect exist, and visuospatial neglect is also observed following damage to the cingulate cortex, to the superior temporal Fig. 8 The impact of unilateral lesion of all contiguous visual areas on 2DG uptake in the superficial (A), middle (B) and deep (C) layers of the contralateral pMS cortex. Conventions and abbreviations as in Fig. 5. cortex or to the frontal eye field in most species studied (Mesulam, 1999; Karnath et al., 2004; Lomber and Payne., 2004; Payne and Rushmore, 2004). While the precise symptomology of neglect is likely to vary with cortical site of damage (Mesulam, 1999), the disparate distribution of these neglect-inducing foci suggests that neglect may result not solely from lesion-induced disruption of processing within these locations alone, but that the processing of a more distal and downstream site may serve as a final common structure in the manifestation of neglect. All regions whose disruption induces neglect maintain strong projections to the SC (Harting et al., 1992), and indeed, lesion of SC in humans also produces neglect-like symptoms (Weddell, 2004). As a consequence, the primary induction of neglect may result from a diaschetic impact of lesion along Functional circuitry of neglect Fig. 9 Summary of results from cats with neglect. (A) Behavioural results from cats with unilateral cooling of pMS sulcus (left) and cats with unilateral lesions of all contiguous visual cortical regions (right). (B) Schematic representation of cooling (grey, left) and lesion (black, right) cats. (C) Representation of impact of manipulation on 2DG uptake in SC. Note concordance of pattern, but not magnitude in the two preparations. (One arrow represents between 5 and 10% change from intact values, two arrows represent a 10–20% change, three arrows represent a 20–30% change, 0 represents no change). efferent projections to the midbrain. This would explain the convergence of symptoms from disparate cortical regions and would suggest that the SC functions as a final common structure mediating and/or exacerbating the effects of neglect. In this interpretation, permanent or reversible deactivation of parietal, frontal or cingulate cortical fields results in a condition of reduced excitability in the ipsilesional SC (Payne and Rushmore, 2004). This condition then initiates a disinhibition of the contralateral SC by reducing the activity of intertectal inhibitory fibres. Finally, the disinhibited contralateral SC exerts a higher inhibitory tone on the ipsilesional SC and inhibits it further (Hilgetag et al., 1999; Payne and Rushmore, 2004). Thus, the visual deficit observed after lesion or deactivation of cortex is a consequence of two factors: the removal of parietal cortex processing and the cascade of interactions arising from the subsequent disruption of collicular activity. The origin of these ideas lies in findings that a cortical lesion-induced visual deficit could be reversed by aspiration lesion of the contralateral SC or after division of the intertectal commissure (Sprague, 1966); to explain these results, Brain (2006), 129, 1803–1821 1815 Sprague suggested that the SC contralateral to lesion exerted a compounding inhibitory influence on the ipsilesional SC (Sprague, 1966, 1996). Other results suggested that inhibitory influences may arise from an extrinsic source whose axons passed through the superficial layers of the SC to access the intertectal commissure (Wallace et al., 1989). As such, ibotenic acid lesions of the SC, which damaged cells but not axons, were insufficient to reinstate behaviour in the heminanopic field, whereas adding a transection of the caudal intertectal commissure did restore visual orienting (Wallace et al., 1989). Subsequent studies identified the substantia nigra pars reticulata (SNpr; Wallace et al., 1990) and the pendunculopontine tegmentum (PPT; Durmer and Rosenquist, 2001) as providing anatomical sources of the inhibitory influence; ibotenic acid lesions of either structure were sufficient to restore visual orienting following a cortical lesion (Wallace et al., 1990; Durmer and Rosenquist, 2001). These findings cannot be easily brought to terms with the present behavioural and functional findings (Lomber et al., 2002; present results) and with anatomical results (Appell and Behan, 1990; Olivier et al., 1998; Takahashi et al., 2006). As such, the heightened activation of the contralateral SC may be due to the deactivation of a projection that courses in the intertectal commissure and arises from the ipsilesional SNpr (Harting et al., 1988; Jiang et al., 2003), the contralateral SC (Edwards, 1977; Olivier et al., 1998) or both. In any case, the requirements for involvement are a relatively tight coupling of activity with parietal cortex, a requirement that fits both proposed structures. Unfortunately, we could not examine the SNpr in the current study owing to methodological constraints: neurons of the nigrocollicular pathway are interspersed with other cell types (Jiang et al., 2003), and the presence of the perforating fibres of the cerebral peduncle makes accurate detection of changes difficult at best with the 2DG method. As a consequence, we cannot exclude or include an involvement of the substantia nigra as a component in the functional circuitry. Regardless, our data show that the presence of neglect is associated with a hyperexcitability in the contralesional SO, SGI and SGP layers of the SC, but not in the SGS layer. In addition, there does not seem to be any lesion-related inhibition of the ipsilateral SC apart from that expected following the functional removal of corticotectal projections; we expected a diminished 2DG uptake signal throughout SGI and SGP, but found none. Moreover, we found that by reducing the hyperexcitability of the contralesional collicular layers, either directly or by deactivating highly connected distant regions, the capacity to respond to moving stimuli in the contralesional visual field was restored. Unexpected findings in SC patterns after induction of visual deficits Previous investigation into the functional organization of this system suggested that the initial cortical lesion-induced 1816 Brain (2006), 129, 1803–1821 R. J. Rushmore et al. Fig. 10 The impact of adding cooling deactivation of pMS cortex (grey, right hemisphere) to a unilateral lesion of all contiguous visual areas (black, left hemisphere) on 2DG uptake in the SC. Open diamonds represent intact (n = 6) values and filled symbols represent cats with unilateral lesion of all contiguous visual areas combined with contralateral pMS cooling deactivation (n = 2; filled squares: ipsilesional, filled triangles: ipsicooled). Abbreviations and conventions as in Fig. 4. Significance for this comparison was set at P < 0.10 and denoted by ‘+’ symbols. Functional circuitry of neglect Brain (2006), 129, 1803–1821 1817 Lesion Fig. 11 The impact of adding cooling deactivation of SC (grey, right SC) to a unilateral lesion of all contiguous visual areas (black, left hemisphere) on 2DG uptake in the SC. Open symbols represent intact (n = 6) values and filled symbols represent a single case with unilateral lesion of all contiguous visual areas combined with contralateral SC cooling deactivation. Error bars represent 0.0001 confidence interval of the mean and · symbols indicate points in which values from the experimental case did not fall within the confidence interval of the intact group and was thus considered to be significantly different. All other conventions and abbreviations as in Fig. 4. 1818 Brain (2006), 129, 1803–1821 R. J. Rushmore et al. A. B. C. Fig. 12 Summary of experimental results. The first row (A) represents orienting behaviour in cats with lesion (left), cats with lesion and contralateral pMS cooling (middle) and cats with lesion and contralateral SC cooling (right). (B) The experimental preparation of each of the three groups—black represents resected cortex and grey indicates cooling deactivation. (C) The impact of different conditions on the layers of the superior colliculus. (One arrow represents between a 5 and 10% change from intact values, two arrows represents a 10–20% change, three arrows represents a 20–30% change, four arrows represents >30% change from intact values; 0 represents no change). (A) redran from Lomber et al. (2002). neglect was maintained and compounded by a suddenly unbalanced midbrain system (Kinsbourne, 1987; Hilgetag et al., 1999; Payne and Rushmore, 2004) via induction of asymmetry between the cortical hemispheres and between the midbrain SC. Our findings provide a test of this idea and force several modifications. The contralateral pMS cortex Contrary to speculations that the initial neglect-inducing lesion would reduce or elevate excitability in the homotypic contralateral cortical region and thus on the contralateral SC (Hilgetag et al., 2001; Payne and Rushmore, 2004), we found no consistent or statistically reliable evidence to suggest that the pMS contralateral to lesion or cooling was affected by the deactivation of the contralateral homotypic region. Thus, despite the large anatomical callosal projection (Keller and Innocenti, 1981; Segraves and Rosenquist, 1982), lesion or cooling do not appear to have an impact on the contralateral pMS cortex. These findings are supported by the presence of the multiple kinds of axons in the corpus callosum (Peters et al., 1990) and the spectrum of functional impacts of callosal projections (Payne et al., 1991). Finally, behavioural data from Sherman (Sherman, 1974) indicate that the contralateral cerebral cortex is not necessary for recovery. Overall, our data show that contralateral parietal cortex is not affected by a neglect-inducing manipulation of the contralateral homologue. The ipsilesional SGI A strong decrease in 2DG uptake in the ipsilesional/ ipsicooled SC was expected on the basis of two arguments. First, we anticipated a functional denervation of cortical inputs would decrease the excitability of superficial collicular layers and vertically telegraph to the intermediate (SGI) and deep (SGP) collicular layers (Behan and Appell, 1992) and render the entire SC hypoexcitable. Second, the impact of a lesion on crossed inhibitory mechanisms between the two SC would be expected to feed back and further inhibit the ipsilesional intermediate and deeper layers (Hilgetag et al., 1999). In the present study, however, we found a decrease in 2DG uptake in the superficial layers SGS and SO, but 2DG uptake in intermediate layers was not different from normal. This constancy of SGI activity confirms previous data that indicate that the superficial layers are functionally isolated from the deeper layers (Wickelgren and Sterling, 1969; Rizzolatti et al., 1970; Stein and Arigbede, 1972; Berman and Cynader, 1975; Ogasawara et al., 1984); alternatively, the normal activity level of the ipsilesional SGI may represent functional compensation by non-visual inputs, which provide substantial inputs to SGI neurons (Huerta and Harting, Functional circuitry of neglect 1984; Harting et al., 1992; Meredith and Stein, 1993; Harting, 2004). Experimental models of visual neglect Both large visual cortical lesions and focal cooling deactivations of pMS cortex are presented as models of neglect, and here we discuss the suitability and limitations of each as appropriate models of neglect. Restricted cooling deactivation of pMS cortex in the cat silences neurons under the cooling loop and produces visual deficits consistent with neglect (Lomber and Payne, 1996, 2004; Payne et al., 1996a). Cooling is a focal manipulation and neurons are functionally silenced, but structurally intact (Lomber, 1999); as such, adaptive or maladaptive mechanisms following cortical damage are not invoked. Comparable functional deficits cannot simply be created by focal permanent lesions of pMS, since in such cases visual orienting spontaneously recovers within a matter of days, presumably as adjacent cortical regions compensate for the function of the damaged pMS cortex (Payne et al., 1996b). Large lesions incorporating pMS and regions of cortex that potentially compensate are required to produce a stable form of neglect. This model is more similar to the human condition, wherein neglect is elicited by brain damage that is frequently quite extensive, and in which case neglect and hemianopia often co-occur. However, the experimental model involving large cortical lesion suffers from a lack of specificity. Overall, we are confident that the cooling and lesion approaches are comparable for three reasons. First, the cancellation of visual deficits argues that we are examining recovery from neglect, and not hemianopia, because unlike neglect, hemianopia cannot be reversed—the grain of analysis that striate cortex exerts on the visual field appears irreplaceable. Second, visual deficits induced by a unilateral pMS cooling or by a large visual cortex lesion can both be reversed by cooling precisely the same region of cortex in the intact side (Lomber and Payne, 1996; Lomber et al., 2001). Finally, both approaches produce a pattern on the SC that is similar in sign, but not magnitude, a result concordant with the extent of the cortical manipulation. We should also note that there is a piece of the puzzle remaining to completely establish this comparison between the two models: the impact of bilateral cooling deactivation of pMS on 2DG uptake in the SC has not yet been performed. While on the basis of the current data and from the data of Lomber and Payne (1996) we would expect the second cooling to reduce 2DG uptake levels to normal or subnormal levels, at this point we can only speculate. Brain (2006), 129, 1803–1821 1819 the SC, orienting to stimuli in the previously neglected hemifield returns (Fig. 12; Lomber et al., 2002). These findings are in broad agreement with models of midbrain function (Hilgetag et al., 1999; Payne and Rushmore, 2004), but several features do not reflect theoretical predictions. In particular, we did not find any evidence that the ipsilateral SC is being inhibited beyond the inhibition expected by functional denervation of corticotectal inputs. Indeed, cancelling neglect is associated with a further decrease in 2DG uptake in the ipsilesional SO layer, and unchanged activity profiles in the SGI. Thus, the simplest explanation we can proffer is that the hyperexcitability of the contralesional SC is sufficient in itself to produce symptoms of neglect. As such, ameliorative manipulations taken previously, including transection of the collicular commissure, ablation of the contralateral SC (Sprague, 1966), as well as the recent findings using cooling (Lomber et al., 2002) were effective because they eliminated the excitability generated in contralateral SC. This mechanism also suggests that therapies that transiently eliminate symptoms of neglect, such as caloric stimulation or neck vibration treatments, may produce their effects by temporarily diminishing excitability in the contralesional SC. It is important to note, however, that this interpretation is germane for the understanding of spatial neglect, but unlikely to account for more complex features or forms of neglect, such as representational neglect (e.g. Bisiach and Luzzatti, 1978). Summary We found evidence that the presence of neglect is associated with a specific pattern of activity in the SC, a pattern that indicates a hypoactivity of the layers of the ipsilateral SC receiving input from the deactivation or lesion, and a hyperactivity of the contralateral SC. Depressing the hyperactivity in the intact side with cooling resulted in a cancellation of neglect. Our results encourage a relatively straightforward approach to the treatment of neglect in humans: the moderation of excitability in the contralesional SC. Owing to the deep location of the SC, and the tendency of neglect to naturally ameliorate in humans, an invasive approach is counterindicated. Instead, since we have shown that neglect may be reversed by modulating activity in strongly coupled cerebral structures, it follows that non-invasive methods such as transcranial magnetic stimulation or electrical stimulation may serve to diminish the activity in the SC through long-range connections (Oliveri et al., 2001; Brighina et al., 2003; Valero-Cabre et al., 2005). Obvious candidate regions would be the frontal eye fields and the parietal cortex. Neglect and its restoration Acknowledgements Our findings suggest that neglect is associated with an increase in excitability in the contralateral SC; when that excitability is diminished, either indirectly through cooling deactivation of the pMS cortex or directly through cooling of We thank the following people who read and made valuable comments on previous versions of this manuscript: Mark Moss, Julie Sandell, Álvaro Pascual-Leone, Alan Peters and Louis Toth. This work was supported by grants NIH 1820 Brain (2006), 129, 1803–1821 NS044624 (R.J.R.), NS032137 and NS47754 (B.R.P., S.G.L. and A.V.C.), NSF IBM9906443 (S.G.L.) and the Wellcome Trust (C.C.H.). References Appell PP, Behan M. Sources of GABAergic projections to the superior colliculus in the cat. J Comp Neurol 1990; 302: 143–58. Behan M, Appell PP. Intrinsic circuitry in the cat superior colliculus: projections from the superficial layers. J Comp Neurol 1992; 315: 230–43. Berman N, Cynader M. Receptive fields in cat superior colliculus after visual cortex lesions. J Physiol 1975; 245: 261–70. Bisiach E, Luzzatti C. Unilateral neglect of representational space. Cortex 1978; 14: 29–133. Brighina F, Bisiach E, Oliveri M, Piazza A, La Bua V, Daniele O, Fierro B. 1 Hz repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the unaffected hemisphere ameliorates contralesional visuospatial neglect in humans. Neurosci Lett 2003; 336:131-3. Driver J, Vuilleumier P. Perceptual awareness and its loss in unilateral neglect and extinction. Cognition 2001; 79: 39–88. Durmer JS, Rosenquist AC. Ibotenic acid lesions in the pedunculopontine region result in recovery of visual orienting in the hemianopic cat. Neuroscience 2001; 106: 765–81. Edwards SB. The commissural projection of the superior colliculus in the cat. J Comp Neurol 1977; 173: 23–40. Feldon P, Kruger L. Topography of the retinal projection upon the superior colliculus of the cat. Vision Res 1970; 10: 135–43. Geneser-Jensen FA, Blackstad TW. Distribution of acetyl cholinesterase in the hippocampal region of the guinea pig. I. Entorhinal area, parasubiculum, and presubiculum. Z Zellforsch Mikrosk Anat 1971; 114: 460–81. Hardy SC, Stein BE. Small lateral suprasylvian cortex lesions produce visual neglect and decreased visual activity in the superior colliculus. J Comp Neurol 1988; 273: 527–42. Harting JK. Puffs and patches: a brief chronological review. In: Hall WC, Moschovakis A, editors. The superior colliculus: new approaches for studying sensorimotor integration. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press; 2004. p. 83–106. Harting JK, Huerta MF, Hashikawa T, Weber JT, Van Lieshout DP. Neuroanatomical studies of the nigrotectal projection in the cat. J Comp Neurol 1988; 278: 615–31. Harting JK, Updyke BV, Van Lieshout DP. Corticotectal projections in the cat: anterograde transport studies of twenty-five cortical areas. J Comp Neurol 1992; 324: 379–414. Heilman KM, Valenstein E. Clinical neuropsychology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1993. Hilgetag CC, Kotter R, Young MP. Inter-hemispheric competition of subcortical structures is a crucial mechanism in paradoxical lesion effects and spatial neglect. Prog Brain Res 1999; 121: 121–41. Hilgetag CC, Theoret H, Pascual-Leone A. Enhanced visual spatial attention ipsilateral to rTMS-induced ‘virtual lesions’ of human parietal cortex. Nat Neurosci 2001; 4: 953–7. Huerta MF, Harting JK. The mammalian superior colliculus: studies of its morphology and connections. In: Venagas H, editor. Comparative neurology of the optic tectum. New York: Plenum; 1984. p. 687–773. Jiang H, Stein BE, McHaffie JG. Opposing basal ganglia processes shape midbrain visuomotor activity bilaterally. Nature 2003; 423: 982–6. Kanaseki T, Sprague JM. Anatomical organization of pretectal nuclei and tectal laminae in the cat. J Comp Neurol 1974; 158: 319–37. Karnath HO, Fruhmann Berger M, Kuker W, Rorden C. The anatomy of spatial neglect based on voxelwise statistical analysis: a study of 140 patients. Cereb Cortex 2004; 14: 1164–72. Keller G, Innocenti GM. Callosal connections of suprasylvian visual areas in the cat. Neuroscience 1981; 6: 703–12. Kerkhoff G. Spatial hemineglect in humans. Prog Neurobiol 2001; 63: 1–27. R. J. Rushmore et al. Kerkhoff G. Modulation and rehabilitation of spatial neglect by sensory stimulation. Prog Brain Res 2003; 142: 257–71. Kinsbourne M. Mechanisms of hemispheric interaction in man. In: Kinsbourne M, Smith WL, editors. Hemispheric disconnection and cerebral function. Springfield, IL: Thomas; 1974. p. 260–85. Kinsbourne M. Mechanisms of unilateral neglect. In: Jeannerod M, editor. Neurophsyiological and neuropsychological aspects of unilateral neglect. North Holland: Elsevier; 1987. p. 69–86. Kinsbourne M. Orientation bias model of unilateral neglect: evidence from attentional gradients within hemispace. In: Robertson I, Marshall J, editors. Unilateral neglect: clinical and experimental studies. New York: L. Erlbaum Associates; 1993. p. 63–86. Lomber SG, Payne BR. Removal of two halves restores the whole: reversal of visual hemineglect during bilateral cortical or collicular inactivation in the cat. Vis Neurosci 1996; 13: 1143–56. Lomber SG, Payne BR. Task-specific reversal of visual hemineglect following bilateral reversible deactivation of posterior parietal cortex: a comparison with deactivation of the superior colliculus. Vis Neurosci 2001; 18: 487–99. Lomber SG, Payne BR. Cerebral areas mediating visual orienting of the head: cooling deactivation of fifteen loci in the cat. J Comp Neurol 2004; 474: 190–208. Lomber SG. The advantages and limitations of permanent or reversible deactivation techniques in the assessment of neural function. J Neurosci Methods 1999; 86:109-17. Lomber SG, Payne BR, Horel JA. The cryoloop: an adaptable reversible cooling deactivation method for behavioral or electrophysiological assessment of neural function. J Neurosci Methods 1999; 86: 179–94. Lomber SG, Payne BR, Cornwell P. Role of the superior colliculus in analyses of space: superficial and intermediate layer contributions to visual orienting, auditory orienting, and visuospatial discriminations during unilateral and bilateral deactivations. J Comp Neurol 2001; 441: 44–57. Lomber SG, Payne BR, Hilgetag CC, Rushmore J. Restoration of visual orienting into a cortically blind hemifield by reversible deactivation of posterior parietal cortex or the superior colliculus. Exp Brain Res 2002; 142: 463–74. Meredith MA, Stein BE. The merging of the senses. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1993. Mesulam MM. Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal and cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional targeting of salient extrapersonal events. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1999; 354: 1325–46. Mort DJ, Malhotra P, Mannan SK, Rorden C, Pambakian A, Kennard C, et al. The anatomy of visual neglect. Brain 2003; 126: 1986–97. Ogasawara K, McHaffie JG, Stein BE. Two visual corticotectal systems in cat. J Neurophysiol 1984; 52: 1226–45. Olivier E, Porter JD, May PJ. Comparison of the distribution and somatodendritic morphology of tectotectal neurons in the cat and monkey. Vis Neurosci 1998; 15: 903–22. Oliveri M, Bisiach E, Brighina F, Piazza A, La Bua V, Bufa D, Fierro B. rTMS of the unaffected hemisphere transiently reduces contralesional visuospatial hemineglect. Neurology 2001; 57:1338-40 Palmer LA, Rosenquist AC, Tusa RJ. The retinotopic organization of lateral suprasylvian visual areas in the cat. J Comp Neurol 1978; 177: 237–56. Payne BR, Rushmore RJ. Functional circuitry underlying natural and interventional cancellation of visual neglect. Exp Brain Res 2004; 154: 127–53. Payne BR, Siwek DF, Lomber SG. Complex transcallosal interactions in visual cortex. Vis Neurosci 1991; 6: 283–9. Payne BR, Lomber SG, Geeraerts S, van der Gucht E, Vandenbussche E. Reversible visual hemineglect. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1996a; 93: 290–4. Payne BR, Lomber SG, Villa AE, Bullier J. Reversible deactivation of cerebral network components. Trends Neurosci 1996b; 19: 535–42. Peters A, Payne BR, Josephson K. Transcallosal non-pyramidal cell projections from visual cortex in the cat. J Comp Neurol 1990; 302: 124–42. Pierce SR, Buxbaum LJ. Treatments of unilateral neglect: a review. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2002; 83: 256–68. Functional circuitry of neglect Reinoso-Suarez F. Topographischer Hirnatlas der Katz fur experimentale physiologische Untersuchungen [Topographic atlas of the cat brain for experimental physiology research]. Darmstadt, FDR: Merck; 1961. Rizzolatti G, Tradardi V, Camarda R. Unit responses to visual stimuli in the cat’s superior colliculus after removal of the visual cortex. Brain Res 1970; 24: 336–9. RosenquistA.Connections ofvisual corticalareasinthecat. In:Peters A,JonesE, editors.Cerebralcortex,Vol.3. NewYork,NY:PlenumPress;1985.p.81–117. Rushmore RJ, Payne BR. Bilateral impact of unilateral visual cortex lesions on the superior colliculus. Exp Brain Res 2003; 151: 542–7. Rushmore RJ, Payne BR, Lomber SG. Functional impact of primary visual cortex deactivation on subcortical target structures in the thalamus and midbrain. J Comp Neurol 2005; 488: 414–26. Schindler I, Kerkhoff G, Karnath HO, Keller I, Goldenberg G. Neck muscle vibration induces lasting recovery in spatial neglect. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2002; 73: 412–9. Schmued LC. A rapid, sensitive histochemical stain for myelin in frozen brain sections. J Histochem Cytochem 1990; 38: 717–20. Segal RL, Beckstead RM. The lateral suprasylvian corticotectal projection in cats. J Comp Neurol 1984; 225: 259–75. Segraves MA, Rosenquist AC. The afferent and efferent callosal connections of retinotopically defined areas in cat cortex. J Neurosci 1982; 2: 1090–107. Sharp FR, Kilduff TS, Bzorgchami S, Heller HC, Ryan AF. The relationship of local cerebral glucose utilization to optical density ratios. Brain Res 1983; 263: 97–103. Sherman SM. Visual fields of cats with cortical and tectal lesions. Science 1974; 185: 355–7. Sprague JM. Interaction of cortex and superior colliculus in mediation of visually guided behavior in the cat. Science 1966; 153: 1544–7. Sprague JM. Neural mechanisms of visual orienting responses. Prog Brain Res 1996; 112: 1–15. Brain (2006), 129, 1803–1821 1821 Stein BE, Arigbede MO. Unimodal and multimodal response properties of neurons in the cat’s superior colliculus. Exp Neurol 1972; 36: 179–96. Takahashi M, Sugiuchi Y, Izawa Y, Shinoda Y. Commissural excitation and inhibition by the superior colliculus in tectoreticular neurons projecting to omnipause neuron and inhibitory burst neuron regions. J Neurophysiol 2006; 94: 1707–26. Valero-Cabre A, Maetani T, Pascual-Leone A, Payne B, Rushmore RJ. Longlasting restitution of visuospatial attention with low-frequency transcranial magnetic stimulation on the parietal cortex. Program Number 642.17, 2005 Abstract Viewer and Itinerary Planner. Washington, DC: Society for Neuroscience, 2005.. Vanduffel W, Vandenbussche E, Singer W, Orban GA. Metabolic mapping of visual areas in the behaving cat: a [14C]2-deoxyglucose study. J Comp Neurol 1995; 354: 161–80. Vanduffel W, Payne BR, Lomber SG, Orban GA. Functional impact of cerebral connections. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1997; 94: 7617–20. Vanduffel W, Orban GA, Lomber SG, Payne BR. Functional impact of cerebral projection systems. Mol Psychiatry 1998; 3: 215–9. Wallace SF, Rosenquist AC, Sprague JM. Recovery from cortical blindness mediated by destruction of nontectotectal fibers in the commissure of the superior colliculus in the cat. J Comp Neurol 1989; 284: 429–50. Wallace SF, Rosenquist AC, Sprague JM. Ibotenic acid lesions of the lateral substantia nigra restore visual orientation behavior in the hemianopic cat. J Comp Neurol 1990; 296: 222–52. Weddell RA. Subcortical modulation of spatial attention including evidence that the Sprague effect extends to man. Brain Cogn 2004; 55: 497–506. Wickelgren BG, Sterling P. Influence of visual cortex on receptive fields in the superior colliculus of the cat. J Neurophysiol 1969; 32: 16–23. Wong-Riley M. Changes in the visual system of monocularly sutured or enucleated cats demonstrable with cytochrome oxidase histochemistry. Brain Res 1979; 171: 11–28.