* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Hippocampal contributions to language
Cognitive neuroscience wikipedia , lookup
Aging brain wikipedia , lookup
Neurolinguistics wikipedia , lookup
Lateralization of brain function wikipedia , lookup
Emotional lateralization wikipedia , lookup
Cognitive neuroscience of music wikipedia , lookup
Hippocampus wikipedia , lookup
Holonomic brain theory wikipedia , lookup
Prenatal memory wikipedia , lookup
Exceptional memory wikipedia , lookup
Adaptive memory wikipedia , lookup
State-dependent memory wikipedia , lookup
Emotion and memory wikipedia , lookup
Sex differences in cognition wikipedia , lookup
Childhood memory wikipedia , lookup
Collective memory wikipedia , lookup
Misattribution of memory wikipedia , lookup
Eyewitness memory (child testimony) wikipedia , lookup
Memory consolidation wikipedia , lookup
Source amnesia wikipedia , lookup
Memory and aging wikipedia , lookup
Socioeconomic status and memory wikipedia , lookup
Limbic system wikipedia , lookup
De novo protein synthesis theory of memory formation wikipedia , lookup
University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Theses and Dissertations Spring 2014 Hippocampal contributions to language: an examination of referential processing and narrative in amnesia Jake Christopher Kurczek University of Iowa Copyright 2014 Jake C. Kurczek This dissertation is available at Iowa Research Online: http://ir.uiowa.edu/etd/4672 Recommended Citation Kurczek, Jake Christopher. "Hippocampal contributions to language: an examination of referential processing and narrative in amnesia." PhD (Doctor of Philosophy) thesis, University of Iowa, 2014. http://ir.uiowa.edu/etd/4672. Follow this and additional works at: http://ir.uiowa.edu/etd Part of the Neuroscience and Neurobiology Commons HIPPOCAMPAL CONTRIBUTIONS TO LANGUAGE: AN EXAMINATION OF REFERENTIAL PROCESSING AND NARRATIVE IN AMNESIA by Jake Christopher Kurczek A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Doctor of Philosophy degree in Neuroscience in the Graduate College of The University of Iowa May 2014 Thesis Supervisor: Assistant Professor Melissa C. Duff Graduate College The University of Iowa Iowa City, Iowa CERTIFICATE OF APPROVAL _______________________ PH.D. THESIS _______________ This is to certify that the Ph.D. thesis of Jake Christopher Kurczek has been approved by the Examining Committee for the thesis requirement for the Doctor of Philosophy degree in Neuroscience at the May 2014 graduation. Thesis Committee: ___________________________________ Melissa C. Duff, Thesis Supervisor ___________________________________ Steven Anderson ___________________________________ Neal Cohen ___________________________________ Daniel Tranel ___________________________________ Michelle Voss ___________________________________ Kristine Williams The story is one that you and I will construct together in your memory. If the story means anything to you at all, then when you remember it afterward, think of it, not as something I created, but rather as something that we made together. Orson Scott Card Ender’s Game ii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I am extremely grateful to all of the people without whom this research would not be possible. First and foremost, I’m grateful to the research participants and their families for donating their time and effort. In the last five years, I have learned so much from these amazing and interesting people, and I want to thank them for sharing their time, stories and lives with me. Secondly, I would like to thank my mentor Melissa Duff, who has done much more than “throw me a bone” as I requested in our first meeting. She has done everything possible in order to help me succeed as a scientist, educator and mentor in the past five years and I wouldn’t be any where near the person/scientist/teacher that I am today without her. I would also like to thank the members of my thesis committee, Daniel Tranel, Neal Cohen, Steven Anderson, Michelle Voss and Kristine Williams for their helpful contributions to shaping this work in particular. I’d like to thank my friends in colleagues who have contributed to my education at Iowa and my ability to be a researcher in neuroscience. I’d like to thank Ruth Henson and Keary Saul for their work scheduling participants and reminding me how to be a good person and researcher. Thank you to Joel Bruss and Nick Jones for helping me to understand computers and technology better and for our always helpful conversations. Without the help of Isabelle Hardy, Linda Hurst, Anita Kafer and Dan Tranel, navigating the road of being a graduate student would be much more difficult. I also want to thank the many members of the Communication and Memory Lab for their help transcribing, coding and testing, especially, Samantha Shune, Laura Savicki, Margaret Miller, Sarah Kirk, Janelle Beadle, Samantha Crooks, and in particular for this dissertation Allison Alpers and Roxanne Calderwood. I’d also like to thank some of the people that have contributed to my graduate education more broadly. I’d like to thank Mitchell Kelly, Mike Baker, Dan Lehn and Wendy Dunn for all that they have done to help me become a better educator. I’d like to iii thank Dave Warren for his helping me to become a more sophisticated thinker and a better scientific programmer. I’d like to thank the Obermann Center for Advanced Studies and Iowa Campus Compact for their work bringing research, scholarship and the community together. I’d also like to acknowledge and thank my fellow students in the neuroscience program for their guidance, help, and support especially Bradley TaberThomas, Rupa Gupta Gordon, and Erik Asp. Finally, I want to thank my family for their love and support. I am extremely thankful to my wife Kameko Halfmann who helps to keep me grounded and whose patience, understanding and thoughtfulness has been much appreciated throughout graduate school and life. iv ABSTRACT Language production is characterized by an unlimited expressive capacity and creative flexibility that allows speakers to rapidly generate novel and complex utterances. In turn, listeners interpret language “on-line”, incrementally integrating diverse representations to create meaning in real-time. A challenge for theories of language has been to understand how speakers generate, integrate, and maintain representations in service of language use and processing and how this is accomplished in the brain. Much of this work has focused prefrontal cortex mechanisms such as “working memory”. The goal of this dissertation is to understand the role of the hippocampal declarative memory system (HDMS) in language use and processing, specifically in referential processing and narrative construction. To test the role of the hippocampus in referential processing, healthy comparisons, brain damaged comparisons (BDC), individuals with bilateral hippocampal damage participated in an eyetracking experiment in which individuals viewed scenes and listened to short stories. The amount of time participants spent looking at the characters after a pronoun reference was recorded. Healthy comparisons and BDC participants preferentially targeted the first mentioned character while participants with hippocampal damage did not to the same degree, suggesting that the hippocampus plays a role in maintaining and integrating information, even in short discourse history. In a second experiment, participants with bilateral hippocampal damage and healthy comparisons told narratives multiple times over the course of a month. The narratives were analyzed for the number of words, the number of episodic details, the number of semantic details, the number of editorials and the consistency of details over the multiple tellings. The patients with hippocampal damage told stories that were significantly shorter, more semanticized and less consistent from telling to telling than healthy comparisons. v The final goal of this study was to understand the effects of unilateral hippocampal damage on language processing. Individuals with unilateral hippocampal damage participated in all of the previous experiments. It was predicted that individuals with left hippocampal damage would perform worse than individuals with right hippocampal damage, though there was a trend towards a deficit their performance was not significantly impaired across measures. This suggests that the left hippocampus may be particularly important for processing linguistic material outside of even verbal memory. This dissertation enters a line of work demonstrating the contributions of the hippocampus to cognition outside memory. We are beginning to see the mark of the hippocampus across all areas of cognition, both early and continuously. This and other evidence appears to suggest that the processing capacities of the hippocampus are called upon and used by many (most) other neural structures in the formation and establishment of complex human behavior including language processing. vi TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ............................................................................................................. ix LIST OF FIGURES ............................................................................................................X CHAPTER 1: SIGNIFICANCE AND SPECIFIC AIMS....................................................1! CHAPTER 2: NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL INVESTIGATION INTO LANGUAGE AND MEMORY .......................................................................6 2.1. Neuropsychology overview .......................................................................6! 2.2. Language..................................................................................................10! 2.2.1. Language processes .......................................................................10! 2.2.2. Neural substrates ...........................................................................12! 2.2.3. Language interim summary ...........................................................18! 2.3. Memory....................................................................................................18! 2.3.1. Memory processes .........................................................................18! 2.3.2. Neural substrates ...........................................................................20! 2.3.3. Cognitive processes and theories...................................................28! 2.4. Memory and Language ............................................................................32! 2.4.1. Connecting the hippocampus, declarative memory, and language ...................................................................................................33! 2.4.2. Hippocampal Declarative (Relational) Memory System: Contributions to Language ......................................................................37! CHAPTER 3: GENERAL METHODOLOGY .................................................................43 3.1 Participants ...............................................................................................43! 3.2. Statistical Approach .................................................................................44! CHAPTER 4: REFERENTIAL PROCESSING - FACILITATING PRONOUN COMPREHENSION IN INDIVIDUALS WITH PROFOUND MEMORY IMPAIRMENT ............................................................................45 4.1. Background/Rationale .............................................................................45! 4.2. Specific Aims and Hypotheses ................................................................50! 4.3. Methods ...................................................................................................51! 4.3.1. Participants ....................................................................................51! 4.3.2. Materials ........................................................................................52! 4.3.3. Procedure .......................................................................................53! 4.3.4. Analysis .........................................................................................54! 4.4. Results......................................................................................................55! 4.4.1. Offline Data. ..................................................................................55! 4.4.2. Eye-movement Data. .....................................................................56! 4.5. Discussion ................................................................................................58 vii ! CHAPTER 5: NARRATIVE IN AMNESIA: THE EFFECTS OF REPEATED TELLINGS ON NARRATIVE CONSTRUCTION ......................................68 5.1. Background/Rationale .............................................................................68! 5.2. Specific Aims and Hypotheses ................................................................75! 5.3. Methods ...................................................................................................76! 5.3.1. Participants ....................................................................................76! 5.3.2. Materials ........................................................................................76! 5.3.3. Procedure .......................................................................................77! 5.3.4. Analysis .........................................................................................78! 5.4. Results......................................................................................................79! 5.4.1. Multiple Tellings Autobiographical Memory Interview Analysis (Condition 1) ............................................................................79! 5.4.2. Multiple Tellings Consistency Analysis for hippocampal amnesics and their comparisons ..............................................................80! 5.4.3. Self Ratings of Multiple Tellings for hippocampal amnesics and their comparisons ..............................................................................80! 5.4.4. War of the Ghosts Analysis (Condition 2) ....................................81! 5.5. Discussion ................................................................................................81! CHAPTER 6: HIPPOCAMPAL LATERALITY: THE EFFECT OF UNILATERAL HIPPOCAMPAL DAMAGE ON LANGUAGE USE .......100! 6.1. Background ............................................................................................100! 6.2. Specific Aims and Hypotheses ..............................................................103! 6.3. Methods .................................................................................................104! 6.3.1. Participants ..................................................................................104! 6.3.2. Experimental protocol .................................................................104! 6.4. Results....................................................................................................105! 6.4.1. Referential processing .................................................................105! 6.4.2. Narrative construction analysis for patients with unilateral hippocampal damage .............................................................................108! 6.5. Discussion ..............................................................................................110! CHAPTER 7: CONCLUSIONS ......................................................................................121! APPENDIX: THE WAR OF THE GHOSTS ..................................................................127! REFERENCES ................................................................................................................128! viii LIST OF TABLES Table 1. Demographic and neuropsychological characteristics of hippocampal amnesic and BDC participants..................................................................................64! Table 2. Study 1: On-line referential processing experimental conditions. .......................65! Table 3. Demographic and neuropsychological characteristics of hippocampal amnesic participants..................................................................................................90! Table 4. Comparison of amnesic and healthy comparison narratives over time. ..............94! Table 5. Comparison of hippocampal amnesic and healthy comparison performance on War of the Ghosts retelling. .................................................................................98! Table 6. Demographic characteristics of patients with unilateral hippocampal damage. ...................................................................................................................114! Table 7. Neuropsychological characteristics of patients with unilateral hippocampal damage. ...................................................................................................................115! ix LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1. Offline judgments of story-picture match for hippocampal amnesics, BDC and comparison participants. ...........................................................................66! Figure 2. Time course of fixation preferences for hippocampal amnesics, BDC participants and healthy comparison participants. ....................................................67! Figure 3. Memory ratings for condition 1, multiple tellings..............................................91! Figure 4. Internal to overall ratio across multiple tellings for hippocampal amnesics and healthy comparison participants.........................................................................92! Figure 5. Consistency data for multiple tellings for hippocampal amnesics and the healthy comparison participants. ..............................................................................93! Figure 6. Self Ratings for multiple tellings for hippocampal amnesics and their healthy comparisons .................................................................................................96! Figure 7. War of the Ghosts accuracy data for hippocampal amnesics and their healthy comparisons. ................................................................................................97! Figure 8. Off-line performance for unilateral hippocampal patients. ..............................117! Figure 9. Time course of fixation preference data for unilateral hippocampal patients. ...................................................................................................................118! Figure 10. Internal details across multiple tellings for unilateral hippocampal patients. ...................................................................................................................119! Figure 11. Consistency data for unilateral hippocampal patients in the multiple telling. .....................................................................................................................120 x 1 CHAPTER 1: SIGNIFICANCE AND SPECIFIC AIMS This thesis brings together the study of two quintessential human capacities, memory and language, by examining the role of the hippocampal declarative memory system (HDMS) in language processing and use. At the heart of this proposal is the idea that the same processes by which the HDMS contributes to the formation of new memories, and maintains representations on-line to be used in service of complex behavior, are the same processes for using and processing language (Duff & BrownSchmidt, 2012). Evidence in support of this proposal comes from work demonstrating that patients with hippocampal damage and declarative memory impairment have deficits in a variety of linguistic and discursive abilities (e.g., Duff, Hengst, Tranel & Cohen, 2008; MacKay, Stewart & Burke, 1998). The proposed work here extends this proposal by expanding the scope of the potential HDMS contribution to two language phenomena that are ubiquitous in everyday uses of language, referential processing and narrative construction. Informed by theories of language processing and use and cognitive neuroscience theories of memory, the research approach capitalizes on a compelling opportunity to combine neuropsychological methods with eye-tracking and behavioral measures. Performance of patients with hippocampal amnesia, who have bilateral hippocampal damage and severe and selective impairments in declarative memory will be compared to healthy comparison participants. The proposed work is significant theoretically and has potential clinical implications. Defining and characterizing the cognitive and neural systems that support language is a key goal in the neuroscience of language. Although the HDMS has received 2 little consideration as a key neural/cognitive system for language use and processing (outside of lexical acquisition/consolidation), preliminary and anticipated findings from the proposed studies fit with increasing evidence that the language network extends beyond the traditional structures of the left perisylvian areas to include the hippocampus and declarative memory (Duff & Brown-Schmidt, 2012). Recent research demonstrating that the hippocampus also plays a critical role in the generation and use of on-line representations created during ongoing information processing to support behavioral performance positions the hippocampus, for the first time, as a candidate neural system for supporting language (Rubin, Brown-Schmidt, Duff, Tranel, & Cohen, 2011). In the memory literature, there is increasing evidence that the hippocampus plays a critical role in cognitive abilities beyond its traditional role in memory (e.g., perception (Warren, Duff, Tranel & Cohen, 2010; 2011), creativity (Duff, Kurczek, Rubin, Tranel & Cohen 2013). The interdisciplinary and integrated approach taken here to studying the memorylanguage interface is timely as it dovetails with recent trends in the neuroscience literature of examining the coordination of cognitive and neural systems in the production of complex behavior. Finally, linking deficits in language to HDMS impairment offers a more fine-grained characterization of the underlying mechanisms of the observed nonaphasic language deficits common in neurological and psychiatric diseases where HDMS deficits are prominent (e.g., TBI; Alzheimer’s disease, schizophrenia). 3 Aim 1: To define and characterize the role of the hippocampal dependent declarative memory system (HDMS) in on-line referential processing. Efficient and successful language processing relies on the ability to establish and maintain reference (e.g., proper nouns, pronouns) as most of what we talk about involves referring to things in the world. Using reference places a demand on the ability to both track information across time and bind information and representations from potentially rich contexts. It is hypothesized that the HDMS contributes to on-line referential processing. It is predicted that patients with damage to the HDMS will be impaired compared to healthy participants in the ability to form and maintain the representations necessary for successful referential processing in the moment in contexts where referents are established with minimal representation. Specifically, combing neuropsychological and eyetracking methods, participants will view images while listening to narratives. In Aim 1 with an eye towards rehabilitation, I attempt to recover performance of the participants with amnesia by providing more context in the discourse to firmly establish the target character. It is predicted that participants with hippocampal amnesia will benefit from increased representation information and preferentially view the target character. Aim 2: To define and characterize the role of the HDMS in narrative construction. Personal narrative allows one to use language to give life events a temporal order, to demystify them, and to establish coherence across a life lived, living and yet to be lived (Ochs & Capps, 2001). Recent work on the functions of the hippocampus suggest it is engaged in behaviors outside of (re)constructing memories for the past (and future), 4 and may potentially by necessary for any behavior that requires the flexible generation, and subsequent (re)combination of perceptually distant and distinct mental representations on-line and in response to novel contexts (Shohamy & Turk-Brown, 2013). It is hypothesized that the HDMS plays a critical role in the generation and flexible use of representations that compose narratives. In Aim 2, I will investigate the impact of hippocampal damage on the repeated tellings of either episodic or semantic narratives over time. In healthy populations, participants have been shown to become less detailed (i.e., less words/details) in their tellings of semantic narratives while they become more detailed in their telling of their own episodic narratives. It is predicted that patients with hippocampal amnesia will not be able to narrate the semantic stories while performance on the episodic narratives will be impaired because hippocampal damage will cause greater variability in the reproduction of narratives (i.e. inconsistent telling of details) over time. Specific Aim #3: To investigate the contribution of left versus right damage to the hippocampus on referential processing and narrative construction. Functional lateralization of the brain has indicated that the left hemisphere is primarily responsible for processing language while the right hemisphere is responsible for processing spatial information. After unilateral damage to the temporal lobe, mild deficits in material specific recall as been observed (Milner 1968; 1990; Naugle, 1992) with damage to the left hemisphere causing problems in verbal recall and damage to the right hemisphere causing deficits in spatial recall. However there is a question of whether these deficits are deficits due to damage to the hippocampus or a loss of connection to the 5 other cortical areas. In other words are the deficits observed after unilateral damage to the hippocampus due to a loss of hippocampal functioning or the loss of the ability to connect to higher cortical areas. It is hypothesized that damage to the left hippocampus will disrupt hippocampal functioning and language processing capacities of the hippocampus to a greater extent than right hippocampal damage. Therefore, it is predicted that patients with left hippocampal damage will perform significantly worse than patients with right hippocampal damage on both the referential processing and narrative construction experimental measures. 6 CHAPTER 2: NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL INVESTIGATION INTO LANGUAGE AND MEMORY 2.1. Neuropsychology overview Language (Hauser, Chomsky, & Fitch, 2002; Pinker & Jackendoff, 2003; Ujhelyi, 1996) and episodic memory (Roberts & Feeney, 2009; Suddendorf, 1994; Tulving & Markowitsch, 1998) are two cognitive abilities that are thought to separate mankind from the rest of the animal kingdom. In the history of mankind’s investigations into the inner workings of his brain, through disciplines such as philosophy, psychology, and more recently neuroscience, the prevailing notion has been to study each type cognitive ability and each brain area separately, before attempting to study them together (Fodor, 1983). While the history of brain-behavior relationships stretches back to the dawn of mankind (e.g. Egyptian physicians performing trepanation; Arnott, Finger & Smith, 2003; Hippocrates’ work with Greek gladiators; Hippocrates, 460 BCE), its modern investigation can only be traced back to the 19th century. Franz Gall, inspired or perhaps limited by faculty psychology, hypothesized that the brain consisted of a number of separate organs, each responsible for a basic trait. Further, he reasoned that skill or capacity of various traits would increase the size of that brain area and affect the size of the skull (Gall, 1825). This set of hypotheses, termed phrenology, formed the early basis for the localization theory of brain function. A number of reports in the middle of the century helped to lend credence to the idea of localized brain functions. The first, occurred in 1848, when a railroad foreman suffered a brain injury to the frontal lobe caused by a tamping iron (Harlow, 1868). Phineas Gage had been described as an adept, mild-mannered and well-respected 7 foreman of the railroad before his injury, and as fitful, irreverent, profane and impatient, while on the whole largely intact in all other mental capacities after (Harlow, 1868). The description of Phineas Gage is one of the first to account for a circumscribed lesion and its resulting loss of a specific set of abilities. However, one of the most famous cases of cognitive deficit localizability in lesion damage occurred in 1861 when Paul Broca announced the most well known breakthroughs in localization of higher cognitive functioning, noting that motor speech was specifically located in the posterior, inferior region of the left frontal lobe (Broca, 1861a, 1861b). It has been noted that this was one of the first indications that specific locations in the brain support specific functions and also that the left and right hemispheres separate their contributions to various functions (Zillmer, Spiers & Culbertson, 2008). A decade later, Carl Wernicke announced that understanding of speech was located in the superior, posterior temporal lobe, when he discovered patients who had fluent speech but were unable to understand speech (Wernicke, 1874). In a different cognitive domain, memory, it wasn’t until the early 1950s that we found evidence for the localizability of memory in patient H.M. (Scoville & Milner, 1957). These three sets of patients provided a foundation in the support of particular cognitive functions being supported by distinct areas of the brain. While localization received much attention, not all of it was in support. A number of investigators criticized the idea of localization, including Sigmund Freud who questioned how to explain partial and mixed aphasias through Broca’s and Wernicke’s understanding (Freud, 1891). Others including Pierre Flourens (who popularized equipotentiality) and later Pierre Marie (Marie, 1906) and Karl Lashley (whose beliefs were more tempered than Flourens, by advocating, multipotentiality and mass action, as 8 well as localization of basic functions; Lashley, 1929) were proponents of a more distributed view of cognitive abilities. As the debate between localization and equipotentiality waged on, evidence continued to mount on the side of localized cognitive functions. John Hughlings Jackson, however, suggested a combination of the two views in which behavior results from the interaction among all the areas of the brain. Jackson’s idea was further adapted by Alexander Luria, who proposed that behavior arises from the interaction of three functional units (e.g. arousal/muscle tone; reception, integration and analysis of sensory information; planning, executing and verifying behavior; Luria, 1964; 1966). While all behavior is represented by the brain as a whole, each brain area has a specific role in shaping behavior. In the fifty years since Luria published his accounts on behavior, the fields of neuropsychology, neuroscience, and cognitive psychology have continued to enhance our understanding of brain functioning by delineating the contributions of various brain areas to particular cognitive functions. While more modern investigation and research techniques have provided a more detailed list of cognitive functions and the brain areas that are both necessary and sufficient for those functions, less is known about how coordinated activity across the brain creates behavior. This history of neuropsychology provides the perfect backdrop for the investigation at hand. Localization of function has provided the opportunity for uncovering a vast number of brain areas and their associated cognitive functions. However, sometimes, when a particular cognitive function becomes attributed to a certain area, research has been focused on that particular relationship, rather than on the contribution of that area to other cognitive functions, or conversely, the contribution of 9 multiple brain structures to a particular function. Of interest to the current investigation are the views that two prominent language scientists held in regard to the contribution of memory to language. Broca (1865) and Wernicke (1874) thought that that language production and comprehension were achieved via separate stages involving units and processes that were dissociable from memory storage and retrieval, meaning that problems in language could be independent of memory disorders. The principles of multipotentiality or pluripotentiality are fundamental to the current investigation. The historical roots of brain behavior relationship in localization of function has largely resulted in the study of cognitive capacities, including memory and language, in isolation. For the past sixty years, researchers have investigated and delineated the contribution of the hippocampus to declarative memory, while perisylvian structures have been probed for close to 150 years in their relationship to language. However, new research is beginning to describe the hippocampus in broader terms outside of memory processing, as contributing more fundamental processing immediately and continually while the language network has been expanding beyond the perisylvian cortical structures of the left hemisphere. This thesis will continue the trend of investigations which have strayed away from the contribution of the hippocampus solely to memory, and will investigate the more recent conceptualization of memory and language functioning which sees those systems as interactive and interdependent, and specifically investigate the contribution of the hippocampus to language processing. What follows is a summary of the work on the localization of language and memory and a framework for examining the interaction/interdependencies of language and memory. 10 2.2. Language 2.2.1. Language processes Language is so pervasive in our lives that it has become an axiom of the human experience. Language is the principle mechanism by which human beings communicate and bond with one another. Language allows us to provide instructions, note our needs and desires and work collaboratively. Through oral traditions and writing, language allows us to connect to our present, our past and the future; it provides continuity to the human experience. Because of the seeming ease by which we speak and understand speech, people often take their language ability for granted. All human languages share a set of fundamental properties including arbitrary relations, incremental processing, generativity, and multi-modality. The language system is composed of a set of arbitrary relations in that, with some limited exceptions, conceptual information cannot be derived from the acoustic signal itself. This is demonstrated by the fact that there are few, if any words across languages that mean the same thing. While some point to onomatopoeic words such as “meow” or “bark” as counter examples to arbitrariness, even these examples rarely hold up across languages where for example in Spanish, the sound dogs make is “guau.” Language is learned over time in a series of presentations between form and a meaning selected randomly with noise involved in both the form (what/how someone says something) and meaning (there are many unique items that combine to form the meaning exemplar; Gasser, 2004). Language processing is incremental, meaning that it occurs over time, at a rate of about 150-200 words per minute (Levelt, 1989; Tauroza & Alison, 1990). The meaning 11 of many words are ambiguous (e.g. lexically, Allopenna, Magnuson, & Tannenhaus, 1998; referentially, Hanna, Tanenhaus, & Trueswell, 2003; and syntactically, Tanenhaus, Spivey-Knowlton, Eberhard, & Sedivy, 1995) until later in the sentence, when multiple sources of information must be generated, integrated, and maintained in real-time to create meaning. In its most strict interpretation, incremental processing means that information is fully integrated with previously processed material as soon as it is available. At the level of syntax, incrementality would mean that each word is integrated into a syntactic structure immediately. As a result of this property, speakers are constantly integrating information and making predictions (Federmeier & Ktuas, 1999; Federmeier, 2007) based on the scenes they are within (Altmann & Kamide, 2007), past scenes (Altmann & Kamide, 2009), the subject (Kamide, Altmann & Haywood, 2003) and the speaker (Van Berkum, Van den Brink, Tesink, Kos, & Haagort, 2008). With this continual building of representations, listeners and speakers are constantly binding representations into rules (e.g. grammar, syntax, semantic, pragmatic) and forming understanding of what is being communicated. Attempts to identify what separates human language capacities from other creatures in the animal kingdom have long pointed to the generative capacity of human language and communication (Chomsky, 1957). Language use is flexible and creative meaning that the symbols of language can be combined and recombined in new and unusual ways to convey meaning. Indeed, particular words can mean very different things in different contexts and to different listeners (Tannen, 1989). Speakers recreate, repurpose and re-contextualize language over time and settings (Bakhtin, 1986; Prior, 2001) in order to convey different meanings. 12 Finally, language use is multi-modal encompassing far more than a stream of spoken words. Language and communication combines words with changes in the body either in the face such as gaze (Meyer, Sleiderink, & Levelt 1998), or in the rest of the body in the case of gesture (Wagner Cook & Tanenhaus, 2009). Language can even extend to objects outside of the person including the way objects in the environment are used (Olson, 1970). While there is some dispute as to the number of properties that are considered to be fundamental to language, which can vary from as few as four to as many as sixteen, what is important in regard to this thesis is the contribution of brain to language processing (Hockett, 1958). So while these fundamental properties of language are considered universal and are accomplished rapidly and with seemingly little effort, how they are accomplished in the brain is not fully understood and much of the work in neuroscience, psycholinguistics and other fields is dedicated to understand the cognitive and neural bases of language. 2.2.2. Neural substrates 2.2.2.1. Left hemisphere language network One hundred and fifty years ago, Paul Broca introduced a revolutionary paper emphasizing the importance of cerebral localization (Broca, 1861a, 1861b) and research regarding language has been under intense study since. However, Broca wasn’t the first to note the role of the left hemisphere in language. Hippocrates (460 BCE) noted that the loss of speech was often accompanied by a loss of movement on the right side. So while this relationship had been noted for 2000 years, its further description began in the 19th 13 century. Marc Dax (1865) was one of the first to indicate the left hemisphere (or hemisphere opposite the dominant hand) was important to language. Although, Dax’s paper is published four years after Broca’s, his findings were originally presented in 1836. The “classic language model” which combines the research of Broca (and Dax) with other neuropsychologists’ findings from aphasic patients has long been the standard bearer in our understanding of language processing. This model proposes frontal motor area for speech production (Broca, 1861a; 1861b) and posterior receptive area for speech understanding (Wernicke, 1874). The connection of these two language areas within the brain formed the basis of the understanding of language as being supported by the perisylvian (i.e., around the sylvian or lateral fissure) structures of the left hemisphere (Catani, Jones, & Ffytche, 2005). These early findings in aphasic patients were fundamental to our early understanding of the localization of cognitive functions and provided a shift in a major debate occurring at the time, which revolved around the idea about whether cognitive functions were localized or processed by the brain as a whole. With the evidence from the aphasics, the prevailing notion of the 19th century came to understand the left hemisphere as the “major” hemisphere, dominant, verbal, analytic and intelligent, while the “minor” right hemisphere was described as nondominant, nonverbal, visuospatial, holistic and creative (Harrington, 1987). Further investigation of language using new technologies has both confirmed and extended the classic language model. Penfield and Roberts (1959) mapped sites in the left hemisphere where electrical stimulation interfered with speech. The conceptualization of language that was formed by lesion investigation continued until the 1960s as more evidence about language localization and lateralization was gathered with a new 14 technique using patients with a different type of brain lesion. A new technique emerged in the 1960s with the use of patients who had undergone copus callosotomy, in which the corpus callosum is sectioned leaving little communication between the hemispheres. Roger Sperry (1961) confirmed the laterality findings that had been demonstrated in the lesion patients, however, they also revealed the involvement of the right hemisphere in language processing. Another advancement in technology, with the development of functional imaging, has begun to reveal more intricacies in the language processing system. Early studies used positron imaging technology (PET) to image the language related images of the left hemisphere while subjects viewed words, listened to words, spoke and generated word associations (Petersen, Fox, Posner, Mintun, Raichle, 1988; Demonet et al., 1992). Another new technology, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), followed PET, and with it we were able to view dynamic changes in brain activation during language processing (Hinke, Hu, Stillman, Kim, Merkle, Salmi, & Ugurbil, 1993; McCarthy, Blamire, Rothman, Gruetter, & Shulman, 1993; Rueckert, Appollonio, Grafman, Jezzard, Johnson, Le Bihan, & Turner, 1994; Binder, Rao, Hammeke, Frost, Bandettini, Jesmanowicz, & Hyde, 1995; Demb, Desmond, Wagner, Vaidya, Glover, & Gabrieli, 1995; Shaywitz, Pugh, Constable, Shaywitz, Bronen, Fulbright, Shankweiler, et al., 1995; Binder, Frost, Hammeke, Cox, Rao, & Prieto,1997). With these new techniques our understanding of language processing beyond the “classic language model” is becoming more and more complicated, especially our understandings of the right hemisphere’s contribution. 15 2.2.2.2. Right hemisphere Language Network While the “classic language model” became the foundation of our understanding of language, even Broca (1865) added caveats to his first description, noting that while the left hemisphere is more efficient language processor, it may not be the exclusive center of language processing. Following this notion, John Hughlings Jackson (1868) proposed an alternative to the “classic language model.” Jackson proposed that Broca’s findings supported the role of the left hemisphere in speech production, not language as a whole. Jackson proposed that right hemisphere shares a role in language processing with the left, however despite his findings, the conceptualization of the “dominant” left hemisphere persisted. Sperry (1961) challenged the exclusivity of language processing in the left hemisphere with their work using split-brain patients, where he found intact language abilities in the left-hemisphere if not always expressive language abilities. Indeed, the right hemisphere is unable to produce language in over 95% of the population (Loring, Meador, Lee, Murro, Smith, Flanigin, et al., 1990). Although the right hemisphere appears to be less able to produce language, researchers have hypothesized the anatomy of the right hemisphere to mirror that of the left, with more anterior portions involved in motor components and more posterior and inferior portions involved in semantics and comprehension. Further, investigators have more thoroughly investigated the language functioning of right hemisphere brain damaged patients with the list of deficits including both the linguistic and extralinguistic domains. Deficits include: difficulty generating inferences, comprehending/producing central themes, reduced sensitivity to 16 communicative context (Myers, 1999), integrating context (Hough, 1990), word fluency, comprehension (Myers & Mackisack, 1990), and understanding non-literal language due to impulsivity, inefficiency and egocentricity (Blake, 2007). Taken together, the language network was understood to be the connection between Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas on (primarily) the left hemisphere with complementary support of extra- and para-linguistic functions on the right hemisphere. 2.2.2.3. Frontal Lobes The frontal lobes are one of the most controversial areas of the human brain in terms of assigning cognitive abilities. Lesions to the frontal lobes often produce a variety of symptoms. Outside the contribution of Broca’s area in the frontal lobe to language production, other subdivisions of the frontal lobes have also been implicated in language processes. Lesions of the anterior and medial left frontal lobe have been implicated in a communication disorder that involves formulation of language and other characteristics of “frontal lobe syndrome” (Benson, 1988). Large lesions of the right frontal hemisphere including areas analogous to Broca’s area and extending into DLPFC have been demonstrated to critically contribute to pragmatic abilities such as communicating coherently (Delis, Wapner, Gardner, & Moses, 1983; Joanette, Goulet, Ska, & Nespoulous, 1986) and avoiding tangential or irrelevant comments (Weylman, Brownell, & Gardner, 1988). Kaczmarek (1984) found in an investigation of 105 patients with frontal lobe lesions that the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex was involved with sequencing the pattern of an utterance, where the orbital cortex was involved with the development of a narrative. Additionally, implications from Baddely and Hitch’s theory of working 17 memory (1974) suggest that deficits in working memory (and thus a deficit in the frontal lobe) would impair language learning. Evidence to support this claim comes from children with specific language impairment who have both a difficulty learning language and an impairment in digit span (Baddely, Papagno, & Vallar, 1998; Gathercole & Baddely, 1990). 2.2.2.4. Subcortical structures Both Wernicke (1874) and Marie (1906) suggested that subcortical structures contributed to language processing. Stimulation of different subcortical structures led to various behavioral performances, where stimulation of the globus pallidus produced a deficit in the ability to produce language (Hermann, Turner, Gillingham, & Gaze, 1966), stimulation of the head of the caudate produced a deficit in the ability to produce comprehensible language (Van Buren, 1963). Although the precise role of the thalamus and basal ganglia in language processing remains unclear, there are clues from patients with subcortical lesions (Nadeau & Crosson, 1997), Parkinson’s disease (Copland, 2003; Copland, Chenery, & Murdoch, 2001), schizophrenia, (Titone, Levy, & Holzman, 2000) and Tourette’s syndrome (Legg, Penn, Temlett, & Sonnenberg, 2005). Broadly, the thalamus has been suggested to play a role in semantic processing (Kraut, Kremen, Moo, Segal, Calhoun, & Hart 2002), while the basal ganglia has been implicated in language control (Friederici, 2006) and grammatical processing (Grossman, Lee, Morris, Stern, & Hurtig, 2002; Ullman, 2001). Evidence from functional imaging has implicated the basal ganglia in syntactic computation (Friederici, Ruschemeyer, Hahne, & Fiebach, 2003; Moro, Tettamanti, Perani, Donati, Cappa, & Fazio 2001). 18 2.2.3. Language interim summary Language processing requires a complicated coordination of a number of topdown and bottom-up processes. Regardless of whether you hear or see a word or sentence, both are going to be built of small pieces of information (phonemes and graphemes respectively) which are combined in some sort of orderly fashion (grammar) made into something meaningful (semantics). The speed with which this occurs, makes language processing seem almost automatic, to the point where we take for granted the complex processing and interactions occurring in order to craft understandable discourse and comprehend the communications of others. The fundamental processing requirements of the language system, arbitrary relations, incremental processing, generativity, and multi-modality, on their face appear complicated and demanding enough to require support from other cognitive systems. Following is a discussion of memory and later a discussion of recent findings indicating that the hippocampal declarative memory system is capable of many of the processing requirements of the language system and is able to contribute on a time-scale necessitated by language. 2.3. Memory 2.3.1. Memory processes Over the centuries, many metaphors have been used to describe memory. An ancient Egyptian legend describes Mnemosyne, the god of memory who gave a wax tablet, in which our perceptions and impressions are stamped into. Since then, a series of metaphors has been proposed for the mechanisms of memory, each working within the confines of the technology of the time. The metaphor of a dovecote or aviary was long used, where memories, like birds would fly in and out of our minds. In the Middle Ages, 19 a more technologically advanced version of the wax tablet, the book, provided an understanding of the functioning of memory (this metaphor is still prevalent to describe our knowledge for words, where the lexicon is imagined to be the brain’s dictionary). In the nineteenth century, the progress of technology led to a series of metaphors about machines including photography (e.g., photographic memory), the telephone system, and the phonograph (which used wax covered cylinders). Even today, our colloquial understanding of memory uses the metaphor of the computer. It is argued that memory is the most basic and important operation of the brain in that many cognitive processes such as language, planning, and problem-solving rely on memory for effective operation (Tranel & Damasio, 2002). But what is memory? An easy why is to describe it is by the processes or mechanisms that memory performs, where memory refers to knowledge that is stored in the brain and to the processes associated with acquiring, encoding, storing, and retrieving this information. Another understanding of memory is to understand what memory is for. Memory holds onto the details of everyday life, what time is the meeting, where did I park my car. But memory is also for holding onto information for a short period of time, attempting mental math or remembering the phone number you just received from someone (when you don’t have your cell phone on you). Memory is also for remembering the aspects of our lives particular to us, the events and people and things particular to each individual, the facts and statistics available to everyone, but also for learning the associations and procedures between common occurrences in life, like the particular rigmarole of each social situation (e.g., the procedures for a proper seminar include arriving early for the snacks, attentively listening and after the lecturer finishes, asking difficult questions; Cohen & Banich, 20 2003). While memory was once thought of as a unitary function (Lashley, 1950), one of the most important developments was the recognition that there is more than one kind of memory supported by different structures of the brain (Cohen, 1984; Tulving, 1985). 2.3.2. Neural substrates 2.3.2.1. Hippocampus and the Medial Temporal Lobe Karl Lashley spent a considerable amount of time looking for the “engram” the structure in the brain where memories could be found or stored. In lesioning the brains of animals across the cortex and in varying amounts with subsequent behavioral tests, he was unable to impair memory performance, leading him to conclude that it may be impossible to locate the “engram” (Lashley, 1950). However, years before, the first hint that the medial temporal lobe plays a critical role in human memory occurred in 1899 when a Russian physician demonstrated the brain of a patient with softened uncus, hippocampus and medial temporal cortex, who had shown a severe memory impairment (Bekhterev, 1899). Moving forward approximately 50 years to 1953, the case of HM marks the seminal case linking the hippocampus and declarative memory. Interestingly, this was just three years after Lashley (1950) declared it impossible to localize the memory trace after being unable to find the memory trace in his search for the engram. HM’s case is often described as serendipitous as his case was one of more than a dozen cases at the time undergoing experimental surgical procedures to treat a number of ailments including epilepsy and psychiatric illness, but because of confounding variables, HM’s neuropsychological profile appeared to be the cleanest of the group (Scoville, 1954; Scoville & Milner, 1957). Before the systematic investigation of HM, memory was 21 once thought to be a unitary faculty (i.e the “engram”), however, the study of HM and other patients with memory impairments has provided evidence that memory is manifested across multiple systems that are supported by functionally and anatomically distinct brain systems and that memory is a fundamental property and natural outcome of the brain’s ongoing processing (Eichenbaum & Cohen, 2001; Gabrieli, 1998; Ranganath & Paller, 1999; Squire & Wixted, 2011). Investigation of HM’s abilities revealed that the hippocampus and medial temporal lobe structures (MTL) were necessary in the consolidation (but not storage) of memories and critical for forming new memories (Scoville & Milner, 1957). Over the next 50 years of investigation, HM demonstrated a number of distinctions in memory systems including a split between declarative (including episodic – specific personal experiences; and semantic – general facts and knowledge; Cohen & Squire, 1980) and non-declarative (e.g. skills, priming, habituation, simple classical conditioning) memory in that the hippocampus is critical for processing declarative but not non-declarative memory (Cohen & Squire, 1980) and a distinction between long-term and short-term memory where the hippocampus is critical for long-term memory, but not short term memory (Baddeley & Warrington, 1970; Warrington & Baddeley, 1974). Despite the severe deficit in declarative memory, HM demonstrated that memory could be distinguished from other cognitive abilities and had intact perceptual, motor, and cognitive functions, immediate memory, and even spared remote memory, and that selective impairments in memory itself could teased apart (Eichenbaum & Cohen, 2001). Recent work in neuroimaging has also implicated the hippocampus in memory processing. Early investigations into the functioning of the hippocampus using functional 22 imaging focused on the role of the hippocampus during encoding and retrieval and found that activity of the hippocampus was similar during encoding and retrieval (Small, Nava, Perera, DeLaPaz, Mayeux, & Stern, 2001) and activity during encoding could predict the success of retrieval (Stark & Okado, 2003). The hippocampus is active in relation to both visual and spatial memory (Bellgowan, Saad, & Bandettini, 2003) Hippocampal activation has been observed when participants recollect a studied event (Eldridge, Knowlton, Furmanski, Bookheimer, & Engle, 2000), remember the source of an item (Casino, Maquet, Dolan, & Rugg, 2002; Davachi, Mitchell, & Wagner, 2003) and remember the associations amongst items (Giovanello, Schnyer, & Verfaellie, 2004). 2.3.2.2. Extended MTL A number of medial temporal lobe structures beyond the hippocampus including perirhinal cortex, parahippocampal cortex and entorhinal cortex have been implicated in various memory functions. While the hippocampus has been primarily implicated in recollection, familiarity has been suggested to depend on adjacent cortex (Brown & Aggleton, 2001; Rugg & Yonelinas, 2003). Another subdivision of memory contributions within the medial temporal lobe has been between relational and item memory, where relational memory depends primarily on the hippocampus, the perirhinal cortex and other regions neighboring hippocampus support item memory (i.e., the binding of inflexible relations; Cohen, Poldrack, Eichenbaum, 1997). Several functional imaging studies have suggested that the hippocampus and parahippocampal cortex is active in both the retrieval of associations and also recollective memory (Davachi et al., 2003; Kirwan & Stark, 2004; Ranganath, Yonelinas, Cohen, Dy, Tom, & D’Esposito, 2003), but many studies 23 have found evidence for activation of the entorhinal and perirhinal cortices (Dobbins, Rice, Wagner, & Schacter, 2002; Davachi et al., 2003) and parahippocampal cortex (Kirwan & Stark, 2004) in familiarity based memory. 2.3.2.3. Frontal Lobes While hippocampal damage impairs long-term memory, it has traditionally been thought to leave short-term or working memory intact. The first well known patient with a deficit was KF whose left tempo-parietal lesion impaired his ability to hold onto even short strings of words or digits despite long-term memory (Shallice & Warrington, 1970). Other deficits have been thought to be linked to various aspects of language processing, comprehension versus production (Caramazza, Miceli, Silveri, & Laudanna, 1985), or the visuospatial workpad (Baddely, 1986) in the case of visual verbal working memory impairments. Evidence from the animal literature has suggested that working memory can be linked to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) as lesions in monkeys impairs their ability to find rewards in a delayed-response task with delays as short as even one second (Goldman-Rakic, 1988; Goldman-Rakic, 1995). Converging methodology in human imaging has also indicated the importance of the DLPFC in holding information on-line about spatial location (Smith & Jonides, 1999). The frontal lobes have also been implicated in encoding memories. Ventrolateral regions of the frontal lobes are activated during acquisition and encoding of information (Buckner, 1996; Buckner & Koustaal, 1998; Kelley, Miezin, McDermott, Buckner, Raichel, Cohen, Ollinger, et al., 1998; Poldrack & Gabrieli, 1998). The prefrontal cortex has also been implicated in retrieval as patients with damage to this area, who have 24 problems organizing and monitoring memory retrieval tend to confabulate (Moscovitch & Winocur, 1995) and have problems in recognition tasks where they disproportionate amount of false positives (Schacter, Reiman, Uecker, Polster, Yun, & Cooper, 1995). Other portions of the frontal lobes including the DLPFC have been implicated in retrieval (Buckner, 1996; McDermott, Buckner, Petersen, Kelley, & Sanders 1999; Nyberg, Cabeza, & Tulving, 1996; Wagner, Desmond, Glover, Gabrieli, 1998). Complimentary findings from functional imaging have implicated frontal contributions to memory such as when individuals are asked to maintain information over short delay (Courtney. Ungerleider, Keil, & Haxby, 1997; Zarahn, Aguirre, & D’Esposito, 1999). Activation of the frontal lobes during encoding (Cabeza & Nyberg, 2000; Kim, Daselaar, & Cabeza, 2010) and retrieval (Shimamura, 1995; Blumenfeld & Ranganath, 2007; Blumenfeld, Parks, Yonelinas, & Ranganath, 2011) have also been observed. 2.3.2.4. Lateral Temporal Lobe Semantic memory (conceptual knowledge) is the memory of general knowledge, word meanings, facts and people, without personal connection to place or time (Tulving, 1985). Conceptual knowledge is largely accessible across persons in a culture (although the scope depends on the individual), while episodic memory ties a particular experience to specific times and places. While the interest in how a concept is represented in the brain has been of interest for centuries, the ability to study its representation in the brain has only emerged recently. Early reports on the loss of specific categories of conceptual knowledge relied on reports from patients with widespread damage, such as demented 25 patients or patients with Herpes Simplex Encephalitis (Warrington & Shallice, 1984). More specific brain localization relied on patients with focal brain damage and category specific loss of semantic representation. A report by Damasio and colleagues (1996) used a combination of PET imaging in normal subjects and lesion overlap analysis in neuropsychological patients to investigate lexical retrieval, finding that the normal process of retrieving concrete words depends on distinct regions of the left hemisphere outside of the classic language area. While some work has focused on the theoretical organization of conceptual knowledge, four theories have focused on the neuroanatomical organization. Work with neuropsychological patients has shaped Damasio and colleagues hypotheses of the conceptual system (Damasio et al, 1996; Tranel, Damasio, & Damasio, 1997). Conceptual knowledge is organized in multiple convergence/divergence sites, which direct the reconstruction of explicit pieces of the concept in the relevant sensory cortices (which is essential for conscious processing of the concept). Intermediary regions (distinct regions of the temporal cortex) promote the activation of sensorimotor representations and serve as a flexible feedback/feedforward projection site (Tranel et al., 1997). These intermediary regions are activated by preferential physical characteristics and contexts of entities, which leads to the possibility of neurally distinct, category specific loss of conceptual knowledge. While Damasio and colleagues’ account of conceptual processing appeals to brain areas outside of the classical language network, Jung-Beeman’s bilateral activation, integration, selection (BIAS) theory proposes a framework of multiple bilateral processes to construct meaning (Jung-Beeman, 2005) within the classic language network. The 26 framework assumes three distinct but interactive components representing the activation (posterior middle and superior temporal gyri), integration (anterior superior temporal gyrus, superior temporal sulcus, middle temporal gyrus) and selection (inferior frontal gyrus) of semantic information (Jung-Beeman, 2005). Further, the semantic processes occur bilaterally with each hemisphere contributing a different level of processing of the information, with the right hemisphere providing coarse semantic coding and the left hemisphere providing fine semantic coding (Jung-Beeman, 2005). A third theory of concrete conceptual representation appears to be combine aspects of the previous two. Just and colleagues (2010) present a neurosemantic theory of concrete noun representation in which semantic factors (manipulation, shelter and eating) underpin the representation of concrete noun and are represented in neurally distinct areas (anterior superior parietal, posterior temporal, and posterior lateral frontal respectively; Just et al., 2010). The final theory envisions the organization of the semantic system as a “hub-andspoke” in which semantic information is represented in both modality-dependent cortices (“spokes”) and the anterior temporal lobes convergence zone (an amodal hub; Patterson, Nestor & Rogers, 2007). 2.3.2.5. Parietal Cortex The most recent addition to the neural network of structures underlying memory is the lateral parietal lobe (PL). Typically the PL has been associated with functions such as visuospatial attention and visually-guided reaching. Research into the role of PL regions in memory has grown following Wagner and colleagues (2005) review of the functional neuroimaging literature, which highlighted how PL responses, in particular on 27 the left, are closely linked with episodic retrieval success. A meta-analysis by Simons et al. (2008) identified that PL regions may be consistently activated during recollection than core episodic memory regions (i.e. MTL) and has been echoed in other meta analyses including Skinner and Fernandes (2007). Patient studies have indicated that while the patients with damage to the PL are not amnesic, their memory may not be quite normal on tasks of autobiographical memory (Berryhill, Phuong, Picasso, Cabeza, & Olson, 2007) or recognition (Davidson, Anaki, Ciaramelli, Cohn, Kim, Murphy, Troyer, et al., 2008). 2.3.2.6. Striatum While hippocampal damage impairs declarative memory, it has traditionally been thought to leave non-declarative memory intact, lending thought to the double dissociation between the hippocampus and declarative memory and non-declarative memory and striatal structures (Knowlton, Squire, & Gluck, 1994). Evidence from studies of patients with Parkinson’s disease and Huntington’s disease suggests that the striatum is critical to non-declarative learning. In one task, patients were unable to perform the rotary pursuit task in which a participant tracks a target spinning in a circle with a handheld stylus. Generally, individuals improve their ability to track the target over time, but the patients with striatal damage were unable to (Gabrieli, 1995). Converging methodology from functional imaging has indicated activation in the striatum with learning on this task (Grafton, Nazzuitta, Presty, Friston, Frackowwiak, & Phelps, 1992). In another example of a more cognitive non-declarative task, the serial reaction time task, in which an individual presses certain buttons in response to spatial locations 28 of a target. Unbeknownst to the participant, there is a pattern that is repeated within the trials, that in healthy people is accomplished faster over time. In patients with damage to the striatum, this effect is not observed (Willingham & Koroshetz, 1993; Ferraro, Balota, & Conor, 1993; Pascual-Leone, Grafman, Clark, Stewart, Massaquoi, Lou, et al., 1993). Again, converging evidence from functional imaging reveals activation of the striatum during learning on this (Grafton, Hazeltine, & Ivry, 1995) and similar tasks (Seitz, Roland, Bohm, Greitz, & Stone-Elander, 1990; Seitz & Roland, 1992). More recent literature from neuroimaging has also implicated the striatum in nondeclarative/procedural memory (Kumari, Gray, Honey, Soni, Bullmore, Williams, Ng, et al., 2002; Martis, Wright, McMullin, Shin, & Rauch, 2004; Rauch, Walen, Savage, Curran, Kendrick, Brown, Bush, et al 1997; Zedkova, Woodward, Harding, Tibbo, & Purdon, 2006). 2.3.3. Cognitive processes and theories 2.3.3.1. Declarative memory The relative impairment of episodic and semantic memory by neurological disorders has implications for theories of long-term memory. How we conceptualize the processing and functioning of the hippocampus has significant effects on what cognitive capacities we understand the hippocampus can contribute to. The traditional view of the functioning of the hippocampus comes from our understanding of amnestic disorders. In 1881, Ribot proposed that there is a time gradient in retrograde amnesia where the most recent memories are the most likely to be lost. The ‘standard model’ of memory consolidation (e.g. Squire, 1992) proposes that both episodic and semantic information 29 becomes independent of the hippocampus after consolidation. Hippocampal damage should, therefore, lead to a temporal gradient for both episodic and semantic information with greater sparing of remote than recent information. This view fits well into the understanding of the hippocampus and its critical contribution to long-term memory, where the hippocampus is responsible for both storing memories early and later retrieving memories that have been consolidated in the cortex. Other theories in contrast, for example, the Multiple Trace Theory (MTT; Moscovitch, Rosenbaum, Gilboa, Addis, Westmacott, Grady, & McAndrews, et al. 2005) suggests that semantic but not episodic memory becomes independent of the hippocampus over time. According to MTT, medial temporal lobe damage should lead to a temporally extended impairment of episodic memory; for semantic memory, MTT, like the consolidation model, predicts a standard temporal gradient. Examination of patients with medial temporal lobe damage has produced mixed results; some studies favor the standard consolidation model (e.g. Bayley, Frascino, & Squire, 2005; Kirwan, Wixted & Squire, 2008), and others MTT (e.g. Steinvorth, Levine & Corkin, 2005; Poreh, Winocur, Moscovitch, Backon, Goshen, Ram & Feldman, 2006; Rosenbaum, Moscovitch, Foster, Schnyer, Gao, Kovacevic, Verfaellie, et al., 2008). Previous studies have confirmed the occurrence of remote memory deficits in patients with dysfunctional temporal lobes but have differed on their precise nature. Some studies have revealed an impairment in autobiographical memory throughout the entire life span (e.g. Viskontas, McAndrews, & Moscovitch, 2000; Noulhiane, Piolino, Hasboun, Clemenceau, Baulac, & Samson, 2007), whereas in others the deficit extends back as little as 5 years (Kapur, Millar, Colbourn, Abbott, Kennedy, & 30 Docherty, 1997). Viskontas et al. (2000) found autobiographical memory deficits with intact personal semantics, while others have reported deficits in both autobiographical memory and semantic memory for public events with intact personal semantic memory (Lucchelli & Spinnler, 1998; Voltzenlogel Despres, Vignal, Steinhoff, Kehrli, & Manning, 2006) or disproportionate loss of public semantics compared to autobiographical memory (Barr, Goldberg, Wasserstein, & Novelly, 1990; Manning et al., 2005). This evidence is consistent with the dissociations observed between components of remote memory in other contexts (e.g. O’Connor, Butters, Miliotis, Eslinger, & Cermak, 1992; Graham & Hodges, 1997) suggesting there is at least partial independence between these processes (Kapur, 1999). This suggestion converges with neuroimaging evidence that shows neural overlap between components of remote memory as well as unique contributions corresponding to the specific properties of the retrieved memories (e.g. Graham, Lee, Brett, & Patterson, 2003; Levine, Turner, Tisserand, Hevenor, Graham, & McIntosh, 2004; Svoboda, McKinnon & Levine, 2006; Burianova & Grady, 2007). However, the conceptualization of the functioning of the hippocampus in either the standard model or MTT limits its processing capabilities to the domain of declarative memory. 2.3.3.2. Relational memory The critical role of the hippocampus and related medial temporal lobe regions in the formation and subsequent retrieval of new enduring memories (i.e., long-term memory) is incontrovertible as demonstrated by decades of neuroscience and cognitive neuroscience research (Cohen, 1984; Cohen & Eichenbaum, 1993; Eichenbaum & 31 Cohen, 2001; Gabrieli, 1998; Squire, 1987; 1992). Cohen and Eichenbaum (1993) hypothesize that the hippocampal system, is critical for learning and forming new declarative memories because of its processing capacities. Damage to the hippocampus selectively impairs the ability to acquire new declarative memories while non-declarative (procedural) memory remains intact. Declarative representations supported by the hippocampus are uniquely flexible (which stands in stark contrast to the relatively static and inflexible memory representations supported by other brain regions), permitting rapid integration with other representations. These representations are then accessible to other processing systems (as when a rich, multisensory autobiographical memory is evoked by the sight of a familiar face or the sound of a familiar song), and permitting representations to be used in novel contexts (Cohen, 1984; Dusek & Eichenbaum, 1997; Eichenbaum & Cohen, 2001; O’Keefe & Nadel, 1978; Squire, 1992). Rather than serve as the storage site of declarative memories, the hippocampus serves to bind together and interact with representations between various neocortical processors (e.g., language processors, spatial processors, face processors) forming relational representations. Further, the hippocampus supports the creation and integration of representations for events including information about the co- occurrences of people, places, and things, and the ability to link the spatial, temporal and interactional relations among them across time (e.g., a word and its meaning; a pronoun and its referent; a narrative and a specific speaker) (Cohen & Banich, 2003; Davachi, 2006; Eichenbaum & Cohen, 2001; Giovanello, Verfaellie, & Keane, 2003; Konkel, Warren, Duff, Tranel, & Cohen, 2008). These cortical processors thus act as the storage sites of memory with each processor storing the particular elements/attributes of the experience or event that it 32 processed (i.e., linguistic elements are stored in language processors; visual representations are stored in or near primary visual cortex). The view of the hippocampus in relational memory theory, where its processing capacities allow for the rapid and on-going access and manipulation of cortically bound representations, allows for a more flexible understanding of the processing capacities of the hippocampus. Rather than just limited to memory processing, the hippocampus is able to flexibly contribute to cognition more generally where a rapid, on-going manipulation of representations is required. This thesis is grounded in this theory as its conceptualization of the hippocampus allows the hippocampus to promiscuously contribute to its fundamental processing capacities to a number of cognitive processes. Below, I discuss how the processing demands of language and the processing capabilities of the hippocampus overlap to a great extent and that the hippocampus may be a critical neural substrate of language capacities that rely on the processing contributions of the hippocampus. 2.4. Memory and Language There have been longstanding efforts to link language to cognitive and neural systems outside of the “traditional language network.” The idea that language relies on other cognitive domains is not new. In healthy and disordered populations the effective use of language has been linked to executive functioning (e.g., Coelho, 2002; Novick, Trueswell, & Thompson-Schill, 2005) and attention (e.g., McNeil, Odell, & Tseng, 1991; Murray, Holland & Beeson, 1998) to name a few. The relationship between memory and language has been of special interest and specific hypotheses have been made regarding 33 the mapping of particular language components (e.g. phonology, lexicon, grammar) and processes (e.g. production, comprehension) to multiple forms of memory (e.g. working memory, declarative memory, procedural memory; e.g., Caplan & Waters, 1998; Crosson, 1992; Gathercole & Baddeley, 1993; Gupta & MacWhinney, 1997; Just & Carpenter, 1992; Ullman, 2004). With the exception of vocabulary acquisition and the role of explicit remembering on extended discourse and text production/processing, the contribution of declarative memory to the processing and use of language has received little attention. Instead, the bulk of work directed at linking language to an aspect of memory has focused on a relationship with working memory (e.g., Caplan & Waters, 1998; Just & Carpenter, 1992). This has been true even in clinical populations for whom profound declarative memory impairments are a hallmark such as traumatic brain injury and Alzheimer’s disease (e.g., Bayles, 2003; Dijkstra, Bourgeouis, Allen, & Burgio, 2004; Youse & Coelho, 2005). 2.4.1. Connecting the hippocampus, declarative memory, and language Although the relationship between memory and language has received a lot of attention, outside of word learning, the hippocampal declarative memory system has received little attention. For example, one model that has received some attention in terms of the differential contribution of memory systems to language is the declarative/procedural model proposed by Ullman and colleagues (Ullman, 2001; Ullman, 2004; Ullman, Corkin, Coppola, Hickok, Growdon, Koroshetz, & Pinker 1997) which argues that language depends directly on the same brain structures that also 34 subserve memory. In this model Ullman (2001, 2004) argues that temporal lobe structures and the declarative memory system subserve the mental lexicon of memorized word-specific knowledge. In contrast, a network of frontal, basal ganglia and cerebellar structures, underlying procedural memory are responsible for mental grammar, the rulegoverned combination of lexical items into complex representation. The declarative/procedural model has mapped onto the spared and intact abilities following amnesia. Kensinger, Ullman, and Corkin, (2001) found that the lexical and grammatical knowledge that the amnesic patient H.M. acquired during his childhood and prior to his surgery did not differ from normal age- and education-matched control subjects, while, Gabrieli and colleagues (1988) found that H.M. was unable to acquire new lexical information (i.e., words and their definitions) following his amnesia. In an investigation of procedural knowledge following hippocampal damage, Knowlton, Ramus, and Squire (1992) found that 13 amnesic participants performed as well as comparison participants on an artificial grammar task suggesting that learning novel grammatical structures can be supported by nondeclarative memory systems, which are intact in amnesia. The declarative/procedural model of language easily maps onto the standard model of memory, where the hippocampus serves to store and retrieve long-term memories, in this case, words. However, what if we view the capacities of the hippocampus through the relational memory theory, are there other language abilities that the hippocampus may contribute to? Much of our understanding of the contribution (and perceived lack there of) of declarative memory to language stems from the study of the famous neurological patient H.M. In the earliest descriptions of his profile (Milner, 35 Corkin, & Teuber, 1968; Scoville & Milner, 1957), the researchers were struck by the severity and selectiveness of the deficit of his ability to form new long term memories, indicating that he had many intact abilities including intelligence and perceptual abilities. The integrity of language in amnesia has been controversial and of great interest since the beginning of H.M.’s formal testing. There are many opinions on the nature of H.M.’s language deficits and whether language deficits exist at all; some claimed H.M. had a pure memory deficit and that language comprehension was undisturbed (Milner, Corkin, and Teuber, 1968). Neuropsychological testing of the language abilities of individuals with amnesia has revealed that they do not have aphasia and furthermore, the ability of amnesic individuals to engage in sophisticated conversations (MacKay, Burke, & Stewart, 1998) has lead to the assumption that language production abilities were intact. However, despite the assertion of normal language, Ogden and Corkin (1991) noted that while H.M.’s was able to discuss the current topic, his language often became markedly tangential and at other times rote and repetitious. As early as 1974, researchers began to more closely examine his language following the thinking that his lack of deficits in language functioning could be due to the lack of probing of the full capacity of language (Lackner, 1974). Lackner (1974) found that while H.M.’s ability to detect ambiguity appeared intact, his performance in segmenting ongoing perceptual speech is not. Further investigation however have indicated that H.M.’s ability has remained intact in a number of language abilities including, lexical retrieval (Skotko, Rubin, & Tupler, 2008), phonology, morphology, direct references, phatic function, conative response, metalinguistic abilities, poetic 36 abilities and extended narrative ability among others (Skotko, Andrews, & Einstein, 2005) syntax (Corkin, 1984; Skotko, Andrews, & Einstein, 2005), semantics (Schmolck, Kensigner, Corkin, & Squire, 2002; Skotko, Andrews, & Einstein, 2005) or lexical and grammatical processing (Kensigner, Ullman, & Corkin, 2001). While others have called into question the language performance of H.M. (c.f. MacKay, Burke & Stewart, 1998; MacKay & James, 2001), and others have criticized the work with H.M. because of extended pathologies (e.g., cerebellar damage), complicating factors (age, long-term medication usage, and seizure disorder) and a lack of contact with current memory theories (Squire, 2011) few of these investigations of H.M.’s language performance finely probe abilities that place high demand on the creative and flexible manipulation of multiple representations. It appears when an individual with amnesia is required to balance, match or compare multiple representations, whether they be time (e.g. the past and present or present and future in reporting speech or describing an event), person (e.g. that he refers to the boy and not the police man), or item (e.g. that the game refers to a game we both have access to and knowledge of), deficits in the ability to narrate those instances becomes more difficult. If individuals with amnesia and hippocampal damage do indeed show deficits on language tasks that probe the processing demands of language that overlap with the processing capacities of the hippocampus, they may provide evidence that the hippocampus plays a more fundamental role in cognition besides contributing to the storage and retrieval of declarative memories and rather serves to generate and relationally bind together representations that can be used in service of multiple cognitive abilities (Cohen & Eichenbaum, 1993; Eichenbaum & Cohen, 2001) including 37 declarative memory, perception (Warren et al., 2010), creativity (Duff, et al., 2013), and language. 2.4.2. Hippocampal Declarative (Relational) Memory System: Contributions to Language The demands of language production and comprehension are immense. Fundamental to all human languages is an unlimited expressive capacity and a creative flexibility that allow speakers to rapidly generate novel and complex utterances, but a challenge for theories of language processing has been to understand how listeners generate, maintain and integrate representations while interpreting language rapidly, incrementally and in real time. Much of the focus on working memory has been due to its purported ability to maintain information on line. However, given the recent discovery of the functionality of the hippocampal declarative memory system (i.e., its generativity and flexibility) and the implication emerging work that new hippocampal-dependent representations are available rapidly enough to influence ongoing processing (e.g., Warren et al., 2010), this form of memory seems uniquely suited to meet the demands of understanding and using language. The processing demands of language, arbitrary relations, processing incrementally over time, being flexible/creative and having multi-modal characteristics are the same processing capacities that are brought to bear in the hippocampus’ contribution to declarative memory. For example, the in language we are required to bind arbitrary speech sounds to meanings and evidence has shown that the hippocampus engages in arbitrary bindings. Subsequently, patients with hippocampal damage are impaired at 38 learning arbitrary word pairs (Gabrieli et al., 1988; Winocur & Weiskrantz, 1976). Another processing demand of language, incremental processing has also been demonstrated as a function of the hippocampus, where the hippocampus is engaged over short time courses (Cabeza, Dolcos, Graham, & Nyberg, 2002; Öztekin, McElree, Staresina, & Davachi, 2009; Ranganath, Cohen, & Brozinsky, 2005; Ranganath & D'Esposito, 2001). Accordingly, when individuals with amnesia are asked to maintain representations over short-time courses their performance is impaired compared to healthy performance (Hannula et al., 2006; Warren et al., 2010). Flexibility and creativity are yet more processing demands of language that have been demonstrated as capacities of the hippocampus (Duff et al., accepted). Again, when patients are assessed for their ability to be flexible and creative with their language, their performance is impaired compared to healthy performance (Duff, Hengst, Tranel & Cohen, 2009). A final processing demand of language, multi-modality, meaning that a word and its meaning includes the physical, visual, auditory and other physical sensations as well as how it is expressed with in the body and environmental context, is yet another processing capacity of the hippocampus (Damasio, Graff-Radford, Eslinger, Damasio & Kassell, 1985). In a tightly controlled series of experiments, participants were asked to identify material that had been altered across spatial, sequential (temporal) and associative (co-occurrence) and found that individuals with amnesia were deficient across all tests of relations (Konkel et al., 2008). Thus, there are compelling reasons to predict that disruptions in hippocampal functioning would be revealed in language use (see MacKay et al., 1998 for an alternative account). 39 Indeed, Duff and colleagues, grounding their work in the relational memory theory, have shown that hippocampal amnesia impairs various aspects of everyday language use, when the language use demands the processing capacities of the hippocampus, including disruptions in collaborative referencing (Duff et al., 2008), verbal play (Duff et al., 2009), procedural discourse (Duff, Hengst, Tengshe, Krema, Tranel, & Cohen 2008), reported speech (Duff, Hengst, Tranel, & Cohen, 2007), cohesion and coherence (Kurczek & Duff, 2011) and definite references such as “the dissertation” as opposed to “a dissertation” (Duff, Gupta, Hengst, Tranel & Cohen, 2011; Duff, Hengst, Gupta, Tranel, & Cohen, 2011). Recently, this work has been extended to examine the role of hippocampaldependent declarative memory in perspective-taking processes using on-line language measures (e.g., eye tracking), revealing striking failures in amnesia patients to integrate self- versus other-perspective information into on-line ambiguity resolution processes (Rubin et al., 2011). Rubin, et al. (2011) asked eye-tracked amnesic patients and healthy comparison participants to interpret definite references such as “the duck” in scenes containing two ducks, one of which was known to a discourse partner, and one of which was not. When the identity of the shared duck was visually marked in the scene or mentioned in an immediately preceding discourse, amnesic patients, like healthy comparisons were largely successful at looking to the shared duck. By contrast, when a 40s unrelated delay was introduced between identifying the shared duck and subsequently referring to it, amnesic patients (but not comparisons) were unable to identify the shared duck. These findings suggest potential deficits in referential processing when processing of the referential expression requires maintaining a 40 representation of a previous discourse across an intervening unrelated delay (Rubin et al., 2011). Kurczek and colleagues (2013) investigated the contributions of the hippocampus to on-line referential processing. Using a visual real world paradigm, they monitored eye movements of individuals with hippocampal amnesia, brain-damaged comparisons and healthy comparisons while they viewed a scene and listened to a brief, two-sentence narrative. They found that individuals with amnesia experienced difficulty in integrating and maintaining information about the discourse even over a very short discourse history (Kurczek, Brown-Schmidt & Duff, 2013). This work not only extended the cognitive contributions of the hippocampus to referential processing during comprehension but also provided further evidence for the contribution of the hippocampus to on-line processing. While the notion that the language abilities of H.M. and other patients with hippocampal damage are fully intact has been challenged (e.g., Duff et al., 2007; MacKay, Stewart, & Burke, 1998; Park, St-Laurent, McAndrews, & Moscovitch, 2011), these studies all used off-line methodologies that tapped the final, ultimate interpretation/production of an utterance. Thus, little is known about the time-course of language processing in amnesia—that is, how impairments to the processes provided by the hippocampus affect processing as it unfolds in real time, leading to the possibility that these patients exhibit even more subtle deficits in the time-course with which they access and process linguistic information, even in cases where they are ultimately successful. For example, consider the amnesic patients’ performance on the Token Test (see Table 1), a standardized neuropsychological assessment of language comprehension where subjects are asked to “Point to a circle” or “Pick up the small yellow circle and the large 41 black square” when presented with small and large squares and circles of different colors. This test was designed to capture the more deleterious language impairments associated with aphasia, and thus amnesia patients are nearly all at ceiling levels of performance. Yet, the results of the present study raises the possibility that even though the patients are ultimately successful at performing the command they may be challenged in the incremental processing of the unfolding utterance with respect to the context (e.g., small yellow circle). If so, such disruptions, which are not evident in the end-state of performance (i.e., picking up the right object), may be revealed using the type of on-line assessment deployed here. Indeed, such a pattern of behavioral accuracy and disrupted language processing as measured by participant eye movements has been shown in adolescents with specific language impairment (McMurray, Samelson, Lee, & Tomblin, 2010). Building on the work that has investigated the contributions of the hippocampus to language, this thesis expands that work and investigates how the hippocampus may contribute to other aspects of language processing not previously explored. The traditional view of hippocampal amnesia has been as a pure memory deficit, leaving language (and other cognitive) capacities unaffected. However, the repeated observations demonstrating disruptions of language, however, suggests that the same processes by which the hippocampus creates and integrates representations in the formation of new memories, and maintaining representations on-line to be evaluated and used in service of behavioral performance, could be the same processes necessary for the on-line processing and use of language. 42 I have chosen the lesion method because, despite advances in imaging and other functional methodology, it remains a fundamental and indispensable scientific approach in cognitive neuroscience (Chatterjee, 2004; Fellows, Heberlein, Morales, Shivde, Waller, & Wu, 2005; Fellows, Stark, Berg, & Chatterjee, 2008; Poldrack, 2006; Rorden & Karnath, 2004). It is often the first source of evidence for brain-behavior relationships, has been provided by lesion studies (including the importance of the hippocampus to new learning and recent memory from observations of patient H.M.). These results, in fact, are often used to validate the use of newer functional methodologies, which are then used as complementary evidence for the lesion studies. Unlike neuroimaging methodology, the lesion method provides a critical test of whether a particular structure is actually necessary for one or another cognitive process or behavioral performance. In this dissertation, I will use lesion methodology to provide a critical test of the necessity of the hippocampus to referential processing and narrative construction. This dissertation focuses on the role of hippocampal dependent declarative memory in the processing and use of language. More specifically, I propose that this form of memory plays a critical role in the creation, maintenance, updating, and use of representations necessary for the processing, use, and understanding of language including in referential processing and in narrative construction. I propose, and aim to test in the experiments below, that on-line linguistic representations depend critically on processing capacities of the hippocampal declarative memory system and that damage to the hippocampal declarative memory system will disrupt referential processing and narrative construction. 43 CHAPTER 3: GENERAL METHODOLOGY 3.1 Participants The participant sample will include a participant group with bilateral hippocampal damage (AM), a brain damaged comparison (BDC) group of individuals with brain damage outside of the hippocampus and medial temporal lobes (specifically, the ventromedial prefrontal cortex), a group of individuals with unilateral temporal lobe damage (TL) and a healthy non-brain injured comparison group (NC). Participants with brain damage were selected from the Iowa Patient Registry of the Department of Neurology at the University of Iowa. The registry serves as a database of participants with focal brain damage and specified behavioral deficits. All of the patients are in the chronic epoch (greater than three months post onset of lesion) and are well characterized neuroanatomically and neuropsychologically according to the standard protocols of the Benton Neuropsychology Laboratory (Frank, Damasio & Grabowski, 1997; Tranel, 2007) and the Laboratory of Brain Imaging and Cognitive Neuroscience (Damasio, 1995, Damasio & Damasio, 1989). Inclusion criteria include normal or corrected vision and hearing, negative history of language impairment or premorbid learning disabilities, and monolingual English speakers. Additionally, patients in this study had a negative history of language impairment as indicated by normal scores on the Boston Naming Test and Token Test, and WAIS-III or WAIS-IV scores greater than 80. Patients with a history of psychiatric disorders, mental retardation/intellectual disability, dementia, psychiatric disease, drug/alcohol abuse, or other neurological illness were excluded. Additionally, for 44 participants to be diagnosed with amnesia their WMS-III GMI were at least 25 points below their WAIS-III /IV IQ. 3.2. Statistical Approach Across all analyses the participant groups were compared using the appropriate test (e.g. T-test, ANOVA, Linear mixed effects modeling). In Aim 1, each patient group (AM and BDC) were compared separately to the healthy comparison group. For all ANOVA tests performed, if there was any violation of the underlying assumptions, the appropriate kluge was applied (e.g., for unequal variances the Greenhouse-Geisser correction was applied). For all tests in which multiple comparisons were performed, the Šidák correction was applied. 45 CHAPTER 4: REFERENTIAL PROCESSING - FACILITATING PRONOUN COMPREHENSION IN INDIVIDUALS WITH PROFOUND MEMORY IMPAIRMENT 4.1. Background/Rationale Efficient and successful language processing relies on the ability to establish and maintain reference as most of what we talk about involves referring to things in the world. Using reference places a demand on the ability to both track information across time and bind information and representations from potentially rich contexts. When interpreting pronouns such as she, it, or him, the addressee must bind features of the pronoun (gender, animacy, etc.) with representations of the salience of relevant discourse referents. However, while this process appears to be quite complex, this process occurs rapidly, with evidence from eye-tracking tasks demonstrating that healthy adults preferentially fixate on salient referents in ambiguous discourse within 200-400ms of the onset of a pronoun like she (Arnold et al., 2000). In terms of the neural systems that could support these abilities, there are a number of reasons to consider the hippocampal declarative memory system. This system supports the creation of representations for successive events including information about the co-occurrences of people, places, and things and the ability to link the spatial, temporal and interactional relations among them across time. Referential processing requires maintaining a representation of unfolding discourse history and potential referents, and integration of information about binding representations together, for example, referential form and context. Research indicating that HDMS representations are available rapidly enough to influence ongoing processing suggests HDMS 46 involvement in integrating distant and immediately available information necessary for referential processing. Discursive cohesion and coherence gives our communication continuity (Ferstle & von Cramon, 2001). One way to study referential processing is to look at cohesion, how speakers use references and pronouns to link ideas within language (the boy…. He….; Halliday & Hasan, 1976). Another way we can examine referential processing is through coherence, the effect that those cohesive links have on how well the story flows or how connected one utterance is to another (Gloser & Deser, 1991). Three preliminary studies have been completed that support the hypothesis that the HDMS contributes to referential processing. In two studies I looked at discourse production and examined the ability of patients with hippocampal damage (Kurczek & Duff, 2011), patients with vmPFC damage (and no hippocampal damage or declarative memory impairment; Kurczek & Duff, 2012) and healthy comparison participants (Kurczek & Duff, 2011; 2012) while in preliminary study 3 I investigated discourse comprehension in patients with hippocampal damage, patients with vmPFC damage and healthy comparisons (Kurczek, Brown-Schmidt & Duff, 2013). In preliminary study 1 participants were asked to produce language across a number of discourse settings including conversation, story-telling, picture description and procedural discourse. These language samples were then analyzed for two linguistic resources, one that investigated how cohesive the language tied together within the sample while the other looked at how coherent the language was within the sample. In comparison to healthy participants amnesia participants (1) produced fewer cohesive ties across all discourse types; (2) the adequacy of their ties were judged to be incomplete 47 more often, and (3) and the ratings of their local coherence were consistently lower. This finding suggests that the hippocampal declarative memory system critically contributes to referential processing as measured by discursive production of cohesion and coherence. In preliminary study 2 I replicated the procedures from preliminary study 1 with a group of patients with damage to the frontal lobes, an area of the brain that was thought to be important for the linguistic resources I had demonstrated to critically rely on the hippocampus. I found that bilateral frontal lobe damage does not impair cohesion and coherence in spoken discourse. This study provides insights into the neural systems that provide critical contributions to referential processing. This study further strengthens the potential of hippocampal specificity within referential processing by ruling out brain injury in general and the prefrontal cortex specifically, where much of the previous work has attempted to make the link between neural systems and referential processing (Kurczek & Duff, 2011; 2012). The first two preliminary studies point to the critical contribution of the hippocampus in reference production. Little is known about the real-time languageprocessing abilities of individuals with amnesia, as most previous work has used off-line measures or explicit judgments (e.g., Kurczek & Duff, 2011; MacKay, Burke, & Stewart, 1998). Here I examine the time course with which participants with hippocampal amnesia interpret discourse in real-time during comprehension. In preliminary study 3, by tracking participants’ eye movements as they process language in real-time, I provide novel insights into if and how hippocampal declarative memory contributes to on-line processing. 48 In preliminary study 3, (Kurczek, Brown-Schmidt, & Duff, 2013), I find support for the hypothesis that the HDMS contributes to referential processing on-line, during comprehension. This study investigated referential comprehension and examined the ability of patients with hippocampal damage), patients with frontal damage (and no hippocampal damage or declarative memory impairment) and healthy comparison participants. Participants were asked to listen to a short narrative and decide whether the narrative matched a picture they saw or not while being eyetracked. Patients with hippocampal damage did not track the referents of the discourse in the same way as the other comparison groups (i.e., they did not look at the correct characters at the correct times) indicating the critical and selective contribution of the hippocampus to on-line referential processing during comprehension. Consistent with previous work, analysis of eye-gaze showed that healthy comparison participants rapidly identified the intended referent of the pronoun when gender uniquely identified the referent, and when it did not, they showed a preference to interpret the pronoun as referring to the first-mentioned character. By contrast, hippocampal patients, while exhibiting a similar gender effect, were significantly impaired in their ability to use information about which character had been mentioned first to interpret the pronoun. This finding suggests that the hippocampus plays a role in maintaining and integrating information even over a very short discourse history. These observed disruptions in referential processing demonstrate how promiscuously the hallmark processing features of the hippocampus are used in service of a variety of cognitive domains including language. 49 In preliminary study 3, the results demonstrated a striking impairment in the ability to identify referents in discourse. The goals of this study are to better understand the time course and extent of the observed deficit in study 1 and to test efforts to facilitate pronoun compression with an eye towards informing rehabilitative efforts. Communicating with individuals with memory impairments offers many challenges. Rehabilitation efforts in memory training also prove difficult as a recent meta-analysis revealed no significant effects of treatment (Rohling, Faust, Beverly, & Demakis, 2009). However there is significant evidence that language treatments, especially in individuals with aphasia, are effective (Rohling et al., 2009). Communication and social interaction are intimately intertwined and positive social interactions provide a number of benefits (Clark, 1993). In amnesia, the experience of emotion outlasts the memory of the experience, which means that even after the memory of an interaction is gone, the positive benefits of that social interaction will exist (Feinstein, Duff & Tranel, 2010). However in individuals with amnesia, there are a number of impairments in language that may make communication more difficult (Duff et al., 2007, 2008, 2009), but this also makes attempts at rehabilitation in language that more important. In other literatures, researchers have targeted improving communication as a way to improve social interaction (Williams & Hummert, 2004) and cognitive decline (Williams, 2010). The potential for recovering performance in language in the presence of a memory disorder holds important implications in the ability to improve the quality of life after a brain injury. In this experiment I explored the integrity and stability of discourse representations by using ambiguous same-gender scenes and manipulating the degree to 50 which the pronoun is clearly established across the discourse. In one condition, additional discourse context is provided in an attempt to improve amnesic performance from baseline (of potential therapeutic benefit) while in a second condition, there is no additional context (similar to preliminary study 3). The repetition of the target character is meant to bolster the representation of the referent by drawing increased attention to the referent and allowing for more time to integrate the discourse information. Participants with brain damage outside of the hippocampus and MTL were included to investigate whether the results we find in the hippocampal amnesics are specific to damage to the hippocampus or a result of brain damage in general. If the performance of individuals with amnesia is improved in this study compared to preliminary study 3, it will be evidence that altering how we communicate with individuals with memory disorders can facilitate their ability to follow a conversation. It will also open an opportunity to investigate other avenues of research in areas of language that individuals with memory disorders have performed disadvantageously. An important aspect to note is that if the performance of the individuals with amnesia is improved in this study, it will not indicate that we should undertake lines of rehabilitative or therapeutic interventions with individuals with memory disorders, but rather that we should pursue interventions with familiar communication partners in order to devise communication strategies that reduce demands on hippocampal processing. 4.2. Specific Aims and Hypotheses Aim #1: To define and characterize the role of the hippocampal dependent declarative memory system in referential processing. It is hypothesized that the 51 HDMS critically contributes to referential processing and it is predicted that individuals with damage to the hippocampus will be impaired at maintaining a representation of ongoing discourse in the no additional context condition. It is predicted that additional context will aid patients with hippocampal amnesia allowing them to correctly direct their gaze after the pronoun. 4.3. Methods 4.3.1. Participants At the time of data collection, the four amnesic participants were in the chronic epoch of amnesia, with time-post-onset ranging from 14 to 21 years, were, on average, 56.5 years old (range 51 – 59 years), and had, on average, 16.5 years of education (range 14 – 18 years). Etiologies of participants with amnesia included anoxia/hypoxia (1846, 2363, 2563), resulting in bilateral hippocampal damage, and herpes simplex encephalitis (HSE) (2308), resulting in more extensive bilateral medial temporal lobe damage affecting hippocampus, amygdala, and surrounding cortices. Four brain damaged comparison (BDC) subjects with brain damage outside of the hippocampus and medial temporal lobes (damage had the greatest overlap within the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC)) and no declarative memory impairment also participated. These BDC participants were significantly older (p = 0.019) with higher IQ (p = 0.034) and WMSGMI (p < 0.001) scores than the bilateral hippocampal amnesic patients. Table 1 presents the demographic and neuroanatomical information for each of the amnesic and BDC participants. Ten non-brain injured comparison participants were matched to the participants with hippocampal amnesia on age, sex, educations, and handedness and 52 recruited from the Iowa City and surrounding community through existing participant registries in the Department of Neurology. As there was no effect of age in preliminary study 3, the same healthy comparison group will be used for comparison to both the amnesia and BDC groups. 4.3.2. Materials Items consisted of pictures and narratives (that were a subset of the materials from preliminary study 3 – the same gender trials); the participants’ task was to decide if the picture and narrative matched or not. Picture stimuli, which were prepared in Adobe Photoshop, used scenes with known Disney cartoon characters (e.g. Mickey Mouse and Donald Duck; see Table 2). Before the experiment, I confirmed that participants were familiar with the Disney characters, and had three practice items that introduced all of the characters by name. The critical stimuli consisted of 32 target items for which the narrative always matched the picture. There were four variants of each target item that manipulated a) the integrity of discourse representation (no set-up versus 2 clause set-up – i.e. repetition of the target character) and b) order of mention (1st versus 2nd mentioned character). These variables were manipulated within-subject and within-item, creating 128 critical trials, plus 32 filler items for a total of 160 total trials per participant. For each critical item, the narrative had two sentences, consisting of multiple clauses (see Table 2). The first clause mentioned the two characters (e.g. Minnie is playing violin for Daisy), and was used to establish their relative salience (i.e., Minnie is more salient because she was mentioned first). The second clause manipulated the integrity of representation, (i.e. nothing vs increased integrity - repetition). The third clause was a 53 distracting phrase that mentioned some other object in the picture (e.g. as the sun is shining overhead.), and was designed to shift eye-gaze away from the characters and to an unrelated image in the scene. The fourth clause asked a question with the critical pronoun referring to one character or the other (e.g. And what is she wearing?). The next clause was then used to disambiguate the reference, the point at which the sentence uniquely identified which character was the intended referent (e.g., Look, she’s wearing a yellow/purple bracelet) in the present example both characters wore a bracelet, but only the target referent wore a yellow/purple bracelet; see Table 2). The final clause provided concluding information without mentioning either character individually (e.g. and it looks like the song is being played well). Practice trials introduced all four characters and then established the structure of the narratives, allowing participants to get used to comparing the narratives and pictures. Filler trials were designed to have a similar structure to critical trials, but did not contain ambiguous pronouns. In order to create situations in which the answer to the narrative-picture comparison task was “no match”, 24 of the filler narratives did not match the picture and thus required participants to indicate a lack of match. 4.3.3. Procedure Participants' eye movements were recorded while they viewed a scene and listened to a narrative describing the picture. A drift check preceded each stimulus. After advancing to the picture, participants explored the picture for four seconds before the narrative began. When the narrative finished playing, participants indicated whether the narratives were consistent with the picture by pressing `match' or `mismatch' on the 54 screen at the end of the story. After confirming knowledge of the characters and completing the three practice items (2 `match', 1 `mismatch' answers), participants completed the 128 critical trials (always `match' answers), interspersed with the 32 fillers trials (8 `match', 24 `mismatch'). The critical and filler trials were presented in the same randomized order to all participants. All healthy comparison participants completed one session of all trials, while all participants with brain damage completed two sessions of all trials. Due to scheduling constraints, one individual with amnesia (2363) completed just one session. Additionally 1 and 1/3 sessions of comparison data were lost to technical error. 4.3.4. Analysis Following Kurczek et al., (2013) our measure of interest was the eye-fixations participants made following the onset of the pronoun. For each stimulus, I identified visual regions of interest where the participant was fixating: target (the referent of the pronoun), competitor (the other character), other (something else in or off the picture). The time-regions for the analysis were offset by 200ms to account for the time needed to program and launch an eye movement (Hallett, 1986). Fixations were binned into three critical time regions, baseline (-200 to 200 ms), immediately following pronoun onset (200-1000ms), and late (1000 to 1800 ms following pronoun onset), and were analyzed with mixed-effects models with a maximal random effects structure for both subjects and items using R’s lmer function (see Jaeger, 2008; Barr, Levy, Scheepers, & Tily, 2013). Fixed effects of repetition and order-of-mention were entered with orthogonal contrast codes; the time-regions were entered as a fixed effect with the baseline region as 55 the reference category (i.e., 0, 1, 2). The dependent measure was the empirical logit of the ratio of the proportion of fixations to the target over the proportion of fixations to the competitor, calculated separately for each trial. By calculating this target preference score on a trial-by-trial basis, I was able to include both subjects and items as crossed random effects. To handle trials on which the proportion of fixations to the competitor was zero, and the ratio is undefined, 0.05 was added to both numerator and denominator (see similar empirical logit approach in Barr, 2008; Fine & Jaeger, 2013; Heller, Grodner, & Tanenhaus, 2008). Fixed effects were considered significant if the t-statistic reached 2.0 or higher. 4.4. Results 4.4.1. Offline Data. The participants were asked at the end of the narrative to decide whether the narrative matched the story or not. The critical items were all designed so that they all matched, but the offline data represent the participants’ ultimate interpretation of the pronoun. If they decide the narrative and picture did not match, then they interpreted the competitor referent as the target. Here in the ambiguous same-gender stories, we should expect lower endorsement rates when the pronoun refers to the 2nd mentioned character. Analysis of the response to the judgment task (whether the picture matched the narrative) revealed that both participants with hippocampal damage and BDC participants performed differently than comparison subjects, but in different ways. Comparing amnesia to healthy comparison participants revealed significant effects of repetition (z=2.70, p<.01), order of mention (z=-9.07, p<.0001) and a order of mention*group 56 interaction (z=-4.24, p<.0001; see Figure 1) where amnesic subjects indicated more matches in the second character conditions than comparison subjects. As expected comparison participants interpreted the first mentioned character as the target in the firstmentioned conditions and had a lower endorsement rate for the picture matching in the second-mentioned character condition. Amnesia participants show a similar pattern, but did not interpret the narrative as mismatching the scene to the same degree as comparison participants in the second-mentioned condition. Comparing BDC participants and healthy comparison participants revealed an effect of order of mention (z=-7.63, p<.0001) and a three-way interaction of group, repetition and order of mention (z=9.21, p<.0001). The BDC participants had the same interpretation for the no repetition conditions but responded in the opposite way for the repeated conditions. 4.4.2. Eye-movement Data. Analyses of the eye movements made during interpretation of the pronoun are used to test for group differences in the use of discourse context (order of mention and repetition) to interpret a potentially ambiguous pronoun (as all characters were the same gender) in real time. We compared eye-fixations for the amnesic patients and the healthy comparison participants in one analysis, and the BDC participants and the comparison participants in a second analysis. These analyses included repetition, order of mention and participant group (patient vs. comparison) as orthogonal factors, as well as timewindow, with the baseline window coded as reference. The first analysis compared amnesic patients to the healthy comparison participants. During the pronoun region there was a significant order of mention (t = - 57 7.25, p < 0.001), an order of mention by repetition (t = -6.38, p < 0.001) interaction and an order of mention by group interaction (t = -2.92, p < 0.001) demonstrating that the amnesic patients and comparison participants were differentially sensitive to the order of mention cue, where comparison participants had a stronger order of mention (t = -25.81, p < 0.001) and order of mention by repetition effect (t = -6.16, p < 0.001) than amnesia participants, (t = -7.50, p < 0.001; (t = -2.59, p = 0.01; respectively). This effect continued into the late region, where there was a significant order of mention (t = -7.45, p < 0.001), an order of mention by repetition interaction (t = -3.88, p < 0.001) and an order of mention by group interaction (t = -2.84, p < 0.001) demonstrating that the amnesic patients and comparison participants were differentially sensitive to the order of mention cue, where comparison participants had a stronger order of mention (t = -27.11, p < 0.001) and order of mention by repetition effect (t = -3.54, p < 0.001) than amnesia participants, (t = -10.66, p < 0.001; t = -1.78, p = 0.07; respectively). Thus, while both comparison and amnesia participants were affected by the order in which the characters were mentioned, comparison participants had a stronger effect (looked at the target at a higher proportion) and only the comparison participants benefited from the repetition (in the first mentioned condition). Comparison of the healthy comparison participants and BDC participants during the pronoun window revealed a significant of order of mention (t = -39.36, p < 0.001), an order of mention by repetition interaction (t = -6.61, p < 0.001) and an order of mention by repetition by group interaction (t = -2.59, p < 0.001), where comparison (t = -6.16, p < 0.001) participants had a larger order of mention by repetition effect than BDC participants (t = -2.95, p = 0.003). The effect continued into the late window. A 58 significant of order of mention (t = -39.85, p < 0.001), an order of mention by repetition (t = -3.00, p < 0.001) interaction and an order of mention by repetition by group interaction (t = -2.61, p < 0.001) was observed, where comparison participants had an order of mention by repetition interaction (t = -3.54, p < 0.001), but BDC participants did not (t = -0.23, p = 0.818). Although like the amnesia participants, the BDC participants performed differently than comparison participants, their difference was not in the strength of their target preference depending on order of mention, but rather because of a lack of a boost in performance from the repetition of the target. 4.5. Discussion The results from the no repetition condition replicate the findings from the initial investigation (Kurczek et al., 2013) where healthy individuals and BDC participants were drawn to look at the first mentioned character. In the first study, amnesic participants were impaired in their ability to preferentially view the target character (i.e., no order of mention effect), but in this study, amnesia participants did show an order of mention effect. However, the goal of this study was to improve the performance of patients with bilateral hippocampal damage who did not preferentially view the target character regardless of condition in the first study. By repeating the mention of the target character, the goal was to increase the saliency and robustness of the character representation and improve performance (i.e., increase gaze to target). Our findings however, demonstrated that amnesic patients experienced difficulty in integrating and maintaining information even over a very short discourse history and did not benefit from the repetition (i.e., increasing the saliency/representation) of the target character. Amnesic patients were 59 significantly impaired in their ability to use information about the relative salience of two very recently mentioned discourse referents to disambiguate a pronoun. By contrast, healthy adults and brain-damaged comparison (BDC) participants, recruited this information, and used it to begin guiding the on-line interpretation of the pronoun within the first second of pronoun onset. That amnesic patients performed significantly worse than the BDCs suggests a strong link between the functionality of the hippocampus and demands of referential processing rather than a general consequence of brain damage. The observed disruption in referential comprehension in amnesia, but not in frontal lobe patients is consistent with previous work on language production (e.g., Kurczek & Duff, 2011, 2012; Kurczek, et al., 2013). Importantly, however, unlike in preliminary study 3, amnesic participants did exhibit an order of mention effect, even if they did not exhibit a boost from repetition of the target character. Although, this negative finding of repetition on the performance of individuals with amnesia should be qualified by the repetition by order of mention interaction found in the healthy comparisons where repetition only boosted performance in first mentioned character condition. Perhaps in healthy participants, there was not a main effect of repetition because a 40-60% target advantage is near ceiling. While the repetition of information may have helped healthy participants (in one condition), it may have been the case that in the presence of a damaged hippocampal declarative memory system, the extra representations did nothing to integrate into the representation and guide subsequent behavior. The BDC off-line judgment data are also pause for concern as theses participants behaved differently in the repeated conditions than the non-repeated conditions compared 60 to healthy comparisons. Although they did benefit from the repetition in the first mentioned condition (only during the pronoun window), the magnitude of their order of mention affect was similar to healthy comparisons and may have suffered from the same potential ceiling effect. The results here suggest that repetition can influence the behavior of both healthy individuals and individuals with brain damage outside of the medial temporal lobes. Repetition however did not improve the performance of individuals with hippocampal amnesia. This finding speaks to the important theoretical underpinnings that should guide rehabilitative efforts. Others have investigated the effect of repetition on behavior (Verfaellie, Rajaram, Fossum, & Williams, 2008) and concluded that repetition of information in varied contexts, enhances recollection of individuals with intact memory but only affects familiarity in patients with severe amnesia. Others have taken the benefits of repetition and implemented it into errorless learning paradigms. Although errorless learning may provide a benefit over errorful learning (Wilson, Baddeley, Evans & Shiel, 1994), the effect appears to be highly context specific. The small benefit of repetition on healthy performance and even smaller benefit on the performance of individuals with brain damage outside of the medial temporal lobes may be sign to rethink the use of repetition in rehabilitation. Others have suggested rethinking verbatim repetition in rehabilitation (Hengst, Duff, & Dettmer, 2010) and that sentiment is echoed in the results here. Hengst and colleagues (2010) suggest repeated engagement rather than context specific repetition be a greater benefit to improving realworld behavior. Perhaps the repetition of information in a specific context with little 61 opportunity to act on or engage with the material in this task did not allow for patients with hippocampal amnesia to benefit from repetition. The benefit of engagement over repetition has been demonstrated in amnesia previously. Duff and colleagues (2006) asked individuals with amnesia to work with a familiar communication partner to collaboratively place tangrams on a board in a certain order. When the patients were able to generate the labels themselves in collaboration and coordination with their communication partner they showed a learning rate similar to healthy comparisons. However, when they attempted to learn arbitrary labels for the tangrams, they showed no learning at all (Duff et al., 2006). The repeated engagement with the materials in a rich, purpose driven context allowed patients to leverage intact neural and cognitive systems to successfully navigate the task. As Verfaellie and colleagues (2008) demonstrated in their investigation of repetition, varied contexts aid learning. Future rehabilitative efforts should focus on the engagement of the individual within a purposeful task rather than drilling or repeating within a contextless task. In this task, we may have focused on just two characters (e.g., Mickey and Donald) and looked to build representations over two very different characters over the course of the task (i.e., Mickey as the “good” character and Donald as the “bad”). By establishing different contexts of the characters across a variety of situations, perhaps over the course of the experiment, attempts to bias performance with repetition within a single context could be achieved. However, while this and other work may suggest engagement as a good practice for rehabilitative efforts for individuals with amnesia, previous attempts to rehabilitate performance in amnesia have proven both slow and difficult (Squires, Hunkin, & Parkin, 62 1997) and the results from this experiment may extend those findings. While we were able to find an order of mention effect in this experiment, as opposed to the previous version of the experiment, there were twice as many opportunities to observe the effect within the same trial condition and even yet, the order of mention effect is attenuated in amnesia (i.e., so even though there is a difference within amnesia, the effect is still not the same as in healthy comparisons and brain damaged comparisons). These findings may indicate the robust difficulties in attempting rehabilitative efforts in patients with declarative memory impairments. Recommendations for speaking with individuals with memory disorders suggests using a number of strategies as a communication partner, from speaking more slowly and using close ended questions to make communication more successful (Tappen, WilliamsBurgess, Edelstein, Touhy, Fisherman, 1997). However, these findings paint a dim outlook for even rehabilitative strategies that are contained within the environment rather than the individual for people with memory disorders. While the participants with amnesia did improve one aspect of their performance, it was not to the same degree as other participants and still lacked another important effect. These findings here suggest that even techniques that attempt to alter the environment of individuals with memory disorders don’t help much or have limited benefits. Previous rehabilitation efforts have attempted to place interventions within the domains of (supposedly) intact cognitive domains. However this and other studies are demonstrating the promiscuity of the hippocampus and its ubiquitous contribution across cognitive domains, early and continuously. Attempts to place demand on (supposedly) intact neural structures or 63 cognitive abilities, may simply be placing demands on the hippocampal declarative memory system at different time points in cognition, or in different, less overt ways. One of the most popular ways to attempt to teach individuals with memory impairments new information has been to leverage the procedural/non-declarative memory system (Squires et al., 1997), but recent evidence has implicated the hippocampus in even non-declarative processes (Foerde, Race, Verfaellie, & Shohamy, 2013), so rehabilitation efforts like errorless learning or adding gesture may not work. Future research within this area may try to replace the pronouns used to increase access with the proper names, but this and other potential augmentations may dramatically decrease the real-world application or similarity (as we often don’t repeat a proper name after it has been established, though individuals with amnesia do, see Kurczek & Duff, 2011). 64 Table 1. Demographic and neuropsychological characteristics of hippocampal amnesic and BDC participants. Sex Ed Age Etiology HC Vol Patient Intelligence Memory Language WAIS-III WMS-III Token BNT FSIQ GMI Test 1846 F 14 51 Anoxia -4.23 84 57 41 43 2308 M 18 58 HSE N/A 98 45 44 52 2363 M 18 58 Anoxia -2.64 98 73 44 58 2563 M 16 59 Anoxia N/A 94 63 44 52 AM 16.5 56.5 N/A -3.4 93.5 59.5 43.3 51.3 Summary ±1.9 ±3.7 ±1.1 ±6.6 ±11.7 ±1.5 ±6.2 318 M 14 71 MR N/A 143 109 44 60 2025 F 16 63 ACoA N/A 115 114 44 59 2352 F 14 62 SaH; N/A 106 109 44 54 MR N/A 109 132 43 57 14.3 65.3 N/A N/A 118.3 116.0 43.8 57.5 ACoA 2391 BDC F 13 65 Summary ±1.3 ± 4.0 ±16.9 ±10.9 ±0.5 ±2.7 Difference t-val 1.91 3.05 2.62 6.30 0.62 1.81 p-val 0.097 0.019 0.034 0.001 0.550 0.112 Note. AM = Hippocampal amnesic participants; BDC = Brain damaged comparison; t-val = T-statistic value; p-val = p-value; F = Female; M = Male; HC = Hippocampus; Ed = Education; HSE = Herpes simplex encephalitis; MR = Meningioma Resection; ACoA = Anterior communicating artery aneurysm; HC Vol = Hippocampal Volume; Hippocampal z-scores represent the combined (left and right hemisphere) studentised residuals of hippocampal volume relative to a group of comparison subjects (Allen et al., 2006; Buchanan et al., 2005). WAIS-III = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; FSIQ = Full Scale Intelligence Quotient, WMS-III GMI = Wechsler Memory Scale-III; GMI = General Memory Index; CFT = Complex Figure test BN = Boston Naming Test 65 Table 2. Study 1: On-line referential processing experimental conditions. Narrative Sentences of Narrative Conditions Establish Mickey is painting a portrait of Characters Donald, C1 – Limited … accessibility C2 – Greater He’s trying really hard to get the increase in portrait just right, because he wants to accessibility be a famous artist someday Redirect gaze and some paint is spilling on the floor Critical And what is he wearing Concluding Look, he’s wearing red shoes and it information looks like they’re having fun Note. C = condition. Picture Stimulus 66 Percent "Match" Response Figure 1. Offline judgments of story-picture match for hippocampal amnesics, BDC and comparison participants. 1 0.9 0.8 0.7 0.6 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 1s0 1s3 2s0 2s3 Condition Comparison! Amnesia! BDC! Note: Error bars indicate one standard error. 1 = First mentioned character as target; 2 = second mentioned character as target; 0 = no repetition; 3 = repetition of target. 67 Figure 2. Time course of fixation preferences for hippocampal amnesics, BDC participants and healthy comparison participants. Note: Plotted as the difference between target and competitor fixations (proportion target minus proportion competitor) at each point in time. Data plotted separately by condition and group. Positive values indicate target preference. 0ms= pronoun onset. Panel A = healthy comparison participants matched to amnesia patients; Panel B = brain-damaged comparison participants; Panel C = amnesia participants; 1 = First mentioned character as target; 2 = second mentioned character as target; 0 = no repetition; 3 = repetition of target. 68 CHAPTER 5: NARRATIVE IN AMNESIA: THE EFFECTS OF REPEATED TELLINGS ON NARRATIVE CONSTRUCTION 5.1. Background/Rationale Narratives are connected events and are an important aspect of culture and communication (Flanagan, 1993). Personal narrative allows one to use language to give life events a temporal order, to demystify them and to establish coherence across a life lived, living and yet to be lived (Ochs & Capps, 2001). Autobiographical memories influence how we tell and interpret narratives. However, the relationship between narrative and memory is bidirectional, as autobiographical memory provides the building blocks upon which narratives are crafted, while narrative practices in turn, influence how we encode, retain and revise experiences in memory. Beyond the cognitive relationship between narrative and memory, recent evidence suggests a common neural instantiation. Recent evidence suggests that the hippocampus is engaged in basic functions (flexible and relational binding) immediately and continuously with the hippocampus maintaining representations on-line (Hannula, Tranel & Cohen, 2006; Warren, Duff, Jensen, Tranel, & Cohen, 2012). These functions together, suggest that the hippocampus can contribute to behaviors outside of (re)constructing memories for the past (and future), and may potentially including any behavior that requires the flexible generation, and subsequent (re)combination of perceptually distant and distinct mental representations on-line and in response to novel contexts. Examples of such behaviors include perception (Warren et al., 2011), creativity (Duff, Hengst, Tranel & Cohen, 2009) and language (Duff & Brown-Schmidt, 2012; MacKay, Stewart, & Burke, 1998). Together, these 69 results suggest that the role of the hippocampal dependent declarative memory system may extend beyond memory to broadly include generating and binding together representations regardless of memory demands on the hippocampal system. The implications of this view see the hippocampus as playing a fundamental role in the earliest phases of cognition, generating and binding together, on-line, representations that can be used in service of a number of cognitive capacities including its traditional roles in memory/future thinking, but also extending to other capacities such language. Narrative construction appears to be one capacity that places high demands on the processing capacities of the HDMS, particularly representational flexibility, as speakers creatively (re)construct events and chose what information and details to represent or omit in response to contextual and audience cues. While creative thinking has been examined in hippocampal amnesia, with patients with hippocampal damage showing impairments in imagining events (Hassabis, Kumaran, Vann, & Maguire, 2007) in the creative use of language (Duff et al., 2009) and in creativity (broadly defined; Duff et al., 2013), I propose that the processing features of the hippocampus that support aspects of creativity, will accordingly, critically contribute to the flexible and creative manipulation of representations that are bound together in order to construct a narrative. While there is some evidence that creative aspects of linguistic behavior are disrupted in amnesia, the role of HDMS in narrative construction is an open question. Disentangling memory from language is difficult to do because we generally assess episodic memories by asking individuals to tell us their memories. While the literature is full of examples of deficits in individuals with amnesia’s ability to include episodic details in their narrations, there is an on-going debate as to whether those 70 deficits are a confined impairment within the memory system or a more basic impairment in cognitive functioning outside of memory. An on-going debate in the field is the whether deficits in the production of narrative elements across either personal stories or picture descriptions/narratives represent solely an impairment in memory or a more basic impairment in cognitive functioning outside of memory. Evidence for a more basic impairment come from a relationship between deficits in narrative construction and future thinking impairments in older adults (Gaesser, Sacchetti, Addis, & Schacter, 2011) and deficits in amnesics ability to narrate pictures and real-life settings (Zeman, Beschin, Dewar, & Della Sala, 2012). However, Race and colleagues (2011; 2013) investigated the contribution of the hippocampus to 1) memory/future thinking and 2) picture narrative construction, and found only a deficit in narratives that placed a demand memory/future thinking and not in narratives cued by a picture as indicated by the number and type of details from both types of narrative. In a preliminary study, I find support for the hypothesis that the HDMS contributes to narrative construction. I examined the role of the hippocampus (with 6 individuals with amnesia and their matched comparison participants) in the active and flexible manipulation of acquired declarative memory representations to be used in service of thinking about and telling narratives across constructed events (past events, imagined past events, imagined present events, future events), personal past events and pictures. Results indicate that regardless of the type of narrative produced, amnesic participants were impaired on both memory and language analyses when compared to normal healthy participants. Assaying memory and language has long been conflated. In order to assess someone’s memory, one has asked them to construct a memory about a 71 personal past episode or imagine a fictional episode, while assaying someone’s language again one may ask them to construct a narrative about a personal past episode or describe a picture. By using both memory and language tasks and analyses I am able to assess the fundamental capacities that underlie narrative construction (Duff & Kurczek, 2013) Individuals with hippocampal damage were impaired across measures, both memory and language compared to comparison participants. This indicates that the hippocampus is engaging in a similar, fundamental mechanism when constructing narratives regardless of the demand on memory, per se. These results suggest that the hippocampus is necessary for generating, manipulating and combining representations that later consist of the elements of the narrative and the linguistic/grammatical combination of those elements. In the preliminary study, I found disruptions in narrative construction after hippocampal damage as measured by both language and memory analyses, here in study 2 I address narrative from another perspective. A critical aspect of our memories is that they are constructed, which means that not only do they change over time, but we can actively change them by emphasizing or leaving out details based on the demands of the moment. In other words, episodic memory is the result of ongoing encoding within a highly contextualized and dynamically changing environment (Tulving 1983; 2002; Suddendorf and Busby 2005). The goal of study 2 is to investigate the effect of hippocampal damage on the repeated recall of memories over time. The consolidation view of memory over time inherently assumes that memories over time remain a faithful record of the original event (Squire, Cohen, & Nadel, 1984). The view was challenged as early as 1932 when Bartlett demonstrated that memory was a 72 constructive process and not just a reply of the past. Using the “War of the Ghosts” story and mask drawing task, he demonstrated that repeated production of the story typically led to a shorted, more stereotyped version of it, with details, changed, removed, or in cases added. Further investigation of the effect has produced mixed results. Ahlberg and Sharps (2010) were able to replicate Bartlett’s broader conclusions with both older and younger adults reconfiguring stories and recall diminished over time. Bergman and Roediger (1999) were also able to replicate Bartlett’s findings with subjects forgetting more over time and changed the story (with rationalizations and distortions) as the interval increased. In contrast, Carbon & Albrecht (2012) in a series of experiments testing potential retrieval biases in the portrait d’homme reproductions were unable to reproduce Bartlett’s findings although, the did find that each of the reproductions changed over time, indicating the general effect of reconstruction, however, not the specific effect of schematic rationalization over time. Wheeler and Roediger (1992) found that when the interval distribution of the recall was changed, there were differences in the recall of the stories. When the intervals were short, there was improvement between the tests, while when the intervals were long, forgetting occurred (Wheeler & Roediger, 1992). In summary, these results appear to indicate that in the retelling on nonpersonal or non-episodic stories, individuals recall and retell fewer details overtime. The hallmark characteristic of amnesia, a significantly decreased ability to learn new declarative memory, would suggest that individuals with amnesia would be able to recall little to no details from the story after just 30 minutes. However, the tests in Bartlett’s studies required learning new material and then attempting to reproduce over time, what happens when you ask someone to reproduce 73 autobiographical memories over time? Wynn and Logie (1998) asked students in three different groups to recall a description of a room over varying amounts of time and found no differences in the amount or type of words produced over time. Neisser (1981) was able to probe the memory of John Dean (counsel to President Richard Nixon) and compare it to his Watergate testimony from years earlier. Neisser (1981) found that Dean’s memory reflected distortions in some instances (e.g. seeing himself more central to the case), and errors (e.g. recall of conversations) but was overall fairly accurate in the general happenings. Moscovitch and colleagues have investigated the recall of autobiographical information over time using both behavioral and functional imaging techniques in order to test the Multiple Trace Theory (MTT; Nadal & Moscovitch, 1997) that suggests repeated recall of information should strengthen the storage of a memory. Nadal and colleagues (2007) asked participants to tell 24 memories and repeatedly told 12 of them 3 times over the course of a month. Two days after the last retelling, participants told the 12 stories only told once, the 12 stories that were repeated and 12 new stories. Results showed that the repeated memories were retrieved faster and became longer over time and more consistent. Imaging results showed greater cortical activation in episodic memory networks which was interpreted as increased reconsolidation as is suggested by MTT (Nadal et al., 2007). In a follow-up study Campbell and colleagues (2011) asked 8 of the 12 participants from Nadal et al., (2007) to recall their narratives approximately 1.4 years later. They found that, in contrast to Bartlett’s (1932) findings, the autobiographical memories became more detailed and longer over time (Campbell et al., 2011). In summary, these results indicate that individuals retelling personal or 74 episodic stories, in general, tell roughly the same story, but may retell more details over time. How does amnesia and damage to the hippocampus effect the telling of narratives over time? The only evidence to date comes from patients with depression (who have been suggested to have memory impairments, c.f., Williams, Barnhofer, Crane, Hermans, Raes, Watkins, & Dalgleish, 2007; King, Macdougall, Gerris, Levine, Macqueen & McKinnon, 2010; Sumner, Griffth, & Mineka, 2010). Using the Columbia Autobiographical Memory Interview – Short Form administered over a two month or six month interval, found that while all participants were less consistent in the follow-up, only the patients still exhibiting depressive symptoms had significantly lower scores than remitters on episodic information (Semkovska et al., 2012). This may suggest that patients with hippocampal damage when retrieving memories repeatedly over time may be less consistent and variable in all aspects of performance. However, I have also been struck by some patients’ “riffs”, their short, highly repetitive sentences that are often offered after a break in conversation or topic. These “riffs” appear to ground the conversation in something in which the patient with hippocampal damage is confortable or an expert. Thus, one hypothesis may be that severe cases of amnesia will lead to rote retellings of the same narratives, in other words demonstrate no variability and tell the same, story or riff time after time, while another hypothesis for the effect of hippocampal damage on the repeated telling of autobiographical memories may be that patients will demonstrate variability from telling to telling as was demonstrated by the patients with depression (Semkovska et al., 2012). A reconciled view of these two opposing hypotheses may be that we see a distinction in the performance of individuals with less 75 (anoxic) and more (herpes simplex encephalitis) hippocampal damage, where individuals with less hippocampal damage become more variable (i.e., the number of words/details and/or type of details may increase or decrease on subsequent tellings) in their performance compared to healthy comparisons (who will show a consistent trend of increasing the number of details over time), while individuals with more hippocampal damage become less variable compared to healthy comparisons (i.e., they tell the same rote story every time). Here participants will be asked to undergo two conditions of story re-telling. In the first condition, the semantic condition, participants will be asked to read through the “War of the Ghosts” story twice and then recall it at four time intervals, fifteen minutes later, one hour later, one week later and one month later. In the second condition, participants will tell six personal episodic memories and recall them along the same time intervals. 5.2. Specific Aims and Hypotheses Aim #2: To define and characterize the role of the HDMS in narrative construction. It is hypothesized that the HDMS plays a critical role in the generation and flexible use of representations critical to the construction of narratives. It is predicted that the individuals with damage to the hippocampus will be significantly impaired at recalling and retelling the “War of the Ghosts” story and impaired at generating representations necessary for constructing narratives from their own lives. More specifically, it is predicted that patients with bilateral hippocampal damage will produce 76 narratives that will be shorter, more semanticized and less consistent than healthy comparisons over multiple tellings of the narratives. 5.3. Methods 5.3.1. Participants At the time of data collection, five amnesic participants were in the chronic epoch of amnesia, with time-post-onset ranging from 14 to 34 years, were, on average, 57.6 years old (range 51 – 62 years), and had, on average, 16.4 years of education (range 14 – 18 years). Etiologies of participants with amnesia included anoxia/hypoxia (1846, 2363, 2563), resulting in bilateral hippocampal damage, and herpes simplex encephalitis (HSE) (1951, 2308), resulting in more extensive bilateral medial temporal lobe damage affecting hippocampus, amygdala, and surrounding cortices. Table 3 presents the demographic and neuroanatomical information for each of the amnesic participants. Ten non-brain injured comparison participants were matched to the participants with hippocampal amnesia on age, sex, educations, and handedness and were recruited from the Iowa City and surrounding community through existing participant registries in the Department of Neurology. 5.3.2. Materials In condition one, a list of typical life events from Levine et al., (2002), such as “your first job” or “falling in love” was used to generate memory prompting cues for the memory retrieval sessions. For each memory, the event was required to have occurred before they were 25 years old, have occurred in a specific time and place and have 77 happened only once. Participants were instructed to visualize the details of the event, playing the event out as if it were a scene in a movie and describe all the details that you can remember. Those details could have included what happened, who was there, where you were, the time of day, and the physical details of the scene. After the participant completed each recall, they were asked to rate the memory on several scales (Figure 3). In condition two, participants read “The War of the Ghosts” (Appendix). 5.3.3. Procedure In condition one, during the initial retrieval session, participants were asked to generate six different memories. After each memory, they were asked to rate the memory on two likert scales, for six questions they used a 5-point scale (from 1-not at all to 5extremely) and for one question they used a 7-point scale (from -3-very negative to 3very positive; see Figure 3). Participants were then asked to recall the memories three more times over various time intervals including 1 hours, 1 week, and 1 month. In each subsequent interview, participants were instructed to recall each memory in a randomized order and subsequently rate each memory again. In condition two, participants read through “The War of the Ghosts” twice at their own reading pace. They were then asked to recall the story at four time intervals, fifteen minutes later, one hour later, one week later and one month later. In subsequent retellings, participants were prompted by being asked to retell “The War of the Ghosts” story. Due to scheduling constraints two participants with amnesia were unable to participate in the “War of the Ghosts” condition (1846, 2563), and due to a recording failure, one comparison participant’s second telling of the “War of the Ghosts” was lost. 78 For condition one, the telling of personal stories, one participant with amnesia was unable to participate in the third and fourth follow-ups of the multiple tellings (2563), one participant with amnesia was unable to provide six stories and instead talked about three stories but did not provide ratings for the stories (2308), and one comparison participant was unable to participate in the third telling of the multiple tellings. 5.3.4. Analysis Audio recordings of each of the retrieval sessions were transcribed for analysis. Following Levine et al., (2002) and Nadel and colleagues (2007) each memory was subjected to autobiographical memory analysis and the memories were divided into different detail types including internal (i.e., episodic), external (i.e., semantic) and editorial (details that reflected uncertainty – e.g., I think it was). For each retrieval session after the first, the number of details that were consistent from the previous retrieval session (i.e., comparing session 2 to 1, 3 to 2 and 4 to 3) were measured and expressed as a proportion of the session’s total details. Details that were changed (e.g., Session 1 – We got married at the Baptist Church; Session 2 – We got married at the Presbyterian Church) were measured and expressed as a proportion of the session’s total details. Details that were new or completely different (e.g. Session 1 – I flew to the hospital; Session 2 – I flew to the hospital and died on the ride) were also measured and expressed as a proportion of the session’s total details. The total number of words spoken by the participant for each narrative were obtained using the word counting function of Microsoft Word. The War of the Ghosts stories were scored using Mandler and Johnson’s parsing (1977). Details that were correctly produced were scored as a one, while details 79 that were close were scored as a half (e.g., two braves instead of two young men) and details that were missing were scored a zero. Summing the score and dividing by 43 (the total number of parsed details) provided an accuracy measure for recall of the story. 5.4. Results 5.4.1. Multiple Tellings Autobiographical Memory Interview Analysis (Condition 1) We used the Autobiographical Memory Interview scoring method to score the narratives of participants own personal life stories to calculate a ratio of internal details to overall details, which is used to capture the “autobiographicalness” of narratives without being influenced by length, as there was a significant difference (F(1,11) = 8.59, p = 0.014, in the length of the narratives, where comparison participants (mean = 280.38; SD = 64.22) produced narratives with twice as many words that amnesia participants (mean = 119.75; SD = 38.24). A 2 X 4 (group – AM, CP; time – first telling, second telling, third telling, fourth telling) ANOVA on the internal to overall details revealed a main effect of group, F(1, 11) = 8.34, p = 0.015, with a significant effect of time elicitation, F(3, 33) = 5.67, p = 0.003, but no group by time interaction, F(3, 33) = 2.72, p = 0.06, indicating that the amensic participants had a lower ratio of internal details to overall details (mean = 0.60, SD = 0.04) than comparison participants (0.76, SD = 0.04), and thus a lower “episodic re-experiencing” across all time retellings (see Figure 4). Followup comparisons of the time periods revealed no significant differences between the time periods. 80 5.4.2. Multiple Tellings Consistency Analysis for hippocampal amnesics and their comparisons Analysis of the consistency of story details revealed a main effect of group on both repeated, F(1, 11) = 29.08, p < 0.001, and added details, F(1, 11) = 57.78, p < 0.001. There was no effect of time on either repeated, F(2, 22) = 1.95, p = 0.166, or added details, F(2, 22) = 1.69, p = 0.208, nor any group by time interactions for either repeated, F(2, 22) = 0.08, p = 0.927, or added details, F(2, 22) = 0.13, p < 0.883 (see Figure 5; Table 4). This indicates that comparison participants were much more consistent in the telling of the details of their stories from the current telling to the proceeding, while participants with amnesia told very different stories from telling to telling (meaning that even with one hour separating the telling of the stories, participants with amnesia were telling narratives that were very different from the one told an hour earlier). 5.4.3. Self Ratings of Multiple Tellings for hippocampal amnesics and their comparisons Analysis of the seven ratings of the narratives after each telling revealed significant group differences in two of the ratings, how important the event was at the time, F(1, 10) = 4.90, p = 0.017, and how emotional the event was at the time, F(1, 10) = 6.47, p = 0.027, with comparison participants having higher ratings (i.e., more important and more emotional) than amnesia participants across all tellings (see Figure 6). Significant group*time interactions were revealed for how vivid their recall, F(3, 30) = 3.062, p = 0.043, was and how positive or negative the event was, F(3, 30) = 3.591, p = 0.025, where comparison participants rated the vividness of their recall as significantly 81 higher than amnesia participants for all but the final retelling (ps < 0.05) and anmnesia participants rated their final retelling as significantly more positive than other retellings while comparison participants ratings stayed the same. Additional analysis of the variability of the ratings from telling to telling revealed a significant difference in the importance at the time, F(1, 10) = 19.36, p < 0.001, where amnesic participants were much more variable in their ratings from telling to telling. 5.4.4. War of the Ghosts Analysis (Condition 2) Scheduling issues prevented two participants (1846, 2563) from participating. Further, two of the three participants (1951, 2308) with amnesia who participated were unable to recall any details from the story after the first fifteen-minute break. Accordingly, all statistical tests revealed profound impairments of the individuals with amnesia to tell the story compared to comparison participants. Amnesia participants told significantly shorter (mean = 14.67; SD = 17.4) and less accurate (mean = 1.0; SD = 2.0) narratives than comparison participants (mean = 247.78; SD = 15.9; F(1,10) = 34.75; p < 0.001; mean = 36.37; SD = 3.0; F(1,10) = 106.13; p < 0.001; respectively; see Figure 7; Table 5). The only within subject comparison to reach statistical significance was the accuracy of the stories, F(3, 30) = 6.36, p = 0.017, with follow-up comparisons revealing that the second retelling was told more accurately than the fourth (p = 0.038), indicating that increases in the demands on declarative memory in healthy subjects led to a decrement in performance. 5.5. Discussion I examined the role of the hippocampus in active and flexible manipulation of acquired declarative memory representations to be used in service of thinking about and 82 telling narratives across multiple tellings. I have previously reported deficits in individuals with amnesia to tell narratives across time (i.e., past, present, and future), imagination (i.e., real vs imagined) and type (i.e., personal stories vs picture narratives) in both their “episodicness” (i.e., how many details of the stories are related to a specific time and place) and in language measures (i.e., how cohesive and coherent the narratives are). The current study investigates the telling of semantic (e.g., “War of the Ghosts”) personal narratives over time. Results indicate that regardless of the time of follow-up of the narrative produced, amnesic participants were impaired in their episodic telling of the stories and how consistent their story stayed from telling to telling. Through the lens of relational memory theory, this pattern of impairment across all time conditions is easily understood (Cohen & Eichenbaum, 1993; Eichenbaum & Cohen, 2001). In this view of declarative memory there are two hallmark features or properties, which are critical to the instantiation of autobiographical memory and the construction of mental representations. First, declarative memory supports flexible expression of memory (Bunsey & Eichenbaum, 1996; Cohen, 1984; Cohen & Eichenbaum, 1993; Dusek & Eichenbaum, 1997; Eichenbaum & Cohen, 2001; O’Keefe & Nadel, 1978; Squire, 1992). The representational flexibility permits accessibility across processing systems and can be used in novel situations. Second, is the importance of the relational (associative) binding that the hippocampus engages in. Hippocampus encodes into long-term memories the co-occurrences of the elements that constitute memories along with the relations (e.g. spatial, temporal, and interactional) among them (see Konkel et al., 2008; Konkel & Cohen, 2009). The hippocampus also encodes 83 representations of relationships among events, providing the basis for the larger record of one’s experience. Thus in retelling narratives, a lot demand is placed on these two properties. Not only are the representations of the narrative activated and brought together, but how they are brought together, and how they have been brought together in the past (i.e., as a part of the larger record of one’s experience), and this plays an important role in how the narratives are told. Healthy comparisons in telling their own narratives were able to activate the experiences of the narrative itself as well as the previous experiences of having told that narrative in the past. The interaction of those episodes drove how they told the narratives in the subsequent retellings. Individuals with hippocampal amnesia not only had a difficult time of generating and binding the representations particular to the experience from the time that it occurred, but were unable to generate the experiences of having told the narrative in the past, which did not allow them to tell the narratives in the same way as healthy comparisons. A particularly interesting result from past investigations has been that despite amnesia, patients with focal hippocampal or MTL damage display relatively intact remote past memories (Squire et al., 2010; Kirwan et al., 2008; Bayley, Gold, Hopkins, & Squire, 2005; Bayley, Hopkins, & Squire, 2006; Maguire et al., 2010). In contrast, patients with more widespread damage (Rosenbaum, McKinnon, Levine & Moscovitch, 2004; Rosenbaum, Moscovitch, Foster, Schnyer, Gao, Kovacevic, Verfaellie, et al., 2008) and developmental damage (Kwan et al., 2010) have been reported to have more deficits in remote past memory. However, while a prevailing notion of amnesia is the presence of intact remote memories, different methodological approaches and recent advances in our 84 understanding of the declarative memory system have suggested that remote memories in amnesia may be different. While individuals with amnesia can report remote memories these memories appear to be different than healthy comparisons. Particularly relevant to the experiment reported here are the social aspects of telling memories. Memories are told for social benefits and the sharing of memories may help build a sense of self (Bluck, Alea, & Habermas, 2005). The ability to recognize what and how you have told a story in the past will shape how you tell the story in the future. The healthy comparisons in this task tended to tell fairly consistent stories over time that trended towards becoming more concise with time. The individuals with amnesia on the other hand did not perform like the healthy comparisons. They told stories that were less consistent (to the point of being different stories) that did not have a sense of continuity to previous tellings. The social implications of this deficit are profound. The ability to tailor your narrative to the person and the context allow you to shape the interaction and the experience. The presence of intact declarative memory systems in the healthy comparison participants allowed for them to shape their stories in a certain way from telling to telling. The individuals with amnesia, were unable to recover the previous episodes and thus unable to shape their stories in a socially beneficial way. One particularly striking example of the difference in how individuals with amnesia tell stories multiple times over time to the same communication partner is provided in Table 4. Just one hour after telling his story, “Being Burglarized” about a bike being stolen during his childhood, participant 2363 was unable to recover the previous interaction and told a completely different story of his car being stolen in his early 20s. The social ramifications for this difference in how stories are told over time 85 could be profound. This is yet another demonstration of the contribution of the hippocampal declarative memory system to cognition outside of memory. How, where and why we tell stories matter because our lives narratives often form the glue that binds us together. It would be extremely difficult to navigate social interactions without the ability to know when, if or how you had spoken about different events or topics, it would be quite maladaptive to have memories that were fragmented, devoid of detail or were confused over time. The damaged hippocampus may be particularly affected by the present causing lability in their performance of telling stories. The inability to recover past experience decreases the ability to live or experience or recover the continuity of life. Different contexts may drive or require the telling of different details. Take the telling of your wedding day. After telling the story many times, you may have a cut and dried version that you can tell at anytime, but imagine how you would tell the story to your significant other, an individual from the wedding party, a co-worker that just became engaged and a researcher from a psychology lab. Each one of those individuals will have certain specific knowledge of you, weddings and a particular social relation to you, and the combination and interaction of those relationships and knowledge will alter how you tell the story. However, difficulty in generating the representations of the wedding day itself as well as how and what your communication knows about you and weddings will create problems in how you shape the narrative to the demands of the current context. Although the narratives of amnesic participants were highly variable in their content, there were not many significant differences in the comparison of their ratings of 86 their events compared to healthy participants ratings of their own events. Interestingly, the amensic participants rated the importance and emotion of the event at the time significantly lower than comparison participants. Perhaps the decrease in “episodicness” of their memories did not allow them to accurately assess their subjective feeling at the time of the event or dampened the phenomenological experience during recall and reexperience. This disconnect between telling very different stories (e.g., patient 2363 telling of his bike being stolen at age 8 versus car being stolen at age 22) yet rating the stories similarly may provide evidence for a lack of autonoetic experience when telling these past remote memories. Reexperiencing in the moment, may allow for more episodic details to be recovered and shared, but may also guide accurate assessment of the feeling at the time. This lability in performance is similar to findings from decision making in patients with hippocampal damage, where they were impaired on the Iowa Gambling Task. Their performance was impaired as they were not performing advantageously (like healthy participants) or disadvantageously (like patients with frontal lobe damage; Gupta, Duff, Denburg, Cohen, Bechara, & Tranel, 2009). Rather their performance was seemingly random and changed drastically from event to event. We see similar performance in the telling of these narratives, where past experience does not guide present behavior. Where healthy comparisons appeared to censure themselves and tell roughly the same story from telling to telling, individuals with amnesia told drastically different stories from time to time. The deficits we observe in active and flexible manipulation of acquired declarative memory representations being used in service of thinking about and telling 87 narratives across time conditions appears obvious when viewing the issue from the relational memory standpoint. The role of the hippocampus is flexibly bind together the co-experiences of particular events and moments. One of these pieces of information is time, however, the system is not then ordered by time. Thus, damage to the hippocampus should affect the ability to flexibly and dynamically pull together mental representations of memory will thinking about narratives regardless of time. The current results paired with past investigations reveal the importance of the hippocampal declarative memory system in shaping future advantageous performance. In the decision-making literature, an intact hippocampal declarative memory system allows one to make advantageous decisions, while in narratives an intact hippocampal declarative memory system allows one to tell narratives tuned to the particular demands of the interaction, allowing one to leverage narratives to the social demands of the moment. An interesting point raised by Squire et al. (2011) is the issue of experimentally measuring the hippocampus’ role in recollecting past and imaging future events is that it depends on spoken narrative. A problem with this method is that hippocampal or other brain damage may contribute to problems in language or narrative abilities. Perhaps using complementary methods such as functional imaging we can ask individuals to think of or imagine stories without telling them and look for activation of hippocampal and other brain areas activation without relying on the actual narration of the stories. Contrary to previous findings for the retelling of autobiographical memory we did not see an increase in the length or repetition of story details from telling to telling across the healthy comparisons (Campbell, Nadel, Duke & Ryan, 2011; Nadel, Campbell, & Ryan, 2007). For the “War of the Ghosts” stories I demonstrated the main finding in 88 amnesia that individuals with amnesia are profoundly impaired in their ability to learn new declarative information. The comparison subjects showed the general trend found in previous investigations of the “War if the Ghosts” where they became less accurate over time, however, there was no significant effect of time on repeated details, indicating that they were fairly consistent in keeping the few accurate (or inaccurate) details from telling to telling (Bartlett, 1932). An interesting future investigation of the “War of the Ghosts” narratives from the healthy comparisons may be to look into language measures such as cohesion and coherence which has previously been demonstrated to rely on declarative memory (Kurczek & Duff, 2011) and see if those measures decrease over time (i.e., as demand on declarative memory increases). While we were unable to investigate the impact of deficits in declarative memory within this task with patients with hippocampal amnesia, increasing demands on declarative memory within healthy participants may reveal similar effects as we observed within the tellings of personal stories. Another interesting potential future investigation is into audience design in patients with amnesia. While narrative production appears to place high demands on the HDMS, particularly representational flexibility, as speakers creatively (re)construct events and chose what information and details to represent or omit in response to contextual and audience cues (e.g., healthy comparisons telling similar stories to the same experimenter four times in the present experiment). While there is some evidence that creative aspects of linguistic behavior are disrupted in amnesia, the role of HDMS in the social aspects of narrative construction is a fairly open question. 89 To better understand how speakers use contextual and audience cues to tailor narratives, we could ask participants to look at two stimuli (paintings produced by an adult or child) and construct a narrative to be told to either a child or adult. Comparing lexical and syntactic characteristics as well as subjective ratings, we could focus on the ability of the speaker to use context and audience information to create distinct and appropriate narratives. Traditionally, patients with hippocampal amnesia have been described as having a severe and selective memory impairment with other cognitive abilities left intact. Here, I begin to explore the interactions and interdependencies of multiple cognitive functions. Specifically, I provide evidence of the early and fundamental role of the declarative memory underlying other cognitive processes such as language and social interaction. Without an intact declarative memory system, patients with hippocampal amnesia tell narratives in a fundamentally different way across time. This indicates that individuals with hippocampal amnesia may not be able to flexibly adapt to different contexts or perform advantageous in multiple contexts. 90 Table 3. Demographic and neuropsychological characteristics of hippocampal amnesic participants. Sex Ed Age Etiology HC Intelligence Memory Language Volume Patient WAIS-III WMS-III Token BNT FSIQ GMI Test 1846 F 14 51 Anoxia -4.23 84 57 41 43 1951 M 16 62 HSE -8.1 106 57 44 49 2308 M 18 58 HSE N/A 98 45 44 52 2363 M 18 58 Anoxia -2.64 98 73 44 58 2563 M 16 59 Anoxia N/A 94 63 44 52 N/A -5.0 96.0 59.0 43.4 50.8 ±2.8 ±8.0 ±10.2 ±1.3 ±5.4 AM 16.4 57.6 Summary ±1.7 ±4.0 Note. AM = Hippocampal amnesic participants; F = Female; M = Male; Ed = Education; HSE = Herpes simplex encephalitis; HC Volume = Hippocampal z-scores represent the combined (left and right hemisphere) studentised residuals of hippocampal volume relative to a group of comparison subjects (Allen et al., 2006; Buchanan et al., 2005). WAIS-III = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; FSIQ = Full Scale Intelligence Quotient, WMS-III GMI = Wechsler Memory Scale-III; GMI = General Memory Index; CFT = Complex Figure test BN = Boston Naming Test 91 Figure 3. Memory ratings for condition 1, multiple tellings. Please use the following scale to rate the questions below: 1 2 3 4 Not at all Somewhat Moderately Very 5 Extremely 1) The importance of the event at the time it occurred? 2) The importance of the event currently? 3) The emotionality of the event at the time it occurred? 4) The emotionality of the event currently? 5) How vividly was your memory recalled? 6) What was your overall arousal or energy level at the time of the event? 7) How positive or negative was the event at the time it occurred -3 -2 Very Somewhat Negative Negative -1 0 Neutral +1 +2 +3 Somewhat Very Positive Positive 92 Figure 4. Internal to overall ratio across multiple tellings for hippocampal amnesics and healthy comparison participants. 1 Internal to Overall Detail Ratio 0.9 0.8 * * * * 0.7 0.6 0.5 Comparison 0.4 Amnesia 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 1 2 3 Time Note. * indicates significant at p<0.05 level. 4 * Note. * indicates significant at p<0.05 level. * * * * * Figure 5. Consistency data for multiple tellings for hippocampal amnesics and the healthy comparison participants. 93 93 94 Table 4. Comparison of amnesic and healthy comparison narratives over time. Telling Number Comparison Well I was about four or five uh 1 years old um and I was la-and my parents were leaving for some reason anyway I needed a babysitter the next door neighbor kid young girl or girl much older than I at the time anyway was coming to babysit me and I recall she was coming over to the house and I was outside and I greeted her and I went to uh open the door and in those days the milk man came you got deliveries of milk and they left em in this case on your porch and it was a metal box and uh and I wasn’t tall enough to reach the door- handle to open the door so I stood up on this box and while I did I caught my shin on corner of one of these milk boxes and ripped open my leg uh a big gash on it and I had to be taken to the doctor and-and uh I-I just reremember this excruciating pain while my mom and dad were holdin me down while the doctor stuck this long needle inside my gash, inside the wound and it just remem- it hurt like crazy and I and I have – I still- I have a very long scar on that shin to this day and it was just rather traumatizing. So uh pain is what I remember from that. Amnesia - 2363 Job interview uh okay which job interview do you want me to tell you about? Um well let’s do the Texaco interview. Thy had me over to Houston ‘cause I was livin’ in Beaumont at the time and the uh they had me over and they had me talk to five different people and one turned out to be my boss and the other four were her members of the team that was developing this probe to use for oil exploration and it was a dielectric energy level and- or dielectric measurement device and it was used to tell the uh dielectric properties of the water in the ground and by telling that you could tell between saltwater and uh freshwater, ‘cause freshwater is very- is very uh dielectric- or the dielectric level isis very high the resistance is very high and the uh saltwater is very low so it makes a good demarcation point when you look for oil in the ground and I built a probe that was- consisted of four circuit boards, you know there were four circuit boards and circuitry on each one of ‘em. They were about ’bout two inches wide- I mean two inches wide and uh ‘bout four inches long and there were multiples of those ‘cause we had to put it in a tube that was about roughly one and a half inches wide- or one and a half- two and a half- it woulda been two and a half inches wide and the uh we had to mount those in a string to make the whole circuitry and the uh mm let’s see uh I forgot uh what I was talking about ’boutoh I made the probe but I forgot what the question was job interview okay that’sthat’s what the job was that I interviewed for. Twenty- one I guess. 95 Table 4. Continued. Details 2 Details Words = 235 Internal = 15 External = 10 IOR = 0.6 That’s a story of uh I was a little kid and um I had the next door neighbor girl was gonna babysit me for some reason um and so I met her outside or close to house and I was gonna let her in and in those days we got milk delivered to us by a milkman and that was front porch they had this milk box it was a square box and I was too little I couldn’t reach the d- handle of the door so I went to let her in I was being nice I was let her in I tried st- I stood up on the milk box and I caught the edge corner of uh the lid and it just ripped a gash right into my left shin all the way up and it was nasty it went in and uh parents had to take me to the doctors and I just recall them one holdin my arms the other one holdin my legs and the doctor- Doctor Block that was his name Doctor Block and uh j-just pour- or uh big needle right into the wound and just give me a shot right intointo the wound uh ya know it was horrifying at that being four years old or fi- ya know little like that and uh hurt it hurt and uh so that was that. Words = 130 Internal = 15 External = 0 IOR = 1.0 Editorials = 0 Repeated = 40.9% Changed = 22.7% Added = 36.4% Words = 311 Internal = 4 External = 26 IOR = 0.13 Okay ya want me to repeat it? Okay the job interview at Texaco was conducted at the facility the uh I got to meet the various people in the lab I’d be working in. The uh they showed me around, took me out to dinner- or lunch I mean and then uh asked me various questions about my training and uh that was about it. I was twenty-one or twenty-two at the time. Words = 102 Internal =7 External = 1 IOR = 0.7 Editorials = 2 Repeated =0.0% Changed =0.0% Added = 100.0% Note. Panel A) How important was the event at the time B) How emotional was the event at the time C) How positive or negative was the event at the time D) How vivid is your memory for the event Figure 6. Self Ratings for multiple tellings for hippocampal amnesics and their healthy comparisons 96 96 97 Figure 7. War of the Ghosts accuracy data for hippocampal amnesics and their healthy comparisons. 0.5 0.45 0.4 * * * * Accuracy 0.35 0.3 0.25 Comparison 0.2 Amnesia 0.15 0.1 0.05 0 1 2 3 Time 4 98 Table 5. Comparison of hippocampal amnesic and healthy comparison performance on War of the Ghosts retelling. Telling Comparison Amnesia - 2363 Uh there were two uh m- or I dunno men or boys The war of the ghost went down to the river they were from Ergulac and story. Okay it um they went down and um they were wanting to hunt talked about two young seals and they heard um war cries and they saw men that went out some canoes and one came to the side of the river hunting and they heard where they were at and said uh what do you think some noise off in the come with us we’re going up river to war on some distance and it was people. And one of ’em said um well I might get another tribe that was killed and I don’t have any arrows and they said apparently- or that waswell there’s arrows in the canoe and then he said they th- they thought he didn’t want to go but he told the other guy that was a tribe and they got he could go with them so the one went back home into a fight and the one and the other got in the canoe and went with them of them was killed and and um, um they went up to- starts with a K, the other one paddled Komoldone or something like that and uh anyway back to shore and told they uh started fighting and some of ‘em there the villages that he had were people killed and some of them were killed been attacked by ghosts and then he heard one of ‘em say um let’s get out and that that the other of here the Indian’s been hit and uh so he didn’t one had been killed the think he had been shot but anyway they went back other member who him home and uh let’s see. He went and built a fire and and his friend one of then he uh said to the others that uh uh they said whom had been killed so that’s it. I’d been hit but I didn’t think I had been and so then overnight they said um black stuff started coming out of his mouth and his face was contorted and they said he’s dead. And I forget the about he said about the ghost but Words = 279 Words = 103 Details Accuracy = 51.2% Accuracy = 11.9% 1 99 Table 5. Continued. Telling Comparison Oh the war of the ghost story okay. There were two uh guys I donno if they were boys from Ergulac and they went down to the river to hunt for seals and when they were down on the river um they heard war cries and they saw in the distance um well it was foggy and uh they heard war cries and they saw these canoes out in the distance and they one of the canoes came to the side of the river where they were at and uh said they said one of the guys said what do you think and do you wanna come with us we’re going up to um I donno Kamalon or something like that uh to uh war on this neighboring village um uh one of the guys said well I am afraid I’ll get killed and I don’t have any arrows and they said well we have arrows in the canoe. He said well I don’t really wanna go so he told the other boy or guy that hey you can go with them I’m gonna go back to the village so the one went back to the village and then the other one got into the canoe with the men and um I think there were five of them in the canoe and they went up and stared way with this village um there were people killed on their side and on our side um and then one of the- on of the people the guys that said hey let’s get out of here the Indians have been hit and uh the guy said you know they were talking about me that had been shot but I didn’t think I had been shot um so anyway we went back um when I got home I went and started a fire and uh went in the tent and uh told the people that he had been thought he had been with ghosts and uh overnight dark things- black things started coming out of his mouth and he had a contorted face and one of the people in the village yelled he’s dead. Words = 360 Details Accuracy = 51.2% Repeated Details = 52.3% Added Details = 31.8% 2 Amnesia - 2363 Oh let’s see the war of the ghost story oh boy had something to do with uh two Indians and they were expecting an attack from dfrom a different tribe or something and they uh one went back to warn the people the people and the other one went ahead to spot them or- or find out their position or where they were that’s pretty much all I can remember right now. Words = 73 Accuracy = 6.0% Repeated Details = 33.3% Added Details = 50.0% 100 CHAPTER 6: HIPPOCAMPAL LATERALITY: THE EFFECT OF UNILATERAL HIPPOCAMPAL DAMAGE ON LANGUAGE USE 6.1. Background While individuals with bilateral hippocampal damage have demonstrated deficits in multiple domains of language use (Kurczek, et al., 2013; MacKay, Stewart, & Burke, 1998; Park et al., 2011), an open question is whether there are differential contributions in the material processing of either the left or right hippocampus. While the left hemisphere has been implicated in many language processes (see Chapter 2) and the right hemisphere often associated with the processing of visuospatial information (Giovagnoli, Casazza & Avanzini, 1995; Gleibner, Helmstaedter, & Elger, 1998), it is an open question whether the deficits in language observed after bilateral damage to the hippocampus would be differentially expressed after unilateral hippocampal damage. In the 50 years since HM underwent surgery to relieve complications from epilepsy there has been a concerted effort to understand the cognitive processing of structures in the medial temporal lobe in order to avoid the same consequences suffered by HM after his surgery. Almost all cases of temporal lobe epilepsy are associated with some degree of memory impairment (Fisher, Vickery, Gibson, Hermann, Penovich, Scherer, et al., 2000) and the predominant understanding of the contribution of each hemisphere has been that of material specificity, where the left and right hemispheres process different types of information (Milner, 1970). While global amnestic syndromes are rare (Van Buren, 1987; Walczak, Radtke, McNamara, Lewis, Luther, Thompson, Wilson, et al., 1990), milder deficits after unilateral damage occur in material specific 101 recall (Milner 1968; 1990; Naugle, 1992). The left hemisphere is thought to process verbal material (Mungas, Ehlers, Walton, & McClutchen, 1985; Ojeman & Dodrill, 1985; Rausch & Babb, 1993; Sass, Spencer, Kim, Westerveld, Novelly, & Lencz, 1990), while the right hemisphere is thought to process non-verbal material (Abrahams, Pickering, Polkey, & Morris, 1997; Barr, Chelune, Hermann, Loring, Perrine, Strauss, Trenerry, et al., 1997; Giovagnoli, et al., 1995; Jones-Gotman, 1986; Smith & Milner, 1981; Taylor, 1969; Feigenbaum, Polkey & Morris, 1996), although the findings are less consistent than those found in the left hemisphere (Giovagnoli, et al., 1995; Pigott & Milner, 1993; Smith & Milner, 1981). These conflicting reports have led some to argue that the material specificity hypothesis is too simple and that neural structures in the medial temporal lobe outside of the hippocampus including the perirhinal cortex are part of a more extensive network of structures both within the medial temporal lobe and outside including the hemispheres at large, that contribute to the deficits observed after unilateral damage to the MTL (Saling, 2009). Similarly, Weber and colleagues (2005) suggest that because of the multiple reciprocal connections of the hippocampus to cortical systems and the neural metabolic disturbances observed in more lateral portions of the temporal lobe in epilepsy (Hammers, Koepp, Labbe, Brooks, Thom, Cunningham, et al., 2001), the observed deficits after unilateral MTL damage may be a consequence of dysfunction of the entire hemisphere instead of just the hippocampus. Further complications in the material specificity hypothesis include atypical language lateralization, functional reorganization and bilateral representation of verbal and spatial representations. Although left hemisphere language processing is present in the overwhelming majority of subjects, upwards of 6% of people show atypical language 102 activation in recent functional neuroimaging (Springer, Binder, Hammeke, Swanson, Frost, Brewer, Perry, et al., 1999). However, in temporal lobe epilepsy, atypical language lateralization is more common with rates observed as high as 30% (Helmstaedter, Kurthen, Linke, & Elger, 1997; Rasmussen, & Milner, 1977; Springer, Binder, Hammeke, Swanson, Frost, Bellgowan, Brewer, et al., 1999; Tanriverdi, Al Hinai, Mok, Klein, Poulin, & Olivier, 2009) with some suggesting that epileptic activity (Janszky, Jokeit, Heinemann, Schulz, Woermann, & Ebner, 2003; Janszky, Mertens, Janszky, Ebner, & Woermann, 2006) or hippocampal sclerosis may drive shifts in language lateralization (Brazdil, Chlebus, Mikl, Pazourkova, Krupa, & Rektor, 2005; Brazdil, Zakopcan, Kuba, Fanfrdlova, & Rektor, 2003; Cousin, Baciu, Pichat, Kahane, & Le Bas, 2008). Further, a study by Sass and colleagues (1992) investigated the relationship between hippocampal neuron loss and cognitive performance, finding that only memory correlated with left hippocampal neuron loss, but not language competency or general intellectual ability even in light of worse performance on the Boston Naming task compared to patients with right hippocampal damage. In a similar line of investigation, intracranial recordings have revealed activity in medial temporal lobe neurons in both hemispheres during a verbal paired associates task (Cameron, Yashar, Wilson, & Fried, 2001), while fMRI in patients with left hippocampal pathology show right medial temporal lobe activation during a verbal memory task (Richardson, Strange, Duncan, & Dolan, 2003). It is thought that, similar to the language reorganization observed in developmental cases of amnesia (Vargha-Khadem, Issacs, & Muter, 1994), adults may be 103 capable of neural reorganization that allows homologous structures opposite the damaged side to overtake cognitive processing once subserved by the damaged structures. Others have suggested that the material-specificity effect observed after unilateral MTL damage may be attributable to interactions with neural structures outside the MTL including the frontal lobes and lateral temporal lobes (Pillon, Bazin, Deweer, Ehrle, Baulac, & Dubois, 1999). A functional imaging study by Kelley and colleagues (1998) found differences in the lateralization of activation for words (left), faces (right) and nameable objects (bilateral) in both the frontal and medial temporal lobes. Interestingly, the left MTL was activated by all material types (both verbal and nonverbal), showing the strongest activation for the namable objects, suggesting to them, that the left MTL is not specialized for processing particular stimulus types (Kelley et al., 1998). Similarly Golby and colleagues (2001) found symmetrical activation for scenes and faces in both the frontal and medial temporal lobes. In summary, although there has been some conflicting evidence, a majority of the unilateral contribution of the hippocampus to memory appears to suggest similar contributions observed in the hemispheres at large, with the left hemisphere contributing to language processing and the right hemisphere contributing to spatial processing. 6.2. Specific Aims and Hypotheses Aim #3: To investigate the contribution of left versus right damage to the hippocampus on referential processing and narrative construction. It is hypothesized that damage to the left hippocampus will make a larger, more significant or disproportionate contribution to language processing. Therefore, it is predicted that 104 patients with left hippocampal damage will perform significantly worse than patients with right hippocampal damage in both referential processing and narrative construction. 6.3. Methods 6.3.1. Participants Six people participated in this study, 3 individuals with left temporal lobe (TL) damage and 3 people with right temporal lobe damage. Participants were recruited from the Neurological Patient Registry at the University of Iowa. Inclusion criteria were the same as listed in Chapter 4 for all previous participant groups. Additionally, patients where excluded if their damage included frontal or occipital cortices or if neuropsychological testing revealed significant impairments in language. All six TL participants participated in the Referential Processing (Aim #1) and Narrative (Aim #2) tasks. Demographic and neuropsychological information are listed in Table 6 and Table 7. 6.3.2. Experimental protocol The TL participants participated in all of the experiments previously described in Chapters 4 and 5. For Aim #1 the Referential processing experiment was performed as described in preliminary study 3 (Kurczek, et al., 2013) was conducted (for details about the procedure and analysis see Chapter 4 – the only difference in procedure is in one of the fixed effects – gender (this experiment) versus repetition (Chapter 4); the same analysis approach was used here). For Aim #2, the narrative experiment was also 105 performed just as described in the hippocampal amnesic experiment was conducted (see Chapter 5). Initial analyses compared the patients with unilateral left and right hippocampal damage. Secondary analyses compared the patients with unilateral hippocampal damage to patients with bilateral hippocampal damage and their comparison participants. 6.4. Results 6.4.1. Referential processing 6.4.1.1. Offline Data for patients with unilateral hippocampal damage The offline data speak to the participants’ ultimate interpretation of the pronoun; if we were successful in establishing the first-mentioned character as more prominent in the story, we would expect lower endorsement rates in the same-gender condition when the pronoun refers to the 2nd mentioned character. Analysis of the response to the judgment task (whether the picture matched the narrative) revealed that there were no group differences between left and right hippocampal patients as both were highly likely to endorse the picture as matching the story across three conditions (D1 = M = 97.4%, 96.4%; SD = 1.8%, 1.8%; D2 = M = 87.5%, 93.8%; SD = 9.0%; 4.1% ; S1 =M = 95.8%, 92.2%; SD = 3.3% , 8.3%; left and right respectively), with lower endorsement in the S2 condition (M = 28.1%, 31.3%; SD = 12.2, 13.9%). The analysis revealed that effects of order of mention (OOM) and gender were qualified by a significant gender by OOM interaction (z=-3.32, p<.001; see Figure 8), due to the low endorsement rates in the S2 condition (see Figure 7). This result is consistent with previous findings (Arnold, et al., 2000, Experiment 2; Kurczek et al., 2013) where participants (i.e., healthy undergraduate 106 participants, healthy older adults and individuals with brain damage outside the medial temporal lobes) had high endorsement of agreement in all conditions except the same gender, second-mentioned condition. 6.4.1.2. Eye-movement data for patients with unilateral hippocampal damage Analyses of the eye movements made during interpretation of the pronoun are used to test for group differences in the use of discourse context to interpret a potentially ambiguous pronoun in real time. We directly compared eye-fixations for the left and right hippocampal patients in one analysis which included gender, order of mention and participant group (left vs. right) as orthogonal factors, as well as time-window, with the baseline window coded as reference. The first analysis compared left and right hippocampal patients and found that the groups performed comparably. At baseline there were no significant effects (ts<1.4) indicating no anticipatory effects or group differences. Analysis of the pronoun region revealed significant effects of gender (t=-4.61, p<.05; see Figure 9) and order of mention (t=-3.69, p<.05) that was qualified by a significant gender by order of mention interaction (t=-3.83, p<.05) which is similar to the performance of undergraduates, healthy older comparisons and individuals with brain damage outside of the hippocampus (Kurczek et al., 2013). However, there were no effects of group or group interactions, indicating that L and R TL groups performed similarly during the pronoun region. In the final region, there were significant effects of gender (t=-5.77, p<.05) and order of mention (t=-6.40, p<.05) that was qualified by a significant gender by order of mention 107 interaction (t=-4.85, p<.05), additionally there was a significant effect of group (t=-2.06, p<.05), The participants with right hippocampal damage during interpretation of the pronoun had a larger target preference when the characters were of different gender (t=4.48, p<.01), and when the target was the first-mentioned character (t=4.99, p<.05). In addition, there was a significant gender*order interaction (t=-3.35, p<.05). Participant with left hippocampal during interpretation of the pronoun had a larger target preference when characters were of different gender (t=3.17, p<.01), and when the target was the first-mentioned character (t=2.50, p<.05). In addition, there was a significant gender*order interaction (t=-2.50, p<.05). Comparing the performance of patients with unilateral hippocampal damage to patients with bilateral hippocampal damage and healthy comparisons revealed differential performance between each unilateral patient group, where patients with damage to the left hippocampus perform much more similarly to the bilateral hippocampal amnesics than comparisons while patients with right hippocampal damage perform more similarly to comparisons participants but not patients with bilateral hippocampal damage. During the pronoun window, comparison of left hippocampal patients to healthy comparisons revealed a significant group by gender by order of mention interaction (t=2.60, p<.014), but showed no group main effect (t = -0.46, p = 1.0) or group interactions compared to the bilateral hippocampal amnesic patients (order of mention by group, t = 1.88, p = 0.061; gender by group, t = -1.44, p = 0.366; and order or mention by gender by group, t = 1.71, p = 0.459) indicating that their performance was fairly similar to amnesic performance. 108 In contrast, during the pronoun window, comparison of right hippocampal patients to healthy comparisons revealed no group or group interactions (ps > 0.228) while comparison to the bilateral hippocampal amnesic patients revealed a significant group by order of mention interaction (t = -3.43, p = 0.026), where patients with right hippocampal damage had a larger target preference than the bilateral hippocampal patients. 6.4.2. Narrative construction analysis for patients with unilateral hippocampal damage 6.4.2.1. Autobiographical memory interview and consistency analysis of multiple tellings in patients with unilateral hippocampal damage Analysis of the multiple tellings of personal stories comparing individuals with either left or right hippocampal damage revealed no significant group or group by time interactions. The only tests to reach statistical significance were effects of time on internal details, F(3,12) = 16.54, p < 0.001, total details, F(3,12) = 9.83, p = 0.030, and percentage of added details, F(3,12) = 4.45, p = 0.050. Follow-up contrasts revealed the first telling had more internal details than the third (p = 0.020) and the final telling (p = 0.021), the first telling had more total details than the second (p = 0.016) and no significant pairwise comparisons for the added details. The left and right hippocampal patients were then compared to the bilateral hippocampal patients and their healthy comparisons. Significant effects of group were found for internal details, F(3,15) = 5.33, p = 0.011 (see Figure 10), total details, F(3,15) = 4.92, p = 0.014, words, F(3,15) = 4.72, p = 0.016, repeated details, F(3,15) = 9.07, p < 109 0.001, and added details, F(3,15) = 15.28, p < 0.001 (see Figure 11). Follow-up contrasts revealed that hippocampal amnesic participants performed significantly worse than healthy comparisons across these measures (ps < 0.05). Additionally, hippocampal amnesic participants performed significantly worse than left and right hippocampal patients on repeated and added details, (ps < 0.05). Patients with left or right hippocampal damage did not differ significantly from healthy comparison participants. 6.5.2.2. War of the Ghosts analysis for patients with unilateral hippocampal damage In the analysis of the multiple tellings of the “War of the Ghosts” stories there were no significant group or group by time interactions for the comparison of right and left unilateral hippocampal patients. The only test that was significantly different was the within subjects effect of time on accuracy, F(3,12) = 4.08, p = 0.033, although follow-up tests did not reveal any significant contrasts. The left and right hippocampal patients were then compared to the hippocampal amnesics and their healthy comparisons. A 4X4 repeated measures ANOVA (group – left hippocampus, right hippocampus, bilateral hippocampus and healthy comparison; time – 1, 2, 3, and 4) on accuracy revealed both an effect of time, F(3,45) = 4.60, p = 0.026 and group, F(3,15) = 13.60, p < 0.001. Follow-up contrasts revealed that the bilateral hippocampal amnesics performed significantly worse than healthy comparisons (p < 0.001) and patients with right hippocampal damage (p = 0.007) but not left hippocampal damage (p = 0.095). Additionally, the first telling was more accurate than the final retelling (p = 0.003). There were also significant effects of group on repeated, F(3,15) = 11.85, p < 0.001, changed, F(3,15) = 3.99, p = 0.028 and added details, F(3,15) = 7.54, p 110 = 0.003. Follow-up contrasts revealed that bilateral hippocampal amnesics had less repeated details than healthy comparisons (p < 0.001), and right hippocampal patients, (p = 0.021), less changed details than healthy comparisons, (p = 0.024), and less added details than healthy comparisons, (p = 0.004), and left hippocampal patients, (p = 0.004). 6.5. Discussion The left hemisphere has been implicated in many language processes (see Chapter 2) and the right hemisphere often associated with the processing of visuospatial information (Giovagnoli, Casazza & Avanzini, 1995; Gleibner, Helmstaedter, & Elger, 1998), but an open question is whether the deficits in language observed after bilateral damage to the hippocampus would be differentially expressed after unilateral hippocampal damage. Here I investigated the contribution of unilateral hippocampal damage to referential processing and narrative construction. While the results do not conclusively suggest differential contributions of the left over right to language measures presented in this dissertation, there is a trend in these data that suggests greater contributions of left hippocampus to language than to right. This trend warrants further investigation. While patients with left hippocampal damage displayed decreased performance in the pronoun resolution eyetracking task, they did not perform nearly as impaired as the patients with bilateral hippocampal damage. This is somewhat surprising given the lack of differences in neuropsychological scores in memory and language between the patients with unilateral left and right hippocampal damage, but it may also indicate that language, in particular reference may be a highly sensitive marker for declarative memory processing deficits (Kurczek & Duff, 2011, Kurczek et al., 2013). It may also indicate that eye movement experiments are also a more sensitive measure in uncovering memory 111 performance as others have suggested (Hannula, Baym, Warren, & Cohen, 2012). Beyond the sensitivity of the task and method, the use of linear mixed model regression may also be more sensitive in detecting small differences in performance as both the within subject and within trial variability is accounted for. In tasks and patient groups where performance may be highly variable, this method of analysis may be more powerful in uncovering subtle differences in performance. In the narrative tasks, there were no significant differences in group performance between the unilateral hippocampal patient groups, but there appeared to be a trend towards worse performance for left over right in narrative construction. This trend could become a significant difference with better controlled groups in terms of lesion type and age of seizure onset. This trend does match the predicted outcome of greater impairments in the left versus right hemisphere for language performance. However, this investigation is unable to answer whether this (potential) decrease in performance is due to differential processing capacities of the hippocampus or differential distribution of material specific processing between the hemispheres. Perhaps rather than the left hippocampus contributing more of the functional processing capacities than the right, the loss of afferent and efferent connections to the lateral left temporal lobes impairs performance within linguistic tasks. Perhaps by varying the linguistic and spatial demands within the same task we could tease out the relative processing contributions of the left or right hippocampus. The comparison of the temporal lobe patients to bilateral hippocampal amnesics and healthy comparison participants revealed performance of patients with unilateral hippocampal damage closer to healthy performance than amnesic performance. Additionally there did not appear to be any gradient of performance from healthy to right damage to left damage to bilateral hippocampal damage. Perhaps the same issues from the left versus right comparison are at play in this analysis. It may also indicate the pervasiveness hippocampal processing capacities in the face of damage. Though left and 112 right hippocampal damage may respectively affect verbal and spatial memories, the types of processes that referential processing and narrative construction are attempting to measure, generating and binding representations, may be more resistant to hippocampal damage. This comparison and lack of statistical differences may indicate a lack of differential lateral contributions of the hippocampus to the fundamental processing capacities. In terms of hippocampal contributions to language and neuroanatomical interactions, the results from this study may indicate that rather than the hippocampus being a fundamental aspect of the neuroanatomic language system, the hippocampus may just be a an early and pervasive contributor to cognition. The comparison of the unilateral hippocampal patients to bilateral hippocampal patients and healthy comparisons revealed that the unilateral patients performed more similarly to healthy comparisons. This indicates that functioning of the hippocampus is fairly robust to damage, especially when patients do not show neuropsychological deficits in declarative memory performance. The slight differences in performance when comparing left and right hippocampal patients may indicate hemisphere processing differences rather than lateralized hippocampal functioning. The loss of afferent and efferent connections with in a hemisphere may explain the decrease in performance for patients with left hippocampal damage, where damage to the left hippocampus impairs the ability of the hippocampal complex to access neocortical sites of linguistic materials stored in more lateral left temporal lobe cortices. Thus, perhaps the deficits previously observed in bilateral hippocampal damage on language tasks are not a matter of the hippocampus playing a role in language, but rather that certain aspects of language place a disproportionate amount of demand on the processing capacities of the hippocampus. This principle would apply across other domains of cognition that have been revealed to receive hippocampal contributions like decision making and social interaction. Rather than the hippocampus being recruited for 113 language, decision making and social interaction, the hippocampus may make early and continuous contributions of fundamental processing capacities that receive greater and lesser demands depending on the context. Within language, the hippocampus is thus making contributions to classic perisylvian neocortical circuits through ongoing interaction with higher cortical structures in the frontal and temporal cortices. One issue in the patient group was the variable demographics. A surprising finding was the lack of differences in memory and language performance on neuropsychological tests between the two patient groups with unilateral hippocampal damage. Perhaps a left patient group with deficits in verbal memory would show more dramatic effects within these language measures. The patients were also mixed in their etiology. One patient (1580) developed damage after herpes simplex encephalitis. Perhaps having not lived with seizures for some length of his life made him stand out differently from the other patients. The patients were also variable in their onset of seizures, which could be an important variable if this investigation continues to enroll more participants. As discussed earlier, childhood onset of seizures can potentially drive language development in the brain in different ways than typically developing adults. Some suggested that epileptic activity (Janszky, Jokeit, Heinemann, Schulz, Woermann, & Ebner, 2003; Janszky, Mertens, Janszky, Ebner, & Woermann, 2006) or hippocampal sclerosis may actually drive shifts in language lateralization (Brazdil, Chlebus, Mikl, Pazourkova, Krupa, & Rektor, 2005; Brazdil, Zakopcan, Kuba, Fanfrdlova, & Rektor, 2003; Cousin, Baciu, Pichat, Kahane, & Le Bas, 2008). With larger groups it will be important to separate adult from childhood on-set cases of seizures in order to compare individuals with more similar language development trajectories. 114 Table 6. Demographic characteristics of patients with unilateral hippocampal damage. Patient Sex Hand Age Etiology Chron Sx Onset Ed 2246 F Un 70 Resection 17 0 20 3166 M R 41 Resection 10 3 16 3509 M R 51 Resection 6 42 14 Left TL 54.0 N/A 11.0 15.0 16.7 Summary ± 14.7 ± 5.6 ± 23.4 ± 3.1 1580 M R 43 HSE 23 20 16 2962 M Un 58 Resection 12 8 14 3386 F R 37 Resection 8 17 18 Right 46.0 N/A 14.3 15.0 16.0 Summary ± 10.8 ± 7.8 ± 6.2 ± 2.0 Difference t(4) = 0.76, p = 0.491 t(4) = 0.60, t(2.28) = 0.0, t(4) = 0.32, p = 0.582 p = 0.999 p = 0.768 Note. Left TL = Left temporal lobectomy participants; F = Female; M = Male; Right TL = Right temporal lobectomy participants; Hand = Handedness; Un = Unhanded; R = Right; Resection = Temporal lobectomy resection; HSE = Herpes Simplex Encephalitis; Chron = Chronicity, time since brain damage; Sx Onset = Age at diagnoses of seizure disorder; Ed = Years of education 101.7 ± 7.6 103 105.0 ± 2.0 115 109.0 107 117 109.3 Left TL Summary ± 11.3 104 3509 1580 2962 3386 Right TL Summary ± 6.8 105 107 105 93 96 3166 107 VERB FSIQ 116 WAIS- WAIS-III 2246 Patient ± 9.1 114.7 123 116 105 ± 6.1 110.3 117 105 109 PERF WAIS- Intelligence ± 9.9 111.0 118 104 N/A ± 2.0 93.0 95 93 91 GMI WMS-III ± 3.2 9.7 11 6 12 ± 0.6 7.7 7 8 8 Recall AVLT Memory ± 0.0 44.0 44 44 N/A ± 3.0 41.0 41 38 44 Test Token Table 7. Neuropsychological characteristics of patients with unilateral hippocampal damage. ± 2.3 54.3 57 53 53 ± 4.0 54.0 58 50 54 BNT ± 1.2 41.3 40 42 42 ± 17.6 41.0 49 28 61 COWA Language 115 p = 0.502 p = 0.967 p = 0.530 t(4) = 0.69, p = 0.228 p = 0.394 t(1.05) = 2.54, t(2.13) = 1.06, p = 0.272 t(4) = 1.34, p = 0.907 p = 0.977 t(4) = 0.13, t(2.02) = 0.03, Note. Left TL = Left temporal lobectomy participants; Right TL = Right temporal lobectomy participants; WAIS-III = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; FSIQ = Full Scale Intelligence Quotient; VERB = Verbal subtest composite score; PERF = Performance subtest composite score; WMS-III GMI = Wechsler Memory Scale-III; GMI = General Memory Index; AVLT Recall = Recall trial of the Auditory Verbal Learning Test; CFT = Complex Figure test – Recall Score; BNT = Boston Naming Test; COWA = Controlled Oral Word Association Test t(4) = 0.74, t(4) = 0.04, Table 7. Continued. 116 117 Percent 'yes' response Figure 8. Off-line performance for unilateral hippocampal patients. 1 0.9 0.8 0.7 0.6 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 0 d1 d2 s1 s2 Condition Left Right Note. S = Same; D = Different; 1 = First mentioned character; 2 = Second mentioned character. 118 Figure 9. Time course of fixation preference data for unilateral hippocampal patients. Note. A) Unilateral left patients B) Unilateral right patients; S = Same; D = Different; 1 = First mentioned character; 2 = Second mentioned character. Vertical lines denote time windows. 119 Number of Internal Dtails Figure 10. Internal details across multiple tellings for unilateral hippocampal patients. 18 16 14 12 10 8 6 4 2 0 Left Right 1 2 3 Time 4 120 Figure 11. Consistency data for unilateral hippocampal patients in the multiple telling. Note: A = Percentage of repeated details; B = Percentage of added details. 121 CHAPTER 7: CONCLUSIONS The goal of this dissertation was to investigate the role of the hippocampal declarative memory system’s role in language. This thesis brings together the study of two quintessential human capacities, memory and language, by examining the role of the hippocampal declarative memory system (HDMS) in language processing and use. Aim #1 examined the effects of bilateral damage to the hippocampus on referential processing and the potential for recovering performance. Although we were able to demonstrate a recovery in the use of order of mention information, we were unable to further bias looks to the target character in our repeated mention condition. This slight impairment in performance was limited to patients with hippocampal damage and not patients with brain damage to the frontal lobes. This work, as attempted in Aim #1 has potential clinical and rehabilitative implications. By understanding the contributions of neural systems to deficits in language processing and use, we may be able to develop strategies that place demand on other neural systems. While many efforts in language rehabilitation have attempted to use procedural memory methods to alleviate language impairments, recent work is beginning to demonstrate the contribution of the hippocampus to procedural memory tasks (Foerde et al., 2013). The emerging picture of the hippocampus as a ubiquitous player across domains of cognition and time periods means that individuals with damage to the hippocampus or hippocampal dysfunction may make rehabilitative efforts difficult. Further, repetition as a rehabilitative device when used without context or purpose may serve a poor rehabilitative tool. Individuals with amnesia received no benefit from repetition, while healthy comparisons and individuals with damage outside the 122 hippocampus received only marginal benefit in their performance. Clinicians working with individuals with memory impairments should realize that how repetition is leveraged matters (Verfaellie et al., 2008) and that social and real-world consequences of the rehabilitative practice will have significant consequences. As Hengst and colleagues (2010) cautioned, the aim of rehabilitation takes place within real-world contexts which need to be realized in the rehabilitative setting. In Aim #2, each participant with bilateral hippocampal damage told two types of stories (semantic – “War of the Ghosts” and episodic) multiple times. The performance of the amnesic participants on the “War of the Ghosts” story illustrated the core feature of amnesia where they were extremely impaired in their ability to learn new declarative information. In the performance of telling their own remote declarative memories healthy comparisons were fairly consistent in telling of their stories while participants with amnesia were much more variable in their tellings. This lability of the tellings from time to time may indicate the core processing capacities of the hippocampus, generating and binding representations that are affected by activation of representations in the present moment. Where healthy comparisons were able to recover previous tellings, amnesics were unable or impaired in their ability to recover previous episodes and thus were more susceptible to recently activated representations causing their stories to be drastically different. Particularly relevant to the deficits in amnesic’s performance are the social implications. Previously it has been demonstrated that the hippocampus critically contributes to the use of various interactional discourse resources including reported speech (Duff et al., 2007) and verbal play (Duff et al., 2009). Here, I demonstrate that 123 how patients with bilateral hippocampal damage tell stories across multiple social interactions is different than healthy comparisons. This and other work demonstrating the importance of the hippocampal memory system in social interaction (Davidson, Drouin, Kwan, Moscovitch, & Rosenbaum, 2012) may implicate the critical contribution of hippocampal dysfunction in disorders of social interaction. Perhaps rather than focusing on frontal lobe dysfunction in disorders of diffuse brain damage (e.g., Alzheimer’s Disease, Schizophrenia, Autism), we should look to hippocampal dysfunction and contributions. Finally, Aim #3 explored the effects of unilateral hippocampal damage on language processing and use. While the first experiment investigating referential processing in an eyetracking experiment demonstrated slight impairments for patients with unilateral left hippocampal damage, the second experiment in narrative construction did not reveal any differences in performance between patients with left or right hippocampal damage. The comparison of the patients with unilateral hippocampal damage to patients with bilateral hippocampal damage and healthy comparisons revealed performance much closer to that of the healthy comparisons. The left hemisphere has been implicated in processing verbal material (Mungas, Ehlers, Walton, & McClutchen, 1985; Ojeman & Dodrill, 1985; Rausch & Babb, 1993; Sass, Spencer, Kim, Westerveld, Novelly, & Lencz, 1990), while the right hemisphere is implicated in processing non-verbal material (Abrahams, Pickering, Polkey, & Morris, 1997; Barr, Chelune, Hermann, Loring, Perrine, Strauss, Trenerry, et al., 1997; Giovagnoli, et al., 1995; Jones-Gotman, 1986; Smith & Milner, 1981; Taylor, 1969; Feigenbaum, Polkey & Morris, 1996), and it was hypothesized that left hippocampal 124 damage would lead to greater impairments in language processing. The mixed findings in this investigation could be attributed to variability in the patient groups or differences in the task demands on the hippocampal declarative memory system. However, while the differences in the performance on the narrative task were not significantly different, there was a trend towards impaired performance by patients with left damage. The comparisons in performance of patients with left and right hippocampal damage may speak to the question of whether the hippocampus posses a differential distribution of processing capacities or is part of a hemisphere more adept at processing one material or another (i.e., left for verbal and right for spatial). The performance of patients with unilateral hippocampal damage being closer to healthy comparisons suggests that the hippocampus has similar processing capacities distributed across left and right hippocampus. The slight differences in performance within the patients with unilateral damage suggest that the differences in performance are due to the interactions within the hemisphere that the damaged hippocampus is placed, where patients with left hippocampal damage have small impairments in language measures because of deficits accessing neocortically stored linguistic material. To speak to this question further, future investigations may attempt to vary verbal versus spatial processing demands with in the same task and either use neuropsychological or functional imaging methodology to investigate either impaired left performance or activation of the left hippocampus for verbal materials. This dissertation is grounded in the idea that the processing capacities of the HDMS that contributes to the formation of new memories, and maintains representations on-line to be used in service of complex behavior, are the same processes for using and 125 processing language (Duff & Brown-Schmidt, 2012). This dissertation is the latest in a long line of evidence in support of this proposal from work demonstrating that patients with hippocampal damage and declarative memory impairment have deficits in a variety of linguistic and discursive abilities (e.g., Duff et al., 2006; Duff et al., 2007; Duff et al., 2008; Duff et al., 2009; Duff et al., 2011; MacKay, Stewart & Burke, 1998). Two areas that this dissertation furthers are referential processing and narrative construction. This work provides further evidence for the contribution of the hippocampus to referential processing while also suggesting that rehabilitation of performance will require more than repetition to provide more salience to the target. Particularly important within the referential processing is that the deficit is occurring on such a brief timescale. This work thus extends the evidence of the contribution of the hippocampus to language and its contribution to on-line processing. This dissertation also extends the contribution of the hippocampus to narrative construction. Previous work (Duff & Kurczek, 2013) had implicated the critical contribution of the hippocampus to generating and combining representations necessary for telling narratives. This work extends that contribution to how narratives are told over time and has profound implications of the impact of hippocampal damage on both language and social interaction processing. By defining and characterizing the cognitive and neural systems that support language we gain a better understanding of disorders of language. Although the HDMS has previously received little consideration as a key neural/cognitive system for language use and processing (outside of lexical acquisition/consolidation), mounting evidence points to its critical contribution to the language network that now extends beyond the 126 traditional structures of the left perisylvian areas to include the hippocampus and declarative memory (Duff & Brown-Schmidt, 2012). As evidence mounts to support the role of the hippocampus in the generation and use of on-line representations created during ongoing information processing to support behavioral performance positions the hippocampus (Kurczek et al., 2013; Rubin, Brown-Schmidt, Duff, Tranel, & Cohen, 2011) in support of other cognitive abilities beyond its traditional role in memory (e.g., perception; Warren, Duff, Tranel & Cohen, 2010; 2011 and creativity; Duff, Kurczek, Rubin, Tranel & Cohen 2013). We now begin to see how promiscuous the hippocampus is in its contribution to cognition. We are beginning to see the mark of the hippocampus across all areas of cognition, both early and continuously. Continued research with complementary methods should look into the time-course of interaction between the hippocampus and other neural structures in the coordination of higher cognitive abilities. Early evidence appears to suggest that the processing capacities of the hippocampus are called upon and used by many (most) other neural structures in the formation and establishment of complex human behavior. Amnesia is derived from the Greek word, ἀµνησία, meaning “without memory,” but what are the implications of the use of this word, when the loss of memory is just one of a host of other impaired cognitive functions? 127 APPENDIX: THE WAR OF THE GHOSTS One night two young men from Egulac went down to the river to hunt seals, and while they were there it became foggy and calm. Then they heard war-cries, and they thought: "Maybe this is a war party". They escaped to the shore, and hid behind a log. Now canoes came up, and they heard the noise of paddles, and saw one canoe coming up to them. There were five men in the canoe, and they said: "What do you think? We wish to take you along. We are going up the river to make war on the people". One of the young men said: "I have no arrows". "Arrows are in the canoe", they said. "I will not go along. I might be killed. My relatives do not know where I have gone. But you", he said, turning to the other, "may go with them." So one of the young men went, but the other returned home. And the warriors went on up the river to a town on the other side of Kalama. The people came down to the water, and they began to fight, and many were killed. But presently the young man heard one of the warriors say: "Quick, let us go home: that Indian has been hit". Now he thought: "Oh, they are ghosts". He did not feel sick, but they said he had been shot. So the canoes went back to Egulac, and the young man went ashore to his house, and made a fire. And he told everybody and said: " Behold I accompanied the ghosts, and we went to fight. Many of our fellows were killed, and many of those who attacked us were killed. They said I was hit, and I did not feel sick". He told it all, and then he became quiet. When the sun rose he fell down. Something black came out of his mouth. His face became contorted. The people jumped up and cried. He was dead. 128 REFERENCES Abrahams, S., Pickering, A., Polkey, C.E., & Morris, R.G. (1997). Spatial memory deficits in patients with unilateral damage to right hippocampal formation. Neuropsychologia, 35, 11-24. Ahlberg, S.W. & Sharps, M.J. (2010). Bartlett revisited: Reconfiguration of long-term memory in young and older adults. The Journal of Genetic Psychology: Research and Theory on Human Development. Allen, J. S., Tranel, D., Bruss, J., & Damasio, H. (2006). Correlations between regional brain volumes and memory performance in anoxia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28(4), 457-476. Allopenna, P. D., Magnuson, J. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1998). Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition: evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 419–439. Altmann, G. T. M., and Kamide, Y. (2007). The real-time mediation of visual attention by language and world knowledge: linking anticipatory (and other) eye movements to linguistic processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 502– 518. Altmann, G. T. M., and Kamide, Y. (2009). Discourse-mediation of the mapping between language and the visual world: eye movements and mental representation. Cognition, 111, 55–71. Arnold, J., Eisenband, J., Brown-Schmidt, S., & Trueswell, J. (2000). The rapid use of gender information: Evidence of the time course of pronoun resolution from eyetracking. Cognition, 76, 813-826. Arnott, R., Finger, S., Smith, C. (2003). Trepanation: History Discovery—Theory. Lisse, Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger Publishers. Baddeley, A.D. (1986). Working Memory. New York: Oxford University Press. Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G.H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). New York: Academic Press. Baddeley, A.D., Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1988). When long-term learning depends on short-term storage. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 586-595. 129 Baddeley, A. D., & Warrington, E. K. (1970). Amnesia and the distinction between longand short- term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, 176–189. Bakhtin, M. (1986). Speech Genres and Other Late Essays. Austin: University of Texas Press. Barr, D. J. (2008). Analyzing ‘visual world’ eyetracking data using multilevel logistic regression. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 457–474. Barr, D., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., J. & Tily, H. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255-278. Barr, W.B., Chelune, G.J., Hermann, B.P., Loring, D.W., Perrine, K., Strauss, E., Trenerry, M.R., et al. (1997). The use of figural reproduction tests as measures of nonverbal memory in epilepsy surgery candidates. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 435-443. Barr, W.B., Goldberg, E., Wasserstein, J., & Novelly, R.A. (1990). Retrograde amnesia following unilateral temporal lobectomy. Neuropsychologia, 28, 243–55. Bartlett, F.C. (1932). Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press. Bayles, K.A. (2003). Effects of working memory deficits on the communication functioning of Alzheimer’s dementia patients. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36, 209-219. Bayley, P.J., Frascino, J.C., Squire, L.R. (2005) Robust habit learning in the absence of awareness and independent of the medial temporal lobe. Nature, 436, 550–553. Bayley, P.J., Gold, J.J., Hopkins, R.O., & Squire, L.R. (2005). The neuroanatomy of remote memory. Neuron, 46, 799-810. Bayley, P.J., Hopkins, R.O., & Squire, L.R. (2006). The fate of old memories after medial temporal lobe damage. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(51), 13311-13317. Bekhterev, V.M. (1899). Demonstration Eines Gehims mit zerstorung der vorderen und innerin theile der himrinde beider schlafenlappen. Neuro Zentbl, 19, 990-991. Bellgowan, P.S., Saad, Z.S., & Bandettini, P.A. (2003). Understanding neural system dynamics through task modulation and measurement of functional MRI amplitude, latency, and width. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100, 1415–1419. 130 Benson, D. (1988). Classical syndromes of aphasia. In F. Boller, & J. Grafman (Eds.). Handbook of Neuropsychology, Vol 1 (pp. 267-306). New York: Elsevier. Bergman, E.T., & Roediger, H.L. (1999). Can Bartlett’s repeated reproduction experiments be replicated? Memory & Cognition, 27(6), 937-947. Berryhill, M.E., Phuong, L., Picasso, L., Cabeza, R. & Olson, I. (2008). Parietal lobe and episodic memory: Bilateral damage causes impaired free recall of autobiographical memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 14415-14423. Binder, J.R., Frost, J.A., Hammeke, T.A., Cox, R.W., Rao, S., & Prieto, T. (1997). Human brain language areas identified by functional magnetic resonance imaging. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17(1), 353-362. Binder, J.R., Rao, S.M., Hammeke, T.A., Frost, J.A., Bandettini, P.A., Jesmanowicz, A., Hyde, J.S. (1995). Lateralized human brain language systems demonstrated by task subtraction functional magnetic resonance imaging. Archives of Neurology, 52, 593-601. Blake, M. (2007). Perspectives on treatment for communication deficits associated with right hemisphere brain damage. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 16, 331-342. Bluck, S., Alea, N., & Habermas, T. (2005). A tale of three functions: The self-reported uses of autobiographical memory. Social Cognition, 23, 91-117. Blumenfeld, R.S., & Ranganath, C. (2007) Prefrontal cortex and long-term memory encoding: An integrative review of findings from neuropsychology and neuroimaging. Neuroscientist, 13, 280–291. Blumenfeld, R.S., Parks, C.M., Yonelinas, A.P., & Ranganath, C. (2011). Putting the pieces together: The role od dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in relational memory binding. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(1), 257-265. Bradley, M.M. & Lang, P.J. (1998). International affective digitized sounds (IADS): Stimuli, instruction manual and affective ratings. Gainsville, FL: Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida. Brazdil, M., Chlebus, P., Mikl, M., Pazourkova, M., Krupa, P., & Rektor, I. (2005). Reorganization of language-related neuronal networks in patients with left temporal lobe epilepsy – An fMRI study. European Journal of Neurology, 12, 268-275. Brazdil, M., Zakopcan, J., Kuba, R., Fanfrdlova, Z, & Rektor, I. (2003). Atypical hemispheric language dominance in left temporal lobe epilepsy as a result of the reorganization of language functions. Epilepsy Behavior, 4, 414-419. 131 Broca, P. (1861a) Nouvelle observation d’aphémie produite par une lésion de la troisième circonvolution frontale. Bulletins de la Société d’anatomie (Paris), 2e serie, 6, 398-407. Broca P. (1861b) Perte de la parole: ramollissement chronique et destruction partielle du lobe anterieur gauche du cerveau. Bulletins de la Societe d’anthropologie, 1re serie, 2, 235-8. Broca, P. (1865). Sur le siege de la faculte du langage articule. Bulletin de la Societe d’anthropologie, 6, 337-93. Brown, M.W., & Aggleton, J.P. (2001). Recognition memory: What are the roles of the perirhinal cortex and hippocampus? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 51-61. Buchanan, T. W., Tranel, D., & Adolphs, R. (2005). Emotional Autobiographical Memories in Amnesic Patients with Medial Temporal Lobe Damage. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(12), 3151–3160. Buckner, R.L. (1996). Beyond HERA: contributions of specific prefrontal brain areas to long-term memory retrieval. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 3, 149–158. Buckner, R.L. & Koustaal, W. (1998). Functional neuroimaging studies of encoding, priming, and explicit memory retrieval. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 95, 891–898. Burianova, H., & Grady, C. L. (2007). Common and unique neural activations in autobiographical, episodic, and semantic retrieval. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(9), 1520-1534. Cabeza, R., Dolcos, F., Graham, R., & Nyberg, L. (2002). Similarities and Differences in the Neural Correlates of Episodic Memory Retrieval and Working Memory. Neuroimage , 16, 317-330. Cabeza, R., & Nyberg, L. (2000). Imaging cognition II: An empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(1), 1-47. Cameron, K.A., Yashar, S., Wilson, C.L., & Fried, I. (2001). Human hippocampal neurons predict how well word pairs will be remembered. Neuron, 30, 289-298. Campbell, J., Nadel, L., Duke, D., & Ryan, L. (2011). Remembering all that and then some: Recollection of autobiographical memories after a 1-year delay. Memory, 19(4), 406-415. Cansino, S., Maquet, P., Dolan, R.J., & Rugg, M.D. (2002). Brain activity underlying encoding and retrieval of source memory. Cerebral Cortex, 12, 1048-1056. 132 Caplan, D. & Waters, G. (1998). Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension. Behavior Brain Science, 22(1), 77-94. Caramazza, A., Miceli, G., Silveri, M.C., & Laudanna, A. (1985). Reading mechanisms and the organization of the lexicon: Evidence from acquired dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 2, 81-114. Carbon, C. & Albrecht, S. (2012). Bartlett’s schema theory: The unreplicated “portrait d’homme” series from 1932. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(11), 2258-2270. Catani, M., Jones, D.K., & Ffytche, D.H. (2005). Perisylvian language networks of the human brain. Annals of Neurology, 57(1), 8-16. Chatterjee A. (2004). The neuropsychology of visual artistic production. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1568-1583, 2004. Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic Structures. New York: Mouton de Gruyter. Clark, L.F. (1993). Stress and the cognitive-conversational benefits of social interaction. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 12(1), 25-55. Coelho, C.A. (2002). Story narratives of adults with closed head injury and non-brain injured adults: Influence of socioeconomic status, elicitation task, and executive functioning. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45(6), 12321248. Cohen, N.J. (1984). Preserved learning capacity in amnesia: Evidence for multiple memory systems. In L.R. Squire, & N. Butters (Eds.), Neuropsychology of memory (pp. 83-103). New York: Guilford Press. Cohen, N.J., & Banich, M.T. (2003). Memory. In: Banich, MT., editor. Neuropsychology: The neural bases of mental function (Vol. 2) p. 322-364. Boston, MA: HoughtonMifflin. Cohen, N.J., Poldrack, R.,& Eichenbaum, H. (1997). Memory for items and memory for relations in the procedural/declarative framework. Memory, 5(1), 131-178. Cohen, N. J., & Squire, L. R. (1980). Preserved learning and retention of patternanalyzing skill in amnesia – dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. Science, 210(4466), 207–210. Copland, D. A. (2003). The basal ganglia and semantic engagement: Potential insights from semantic priming in individuals with subcortical vascular lesions, 133 parkinson's disease, and cortical lesions. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 9(7), 1041-1052. Copland, D. A., Chenery, H. J., & Murdoch, B. E. (2001). Discourse priming of homophones in individuals with dominant nonthalamic subcortical lesions, cortical lesions and parkinson's disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23(4), 538-556. Corkin, S. (1984) Lasting consequences of bilateral medial temporal lobectomy: Clinical course and experimental findings in H.M. Seminars in Neurology, 4, 249-259. Courtney, S.M., Ungerleider, L.G., Keil, K., & Haxby, J.V. (1997). Transient and Sustained Activity in a Distributed Neural System for Human Working Memory. Nature, 386, 608-611. Cousin, E., Baciu, M., Pichat, C., Kahane, P., & Le Bas, J. (2008). Functional MRI evidence for language plasticity in adult epileptic patients: Preliminary results. Neuropsychiatric Disease Treatment, 4, 235-246. Crosson, B. (1992). Subcortical functions in language and memory. New York: The Guilford Press. Damasio, H. (1995). Human brain anatomy in computerized images. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press. Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. (1989). Lesion analysis in neuropsychology. New York: Oxford University Press. Damasio, A.R., Graff-Radford, N.R., Eslinger, P.J., Damasio, H. & Kassell, N. (1985). Amnesia following basal forebrain lesions. Archives of Neurology, 42, 263-271. Damasio, H., Grabowski, T.J., Tranel, D., Hichwa, R.D., & Damasio, A.R. (1996). A neural basis for lexical retrieval. Nature, 380(6574), 499-505. Davachi, L. (2006). Items, context, and relational episodic encoding in humans. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16, 693-700. Davachi, L., Mitchell, J.P., & Wagner, A.D. (2003). Multiple routes to memory: Distinct medial temporal lobe processes build item and source memories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100, 2157-2162. Davidson, P.S.R., Anaki, D., Ciaramelli, E., Cohn, M., Kim, A.S.N., Murphy, K., Troyer, A.K., et al., (2008). Does lateral parietal cortex support episodic memory? Evidence from focal lesion patients. Neuropsychologia, 46, 1743-1735. 134 Davidson, P.S., Drouin, H., Kwan, D., Moscovitch, M., & Rosenbaum, R.S. (2012). Memory as social glue: Close interpersonal relationships in amnesic patients. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 531. Dax, M. (1865). Lésions de la moitié gauche de l'encéphale coïncident avec l'oubli des signes de la pensée (lu à Montpellier en 1836). Bulletin hebdomadaire de médecine et de chirurgie, 2me série, 2, 259-62. Delis, D.C., Wapner, W., Gardner, h., & Moses, J.A. (1983). The contribution of the right hemisphere to the organization of paragraphs. Cortex, 19, 43-50. Demb, J.B., Desmond, J.E., Wagner, A.D., Vaidya, C.J., Glover, G.H. Gabrieli, J.D.E. (1995). Semantic encoding and retrieval in the left inferior prefrontal cortex: A functional MRI study of task difficulty and process specificity, Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 5870-5878. Demonet, J.F., Chollet, F., Ramsay, S., Cardebat, D., Nespouslous, J.L., Wise, R., Rascol, A., & Frackowiak, R. (1992). The anatomy of phonological and semantic processing in normal subjects, Brain, 115, 1753-1768. Dijkstra, K., Bourgeois, M. S., Allen, R. S., & Burgio, L. D. (2004). Conversational coherence: Discourse analysis of older adults with and without dementia. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 17, 263–283. Dobbins, I. G., Rice, H. J., Wagner, A. D., & Schacter, D. L. (2003). Memory orientation and success: Separable neurocognitive components underlying episodic recognition. Neuropsychologia, 41, 318-333. Duff, M.C., & Brown-Schmidt, S. (2012). The hippocampus and the flexible use and processing of language. Frontiers of Human Neuroscience, 6, 69. Duff, M.C., Gupta, R., Hengst, J., Tranel, D., & Cohen, N. J. (2011). The use of definite references signals declarative memory: Evidence from patients with hippocampal amnesia. Psychological Science, 22 (5), 666-673. Duff, M.C., Hengst, J., Gupta, R., Tranel, D. & Cohen, N.J. (2011). Distributed impact of cognitive-communication impairment: Disruptions in the use of definite references when speaking to individuals with amnesia. Aphasiology, 25(6-7), 675687. Duff, M.C., Hengst, J., Tranel, D. & Cohen, N.J. (2006). Development of shared information in communication despite hippocampal amnesia. Nature Neuroscience, 9(1), 140-146. 135 Duff, M.C., Hengst, J., Tranel, D., & Cohen, N.J. (2007). Talking across time: Using reported speech as a communicative resource in amnesia. Aphasiology, 21 (6, 7, 8), 702-716 Duff, M.C., Hengst, J., Tranel, D., & Cohen, N. J. (2008). Collaborative discourse facilitates efficient communication and new learning in amnesia. Brain and Language, 106, 41-54. Duff, M.C., Hengst, J., Tranel, D., & Cohen, N.J. (2009). Hippocampal amnesia disrupts verbal play and the creative use of language in social interaction. Aphasiology, 23(7), 926-939. Duff, M.C., Hengst, J., Tengshe, C., Krema, A. Tranel, D., & Cohen, N.J. (2008). Hippocampal amnesia disrupts the flexible use of procedural discourse in social interaction. Aphasiology, 22 (7, 8), 866-880. Duff, M.C., & Kurczek, J. (2013). Hippocampal amnesia disrupts episodic memory and narrative construction. Poster presentation at the Cognitive Neuroscience Society Conference, San Francisco, CA. Dusek, J.A., & Eichenbaum, H. (1997). The hippocampus and memory for orderly stimulus relations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 94(13), 7109-7114. Eichenbaum, H., & Cohen, N. J. (2001). From conditioning to conscious recollection: Memory systems of the brain. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Eldridge, L. L., Knowlton, B. J., Furmanski, C. S., Bookheimer, S. Y. & Engle, S. A. (2000) Remembering episodes: A selective role for the hippocampus during retrieval. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 1149–1152. Federmeier, K. D. (2007). Thinking ahead: the role and roots of prediction in language comprehension. Psychophysiology, 44, 491–505. Federmeier, K. D., & Kutas, M. (1999). A rose by any other name: long-term memory structure and sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 469– 495. Feigenbaum, J.D., Polkey, C.E., & Morris, R.G. (1996). Deficits in spatial working memory after unilateral temporal lobectomy in man. Neuropsychologia, 34, 163176. Feinstein, J.S., Duff, M.C., & Tranel, D. (2010). Sustained experience of emotion after loss of memory in patients with amnesia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(17), 7674-7679. 136 Fellows, L.K., Herberlein, A.S., Morales, D.A., Shivde, G., Waller, S., & Wu, D. (2005). Method matters: An empirical study of impact in cognitive neuroscience. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(6), 850-858. Fellows, L.K., Stark, M., Berg, A. & Chatterjee, A. (2008). Patient registries in cognitive neuroscience research: advantages, challenges, and practical advice. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(6): 1107-1103. Ferraro, F.R., Balota, D.A., & Conor, L.T. (1993). Implicit memory and the formation of new associations in nondemented Parkinson’s disease individuals and individuals with senile dementia of the Alzheimer’s type: A serial reaction time (SRT) investigation. Brain and Cognition, 21(2), 163-180. Ferstle, E., & von Cramon, D.Y. (2001). The role of coherence and cohesion in text comprehension: An event-related fMRI study. Cognitive Brain Research, 11, 325340. Fine, A.B. & Jaeger, T.F. (2013). Evidence for implicit learning in adult language processing. Cognitive Science, 37, 578-591. Fisher, R.S., Vickery, B.G., Gibson, P., Hermann, B.P., Penovich, P., Scherer, A., et al., (2000). The impact of epilepsy from the patients perspective. Descriptions and subjective perceptions. Epilepsy Research, 41, 39-51. Flanagan Jr., O. (1993). Situations and Dispositions. In A.I. Goldman (Ed.). Readings in Philosophy and Cognitive Sciences (pp. 681-695). Cambridge: MIT Press. Foerde, K., Race, E., Verfaellie, M., & Shohamy, D. (2013). A role for the medial temporal lobe in feedback-driven learning: Evidence from amnesia. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(13), 5698-5704. Fordor, J. The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Frank, R. J., Damasio, H., & Grabowski, T. J. (1997). Brainvox: an interactive, multimodal, visualization and analysis system for neuroanatomical imaging. NeuroImage, 3, 13–30. Freud, S. (1891). Zur auffassung der aphasien. Eine kritishe studie. Leipzig/Vienna: Deuticke. Friederici, A. D. (2006). The neural basis of language development and its impairment. Neuron, 52(6), 941-952. Friederici, A. D., Rüschemeyer, S.-A., Hahne, A., & Fiebach, C. J. (2003). The role of left inferior frontal and superior temporal cortex in sentence comprehension: Localizing syntactic and semantic processes. Cerebral Cortex, 13(2), 170-177. 137 Gabrieli J. (1995). Contribution of the basal ganglia to skill learning and working memory in humans. In J.C., Houk, J.L. Davis, & D.G. Beiser (Eds), Models of Information Processing in the Basal Ganglia (pp. 277-294). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Gabrieli, J. D. (1998). Cognitive neuroscience of human memory. Annual Review of Psychology. 49, 87-115. Gabrieli, J.D., Cohen, N.J., & Corkin, S. (1988). The impaired learning of semantic knowledge following bilateral medial temporal-lobe resection. Brain and Cognition, 7(2), 157-77. Gaesser, B., Sacchetti, D., Addis, D., & Schacter, D. (2011). Characterizing age-related changes in remembering the past and imagining the future. Psychology and Aging, 26(1), 80-84. Gall, F. J. (1825). Sur les fonctions du cerveau et sur celles de chacune de ses parties. avec des observations sur la possibilité de reconnaitre les instincts, les penchans, les talens, ou les dispositions morales et intellectuelles des hommes et des animaux, par la configuration de leur cerveau et de leur tête (6 vols.). Paris: J. B. Baillière. Gasser, M. (2004). The origins of the arbitrariness in language. Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 26. Gathercole, S. E. & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). The role of phonological memory in vocabulary acquisition: A study of young children learning new names. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 439-454. Gathercole, S.E., & Baddeley, A.D. (1993). Working memory and language. Hove, U.K.: Erlbaum. Giovagnoli, A.R., Casazza, M. & Avanzini, G. (1995). Visual learning on a selective reminding procedure and delayed recall in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia, 36, 704-711. Giovanello, K.S., Schnyer, D.M., & Verfaellie, M. (2004). A critical role for the anterior hippocampus in relational memory: Evidence from an fMRI study comparing associative and item recognition. Hippocampus, 14(1), 5-8. Giovanello, K.S., Verfaellie, M., & Keane, M.M. (2003). Disproportionate deficit in associative recognition relative to item recognition in global amnesia. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 186-194. 138 Gleibner, U. Helmstaedter, C., & Elger, C.E. (1998). Right hippocampal contribution to visual memory: A presurgical and postsurgical study in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, & Psychiatry, 65, 665-669. Glosser, G., & Deser, T. (1991). Patterns of discourse production among neurological patients with fluent language disorders. Brain and Language, 40, 67–88. Golby, A.J., Poldrack, R.A., Brewer, J.B., Spencer, D., Desmond, J.E., Aron, A.P., & Gabrieli, J.D.E. (2001). Material-specific lateralization in the medial temporal lobe and prefrontal cortex during memory encoding. Brain, 124, 1841-1854. Goldman-Rakic, P. (1988). Topography of cognition: Parallel distributed networks in primate association cortex. Annual Reviews of Neuroscience, 11, 137-156. Goldman-Rakic, P. (1995). Regional and cellular fractionation of working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 93(24), 13473-13480. Grafton, S., Hazeltine, E., & Ivry, R. (1995). Functional mapping of sequence learning in normal humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 497-510. Grafton, S.T., Mazziotta, J.C., Presty, S., Friston, K.J., Frackowiak, R.S.J. & Phelps, M.E. (1992). Functional anatomy of human procedural learning determined with regional cerebral blood flow and PET. Journal of Neuroscience, 12(7), 26422648. Graham, K. S., & Hodges, J. R. (1997). Differentiating the roles of the hippocampal system and the neocortex in long-term memory storage. Neuropsychology, 11, 77–89. Graham, K.S., Lee, A.C.H., Brett, M., Patterson, K., (2003). The neural basis of autobiographical and semantic memory: new evidence from three PET studies. Cognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 234–254. Grossman, M., Lee, C., Morris, J., Stern, M.B. & Hurtig, H.I. (2002). Assessing resource demands during sentence processing in Parkinson’s disease. Brain and Language, 80, 603-616. Gupta, P., & MacWhinney, B. (1997). Vocabulary acquisition and verbal short-term memory: Computational and neural bases. Brain and Language, 59(2), 267-333. Gupta, R., Duff, M.C., Denburg, N.L., Cohen, N.J., Bechara, A., Tranel, D. (2009). Declarative memory is critical for advantageous decision making. Neuropsychologia, 47(7), 1686-1693. 139 Hallett, P. E. (1986). Eye movements. In K. R. Boff, L. Kaufman, & J. P.Thomas (Eds.), Handbook of perception and human performance (pp.10.1.112B13–10). New York, NY: Wiley. Halliday, M., & Hasan, R. (1976). Cohesion in English. London, UK: Longman Group. Hammers, A., Koepp, M., Labbe, C., Brooks, D.J., Thom, M., Cunningham, V.J., et al. (2001). Neocortical abnormalities of [C-11]-flumazenil PET in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurology, 56, 1261-1268. Hanna, J. E., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Trueswell, J. C. (2003). The effects of common ground and perspective on domains of referential interpretation. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 43–61. Hannula, D.E., Bayum, C.L., Warren, D.E., & Cohen, N.J. (2012). The eyes know: Eye movements as a veridical index of memory. Psychological Science, 23(3), 278287. Hannula, D.E., Tranel, D. & Cohen, N.J. (2006). The long and the short of it: Relational memory impairments in amnesia, even at short lags. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(32), 8352-8359. Harlow, J.M. (1868). Passage of an iron rod through the head. Publications of the Massachusetts Medical Society, 2, 327-347. Harrington, A. (1987). Medicine, Mind, and the Double Brain: A Study in NineteenthCentury Thought. Princeton, NJ, Princeton University Press. Hassabis, D., Kumaran, D., Vann, S. & Maguire, E.A. (2007). Patients with hippocampal amnesia cannot imagine new experiences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(5), 1726-1731. Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science’s Compass Review, 298, 1569-1579. Helmstaedter, C., Kurthen, M., Linke, D.B., & Elger, C.E. (1997). Patterns of language dominance in focal left and right hemisphere epilepsies: Relation to MRI findings, EEG, sex, and age at onset of epilepsy. Brain and Cognition, 33, 135-150. Heller, D., Grodner, D., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2008). The role of perspective in identifying domains of reference. Cognition, 108, 831–836. Hengst, J. & Duff, M.C. (2007). Clinicians as communication partners: Developing a mediated discourse elicitation protocol. Topics in Language Disorders, 27, 36-47. 140 Hengst, J., Duff, M.C., & Dettmer, A. (2010). Rethinking repetition in therapy: Repeated engagement as the social ground of learning. Aphasiology, 24(6-8), 887-901. Hengst, J., Frame, S., Neuman-Stritzl, T., & Gannaway, R. (2005). Using others’ words: Conversational use of reported speech by individuals with aphasia and their communication partners. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 48, 137–156. Hermann, K., Turner, J.W., Gillingham, F.H., & Gaze, R.M. (1966). The effects of destructive lesions and stimulation of the basal ganglia on speech mechanisms. Confinia Neurologica, 27, 197-207. Hinke, R.M., Hu, X., Stillman, A.E., Kim, S.G., Merkle, H., Salmi, R., Ugurbil, K. (1993) Functional magnetic resonance imaging of Broca’s area during internal speech. NeuroReport, 4, 675– 678. Hippocrates. (1928). On wounds to the head. (E.T. Withington). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Original work published 460 BCE). Hockett, C.F. (1958). A course in modern linguistics. New York: The Macmillan Company. Hough, M. (1990). Narrative comprehension in adults with right and left hemisphere brain damage: Theme organization. Brain and Language, 38, 253-277. Jackson, J.H. (1868). Notes on the physiology and pathology of the nervous system. Medical Times Gazette. Jaeger, T.F., (2008). Categorical Data Analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards Logit Mixed Models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 434-446. Janszky, J., Jokeit, H., Heinemann, D., Schulz, R., Woermann, F.G., & Ebner, A. (2003). Epileptic activity influences the speech organization in medial temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain, 126, 2043-2051. Janszky, J., Mertens, M., Janszky, I., Ebner, A., & Woermann, F. (2006). Left-sided interictal epileptic activity induces shift of language lateralization in temporal lobe epilepsy: An fMRI study. Epilepsia, 47, 921-927. Jensen, U., Duff, M.C., Adolphs, R., & Tranel, D. (2007, November). Patients with hippocampal amnesia are unable to construct future episodic events. Poster presentation at the Society for Neuroscience Conference, San Diego, CA. 141 Joanette, Y., Goulet, P., Ska, B. & Nespoulous, J. (1986). Informative content of narrative discourse in right-brain-damaged right-handers. Brain and Language, 29, 81-105. Jones-Gotman, M. (1986). Right hippocampal excision impairs learning and recall of a list of abstract designs. Neuropsychologia, 24, 659-670. Jung-Beeman, M. (2005). Bilateral brain processes for comprehending natural language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 512-518. Just, M. & Carpenter, P. (1992). A Capacity Theory of Comprehension: Individual Differences in Working Memory. Psychology Review, 99(1), 122-149. Just, M.A., Cherkassky, V.L. Aryal, S., & Mitchell, T.M. (2010). A neurosemantic theory of concrete noun representation based on the underlying brain codes. PLoS One, 5(1), e8622. Kamide, Y., Altmann, G. T. M., & Haywood, S. L. (2003). The time- course of prediction in incremental sentence processing: evidence from anticipatory eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 133–159. Kaczmarek, B. (1984). Neurolinguistic analysis of verbal utterances in patients with focal lesions of frontal lobes. Brain and Language, 21(1), 52-58. Kapur, N. (1999). Syndromes of retrograde amnesia: A conceptual and empirical synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 800-825. Kapur, N., Millar, J., Colbourn, C., Abbott, P., Kennedy, P., & Docherty, T. (1997) Very long-term amnesia in association with temporal lobe epilepsy: Evidence for multiple-stage consolidation processes. Brain and Cognition, 35, 58–70. Kelley, W., Miezin, F., McDermott, K., Buckner, R.L., Raichel, M.E., Cohen, N.J., Ollinger, J.M., et al. (1998). Hemispheric specialization in human dorsal frontal cortex and medial temporal lobe for verbal and nonverbal memory encoding. Neuron, 20, 927–936. Kensinger, E.A., Ullman, M.T., & Corkin, S. (2001) Bilateral medial temporal lobe damage does not affect lexical or grammatical processing: Evidence from amnesic patient H.M. Hippocampus, 11, 337-346. Kim, H., Daselaar, S., & Cabeza, R., (2010). Overlapping brain activity between episodic memory encoding and retrieval: roles of the task-positive and task-negative networks. NeuroImage, 49, 1045–1054. King, M.J., Macdougall, A.G., Ferris, S.M., Levine, B., Macqueen, G.M., & McKinnon, M.C., (2010). A review of factors that moderate autobiographical memory 142 performance in patients with major depressive disorder. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 32, 1122–1144. Kirwan, C.B., & Stark, C.E.L. (2004). Medial temporal lobe activation during encoding and retrieval of novel face-name pairs. Hippocampus, 14, 919-930. Kirwan, C.B., Wixted, J.T., & Squire, L.R. (2008) Activity in the medial temporal lobe predicts memory strength, whereas activity in the prefrontal cortex predicts recollection. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 10541–10548. Knowlton, B.J., Ramus, S. & Squire, L.R. (1992) Intact artificial grammar learning in amnesia: Dissociation of classification learning and explicit memory for specific instances. Psychological Science, 3, 172-179. Knowlton, B., Squire, L. R., & Gluck, M. A. (1994). Probabilistic classification in amnesia. Learning and Memory, 1, 106-120. Konkel, A., Warren, D., Duff, M.C., Tranel, D. & Cohen, N.J., (2008). Hippocampal damage impairs all manner of relational memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2:15, doi:10.3389/neuro.09.015.2008. Kraut, M. A., Kremen, S., Moo, L. R., Segal, J. B., Calhoun, V., & Hart J., Jr. (2002). Object activation in semantic memory from visual multimodal feature input. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 37–47. Kumari, V. Gray, J., Honey, G., Soni, W., Bullmore, E., Williams, S., Ng, V. et al. (2002). Procedural learning in schizophrenia: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Schizophrenia Research, 57, 97-107. Kurczek, J., Brown Schmidt, S.,& Duff, M.C. (2013). Hippocampal contributions to language: Evidence of referential processing deficits in amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(4), 1346-1354. Kurczek, J. & Duff, M.C. (2011). Cohesion, coherence, and declarative memory: Discourse patterns in individuals with hippocampal amnesia. Aphasiology, 25(67), 700-712. Kurczek, J. & Duff, M.C. (2012). Intact discourse cohesion and coherence following bilateral ventromedial prefrontal cortex damage. Brain and Language, 123(3), 222-227. Kwan, D., Carson, N., Addis, D.R., & Rosenbaum, R.S. (2010). Deficits in remembering extend to future imagining in a case of developmental amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 48, 3179-3186. 143 Lackner, J. R. (1974). Observations on the speech processing capabilities of an amnesic patient: Several aspects of H. M.’s language function. Neuropsychologia, 12, 199–207. Lashley, K. (1929). Brain mechanisms and intelligence. Chicago: Chicago University Press. Lashley, K. (1950). In search of the engram. Society of Experimental Biology Symposium: Psychological Mechanisms in Animal Behavior, 4, 453-482. Legg, C., Penn, C., Temlett, J., & Sonnenberg, B. (2005). Language skills of adolescents with Tourette Syndrome. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 19(1), 15-33. Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From Intention to Articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Levine, B., Svoboda, E., Hay, J., Winocur, G., & Moscovitch, M. (2002). Aging and autobiographical memory: Dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval. Psychology and Aging, 17(4), 677-689. Levine, B., Turner, G. R., Tisserand, D., Hevenor, S. J., Graham, S. J., & McIntosh, A. R. (2004). The functional neuroanatomy of episodic and semantic autobiographical remembering: a prospective functional MRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(9), 1633-1646. Liles, B. Z. (1985). Narrative ability in normal and language-disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 23, 123–133. Loring, D.W., Meador, K.J., Lee, G.P., Murro, A.M., Smith, J.R., Flanigin, H.F. et al. (1990). Cerebral language lateralization: Evidence from intracarotid amobarbital testing. Neuopsychologia, 28, 831-838. Lucchelli, F., & Spinnler, H. (1998). Ephemeral new traces and evaporated remote engrams: A form of neocortical temporal lobe amnesia? A preliminary case report. Neurocase, 4, 447-459. Luria, A.R. (1964). Neuropsychology in the local diagnosis of brain damage. Cortex, 1, 3-18. Luria, A.R. (1966). Higher Cortical functions in man. New York: Basic Books. Maguire, E.A., Vargha-Khadem, F., & Hassabis, D. (2010). Imagining fictious and future experiences: Evidence from developmental amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 48, 31873192. 144 MacKay, D.G., Burke, D.M., & Stewart, R. (1998). H.M.’s language production deficits: Implications for relations between memory, semantic binding and the hippocampal system. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 28-69. MacKay, D.G. & James, L.E. (2001). The binding problem for syntax, semantics, and prosody: H.M.’s selective sentence-reading deficits under the theoreticalsyndrome approach. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 419-460. MacKay, D.G., Stewart, R., & Burke, D.M. (1998). H.M. revisited: Relations between language, comprehension, memory and the hippocampal system. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(3), 377-394. Mandler, J.M., & Johnson, N.S. (1977). Remembrance of things parsed: Story structure and recall. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 111-151. Marie, P. (1906). Revision de la question de l’aphasie: la troisième circonvolution frontale gauche ne joue aucun rôle spécial dans la fonction du langage. La Semaine Medicale (Paris), 26, 241-247. Martis, B., Wright, C. I., McMullin, K. G., Shin, L. M., & Rauch, S. L. (2004). Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for a lack of striatal dysfunction during implicit sequence learning in individuals with animal phobia. The American journal of psychiatry, 161(1), 67-71. McCarthy, G., Blamire, A.M., Rothman, D.L., Gruetter, R., & Shulman, R.G. (1993). Echo-planar magnetic resonance imaging studies of frontal cortex activation during word generation in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 90, 4952-4956. McDermott, K.B., Buckner, R.L., Petersen, S.E., Kelley, W.M., & Sanders, A.L. (1999). Set- and code-specific activation in the frontal cortex: An fMRI study of encoding and retrieval of faces and words. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11, 631-640. McMurry, B., Samelson, V.M., Lee, S.H., & Tomblin, J.B. (2010). Individual differences in online spoken word recognition: Implications for SLI. Cognitive Psychology, 60(1), 1-39. McNeil, M. R., Odell, K., & Tseng, C. (1991). Toward the integration of resource allocation into a general theory of aphasia. In T. Prescott (Ed.). Clinical Aphasiology. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Meyer, A. S., Sleiderink, A., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1998). Viewing and naming objects: eye movements during noun phrase production. Cognition 66, B25–B33. Milner, B. (1968). Visual recognition and recall after right temporal-lobe excision in man. Neuropsychologia, 6, 191-209. 145 Milner, B. (1970). Memory and the medial temporal regions of the brain. In K.H. Pribram, & D.E. Broadbent (Eds.). Biology of Memory (pp. 29-50). New York: Academic Press. Milner, B. (1990). Hippocampal-neocortical interaction in human memory processes. In L.R. Squire, M. Mishkin, & A. Shimamura (Eds.), Learning and Memory, Discussions in Neuroscience (pp. 101-108). Geneva: Elsevier. Milner, B., Corkin, S., & Teuber, H-L. (1968) Further analysis of the hippocampal amnesic syndrome: 14-year follow-up study of H.M. Neuropsychologia, 6, 215234. Moro, A, Tettamanti, M., Perani, D., Donati, C., Cappa, S.F., & Fazio, F. (2001). Syntax and the brain: Disentangling grammar by selective anomalies. Neuroimage, 13, 110-118. Moscovitch, M., Rosenbaum, R.S., Gilboa, A., Addis, D.R., Westmacott, R., Grady, C., & McAndrews, M.P. et al. (2005). Functional neuroanatomy of remote episodic, semantic and spatial memory: A unified account based on multiple trace theory. Journal of Anatomy, 207(1), 35-66. Moscovitch, M. & Winocur, G. (1995). Frontal lobes, memory, and aging. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 769, 119-150. Mungas, D., Ehlers, C., Walton, N. & McCutchen, C.B. (1985). Verbal learning differences in epileptic patients with left and right temporal lobe foci. Epilepsia, 26, 340-345. Murray, L. L., Holland, A. L., & Beeson, P. M. (1998). Spoken language of individuals with mild fluent aphasia under focused and divided attention conditions. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 213–227. Myers, P.S. (1999). Right hemisphere disorder: Disorders of communication and cognition. San Diego: Singular. Myers, P.S. and Mackisack, E.L. (1990). Right Hemisphere Syndrome. In Lapointe, L.L. (Ed.), Aphasia and Related Neurogenic Language Disorders, New York: Thieme. Nadel, L. Cambell, J. & Ryan, L. (2007). Autobiographical memory retrieval and hippocampal activation as a function of repetition and passage of time. Neural Plasticity, 1-14. Nadel, L. & Moscovitch, M. (1997). Memory consolidation, retrograde amnesia and the hippocampal complex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 7(2), 217-227. 146 Nadeau, S.E. & Crosson, B. (1997). Subcortical aphasia. Brain and Language, 58(3), 418-423. Naugle, R.I. (1992). Neuropsychological effects of surgery of epilepsy. In H.O. Luders (Ed.). Epilepsy surgery (pp. 637-645). Philadelphia: Lippincott, Williams & Wilkens. Neisser, U. (1981). John Dean’s memory: A case study. Cognition, 9, 1-22. Noulhiane, M., Piolino, P., Hasboun, D., Clemenceau, S., Baulac, M., & Samson, S. (2007). Autobiographical memory after temporal lobe resection: Neuropsychological and MRI volumetric findings. Brain, 130(12), 3184-3199. Novick, J. M., Trueswell, J. C., Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2005). Cognitive control and parsing: reexamining the role of Broca’s area in sentence comprehension. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 263–281. Nyberg, L., Cabeza, R., & Tulving, E. (1996). PET studies of encoding and retrieval: The HERA model. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 3, 135-148. Ochs, E. & Capps, L. (2001). Living narrative: Creating lives in everyday storytelling, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. O’Connor, M., Butters, N., Miliotis, P., Eslinger, P., & Cermak, L.S. (1992). The dissociation of anterograde and retrograde amnesia in a patient with herpes encephalitis. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 14, 159178. Ogden, J.A. and Corkin, S. (1991) Memories of H.M.. In M. Corballis, K.G. White, & W. Abraham (Eds). Memory Mechanisms: A Tribute to G.V. Goddard (pp. 195-215). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Ojeman, G. & Dodrill, C. (1985). Verbal memory deficit after left temporal lobectomy for epilepsy. Journal of Neurosurgery, 62, 101-107. O’Keefe, J. & Nadel, L. (1978). The hippocampus as a cognitive map. London: Oxford University Press. Olson, D. R. (1970). Language and thought: Aspects of a cognitive theory of semantics. Psychological Review, 77, 257–273. Ozetkin, I., McElree, B., Staresina, B., & Davachi, L. (2009). Working memory retrieval: Contributions of the left prefrontal cortex, the left posterior parietal cortex and the hippocampus. Journal of Cognitive Neurosciecne, 21(3), 518-593. 147 Park, L., St-Laurent, M., McAndrews, M. P., & Moscovitch, M. (2011). The immediacy of recollection: The use of the historical present in narratives of autobiographical episodes by patients with unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy. Neuropsychologia, 49(5), 1171–1176 Pascual-Leone, A., Grafman, J., Clark, K., Stewart, M., Massaquoi, S., Lou, J. S., et al. (1993). Procedural learning in Parkinson’s disease and cerebellar degeneration. Annals of Neurology, 34, 594–602. Patterson, K., Nestor, P.J., Rogers, T.T. (2007). Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(12), 976-987. Penfield, W., & Roberts, L. (1959). Speech and brain mechanisms. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Petersen, S.E., Fox, P.T., Posner, M.I., Mintun, M., Raichle, M.E. (1988). Positron emission tomographic studies of the cortical anatomy of single-word processing. Nature, 331, 585–589. Pigott, S. & Milner, B. (1993). Memory for different aspects of complex visual scenes after unilateral temporal- or frontal-lobe resection. Neuropsychologia, 31, 1-15. Pillon, B., Bazin, B., Dewer, B., Ehrle, N., Baulac, M., & Dubois, B. (1999). Specificity of memory deficits after right or left temporal lobectomy. Cortex, 35, 561-571. Pinker, S. & Jackendoff, R. (2005) What's special about the human language faculty? Cognition, 95(2), 201-236. Poldrack, R.A. (2006). Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 59-63. Poldrack, R. A., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (1998). Memory and the brain: What’s right and what’s left? Cell, 93, 1091-1093. Poreh, A., Winocur, G., Moscovitch, M., Backon, M., Goshen, E., Ram, Z, & Feldman, Z. (2006). Anterograde and Retrograde Amnesia in a person with bilateral fornix lesions following removal of a colloid cyst. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2241-2248. Prior, P. (2001). Voices in text, mind and society: Sociohistoric accounts of discourse acquisition and use. Journal of Second Language Writing, 10, 55–81. Race, E., Keane, M., & Verfaellie, M. (2011). Medial temporal lobe damage causes deficits in episodic memory and episodic future thinking not attributable to deficits in narrative construction. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(28), 10262-10269. 148 Race, E., Keane, M., & Verfaellie, M. (2013). Living in the moment: Patients with MTL amnesia can richly describe the present despite deficits in past and future thought. Cortex, 49(6), 1764-1766. Ranganath, C., Cohen, M., & Brozinsky, C. (2005). Working memory maintenance contributes to long-term memory formation: Neural and behavioral evidence. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(7), 994-1010. Ranganath, C. & D'Esposito, M. (2001). Medial temporal lobe activity associated with active maintenance of novel information. Neuron, 31, 865-873. Ranganath, C., & Paller, K.A. (1999). Frontal brain potentials during recognition are modulated by requirements to retrieve perceptual detail. Neuron, 22, 605-613. Ranganath, C., Yonelinas, A.P., Cohen, M.X., Dy, C.J., Tom, S.M., & D’Esposito, M. (2003). Dissociable correlates of recollection and familiarity within the medial temporal lobes. Neuropsychologia, 42(1), 2-13. Rasmussen, T., & Milner, B. (1977). The role of early left-brain injury in determining lateralization of cerebral speech functions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 299, 355-369. Rauch, S., Walen, P.J., Savage, C.R., Curran, T., Kendrick, A., Brown, H., Bush, G., et al. (1997). Striatal recruitment during an implicit sequence learning task as measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Human Brain Mapping, 5(2), 124-132. Ribot, T. (1881). Les maladies de la memoire [Diseases of memory]. Paris: Germer Bailliere. Richardson, M.P., Strange, B.A., Duncan, J.S., & Dolan, R.J. (2003). Preserved verbal memory function in left medial temporal lobe pathology involves reorganization of function to right medial temporal lobe. Neuroimage, 20, S112-S119. Roberts, W.A., & Feeney, M.C. (2009). The comparative study of mental time travel. Trends in Cognitive Science, 13(6), 271-277. Rohling, M.L., Faust, M.E., Beverly, B. & Demakis, G. (2009). Effectiveness of cognitive rehabilitation following acquired brain injury: A meta-analytic reexamination of Cicerone et al.’s (2000, 2005) systematic reviews. Neuropsychology, 23(1), 20-39. Rorden, C. & Karnath, H.O. (2004). Using human brain lesions to infer function: A relic from a past era in the fMRI age? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(10), 813-819. 149 Rosenbaum, R.S., McKinnon, M.C., Levine, B., & Moscovitch, M. (2004). Visual imagery deficits, impaired strategic retrieval, or memory loss: Disentangling the nature of an amnesic person’s autobiographical memory deficit. Neuropsychologia, 42(12), 1619-1635. Rosenbaum, R.S., Moscovitch, M., Foster, J.K., Schnyer, D.M., Gao, F., Kovacevic, N., Verfaellie, M. et al. (2008). Patterns of autobiographic memory loss in medialtemporal lobe amnesic patients. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 14901506. Rubin, R., Brown-Schmidt, S., Duff, M.C., Tranel, D., & Cohen, N.J. (2011). How do I remember that I know you know that I know? Psychological Science, 22(12), 1574-1582. Rueckert, L., Appollonio, I., Grafman, J., Jezzard, P., Johnson, R., Le Bihan, D., Turner, R. (1994). Magnetic resonance imaging functional activation of left frontal cortex during covert word production. Journal of Neuroimaging, 4, 67-70. Rugg, M.D., Yonelinas, A.P. (2003) Human recognition memory: a cognitive neuroscience perspective. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7, 313–319. Saling, M.M. (2009). Verbal memory in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy: Beyond material specificity. Brain, 132, 570-582. Sass, K.J., Sass, A., Westerveld, M., Lencz, T., Novelly, R.A., Kim, J.H., & Spencer, D.D. (1992). Specificity in the correlation of verbal memory and hippocampal neuron loss: Dissociation of memory, language, and verbal intellectual ability. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 14(5), 662-672. Sass, K.J., Spencer, D.D., Kim, J.H, Westerveld, M., Novelly, R.A., & Lencz, T. (1990). Verbal memory impairment correlates with hippocampal pyramidal cell density. Neurology, 50, 812-817. Schacter, D.L., Reiman, E., Uecker, A., Polster, M.R., Yun, L.S. & Cooper, L.A. (1995). Brain regions associated with retrieval of structurally coherent visual information. Nature, 376, 587-590. Schmolck, H., Kensinger, E.A., Corkin, S. & Squire, L.R. (2002). Semantic knowledge in patient H.M. and other patients with bilateral medial and lateral temporal lobe lesions. Hippocampus, 12(4), 520-533. Scoville, W. B. (1954). The limbic lobe in man. Journal of Neurosurgery, 11(1), 64–66. Scoville, W.B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 20, 11-21. 150 Semkovska, M., Noone, M., Carton, M., & McLoughlin, D.M. (2012). Measuring consistency of autobiographical memory recall in depression. Psychiatry Research, 197, 41-48. Shallice, T., & E. K. Warrington. (1970). Independent functioning of verbal memory stores: A neuropsychological study. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22, 261-273. Shaywitz, B.A., Pugh, K.R., Constable, T., Shaywitz, S.E., Bronen, R.A., Fulbright, R.K., Shankweiler, D.P., et al., (1995). Localization of semantic processing using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Human Brain Mapping, 2, 149-158. Shimamura, A. P. (1995). Memory and the prefrontal cortex. In J. Grafman, K. J. Holyoak, & F. Boller (Eds.) Structure and function of the human prefrontal cortex. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Vol 769, 151-159. Shohamy, D., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (in press). Mechanisms for widespread hippocampal involvement in cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. Seitz, R.J., & Roland, P.E. Learning of sequential finger movements in man: a combined kinematic and positron emission tomography (PET) study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 4, 154–65. Seitz, R.J., Roland, P.E., Bohm, C., Greitz, T., & Stone-Elander, S. (1990). Motor learning in man: a positron emission tomographic study. NeuroReport, 1, 17–20. Simons, J.S., Peers, P.V., Hwang, D.Y., Ally, B.A., Fletcher, P.C., & Budson, A.E. (2008). Is the parietal lobe necessary for recollection in humans? Neuropsychologia, 46(4), 1185-1191. Skinner, E. I., & Fernandes, M. A. (2007). Neural correlates of recollection and familiarity: A review of neuroimaging and patient data. Neuropsychologia, 45(10), 2163–2179. Skotko, B., Andrews, A., & Einstein, G. (2005). Language and the Medial Temporal Lobe: Evidence from H.M.’s Spontaneous Discourse. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 397-415. Skotko, B., Rubin, D., & Tupler, L. (2008). H.M.’s personal crossword puzzles: Understanding memory and language. Memory, 16, 89-96. Small, S.A., Nava, A.S., Perera, G.M., DeLaPaz, R., Mayeux, R., Stern, Y. (2001) Circuit mechanisms underlying memory encoding and retrieval in the long axis of the hippocampal formation. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 442-449. 151 Smith, E., & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science’s Compass Review, 283, 1657-1661. Smith, M.L., & Milner, B. (1989). Right hippocampal impairment in the recall of spatial location: Encoding deficit or rapid forgetting. Neuropsychologia, 27, 71-81. Sperry, R. W. (1961) Cerebral organization and behavior. Science 133, 1749-1757. Springer, J.A., Binder, J.R., Hammeke, T.A., Swanson, S.J., Frost, J.A., Bellgowan, P.S.F., Brewer, C.C. et al. (1999). Language dominance in neurologically normal and epilepsy subjects. A functional MRI study. Brain, 122, 2033-2045. Squire, L. R. (1992). Memory and brain. New York: Oxford University Press. Squire, L. R. (1992). Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychological Review, 99(2), 195-231. Squire, L. R. (2011). The Art of Remembering. Review of Moonwalking with Einstein, by J. Foer. Nature, 472, 33-34. Squire, L.R., Cohen, N.J., & Nadel, L. (1984). The medial temporal region and memory consolidation: A new hypothesis. In: H. Weingartner and E. Parker (Eds.). Memory Consolidation (pp. 185-210), Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Squire, L.R., van der Horst, A.S., McDuff, S.G., Frascino, J.C., Hopkins, R.O., Mauldin, K.N. (2010). Role of the hippocampus in remembering the past and imagining the future. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 107, 19044-19048. Squire, L.R. & Wixted, J. The cognitive neuroscience of human memory since H.M. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 34, 259-288. Squires, E.J., Hunkin, N.M., Parkin, A.J. (1997). Errorless learning of novel associations in amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 35(8), 1103-1111. Stark, C.E., Okado, Y. (2003). Making memories without trying: medial temporal lobe activity associated with incidental memory formation during recognition. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 6748–6753. Steinvorth, S., Levine, B., & Corkin, S. (2005). Medial temporal lobe structures are needed to re-experience remote autobiographical memories: Evidence from H.M. and W.R. Neuropyschologia, 43, 479-496. Strauss, E., & Wada, J. (1983). Lateral preferences and cerebral speech dominance. Cortex, 19, 165-177. 152 Suddendorf, T. (1994). Discovery of the fourth dimension: Mental time travel and human evolution. (Doctoral dissertation). Suddendorf, T. & Busby, J. (2005). Making decisions with the future in mind: Developmental and comparative identification of mental time travel. Learning & Motivation, (Special Issue: Cognitive time travel in people and animals), 36, 110125. Sumner, J.A., Griffith, J.W., & Mineka, S., (2010). Overgeneral autobiographical memory as a predictor of the course of depression: a meta-analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 614–625. Svoboda, E., McKinnon, M. C., & Levine, B. (2006). The functional neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44(12), 21892208. Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M., & Sedivy, J. C. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science, 268, 1632–1634. Tannen, D. (1989). Talking Voices: Repetition, Dialogue, and Imagery in Conversational Discourse. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Tanriverdi, T., Al Hinai, Q., Mok, Kelvin, Klein, D., Poulin, N., & Olivier, A. (2009). Atypical language lateralization in patients with left hippocampal sclerosis: Does the hippocampus affect language lateralization? Turkish Neurosurgery, 19(1), 114. Tappen, R.M., Williams-Burgess, C., Edelstein, J., Touhy, T., Fishman, S. (1997). Communicating with individuals with Alzheimer’s disease: Examination of recommended strategies. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing, 11(5), 249-256. Tauroza, S., & Allison, D. (1990). Speech rates in British English. Applied.Linguistics, 11, 90–105. Taylor, L.B. (1989). Localisation of cerebral lesions by psychological testing. Clinical Neurosurgery, 16, 269-287. Titone, D., Levy, D.L., & Holzman, P.S. (2000). Contextual insensitivity in schizophrenic language processing: Evidence from lexical ambiguity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 761-767. Tranel, D. (2007). Theories of clinical neuropsychology and brain-behavior relationships: Luria and beyond. In J. Morgan & R. Ricker (Eds.), Theories of clinical neuropsychology and brain-behavior relationships: Luria and beyond (pp. 2739). New York: Taylor and Francis. 153 Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (2002). Neurobiological foundations of human memory. In A.D. Baddeley, M. Kopelman, & B. A. Wilson (Eds.), The handbook of memory disorders (2nd ed., pp. 17-56). New York: John Wiley and Sons, Ltd. Tranel, D., Damsio, H. & Damasio, A.R. (1997). A neural basis for the retrieval of conceptual knowledge. Neuropsychologia, 35(10), 1319-1327. Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of Episodic Memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press Tulving, E. (1985). How many memory systems are there? American Psychologist, 40(4), 385-398. Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Reviews of Psychology, 43(1), 1-25. Tulving, E., & Markowitsch, H.J. (1998). Episodic and declarative memory: Role of the hippocampus. Hippocampus, 3(3), 198-204. Ujhelyi, M. (1996). Is there any intermediate stage between animal communication and language? Journal of Theoretical Biology, 180(1), 71-76. Ullman, M. (2001). The neural basis of lexicon and grammar in first and second language: The declarative/procedural model. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4(1), 105-122. Ullman, M. (2004). Contributions of memory circuits to language: The declarative/procedural model. Cognition, 92, 231-270. Ullman, M., Corkin, S., Coppola, M., Hickok, G., Growdon, J., Koroshetz, W., & Pinker, S. (1997). A neural dissociation within language: Evidence that the mental dictionary is part of declarative memory and that grammatical rules are processed by the procedural system. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(2), 266-276. Van Berkum, J. J. A., Van den Brink, D., Tesink, C. M. J. Y., Kos, M., & Hagoort, P. (2008). The neural integration of speaker and message. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 580–591. Van Buren, J.M. (1963) Confusion and disturbance of speech from stimulation in vicinity of the head of the caudate nucleus. Journal of Neurosurgery 20, 148–157. Van Buren, J.M. (1987). Complications of surgical procedures in the diagnosis and treatment of epilepsy. In J. Engel Jr. (Ed.), Surgical treatment of the epilepsies (pp. 465-475). New York: Raven Press. 154 Vargha-Khadem, F., Issacs, E., & Muter, V. (1994). A review of cognitive outcomes after unilateral lesions sustained during childhood. Journal of Child Neurology, 9 Supplemental 2, 67-73. Verfaellie, M., Rajaram, S., Fossum, K., & Williams, L. (2008). Not all repetition is alike: Different benefits of repetition in amnesia and normal memory. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14(3), 365-372. Viskontas, I.V., McAndrews, M.P. & Moscovitch, M. (2000). Remote episodic memory deficits in patients with unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy and excisions. Journal of Neuroscience, 20(15), 5853-5857. Voltzenlogel, V., Despres, O., Vignal, J.P., Steinhoff, B.J., Kehrli, P., & Manning, L. (2006). Remote memory in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia, 47, 1–8. Wagner, A.D., Desmond, J.E., Glover, G.H., & Gabrieli, J.D.E. (1998). Prefrontal cortex and recognition memory: Functional-MRI evidence for context-dependent retrieval processes. Brain, 121, 1985-2002. Wagner, A.D., Shannon, B.H., Kahn, I., & Buckner, R.L. (2005). Parietal lobe contributions to episodic memory retrieval. Trends in Cognitive Science, 9, 445453. Wagner Cook, S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2009). Embodied communication: speakers’ gestures affect listeners’ actions. Cognition, 113, 98–104. Walczak, T., Radtke, R., McNamara, J., Lewis, D.V., Luther, J.S., Thompson, E., Wilson, W.P., et al. (1990). Anterior temporal lobectomy for complex partial seizures: Evaluation, results and long-term follow up in 100 cases. Neurology, 40, 413-418. Warren, D. E., Duff, M. C., Jensen, U., Tranel, D., & Cohen, N. J. (2012). Hiding in plain view: Lesions of the medial temporal lobe impair online representation. Hippocampus, 22 (7), 1577–88. Warren, D.E., Duff, M.C., Tranel, D. & Cohen, N. J. (2010) Medial temporal lobe damage impairs representation of simple stimuli. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 35. Warren, D. E., Duff, M. C., Tranel, D., & Cohen, N. J. (2011). Observing degradation of visual representations over short intervals when medial temporal lobe is damaged. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(12), 3862-3873. Warrington, E.K., & Baddeley, A.D. (1974) Amnesia and memory for visual location. Neuropsychologia, 12, 257–263. 155 Warrington, E.K., & Shallice, T. (1974). Category specific semantic impairments. Brain, 107, 829-854. Wernicke, C. (1874). Der aphasische Symptomencomplex. Eine psychologische Studie auf anatomischer Basis. Breslau, M. Crohn und Weigert. Weylman, S.T., Brownell, H.H., & Gardner, H. (1988). It’s not you mean, not what you say: Pragmatic language use in brain-damaged patients. In F. Plum (Ed.). Language, communication, and the brain (pp. 229–43). New York: Raven. Wheeler, M.A., & Roediger, H.L. (1992). Disparate effects of repeated testing: Reconciling Ballard’s (1913) and Bartlett’s (1932) results. Psychological Science, 3(4), 240-245. Williams, J.M.G., Barnhofer, T., Crane, C., Hermans, D., Raes, F., Watkins, E., & Dalgleish, T., (2007). Autobiographical memory specificity and emotional disorder. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 122–148. Williams, K.N. (2010). Exploring interventions to reduce cognitive decline in aging. Journal of Psychosocial Nursing and Mental Health Services, 48(5), 1-10. Williams, K.N. & Hummert, M. L. (2004). Enhancing communication with older adults: Overcoming elderspeak. Journal of Gerontological Nursing, 30, 1-10. Willingham, D. B., & Koroshetz, W. J. (1993). Evidence for dissociable motor skills in Huntington's disease patients. Psychobiology, 21, 173-182. Wilson, B.A., Baddeley, A., Evans, J., & Shiel, A. (1994). Errorless learning in the rehabilitation of memory impaired people. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 4(3), 307-326. Winocur. G. & Weiskrantz. L. (1976). An investigation of paired-associate learning in amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia 14, 97-110. Weber, B., Wellmer, J., Reuber, M., Mormann, F., Weis, S., Urbach, H., Ruhlmann, J. et al. (2006). Left hippocampal pathology is associated with atypical language lateralization in patients with focal epilepsy. Brain, 129, 346-351. Wynn, V.E., & Logie, R.H. (1998). The veracity of long-term memories – Did Bartlett get it right? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 12, 1-20. Youse, K. M., & Coelho, C. A. (2005). Working memory and discourse production abilities following closed-head injury. Brain Injury, 19(12), 1001–1009. 156 Zedkova, L., Woodward, N. D., Harding, I., Tibbo, P. G., & Purdon, S. E. (2006). Procedural learning in schizophrenia investigated with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Schizophrenia Research, 88, 198-207. Zarahn, E., Aguirre, G.K., & D’Esposito, M., (1999). Temporal isolation of the neural correlates of spatial mnemonic processing with fMRI. Cognitive Brain Research, 7, 255-268. Zeman, A. Beschin, N., Dewar, M., & Della Sala, S. (2012). Imagining the present: Amnesia may impair descriptions of the present as well as of the future and the past. Cortex, 49(3), 637-645. Zillmer, E.A., Spiers, M., & Culbertson, W.C. (2008). Principles of Neuropsychology (2nd ED). Belmont, CA: Thomson Wadsworth.