* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Lecture 2 - wseh2elt
Activity-dependent plasticity wikipedia , lookup
Artificial general intelligence wikipedia , lookup
Blood–brain barrier wikipedia , lookup
Embodied language processing wikipedia , lookup
Evolution of human intelligence wikipedia , lookup
Dual consciousness wikipedia , lookup
Neuroesthetics wikipedia , lookup
Neuroeconomics wikipedia , lookup
Broca's area wikipedia , lookup
Haemodynamic response wikipedia , lookup
Emotional lateralization wikipedia , lookup
Neurotechnology wikipedia , lookup
Donald O. Hebb wikipedia , lookup
Sports-related traumatic brain injury wikipedia , lookup
Aging brain wikipedia , lookup
Selfish brain theory wikipedia , lookup
Neuroinformatics wikipedia , lookup
Human brain wikipedia , lookup
Neuroplasticity wikipedia , lookup
Brain morphometry wikipedia , lookup
Holonomic brain theory wikipedia , lookup
Neuropsychopharmacology wikipedia , lookup
Neurophilosophy wikipedia , lookup
Neuroanatomy wikipedia , lookup
History of neuroimaging wikipedia , lookup
Cognitive neuroscience wikipedia , lookup
Brain Rules wikipedia , lookup
Lateralization of brain function wikipedia , lookup
Metastability in the brain wikipedia , lookup
1 The fact that we – human beings – acquire our mother tongues and are able to learn other languages is the result of the brain we have. We will be looking at this brain of ours, at areas that specialise in language, and also at the brain as a system of systems in which connections between areas are as important as the areas themselves. That is why we will start by asking: What’s the matter? We often refer to our brains by means of a phrase grey matter. In fact (Damasio 1994, among others), there are two kinds of matter: - grey matter, containing 10 billion neurons - white matter, 10 trillion synapses (each neuron having between 1000 and 6000 synaptic connections with other neurons) All neurons (the grey matter) consist of the CELL and a number of projections branching out. These projections make it possible for the neurons to shake hands with other neurons (creating the white-matter network of connections): AXONS reach out sending electro-chemical signals which are received by DENDRITES. The brain consists of two hemispheres - left and right – which asymmetrically control the left and the right side of the body (this means that the left hemisphere controls the right side and vice versa). As a result of this asymmetry as well as hemisphere specialisation the two halves of the brain are often referred to as two brains: THE LEFT BRAIN AND THE RIGHT BRAIN As indicated in the humorous picture in the previous slide, the left brain is orderly, analytical and generally seens as the area of reason; the right brain is emotional, imaginative and highly creative. The areas in charge of language processing are located in the left brain …, … which is an oversimplification, as will be seen soon. Yet, first things first ;-) Broca’s area – responsible for the production of speech (and in charge of grammar) How do we know? Among others, because Broca’s aphasia involves a syntactic deficit – such patients struggle to get their meaning across but they eventually make sense; their main problem is grammar Mr. Ford (a stroke victim; following Pinker 1994): Head, fall, Jesus Christ, me, no good, stroke … Thursdaz, er, er, er, no Fridaz, Bar+ba+ra … wife … and, oh, car … drive … purnpike … you know … rest and tee-vee. He understood questions like: Does a stone float on water? but failed to answer questions requiring grammatical operations like: The lion was killed by the tiger. Which animal is dead? Broca’s area – responsible for the production of speech Wernicke’s area – responsible for the understanding of speech (words and their meanings). How do we know? Among others, from cases of Wernicke’s aphasia, which involves a semantic and lexical deficit – such patients utter strings of words which are more or less grammatical but make no sense. Mr. Gorgan (following Pinker 1994): I’ve done a lot well, I impose a lot while, on the other hand, you know what I mean. I have to run around, look it over, trebbin and all that sort of stuff. Besides, such patients have problems naming objects, coming up with related words or sound distortions: table – chair; clip - plick Broca’s area – responsible for the production of speech Wernicke’s area – responsible for the understanding of speech Arcuate fasciculus (the connection between Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas) Motor cortex (without which we would not be able to control our articulatory apparatus) Yes, there is. We know this, because right brain damage makes some aspects of language processing impossible. These aspects include …. metaphor humour concrete vocabulary word-picture associations intonation singing and language expression writing spatial reasoning handedness and language – in left-handed people: 70% have lannguage in the left hemisphere 15% - in the right hemisphere 15% - in both hemispheres men and women men – spatial reasoning; women – verbal fluency in the case of brain damage – aphasia is 3 times more frequent in men; women have more bilateral control of language (their corpus callosum is bigger) phonological processing – men in the left brain; women – bilaterally Performance on a rhyming task (as seen in the metabolic activity in the brain) (Shaywitz et al. 1995) C same effectiveness !!! 2 Is language modular? = is language incapsulated in one region? The simplified answer may be: YES – left brain, Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas The real answer is more complex ;-) The answer to the question of whether language is incapsulated in one region has been given based on a number of theoretical stances. Some of them include: Phrenology Nativists (Chomsky) Cognitivists (Piaget) Contemporary neuroscience (Damasio) P. is now considered to be pseudoscience. According to phrenologists, every function had a special location in the brain. Language was located under the left eye. Language is domain-specific: “... possession of human language is associated with a specific type of mental organization...” (Chomsky 1972: 70). In the brain there is a special language organ: “language is a natural object, a component of the human mind, physically represented in the brain and part of the biological endowment of the species” (Chomsky 2002: 1) Language is domain-general, we have to look at its development in terms of a change in the level of intelligence (Piaget 1955). Dąbrowska (2004): While Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas may be regions where language is triggered, cerebral connections between this areas and areas in charge of general cognition (categorising, analysing, visualising, etc.) cannot be denied. ◦ there are no centres, there are systems made up of several interconnected units ◦ whatever neurons do depends on the nearby assembly of neurons they belong to ◦ whatever systems do depends on how assemblies influence other assemblies in an architecture of interconnected assemblies; ◦ whatever each assembly contributes to the function of the system to which it belongs depends on its place in the system. … In short then, the brain is a supersystem of systems. (Damasio 1994: 29-30). Some of the systems are language-specific and they are connected to form larger assemblies which, in turn, are connected to other systems non-language-specific systems. (new type of brain modularity) brain is a system of systems. If we realize that, it will be easier to understand how second/foreign languages are learned and processed: how new words are noticed, remembered and linked to concepts, how language chunks are formed, how rules are abstracted from usage, how L1 rules are used to endorse (often negatively) L2/FL rules cerebral location of languages. L2/FL use the same brain regions as L1 but the location differs slightly from that of our native tongue in late bilinguals (in late but not in early bilinguals; based on neuroscience findings). As a result, there will be areas of similarities and areas of difference between the first and every other learned language 3 There appears to be a collection of systems in the human brain consistently dedicated to the goal-oriented thinking process we call reasoning, and to the response selection we call decision making, with a special emphasis on the personal and social domain. This same collection of systems is also involved in emotion and feeling, and is partly dedicated to processing body signals. Reason and emotion “intersect” in the ventromedial prefrontal cortices, and they also intersect in amygdale Because the brain is the captive audience of the body, feelings are winners among equals. And since what comes first constitutes the frame of reference for what comes after, feelings have a say on how the rest of the brain and cognition go about their business. Their influence is immense What exactly happens? The brain evaluates the stimuli it receives via the senses from the language learning situation the brain appraises the stimuli this appraisal leads to emotional response which affects subsequent cognition in a positive or a negative way The amygdala and the orbitofrontal cortex, based on past experience with similar stimuli, automatically appraise the stimuli for the emotional relevance. Both the amygdale and the orbitofrontal cortex project to the brain stem activating the sympathetic and the parasympathetic nervous systems (SNS – preparing body for action and PNS – calming the body) as well as the hypothalamus (controlling endocrine function and influencing glands) (Schumann 1999: 41) Second/Foreign language learners have brains brains have appraisal systems motivation to learn L2/FL is based on appraisal carried out within certain dimensions In language learning such emotional dimensions or “frames of reference” (Gardner 1985 as well as Schmidt & Savage 1992; modified by Schumann 1999) include: novelty/familiarity, pleasantness, goal significance, coping potential and self/social image. Which dimension of each two mentioned below would you say is good and which – bad. Why? novelty/familiarity pleasantness/unpleasantness goal significance: high/low coping potential : high/low self/social image: good/bad The brain is a parallel processor The brain downshifts under threat The search for meaning occurs through patterning The brain is meaning driven Each brain is unique Movement and exercise improve brain functioning Brain growth is enriched by continued learning Bartkowski, B. 2001. “Sieci jednokierunkowe” . Chomsky, N. 1972. Language and Mind. New York: Harcourt Brace Javanovich. Chomsky, N. 2002. On Nature and Language. Cambridge: CUP Christison, M. 2002. “Brain-based research and language teaching”. ET Forum 40, 2; http://exchanges.state.gov/forum/vols/vol40/no2/p02.htm Damasio, R.A. 1994. Descartes’ Error. Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain. New York: G.P. Putnam’s Sons Fitch, W.T. and M.D. Hauser, 2004. "Computational Constraints on Syntactic Processing in a Nonhuman Primate," Science B 303, pp. 377-380. Fodor, J. A. 1983. “The Modularity of Mind: An Essay on Faculty Psychology”. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press. Fromkin, V. and R. Rodman. 1998. An Introduction to Language; 6th edition. New York: Harcourt Brace Gardner R.C. 1985. Social Psychology and Second Language Learning. The Roles of Attitudes and Motivation. London: Edward Arnold Gentner TQ, Fenn KM, Margoliash D, Nusbaum HC. 2006. “Recursive syntactic pattern learning by songbirds”. Nature 440:1204-1207 Goleman, D. 1995. Emotional Intelligence. New York: Bantam Books Gould, E., A. Beylin, P. Tanapat, A. Reeves & T.J. Shors. 1999. “Learning enhances adult neurogenesis in the hippocampal formation”. Nature Neuroscience 2, 3 pp. 260-265. Kasperski, M.J. 2003. Sztuczna inteligencja. Droga do maszyn myślących. Gliwice: Helion Piaget, J. (1955). The Language and Thought of the Child. New York: Meridian Books. Pilcher, H. 2004. “Old dog learns new tricks”. http://www.bioedonline.org/news/news.cfm?art=1012 Pinker, S. 1994. The Language Instinct. London: Penguin Books http://www.neuron.chost.pl/pliki/start.html). Praag, H. G. Kempermann & F.H. Gage. 1999. “Running increases cell proliferation and neurogenesis in adult mouse dentate gyrus”. Nature Neuroscience 2, 3.pp. 266-270. Schumann, J.H. 1999. “A neurobilogical perspective on affect and methodology in second language learning. In: J. Arnold (ed) Affect in Language Learning. CUP Shaywitz, B.A.; Shaywitz, S.E.; Pugh. K.R.; Constable, R.T.; Skudlawski, P.; Fulbright, R.K.; Bronen, R.A.; Fletcher, J.M.; Shankwiler, D.P.; Katz, L.; Gore, J.C. 1995. “Sex differences in the functional organization of the brain for language”. Nature 373 (6515):607-609 Skehan, P. 2003. A Cognitive Approach to Language Learning (5th edition). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Springer, S.P. i G. Deutch. 2004. Lewy mózg, prawy mózg z perspektywy neurobiologii poznawczej. Warszawa: Prószyński i S-ka; Stevick E.W. 1999. “Affect in learning and memory: From alchemy to chemistry”. In J. Arnold (ed) Affect in Language Learning. CUP Vygotsky, L.S. 1986. Language and Thought. Revised Edition. (edited by A. Kazulin). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Whorf, B.J. 1956. Language, Thought and Reality: selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Yule, G. 2002. The Study of Language (8th priniting). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press