Download Alterations to multisensory and unisensory integration by stimulus

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Surface wave detection by animals wikipedia , lookup

Metastability in the brain wikipedia , lookup

Nervous system network models wikipedia , lookup

Emotional lateralization wikipedia , lookup

Perception wikipedia , lookup

Biological neuron model wikipedia , lookup

Neuroethology wikipedia , lookup

Synaptic gating wikipedia , lookup

Caridoid escape reaction wikipedia , lookup

Sensory substitution wikipedia , lookup

Emotion perception wikipedia , lookup

Biology of depression wikipedia , lookup

Visual search wikipedia , lookup

Rheobase wikipedia , lookup

Visual selective attention in dementia wikipedia , lookup

Emotion and memory wikipedia , lookup

Negative priming wikipedia , lookup

Neural correlates of consciousness wikipedia , lookup

Neuroesthetics wikipedia , lookup

Sensory cue wikipedia , lookup

Neural coding wikipedia , lookup

Perception of infrasound wikipedia , lookup

Time perception wikipedia , lookup

Evoked potential wikipedia , lookup

Response priming wikipedia , lookup

Visual extinction wikipedia , lookup

Allochiria wikipedia , lookup

Visual N1 wikipedia , lookup

Psychophysics wikipedia , lookup

C1 and P1 (neuroscience) wikipedia , lookup

Feature detection (nervous system) wikipedia , lookup

Stimulus (physiology) wikipedia , lookup

P200 wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
J Neurophysiol 106: 3091–3101, 2011.
First published September 28, 2011; doi:10.1152/jn.00509.2011.
Alterations to multisensory and unisensory integration
by stimulus competition
Scott R. Pluta, Benjamin A. Rowland, Terrence R. Stanford, and Barry E. Stein
Department of Neurobiology and Anatomy, Wake Forest University School of Medicine, Winston-Salem, North Carolina
Submitted 7 June 2011; accepted in final form 26 September 2011
depression; superior colliculus; target selection; salience
(Kadunce et al. 2001; Meredith et al. 1987; Meredith and Stein
1996; Rowland et al. 2007; Wallace et al. 1998). Theoretically,
such multisensory enhancement should afford cross-modal
stimuli a competitive advantage over their modality-specific
counterparts; however, the rules that govern such competitive
interactions at the level of the single multisensory neuron are
not well understood.
To understand how neurons in the cat SC integrate sensory
information within a competitive multisensory environment,
we presented modality-specific and/or cross-modal stimuli at
two locations simultaneously. Stimuli were placed in opposite
hemifields to simulate target choices that would require interhemispheric competition to resolve mutually exclusive (i.e.,
left/right) demands on the motor apparatus. We evaluated
several postulates pertaining to the potential benefits of multisensory integration by testing the ability of cross-modal and
modality-specific stimuli to both maintain and degrade the
physiological salience of the excitatory stimulus. In addition,
we sought to determine if the activity of single multisensory
neurons is consistent with a simple linear model for integrating
the various stimulus-specific excitatory and inhibitory influences inherent in a given stimulus configuration, or if a novel
combinatorial rule is required to predict outcomes in competitive sensory environments.
METHODS
to rapidly detect, localize, and
orient toward biologically relevant events. The superior colliculus (SC) plays a key role in this task by translating sensory
signals into the motor commands required for orienting the
eyes, ears, and head toward salient visual, auditory, and tactile
stimuli as well as to their various cross-modal combinations
(Fecteau and Munoz 2006; Lomber et al. 2001; Sparks 1986;
Stein and Meredith 1993). In a typical environment containing
multiple potential targets, response selection is a competitive
process in which targets are chosen on the basis of stimulus
attributes (e.g., size, intensity) and intrinsic motivation. While
several studies have shown the neural correlates of competition
among modality-specific stimulus representations within SC
activity (Basso and Wurtz 1997, 1998; Jiang and Stein 2003;
Kadunce et al. 1997; Li and Basso 2005; Mysore et al. 2011;
Rizzolatti et al. 1974), real world scenes are multisensory and
contain concordant cross-modal signals competing with other
stimuli. It is well established that concordant cross-modal
signals are integrated in the cat SC, a process that is known to
significantly increase the physiological salience of such events
SURVIVAL DEPENDS ON THE ABILITY
Address for reprint requests and other correspondence: S. R. Pluta, Wake
Forest School of Medicine, Medical Center Blvd., Winston-Salem, NC 27157
(e-mail: [email protected]).
www.jn.org
All procedures were compliant with the Guide for the Care and Use
of Laboratory Animals (National Institutes of Health Publication
86 –23) and approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee of
Wake Forest University School of Medicine and the Association for
Assessment and Accreditation of Laboratory Animal Care International.
Surgical preparation. Three adult cats were implanted with stainless-steel recording chambers using previously described methods
(Mchaffie and Stein 1983). Each animal was first anesthetized with a
mixture of ketamine hydrochloride (25 mg/kg) and acepromazine
maleate (0.2– 0.4 mg/kg). The animal was then intubated, placed in a
stereotaxic head holder, and anesthetized for surgery with isoflurane
(1.5–3%). A craniotomy was performed to allow access to the SC, and the
recording chamber was attached with stainless steel screws and dental
acrylic. Postsurgical analgesics (0.01 mg/kg buprenorphine and 2
mg/kg ketoprofen) and antibiotics (5 mg/kg enrofloxacin) were administered as needed.
Single neuron recording. In each session the animal was anesthetized with a dose of ketamine hydrochloride (20 mg/kg im) and
acepromazine maleate (0.1– 0.2 mg/kg), intubated, ventilated, and
paralyzed with an injection of pancuronium bromide (0.1 mg/kg iv) to
prevent the eyes from changing position. Respiratory rate and volume,
heart rate, blood pressure, and CO2 were monitored by a VetSpecsVSM7 and corrected as needed. Anesthesia, paralysis, and hydration
were maintained by continuous intravenous infusion of ketamine
0022-3077/11 Copyright © 2011 the American Physiological Society
3091
Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.2 on June 18, 2017
Pluta SR, Rowland BA, Stanford TR, Stein BE. Alterations to
multisensory and unisensory integration by stimulus competition. J
Neurophysiol 106: 3091–3101, 2011. First published September 28,
2011; doi:10.1152/jn.00509.2011.—In environments containing sensory events at competing locations, selecting a target for orienting
requires prioritization of stimulus values. Although the superior colliculus (SC) is causally linked to the stimulus selection process, the
manner in which SC multisensory integration operates in a competitive stimulus environment is unknown. Here we examined how the
activity of visual-auditory SC neurons is affected by placement of a
competing target in the opposite hemifield, a stimulus configuration
that would, in principle, promote interhemispheric competition for
access to downstream motor circuitry. Competitive interactions between the targets were evident in how they altered unisensory and
multisensory responses of individual neurons. Responses elicited by a
cross-modal stimulus (multisensory responses) proved to be substantially more resistant to competitor-induced depression than were
unisensory responses (evoked by the component modality-specific
stimuli). Similarly, when a cross-modal stimulus served as the competitor, it exerted considerably more depression than did its individual
component stimuli, in some cases producing more depression than
predicted by their linear sum. These findings suggest that multisensory
integration can help resolve competition among multiple targets by
enhancing orientation to the location of cross-modal events while
simultaneously suppressing orientation to events at alternate locations.
3092
MULTISENSORY INTEGRATION WITH COMPETING STIMULI
J Neurophysiol • VOL
Fig. 1. Graphical depiction of the simple and competing stimulus paradigms. The
gray circles represent a neuron’s visual (dark gray) and auditory (light gray)
excitatory receptive fields (ERFs). A: the simple paradigm: 2 stimuli are either
separately or jointly presented inside the neuron’s ERFs. This excitatory stimulus
is denoted by the subscript “i.” B: the competing paradigm: stimuli are presented
simultaneously inside (excitatory stimulus) and outside (competitor stimulus) the
neuron’s ERFs in all possible combinations. The competitor stimuli are denoted by
the subscript “c.” The dotted lines represent the vertical meridian.
neurons studied. The mean cumulative impulse count was then converted to the mean cumulative stimulus-driven impulse count (Qsum)
by subtracting, at each moment in time, the number of impulses
expected based on the mean spontaneous firing rate calculated in the
500-ms window preceding the stimulus. Changes in the slope of the
Qsum indicated changes in the underlying instantaneous firing rate.
Response latency was calculated using a geometric function involving
changes in the slope of the Qsum (Rowland et al. 2007).
Multisensory integration. The criterion for multisensory integration
was met when the response to the cross-modal stimulus (i.e., the
multisensory response) was significantly greater than the response to
the largest unisensory response (Meredith and Stein 1983). The
multisensory index (MSI), a relative measure of multisensory integration, was calculated as the percent increase in the multisensory
response over the largest unisensory response. Response depression
was calculated as the percent decrease in stimulus-driven mean
impulse count, but is reported here as percent depression, a positive
quantity. Depression values greater than 100% indicate a decrease in
response magnitude below the spontaneous firing rate. Statistical
comparisons between response magnitudes among conditions were
performed with t-tests and ANOVA.
RESULTS
Fifty-one multisensory neurons in the deep (i.e., multisensory) layers of the SC were analyzed. Their response properties
were evaluated using simple and competing stimulus configurations (Fig. 1). Briefly, in all configurations, cross-modal or
modality-specific stimuli were presented within a neuron’s
ERFs to establish their relative effectiveness. In the competing
configurations, an additional stimulus (cross-modal or modality-specific) was presented simultaneously in the opposite
hemifield to promote interhemispheric competition. This configuration also precluded the possibility that the competing
stimulus encroached on an unidentified subthreshold portion of
the neuron’s ERF (Jiang and Stein 2003; Kadunce et al. 1997;
Stein et al. 1988).
The magnitude and incidence of multisensory integration
observed in the simple stimulus paradigm were similar to prior
observations (Perrault et al. 2005; Stanford et al. 2005). For the
majority (66%, 25/38) of neurons studied with this stimulus
configuration, the multisensory response was significantly (ttest, P ⬍ 0.05) greater than the largest unisensory comparator
response, thereby meeting the criterion for multisensory enhancement (Meredith and Stein 1983). In the competing stimulus configurations, the incidence of neurons exhibiting signif-
106 • DECEMBER 2011 •
www.jn.org
Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.2 on June 18, 2017
hydrochloride (4 – 6 mg·kg⫺1·h⫺1), pancuronium bromide (0.05
mg·kg⫺1·h⫺1), and 5% dextrose in 0.9% saline (3– 6 ml/h). Body
temperature was maintained at 37–38°C using a circulating water pad.
Contact lenses were inserted to correct refractive errors and prevent
corneal drying.
Single neuron extracellular recordings used conventional methods.
The electrode (tip diameter 1–3 ␮m, impedance 2–3.5 M⍀ at 1 kHz)
was positioned on a microdrive stage and lowered into the SC. After
reaching the superficial layers of the SC, the electrode was advanced
with the hydraulic microdrive while monitoring neural activity and
presenting search stimuli. Single neuron activity was recorded, amplified, and then sent to an oscilloscope, audio monitor, and computer.
At the end of an experiment the animal was returned to its home cage
after it regained respiratory and ambulatory control.
Apparatus and search paradigm. Sensory-responsive neurons were
identified with visual and auditory stimuli. Visual search stimuli
consisted of moving bars of light (projected from an LC 4445 Philips
projector) onto a screen located 45 cm from the animal. Auditory
stimuli consisted of broadband (20 –20,000 Hz) noise bursts of
100-ms duration presented from any of a series of speakers positioned
on a mobile hoop 15° apart and 15 cm from the animal’s head. Stimuli
were controlled using custom software operating a NIDAQ controller.
When a multisensory neuron was isolated, its modality-specific visual
and auditory receptive fields were mapped as in the past (Alvarado et
al. 2007).
Test stimuli. Auditory stimuli consisted of brief (100-ms) 50 –56
decibel (dB) sound pressure level (SPL) bursts of broadband noise
presented against ambient background SPL 48.4 dB. Visual stimuli
were 100- to 200-ms duration, 4° ⫻ 1° moving bars of light (1.68 –
13.67 cd/m2) of optimal velocity for each neuron and presented
against a uniform gray background (0.16 cd/m2). When a “withinmodal” pair of visual stimuli was presented, the two bars of light had
a horizontal separation of 5°. During cross-modal stimulation, visual
stimulus onset preceded auditory stimulus onset by 50 ms to maximize the likelihood of multisensory integration (Meredith et al. 1987).
Stimulus paradigms. Only visual and auditory stimuli were employed in the current experiments, and they were arranged in two main
configurations: “simple” and “competing.” In a simple configuration,
visual (1 or 2 bars), auditory, or visual-auditory (i.e., cross-modal)
stimuli were presented within the neuron’s excitatory receptive fields
(ERFs). In a competing configuration, an additional (i.e., “competitor”) stimulus was presented simultaneously in the opposite hemifield
(ipsilateral to the electrode) and outside the neuron’s ERFs. Stimuli
were placed in the opposite hemifield to assure that the motor plans
required to target one or the other would have minimal overlap.
Bilaterally presented auditory stimuli were composed of noise that
was statistically uncorrelated (r2 ⬍ 0.01) and had an azimuthal
separation of at least 75° to reduce summing localization. Visual
stimuli moved in the medial to lateral direction. In blocks testing
multisensory integration in competing configurations, 12 different
stimulus configurations (3 simple, 9 competing) were presented in a
pseudo-randomly interleaved fashion. In blocks testing within-modal
visual integration in competing configurations, 18 different stimulus
configurations (3 simple, 15 competing) were pseudo-randomly interleaved. Fourteen of the 51 neurons were tested in both cross-modal
and within-modal blocks. Figure 1 illustrates the difference between
simple and competing stimulus configurations. An interstimulus interval of 5– 6.5 s was used to minimize habituation.
Data acquisition and analysis. Custom software was used to
acquire raw data waveforms and impulses from single neurons (after
A/D conversion) with action potential amplitudes at least three times
greater than the background noise. Impulse times were recorded with
1-ms resolution. The mean cumulative impulse count was computed
from each raster by counting the number of impulses generated on or
before each time bin and averaging across trials for 300 ms after
stimulus onset. A response window of 300 ms was chosen because it
incorporates the entire stimulus-driven activity of the vast majority of
MULTISENSORY INTEGRATION WITH COMPETING STIMULI
icant multisensory enhancement increased to 71% (27/38) with
concurrent presentation of a visual competitor, 74% (28/38)
with an auditory competitor, and 82% (31/38) with a crossmodal competitor. As described below, increases in both the
incidence and magnitude of multisensory enhancement could
be attributed to a disproportionately greater suppressive effect
3093
of competing stimuli on unisensory responses, whereas the
multisensory responses were substantially more resistant to this
suppressive influence.
The results from an example neuron illustrate the relative
potency of multisensory integration in both resisting and provoking suppression in the competing stimulus configuration (Fig. 2).
Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.2 on June 18, 2017
Fig. 2. The effect of competitive stimulus interactions on unisensory and multisensory responses in an example neuron. A: within the schematics of
visual-auditory space (each concentric circle represents 10°), icons denote the locations of visual (V), auditory (A), and cross-modal (VA) stimuli relative to the
neuron’s ERFs. Visual and auditory stimuli were presented in simple and competing stimulus configurations to generate 12 stimulus conditions (B–E) with
conventions described in Fig. 1. Below the schematics are traces representing the time course of visual (ramp) and auditory (square wave) stimuli. Histograms,
rasters, and a summary bar graph depict the neuron’s unisensory and multisensory responses in simple (B) and competing (C–E) conditions, calculated from the
mean cumulative impulse counts at 300 ms after stimulus onset. A window of 300 ms incorporated the entire stimulus-driven response in the vast majority of
neurons studied. The vertical arrows underneath the PSTHs indicate the time(s) of stimulus onset. The competing visual (C), auditory (D), and cross-modal (E)
stimuli are in the opposite hemifield. Note that the neuron’s unisensory responses were disproportionately depressed in the competing conditions relative to its
multisensory responses, and that the most effective inhibitory competitor was cross-modal. The disproportionate inhibition of unisensory responses increased the
relative magnitude of multisensory responses, thereby significantly increasing the degree of multisensory enhancement from 31% (simple condition) to
61%–924% (competing conditions). *P ⬍ 0.05; **P ⬍ 0.01. Error bars indicate 1 SE. MSI, multisensory index.
J Neurophysiol • VOL
106 • DECEMBER 2011 •
www.jn.org
3094
MULTISENSORY INTEGRATION WITH COMPETING STIMULI
Although in the simple configuration, cross-modal stimuli generally produced significantly larger responses than did either of
their component stimuli; in this particular case, the multisensory response was only marginally greater than the largest
component unisensory (auditory) response (Fig. 2B). The addition of a visual competitor changed the relative magnitudes
of the unisensory and multisensory responses. It moderately
depressed the auditory response, but only slightly degraded the
multisensory response, thereby increasing the relative magnitude of the latter (Fig. 2C). The multisensory response was now
statistically greater (P ⬍ 0.05) than the largest component
unisensory response, achieving the criterion for multisensory
integration, and nearly doubling the MSI to 61% (Fig. 2C). The
effect was considerably greater when an auditory stimulus was
the competitor (Fig. 2D), but it was most dramatic in the
presence of a competing cross-modal stimulus (Fig. 2E). In this
stimulus configuration, the unisensory responses were nearly
extinguished, while the multisensory response retained a substantial degree of its original magnitude (Fig. 2E). Consequently, the MSI increased to 924%.
A second example, shown in Fig. 3, further illustrates the
phenomenon by demonstrating the relative impact of competing stimuli on the temporal profiles of the multisensory and
unisensory responses. In the simple configuration, the firing
rate of the largest unisensory response (visual) and the multisensory response were equivalent for up to 200 ms following
stimulus onset (Fig. 3A). In the competing configuration, a
visual competitor had no effect on the temporal relationship
between the visual and multisensory responses, but competing
auditory and cross-modal stimuli preferentially depressed the
Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.2 on June 18, 2017
Fig. 3. The effect of competitive stimulus interactions on
the temporal profile of responsiveness in an example
neuron. A: the cumulative number of stimulus-driven
impulses (Qsum) in the simple and competing stimulus
conditions. In the simple conditions, the largest unisensory response (visual) had a temporal profile similar to the
multisensory response for 200 ms after stimulus onset.
B: however, in the presence of an auditory or cross-modal
competitor, the visual response was depressed more than
the multisensory response. C: this differential depression
is evident in the early and later phases of the visual
response, resulting in a notable increase in the MSI.
Conventions are the same as in Fig. 2.
J Neurophysiol • VOL
106 • DECEMBER 2011 •
www.jn.org
MULTISENSORY INTEGRATION WITH COMPETING STIMULI
3095
early phase of the visual response, as manifested by a reduction
in the slope and plateau of the Qsum plot. The cross-modal
competitor had the most potent depressive effects (Fig. 3B).
The multisensory response proved most resistant to the depressive influences of competing stimulation, thus accounting for
the increases in MSI observed in these configurations (Fig.
3C). In the simple condition, multisensory response latency
was 7 ms shorter than visual response latency. In conditions
with competing auditory and cross-modal stimuli, multisensory
response latency was 18 ms and 24 ms shorter than visual
response latency, respectively.
In general, the effects seen in these examples were evident
across the population of neurons examined under these conditions (n ⫽ 38, Fig. 4, A and B). In a number of cases, one or
both of the unisensory responses were nearly eliminated by a
competing stimulus, while the multisensory response retained a
larger degree of its original magnitude. The specific differences
in the effectiveness of these different stimulus conditions on
unisensory and multisensory responses are shown in Table 1,
and the relationships are shown for the population in Fig. 4,
C–E. To determine if differences in the effectiveness between
cross-modal and modality-specific ERF stimuli can explain the
observed differences in resistance to depression, the relationship between response depression (cross-modal competitor)
and simple response magnitude was examined. There was no
systematic relationship between response magnitude and depression revealed by the pooled population analysis (R2 ⬍
0.005), regardless of ERF stimulus type. Note that the lack of
Table 1. Differential depression
Visual response (Vi)
Auditory response (Ai)
Vc
Ac
VcAc
74%
61%
42%
89%
76%
84%
The proportion of neurons in each sensory condition where the unisensory
response was depressed more than the multisensory response.
J Neurophysiol • VOL
a relationship in the population does not preclude a relationship
for individual neurons. Interestingly, multisensory responses
(n ⫽ 18, ␮ ⫽ 7.69) were depressed significantly less (44% vs.
80%, P ⬍ 0.001) than auditory responses (n ⫽ 12, ␮ ⫽ 7.34),
but approximately equal (44% vs. 55%, P ⬍ 0.26) to visual
responses (n ⫽ 15, ␮ ⫽ 7.09) of similar magnitude.
Consistent changes in the MSI evoked by competing stimuli
were evident across the sample. Weakly depressive competitors produced correspondingly small increases in MSI, and
strongly suppressive competitors yielded large increases in
MSI. As noted above, competing cross-modal stimuli were
generally the most powerful at degrading responses, disproportionately suppressing unisensory responses. Accordingly,
cross-modal competitors yielded increases in MSI for a high
percentage of neurons (Fig. 5C) that were, on average, larger
than those produced by competing visual or auditory stimuli
(Fig. 5D). By comparison, weakly suppressive visual competitors produced small increases in MSI (Fig. 5, A and D). The
effects of auditory competitors were mixed, producing some of
the largest increases but also decreases in MSI (Fig. 5B),
depending on whether the neuron’s preferred modality was
auditory or visual. Responses to auditory stimuli tended to be
strongly suppressed by competing auditory stimulation (MSI
increased), while visual responses tended to better resist suppression by competing auditory stimulation. In accordance
with previous research investigating the computation underlying multisensory integration (Meredith and Stein 1986),
MSI was inversely related to unisensory response magnitude
(Fig. 5E).
To further illustrate the potency of depression from a crossmodal competitor, the magnitude of its effect was compared
with its largest component depression, induced from either of
its component competitors (Fig. 6, A–C). Depression of visual
(Fig. 6A) and multisensory (Fig. 6C) responses from competing
cross-modal stimuli was significantly (P ⬍ 0.01) greater than
their largest component depressions. An interesting finding
106 • DECEMBER 2011 •
www.jn.org
Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.2 on June 18, 2017
Fig. 4. Population data summarizing the disproportionate depression of unisensory responses from competing
stimulation. A and B: scatter plots of the relative depressive impact of different competitor stimuli on unisensory and multisensory superior colliculus responses
for the population of neurons sampled. The symbol for
each competitor stimulus type is shown in the legend.
Note that multisensory responses are generally more
resistant to the depressive effects of competing stimuli
than are unisensory responses, with auditory responses
being particularly sensitive to degradation from auditory and cross-modal competitors, and visual responses
being particularly sensitive to depression from crossmodal competitors. C–E: bar graphs summarize the
average (n ⫽ 38) depression levels for each competitor
stimulus type and their statistical relationships (paired
t-test). Conventions are the same as in previous figures.
3096
MULTISENSORY INTEGRATION WITH COMPETING STIMULI
was that auditory responses were more depressed on average
by auditory competitors than cross-modal competitors (n ⫽ 38,
P ⬍ 0.05) and thus showed an opposite trend (Fig. 6B). There
are several different interpretations of this seemingly counterintuitive finding, and the reader is referred to the DISCUSSION.
Additional analyses of visual and multisensory response
depression revealed both linear and nonlinear summation of
depression (Fig. 7). A good example of nonlinear summation
of depression is shown for a single neuron in Fig. 7, A and B.
In this neuron, a single visual competitor produced no depression in the neuron’s visual response, an auditory competitor
produced a fair amount of depression, and a cross-modal
competitor almost eliminated the response. Quantitatively, the
depression produced by a competing cross-modal stimulus
configuration was 79%, but the predicted depression by a
simple summation of the depression induced by its two modality-specific component stimuli was only 16%. A population
analysis (n ⫽ 38) confirmed the generality of the observation
(Fig. 7C). The magnitude of depression from competing crossJ Neurophysiol • VOL
Fig. 6. Population data comparing the magnitude of depression from a
cross-modal competitor (VcAc) to its largest component depression (Xc).
A: visual and multisensory response depression induced by a competing
cross-modal stimulus was significantly (P ⬍ 0.01) greater than the largest
depression induced by one of its component stimuli. This trend is evident in the
distribution of data points well above the line of unity. Auditory response
depression from a competing cross-modal stimulus was significantly less (P ⬍
0.05) than that induced by one of its component stimuli. The most effective
component stimulus depressor was nearly always auditory. In this condition,
the cluster of data points is just below the line of unity. B–D: the statistical
differences in depression magnitude are summarized in bar graphs. Conventions are the same as in previous figures.
106 • DECEMBER 2011 •
www.jn.org
Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.2 on June 18, 2017
Fig. 5. The effect of competitive stimulus interactions on the MSI. Scatter plots
and a summary bar graph compare the MSI in simple and competing conditions. The MSI increased significantly in the presence of visual (A, P ⬍ 0.05),
auditory (B, P ⬍ 0.01), and cross-modal (C, P ⬍ 0.01) competitors. A: in the
presence of a visual competitor, most data points clustered slightly above the
line of unity, demonstrating consistent, albeit small increases in the MSI across
the sample of neurons. B: this elevation of MSI was greater in the presence of
an auditory competitor. C: however, it was the strongest on average in the
presence of a cross-modal competitor, where nearly all the data points fell
above the line of unity. D: these effects are compared in a bar graph. E: MSI
was inversely related to unisensory response magnitude. Filled circles represent a competing condition (cross-modal competitor) and open circles represent the simple condition with no competing stimuli. Conventions are the same
as in previous figures.
modal stimulation was superadditive when the modality-specific components were weak (⬍45%, P ⬍ 0.05). However, if
the competing cross-modal stimulus was composed of strongly
effective modality-specific components, the magnitude of depression was accurately predicted by simple linear summation
(P ⬎ 0.23). The effect of a cross-modal competitor on a
cross-modal excitatory stimulus was more linear throughout
the same range, although a similar relationship was still evident
(Fig. 7D).
Cross-modal vs. within-modal comparisons. The observations that multisensory responses are far more resistant to
depression by competing stimuli and are also far more effective
response suppressors when placed in the opposite hemifield are
consistent with the known consequences of multisensory integration. However, it was also possible that this result was
simply due to target redundancy (multisensory aspect was
irrelevant). To examine this possibility, responses to simultaneously presented adjacent pairs of visual stimuli were also
evaluated for resistance to competitor depression. In addition,
the depressive power of within-modal pairs of visual stimuli
was examined. The visual stimulus pairs were created by
replacing the auditory component of the cross-modal stimulus
with a second visual stimulus that was identical to the first.
Responses to the within-modal stimulus lacked resistance to
depression from competing stimuli. Figure 8 shows an example
neuron. In the simple configuration (Fig. 8B), the response
evoked by the within-modal stimulus was marginally better
than that evoked by either of the component visual stimuli.
However, the within-modal response was greatly decreased in
each of the competing stimulus configurations tested (Fig. 8,
C–E), so that its magnitude was rendered approximately equal
MULTISENSORY INTEGRATION WITH COMPETING STIMULI
to, or smaller than, the responses to single visual stimuli. This
phenomenon reflected the general pattern (n ⫽ 27, Fig. 9A) that
within-modal responses were suppressed by competing stimuli
to approximately the same degree as the best single-stimulus
component response, thereby demonstrating that simple target
redundancy does not confer resistance to depression. On average, within-modal excitatory responses were weaker than those
evoked by the largest modality-specific visual response, a
finding consistent with previous observations (Alvarado et al.
2007). These observations suggest that multisensory responses
are resistant to the depressive effects of competing stimuli (Fig.
9B). The depression induced by a cross-modal competitor was
also significantly stronger than that induced by a within-modal
pair of visual stimuli (n ⫽ 27, P ⬍ 0.01, Fig. 10, A and C). In
some notable examples, there was more than a fivefold difference in depression between cross-modal and within-modal
competitors, even though the simple sums of their component
depressions were approximately equal (P ⬎ 0.83; Fig. 10B).
DISCUSSION
The SC participates in guiding orientation behaviors by
translating sensory signals into appropriate motor commands.
Given a competitive stimulus environment, SC sensory maps
can temporarily represent multiple conflicting targets that require resolution before an accurate motor command towards a
selected target can be issued (Keller and McPeek 2002; Li and
J Neurophysiol • VOL
Basso 2005; McPeek and Keller 2002). Numerous prior studies
have shown that multisensory integration of spatiotemporally
concordant modality-specific cues leads to enhanced activity
for individual SC neurons (Jiang et al. 2001; King and Palmer
1985; Meredith et al. 1987; Meredith and Stein 1996; Stein and
Meredith 1993). In principle, this process should prioritize
cross-modal stimuli for access to SC motor output, as multisensory neurons provide input to the brainstem circuits that
control orientation (Meredith and Stein 1985). However, this
hypothesis has not been tested explicitly, as earlier studies on
SC neurons have not tested the impact of multisensory integration in the presence of competing stimuli.
With the present dataset, we demonstrate that the responses
of SC neurons to cross-modal stimuli are considerably more
resistant to the depressive effects of stimulus competition. This
greater resistance relative to component visual responses is
likely due to differences in response magnitude produced by
multisensory integration. Similarly, visual response magnitude
is inversely related to the magnitude of its depression from
competing auditory stimulation (Jiang and Stein 2003). However, in the current study, multisensory responses were suppressed less than auditory responses of equal magnitude, indicating that differences in simple response magnitude do not
explain all of the results (see discussion on sound translocation
below).
Cross-modal stimuli generally produced the greatest depressive effect on responses, presumably due to the multisensory
enhancement of activity at the competing SC location. Consequently, multisensory integration not only enhances the magnitude of sensory signals, but also minimizes the potentially
distracting effects of competing stimuli, which is consistent
with the hypothesis of cross-modal “priority” and “winnertake-all” algorithms (Basso and Kim 2010; Fecteau and Munoz
2006). These findings suggest that in competitive environments, multisensory integration augments the relative salience
of events composed of cross-modal cues, and are largely
consistent with observations in the owl optic tectum that the
strongest stimulus prevails (Mysore et al. 2011). In addition,
these data provide insight into how competitive interactions at
the level of single neurons might underlie some of the benefits
in behavioral localization attributed to multisensory processing
(Corneil et al. 2002; Gingras et al. 2009; Jiang et al. 2002;
Stein et al. 1988).
The present findings suggest that SC multisensory integration augments orientating behavior in complementary
ways: potentially facilitating orientation to cross-modal
events within corresponding sensory space while simultaneously suppressing orientation to events at alternate locations. Several of the input sources required to support the
enhancement role of multisensory integration within the cat
SC have been established (Alvarado et al. 2009; Jiang et al.
2001; Meredith 1999). Those accounting for the depressive
effects of competing stimuli are somewhat less clear, but
likely candidates include inhibitory projections arising from
the opposite SC, crossed inhibitory projections derived from
the basal ganglia, as well as projections from the anterior
ectosylvian sulcus that contact SC GABA-ergic interneurons en route to the multisensory output neurons (FuentesSantamaria et al. 2009; Jiang and Stein 2003; Kadunce et al.
1997). It is well established that the superior colliculi on
opposite sides are mutually inhibitory, each sending a
106 • DECEMBER 2011 •
www.jn.org
Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.2 on June 18, 2017
Fig. 7. The visual and multisensory response depression induced by a competing cross-modal stimulus often exceeded that predicted by the linear sum of
its component stimulus effects. A: an example neuron showing the cumulative
number of stimulus-driven impulses (Qsum) in simple and competing visual
stimulus configurations. In the competing configuration, a cross-modal competitor profoundly degraded the visual response. In contrast, depression induced by its modality-specific components was either absent (visual) or
moderate (auditory). B: a bar graph summarizing the visual response depression from competing modality-specific and cross-modal stimuli. C and D: a
scatter plot of the population data revealed that the magnitude of depression
from a competing cross-modal stimulus exceeded the predicted linear sum
when the modality-specific competitors were individually weakly depressive.
However, the linear sum accurately predicted the magnitude of depression
from a cross-modal competitor when the modality-specific competitors were
strong individually. Conventions are the same as in previous figures.
3097
3098
MULTISENSORY INTEGRATION WITH COMPETING STIMULI
Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.2 on June 18, 2017
Fig. 8. Within-modal pairs of visual stimuli neither induced more depression nor were more resistant to depression than were their individual component stimuli.
A: polar coordinate lines show the location of the visual stimuli (V1i ,V2i ,Vc1,Vc2) relative to the neuron’s ERF. Visual stimuli were presented in simple (B) and
competing (C–E) configurations, and their effects are displayed in a 12-component matrix. Within-modal stimuli and their individual component stimuli did not
differ significantly in their resistance to depression, or the magnitude of depression they induced in any of the stimulus configurations. Conventions are the same
as in previous figures.
GABA-ergic projection via the intercollicular commissure
(Appell and Behan 1990; Munoz and Istvan 1998; Sooksawate et al. 2011; Takahashi et al. 2010). Likewise, recent
studies have demonstrated that the substantia nigra pars
reticulata (SNr), in addition to its well-known inhibitory
projection to the ipsilateral SC (Chevalier et al. 1984;
Hikosaka and Wurtz 1983; Karabelas and Moschovakis
J Neurophysiol • VOL
1985; May and Hall 1984), provides a crossed inhibitory
input to the opposite SC (Jiang et al. 2003; Liu and Basso
2008). Critically, these SNr projections perform complementary functions, simultaneously facilitating contralateral
stimulus/movement representations (via disinhibition) while
suppressing those to the ipsilateral side. In the present study,
competing stimuli were placed in opposite hemifields to
106 • DECEMBER 2011 •
www.jn.org
MULTISENSORY INTEGRATION WITH COMPETING STIMULI
3099
Fig. 9. The differential effect of competitive stimulus interactions on unisensory and multisensory integration. A: bar graphs compare the mean of the
largest modality-specific visual responses (Vxi ) to the mean of the within-modal
visual responses (V1i V2i ). The within-modal visual response was significantly
smaller than the largest visual response, and competing stimuli had little effect
on this relationship. B: however, competing stimuli increased the percent
difference between the multisensory response and the largest unisensory
response (Xi). Conventions are the same as in previous figures.
simulate mutually exclusive alternatives that would pit opposing SCs against one another. However, it is important to
point out that the present findings should generalize beyond
interhemispheric interactions to stimulus/target selection
mechanisms responsible for choosing among potential targets in the same hemifield. Although this may rely more on
GABA-ergic neurons intrinsic to the SC (Fuentes-Santamaria et al. 2009; Meredith and Ramoa 1998; Sooksawate et al.
2011), there is no a priori reason to suggest that the
principles observed here would fail to apply to intracollicular competitive interactions. Nevertheless, this remains an
empirical question for future study.
The resistance of multisensory responses to depression by
competing stimuli and the ability of competing cross-modal
stimuli to produce substantial depression are consistent with
the observed effects of multisensory integration on response
magnitude. As demonstrated, these effects were specific to
cross-modal pairings and could not be replicated by simply
increasing the number of stimuli within or outside the ERFs.
As reported previously, the responses evoked by multiple
modality-specific stimuli were generally not enhanced (AlJ Neurophysiol • VOL
Fig. 10. Cross-modal competitors depressed visual responses more than did
pairs of within-modal competitors. A: a scatter plot compares depression from
cross-modal competitors (VoAo) to depression from within-modal visual competitors (V1oV2o) on modality-specific (V1i or V2i ) and within-modal (V1i V2i )
visual responses. B: a bar graph shows how the linear sum of the depressions
from competing modality-specific stimulation yielded equivalent values for
both visual and auditory components. C: depression from a competing crossmodal stimulus was significantly greater than depression from a competing
within-modal visual stimulus.
106 • DECEMBER 2011 •
www.jn.org
Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.2 on June 18, 2017
varado et al. 2007; Gingras et al. 2009), and, accordingly,
they proved no more resilient to depression than were
responses to the individual component stimuli. However, we
did note one exception to the rule of cross-modal potency.
While a cross-modal competitor was the most effective at
depressing visual and multisensory responses, in many instances, an auditory response was depressed slightly less by
a competing cross-modal stimulus than a competing modality-specific auditory stimulus (Fig. 6B). While this result
might in part be due to activation of a large auditory-specific
inhibitory surround (Kadunce et al. 1997), it may also be
attributable to summing localization, and the associated
translocation of the perceived auditory stimulus (Cranford
1982; Kelly 1974). In this regard, it is interesting to note
that the presence of the visual component in the cross-modal
stimulus may have moderated this effect by “calibrating” or
“capturing” the location of the competing auditory stimulus
(Bishop et al. 2011; Burr and Alais 2004; Donohue et al.
2011; Hairston et al. 2003; Wozny and Shams 2011),
thereby reducing sound translocation and the associated
auditory response suppression. This result is intriguing in its
own right, as it suggests that multisensory integration could
play a role in segregating sounds in noisy environments, an
important issue deserving of future study.
In simple stimulus conditions, the behavioral enhancement
of event detection and reaction time may be attributable in part
to physiological enhancement at a corresponding location
within the SC sensory topography (Stein and Stanford 2008).
The current study has implications for a wider range of mul-
3100
MULTISENSORY INTEGRATION WITH COMPETING STIMULI
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank Nancy London for technical assistance.
GRANTS
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants EY016716 and NS-036916.
DISCLOSURES
No conflicts of interest, financial or otherwise, are declared by the author(s).
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Author contributions: S.R.P., B.A.R., T.R.S., and B.E.S. conception and
design of research; S.R.P. performed experiments; S.R.P. analyzed data;
S.R.P., B.A.R., T.R.S., and B.E.S. interpreted results of experiments; S.R.P.
prepared figures; S.R.P., B.A.R., T.R.S., and B.E.S. drafted manuscript; S.R.P.,
B.A.R., T.R.S., and B.E.S. edited and revised manuscript; S.R.P., B.A.R.,
T.R.S., and B.E.S. approved final version of manuscript.
REFERENCES
Alvarado JC, Stanford TR, Rowland BA, Vaughan JW, Stein BE. Multisensory integration in the superior colliculus requires synergy among corticocollicular inputs. J Neurosci 29: 6580 – 6592, 2009.
Alvarado JC, Vaughan JW, Stanford TR, Stein BE. Multisensory versus
unisensory integration: contrasting modes in the superior colliculus. J
Neurophysiol 97: 3193–3205, 2007.
Appell PP, Behan M. Sources of subcortical gabaergic projections to the
superior colliculus in the cat. J Comp Neurol 302: 143–158, 1990.
Basso MA, Kim B. A probabilistic strategy for understanding action selection.
J Neurosci 30: 2340 –2355, 2010.
Basso MA, Wurtz RH. Modulation of neuronal activity by target uncertainty.
Nature 389: 66 – 69, 1997.
Basso MA, Wurtz RH. Modulation of neuronal activity in superior colliculus
by changes in target probability. J Neurosci 18: 7519 –7534, 1998.
Bishop CW, London S, Miller LM. Visual influences on echo suppression.
Curr Biol 21: 221–225, 2011.
Burr D, Alais D. The ventriloquist effect results from near-optimal bimodal
integration. Curr Biol 14: 257–262, 2004.
Chevalier G, Vacher S, Deniau JM. Inhibitory nigral influence on tectospinal
neurons, a possible implication of basal ganglia in orienting behavior. Exp
Brain Res 53: 320 –326, 1984.
Corneil BD, Van Wanrooij M, Munoz DP, Van Opstal AJ. Auditory-visual
interactions subserving goal-directed saccades in a complex scene. J Neurophysiol 88: 438 – 454, 2002.
J Neurophysiol • VOL
Cranford JL. Localization of paired sound sources in cats - effects of variable
arrival times. J Acoust Soc Am 72: 1309 –1311, 1982.
Donohue SE, Roberts KC, Grent-’t-Jong T, Woldorff MG. The crossmodal spread of attention reveals differential constraints for the temporal
and spatial linking of visual and auditory stimulus events. J Neurosci 31:
7982–7990, 2011.
Fecteau J, Munoz D. Salience, relevance, and firing: a priority map for target
selection. Trends Cogn Sci 10: 382–390, 2006.
Fuentes-Santamaria V, Alvarado JC, McHaffie JG, Stein BE. Axon morphologies and convergence patterns of projections from different sensoryspecific cortices of the anterior ectosylvian sulcus onto multisensory neurons
in the cat superior colliculus. Cereb Cortex 19: 2902–2915, 2009.
Gingras G, Rowland BA, Stein BE. The differing impact of multisensory and
unisensory integration on behavior. J Neurosci 29: 4897– 4902, 2009.
Hairston WD, Wallace MT, Vaughan JW, Stein BE, Norris JL, Schirillo
JA. Visual localization ability influences cross-modal bias. J Cognitive
Neurosci 15: 20 –29, 2003.
Hikosaka O, Wurtz RH. Visual and oculomotor functions of monkey substantia nigra pars reticulata. 4. Relation Of Substantia Nigra to Superior
Colliculus. J Neurophysiol 49: 1285–1301, 1983.
Jiang H, Stein BE, McHaffie JG. Opposing basal ganglia processes shape
midbrain visuomotor activity bilaterally. Nature 423: 982–986, 2003.
Jiang W, Jiang H, Stein BE. Two corticotectal areas facilitate multisensory
orientation behavior. J Cognitive Neurosci 14: 1240 –1255, 2002.
Jiang W, Stein BE. Cortex controls multisensory depression in superior
colliculus. J Neurophysiol 90: 2123–2135, 2003.
Jiang W, Wallace MT, Jiang H, Vaughan JW, Stein BE. Two cortical areas
mediate multisensory integration in superior colliculus neurons. J Neurophysiol 85: 506 –522, 2001.
Kadunce DC, Vaughan JW, Wallace MT, Benedek G, Stein BE. Mechanisms of within- and cross-modality suppression in the superior colliculus.
J Neurophysiol 78: 2834 –2847, 1997.
Kadunce DC, Vaughan JW, Wallace MT, Stein BE. The influence of visual
and auditory receptive field organization on multisensory integration in the
superior colliculus. Exp Brain Res 139: 303–310, 2001.
Karabelas AB, Moschovakis AK. Nigral inhibitory termination on efferent
neurons of the superior colliculus - an intracellular horseradish-peroxidase
study in the cat. J Comp Neurol 239: 309 –329, 1985.
Keller EL, McPeek RM. Neural discharge in the superior colliculus during
target search paradigms. Ann NY Acad Sci 956: 130 –142, 2002.
Kelly JB. Localization of paired sound sources in rat - small time differences.
J Acoust Soc Am 55: 1277–1284, 1974.
King AJ, Palmer AR. Integration of visual and auditory information in
bimodal neurons in the guinea-pig superior colliculus. Exp Brain Res 60:
492–500, 1985.
Li XB, Basso MA. Competitive stimulus interactions within single response
fields of superior colliculus neurons. J Neurosci 25: 11357–11373, 2005.
Liu P, Basso MA. Substantia nigra stimulation influences monkey superior
colliculus neuronal activity bilaterally. J Neurophysiol 100: 1098 –1112,
2008.
Lomber SG, Payne BR, Cornwell P. Role of the superior colliculus in
analyses of space: superficial and intermediate layer contributions to visual
orienting, auditory orienting, and visuospatial discriminations during unilateral and bilateral deactivations. J Comp Neurol 441: 44 –57, 2001.
May PJ, Hall WC. Relationships between the nigrotectal pathway and the
cells of origin of the predorsal bundle. J Comp Neurol 226: 357–376, 1984.
Mchaffie JG, Stein BE. A chronic headholder minimizing facial obstructions.
Brain Res Bull 10: 859 – 860, 1983.
McPeek RM, Keller EL. Superior colliculus activity related to concurrent
processing of saccade goals in a visual search task. J Neurophysiol 87:
1805–1815, 2002.
Meredith MA. The frontal eye fields target multisensory neurons in cat
superior colliculus. Exp Brain Res 128: 460 – 470, 1999.
Meredith MA, Nemitz JW, Stein BE. Determinants of multisensory integration in superior colliculus neurons. 1. Temporal factors. J Neurosci 7:
3215–3229, 1987.
Meredith MA, Ramoa AS. Intrinsic circuitry of the superior colliculus:
pharmacophysiological identification of horizontally oriented inhibitory interneurons. J Neurophysiol 79: 1597–1602, 1998.
Meredith MA, Stein BE. Descending efferents from the superior colliculus
relay integrated multisensory information. Science 227: 657– 659, 1985.
Meredith MA, Stein BE. Interactions among converging sensory inputs in the
superior colliculus. Science 221: 389 –391, 1983.
106 • DECEMBER 2011 •
www.jn.org
Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.2 on June 18, 2017
tisensory effects, particularly those in which there are multiple
response alternatives to be considered. For example, in a study
requiring saccades to target stimuli presented against a background of audio-visual noise, saccades to cross-modal targets
exhibit the accuracy of visually guided saccades while still
maintaining the short latency of auditory-guided saccades
(Corneil et al. 2002). By increasing the number of competitors
for a single response, the addition of extraneous stimuli may
represent a greater cognitive load. In humans, cross-modal cues
have been shown to significantly reduce reaction time under
conditions of high perceptual load, while modality-specific
cues have no effect in this condition (Santangelo and Spence
2007). The present study examines the impact of multisensory
integration on interstimulus competition, a scenario that (like
those just described) more closely resembles the complex
environment in which this capacity evolved. Within SC activity, multisensory integration confers a strong competitive advantage onto cross-modal events at the expense of their modality-specific counterparts, a finding that has strong implications for stimulus-guided orienting behavior in natural scenes.
MULTISENSORY INTEGRATION WITH COMPETING STIMULI
J Neurophysiol • VOL
Sparks DL. Translation of sensory signals into commands for control of
saccadic eye-movements - role of primate superior colliculus. Physiol Rev
66: 118 –171, 1986.
Stanford TR, Quessy S, Stein BE. Evaluating the operations underlying
multisensory integration in the cat superior colliculus. J Neurosci 25:
6499 – 6508, 2005.
Stein BE, Huneycutt WS, Meredith MA. Neurons and behavior - the same
rules of multisensory integration apply. Brain Res 448: 355–358, 1988.
Stein BE, Meredith MA. The merging of the senses. Cambridge, Mass: MIT
Press, 1993, p. xv, 211 p.
Stein BE, Stanford TR. Multisensory integration: current issues from the
perspective of the single neuron. Nat Rev Neurosci 9: 255–266, 2008.
Takahashi M, Sugiuchi Y, Shinoda Y. Topographic organization of excitatory and inhibitory commissural connections in the superior colliculi and
their functional roles in saccade generation. J Neurophysiol 104: 3146 –
3167, 2010.
Wallace MT, Meredith MA, Stein BE. Multisensory integration in the
superior colliculus of the alert cat. J Neurophysiol 80: 1006 –1010,
1998.
Wozny DR, Shams L. Recalibration of auditory space following milliseconds
of cross-modal discrepancy. J Neurosci 31: 4607– 4612, 2011.
106 • DECEMBER 2011 •
www.jn.org
Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.2 on June 18, 2017
Meredith MA, Stein BE. Spatial determinants of multisensory integration in
cat superior colliculus neurons. J Neurophysiol 75: 1843–1857, 1996.
Meredith MA, Stein BE. Visual, auditory, and somatosensory convergence
on cells in superior colliculus results in multisensory integration. J Neurophysiol 56: 640 – 662, 1986.
Munoz DP, Istvan PJ. Lateral inhibitory interactions in the intermediate layers
of the monkey superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 79: 1193–1209, 1998.
Mysore SP, Asadollahi A, Knudsen EI. Signaling of the strongest stimulus
in the owl optic tectum. J Neurosci 31: 5186 –5196, 2011.
Perrault TJ, Vaughan W, Stein BE, Wallace MT. Superior colliculus
neurons use distinct operational modes in the integration of multisensory
stimuli. J Neurophysiol 93: 2575–2586, 2005.
Rizzolatti G, Camarda R, Grupp LA, Pisa M. Inhibitory effect of remote
visual-stimuli on visual responses of cat superior colliculus - spatial and
temporal factors. J Neurophysiol 37: 1262–1275, 1974.
Rowland BA, Quessy S, Stanford TR, Stein BE. Multisensory integration
shortens physiological response latencies. J Neurosci 27: 5879 –5884, 2007.
Santangelo V, Spence C. Multisensory cues capture spatial attention regardless of perceptual load. J Exp Psychol 33: 1311–1321, 2007.
Sooksawate T, Isa K, Behan M, Yanagawa Y, Isa T. Organization of
GABAergic inhibition in the motor output layer of the superior colliculus.
Eur J Neurosci 33: 421– 432, 2011.
3101