* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Frontal lobe dysfunction in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis
Eyeblink conditioning wikipedia , lookup
Cortical cooling wikipedia , lookup
Source amnesia wikipedia , lookup
Premovement neuronal activity wikipedia , lookup
Cognitive neuroscience wikipedia , lookup
Dual consciousness wikipedia , lookup
Neurophilosophy wikipedia , lookup
Executive dysfunction wikipedia , lookup
Neuroplasticity wikipedia , lookup
Human brain wikipedia , lookup
Environmental enrichment wikipedia , lookup
Persistent vegetative state wikipedia , lookup
Synaptic gating wikipedia , lookup
Time perception wikipedia , lookup
Executive functions wikipedia , lookup
Affective neuroscience wikipedia , lookup
Neuroesthetics wikipedia , lookup
Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis wikipedia , lookup
Biology of depression wikipedia , lookup
Neuroeconomics wikipedia , lookup
Impact of health on intelligence wikipedia , lookup
Orbitofrontal cortex wikipedia , lookup
Sex differences in cognition wikipedia , lookup
Visual selective attention in dementia wikipedia , lookup
Aging brain wikipedia , lookup
Emotional lateralization wikipedia , lookup
Cerebral cortex wikipedia , lookup
Embodied language processing wikipedia , lookup
Brain (1996), 119, 2105-2120 Frontal lobe dysfunction in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis A PET study S. Abrahams,1 L. H. Goldstein,1 J. J. M. Kew,3 D. J. Brooks,3 C. M. Lloyd,2 C. D. Frith 34 and P. N. Leigh2 1 Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, the Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Institute of Psychiatry and King's College Hospital Medical School, the 3MRC Cyclotron Unit, Hammersmith Hospital and the ^Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, Institute of Neurology, London, UK 2 Correspondence to: Dr Sharon Abrahams, Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, UK Summary PET measurements of regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) were used to explore frontal lobe dysfunction in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS). An activation paradigm of executive frontal lobe function (verbal fluency), which contrasted rCBF during word generation and word repetition, was used. Two groups of ALS patients, defined by the presence or absence of cognitive impairment (ALSi) (impaired, n = 6; ALSu, unimpaired, n = 6) were compared with healthy age-matched controls (n = 6). Patient selection was based on prior performance on a written test of verbal fluency. Additional neuropsychological assessment of the patients revealed evidence of executive and memory dysfunction in the ALSi group only, with marked deficits in tests of intrinsic generation. The ALSi patients displayed significantly (P < 0.001) impaired activation in cortical and subcortical regions including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC; areas 46 and 9), lateral premotor cortex (areas 8 and 6), medial prefrontal and premotor cortices (areas 8 and 9), insular cortex bilaterally and the anterior thalamic nuclear complex. Although the three groups showed matched word generation performance on the scanning paradigm, the ALSu group displayed a relatively unimpaired pattern of activation. These results support the presence of extra-motor neuronal involvement, particularly along a thalamo-frontal association pathway, in some non-demented ALS patients. In addition, this study suggests dysfunction of DLPFC in some ALS patients with associated cognitive impairments. Keywords: frontal lobes; cognitive impairment; ALS; PET; verbal fluency Abbreviations: ALS = amyotrophic lateral sclerosis; ALSi = cognitively impaired ALS patients; ALSu, = cognitively unimpaired ALS patients; DLPFC = dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; rCBF = regional cerebral blood flow; RMJT = random movement joystick task; SPM = statistical parametric mapping; VFI = Verbal Fluency Index; WCST = Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Introduction Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis is a progressive disorder characterized by degeneration of the corticospinal tract and lower motor neurons of the brainstem and spinal cord. Hence most research on the mechanisms of neurodegeneration in ALS has focused on the motor system. There is, however, increasing evidence that ALS should be regarded as a multisystem disorder and that cortical involvement extends beyond the confines of the primary motor areas (Smith, 1960; Kew et al., 1993a, b; Leigh et al., 1994; Talbot et al., 1995). © Oxford University Press 1996 Dementia occurs in - 3 % of patients with sporadic ALS (Kew and Leigh, 1992, 1994), and such patients display cognitive and behavioural changes characteristic of frontal lobe dysfunction (Neary et al., 1990; Peavy et al., 1992). The underlying pathology in these cases consists of spongiform neuronal degeneration in layers 2 and 3 of the prefrontal and temporal cortex (Hudson, 1981; Wikstrom etal., 1982; Morita et al., 1987; Neary et al., 1990; Wightman et al, 1992) with additional involvement of regions within the limbic system 2106 S. Abrahams et al. (the hippocampal formation, subiculum and amygdala) (Okamoto et al., 1991; Wightman et al., 1992; Kato et al., 1994). In non-demented patients with sporadic ALS cognitive changes have been reported in a number of neuropsychological investigations (Gallassi et al., 1985, 1989; David and Gillham, 1986; Ludolph et al., 1992; Kew et al., 1993b). These studies have revealed a pattern of frontal lobe (executive) and memory deficits, clearly suggestive of cortical involvement extending beyond the motor system. Executive dysfunction has been demonstrated using tests of verbal reasoning, visual attention, picture sequencing and category formation on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) (Gallassi et al., 1985, 1989; David and Gillham, 1986; Talbot et al., 1995). However, the most striking and consistent impairment is found using tests of verbal fluency, which require rapid word generation, typically involving orthographic or category-based procedures (Gallassi et al., 1985, 1989; Ludolph et al., 1992; Kew et al., 1993*). Memory and learning impairments have also been reported using tests of prose recall, word list and verbal paired associate learning, and picture recall (Gallassi et al., 1985, 1989; David and Gillham, 1986; Iwasaki et al., 1990; Kew et al., \993b). ALS patients with cognitive impairment, and also demonstrated that verbal fluency appears to be a sensitive indicator of such cortical dysfunction. In the present investigation, we used PET to further explore frontal lobe functions in ALS using a verbal fluency activation paradigm. The task was based closely on a study by Frith et al. (1991a) and contrasts rCBF increases during two conditions, orthographically based word generation and word repetition. Word generation is known to produce relative activation of the DLPFC (Frith et al., 1991a). The purpose of this study was to identify the neural basis of the marked abnormalities of verbal fluency detected in some ALS subjects and to test the hypothesis that such subjects show impaired activation of prefrontal and subcortical areas, previously shown to be implicated in ALS (Kew et al., 1993a). Material and methods Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis patients Twelve patients with sporadic ALS, recruited from the Maudsley/King's College Hospital, London MND Care and Research Centre, were studied. All patients had clinical and electrophysiological evidence of combined upper and lower motor neuron involvement and were classified as 'definite' or 'probable' ALS according to the El Escorial criteria (Swash and Leigh, 1992; World Federation of Neurology Research Group, 1994). The clinical characteristics of the patients are presented in Table 1. No patient had a history of cerebrovascular disease, hypertension or diabetes, and none was taking psychoactive drugs or any medication that might influence CBF. All patients were right handed. Patients were excluded if severity of speech, swallowing or respiratory symptoms would have hindered their task performance (speaking whilst lying supine in the PET scanner). Prior to the investigation, the patients underwent a pilot of the test Further insight into the neural basis of the cognitive changes in ALS has been provided by a PET study (Kew et al., 1993a, b). This was conducted using a motor paradigm which compared paced joystick movements in a freely chosen (random) direction to movements only in a forward direction (stereotyped). The study showed impaired activation of medial prefrontal cortex (Brodmann areas 10 and 32), anterior cingulate gyrus, parahippocampal gyrus and anterior thalamic nuclear complex, in ALS subjects selected on the basis of abnormal verbal fluency scores (Kew et al., 1993£>). Such dysfunction was less marked in patients with unimpaired verbal fluency. This study therefore revealed clear evidence of medial prefrontal cortex dysfunction, in non-demented Table 1 Clinical characteristics of 12 ALS patients Patient (group) Pseudobulbar Palsy (UMN) Bulbar involvement (LMN) Wasting UL Wasting LL Fasciculations (UL and LL) Spasticity UL Spasticity LL Hyperreflexia Babinski sign 1 (ALSu) 2 (ALSu) 3 (ALSu) 4 (ALSu) 5 (ALSu) 6 (ALSu) 7 (ALSi) 8 (ALSi) 9 (ALSi) 10 (ALSi) 11 (ALSi) 12 (ALSi) — + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + — + + + + + — + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + — + + + + + + + + - Pseudobulbar palsy evidence: brisk jaw jerk, spasticity of factila and or tongue muscles, spastic dysarthria. Bulbar involvement evidence nasal speech, fasciculations, wasting of tongue and weak cough. UMN = upper motor neuron; LMN = lower motor neuron. UL = upper limb; LL = Lower Limb. + = present/positive; - = absent/negative; ALSu = ALS unimpaired; ALSi = ALS impaired. Frontal lobe activation in ALS Table 2 Subject characteristics Age (years) Gender VFI Duration (months) Bulbar function Spinal function Total disability HAD anxiety HAD depression ALSu (n = 6) ALSi (» = 6) Controls (« = 6) 57.3 (10.3) 6M , OF 4.2 (10) 25.7 (17.0) 19.8 (0.4) 15.2 (1.7) 35.0 (1.9) 6.2 (3.0) 4.2 (2.1) 57.3 (13.2) 5M , IF 14.1 (10.8) 22.7 (15.0) 17.5 (3.3) 14.7 (2.9) 32.2 (2.7) 6.8 (2.8) 3.5 (1.9) 57.7 (9.7) 4M , 2F 4.8 (2.0) NA NA NA NA 5.5 (4.1) 3.3 (2.7) Means (and standard deviations). Disability scores (bulbar and spinal functions and total disability) are taken from the ALS severity scale (Hillel et al., 1989; see text). Low scores represent functional impairment. Duration = illness time (months) between onset of symptoms and testing. NA = not applicable; HAD = Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. procedure to establish whether they could comfortably speak whilst lying supine. The patients' severity of bulbar symptoms (particularly swallowing) did not significantly affect their dietary state and none had a percutaneous endoscopic gastrostomy. The patients were selected on the basis of prior performance on a test of written verbal fluency, based on the Thurstone Word Fluency Task (Thurstone and Thurstone, 1962). The task involved the written generation of as many words as possible beginning with a given letter, in a limited time period: Subjects were required to write (i) words beginning with the letter S in 5 min and (ii) four-letter words beginning with the letter C in 4 min. We adapted the task to control for speed of writing by using a copy condition where, following the fluency tests (i) and (ii), the subject was timed as they copied the words they had written previously. This writing control condition enabled the calculation of the Verbal Fluency Index (VFI), which consisted of the average time (s) taken to think of each word, i.e. (total time allowed for test, 9 min - time to copy all words generated)/(total number of words generated). Higher scores represent longer thinking times and greater impairment. Six patients displayed unimpaired VFI scores [ALSu (unimpaired performance)], with scores comparable to the control group (see Table 2 for means and standard deviations). Six were selected for their poor VFI scores [ALSi (impaired performance)]. The scores between the two ALS subgroups were separated by more than 2 SDs. Healthy controls Six age-matched right-handed control subjects were studied. Three of the controls were either friends or spouses of ALS patients, recruited through the local Motor Neuron Disease Association and three were recruited independently and did not know of any friend or relative with ALS. The subjects were asked whether they had any neurological symptoms, 2107 but were not examined in detail neurologically. None had any neurological symptoms or a history of any neurological disease. Details (age and gender) are presented in Table 2. Written informed consent was given by all subjects. The study was approved by the Hammersmith Hospital and Maudsley Hospital ethics committees, and permission to administer radioactive H2I5O was obtained from ARSAC (UK). Design for analysis of subject characteristics and neuropsychological assessment A one-way ANOVA, with one between-subjects factor of group, was used to compare the two patient groups (ALSu and ALSi) or all three subject groups (controls, ALSi, ALSu). In the latter, two specified independent contrasts were employed: (i) comparing all ALS patients with controls (ALSu + ALSi versus controls), (ii) comparing the two patient groups (ALSu versus ALSi). An additional contrast was also employed to determine whether one patient group was significantly different from controls (controls versus ALSi). Only those contrasts with significant findings are quoted below (for values of P > 0.05, n.s. denotes nonsignificant). For subject characteristics, a 95% confidence interval for the difference in means between groups is also quoted, where a value of 0 contained within the interval demonstrates a non-significant difference. Subject characteristics (see Table 2 for means and standard deviations) All three subject groups were matched for age and the analysis revealed no significant differences between groups. The 95% confidence intervals for the difference in mean age between ALSu and ALSi (-1.6, 3.0), controls and ALSu (-3.6, 1.8), controls and ALS (-2.8, 2.4), demonstrates that the differences were not significant. In the analysis of VFI scores, the one-way ANOVA revealed a significant effect of group [F(2,15) = 4.50, P < 0.05] with a significant difference between the ALSi and the ALSu patients [F(l,15) = 7.17, P < 0.025] and between the ALSi group and controls [F(l,15) = 6.29, P < 0.025). The 95% confidence intervals for the difference in mean VFI scores between ALSu and ALSi (-18.6, -1.2), controls and ALSi (-18.0, -0.5), controls and ALSu (-1.2, 2.4), are consistent with the results of the ANOVA. This demonstrates that the ALSi group were clearly impaired on the measure of fluency relative to controls and to ALSu patients, which is consistent with selection criteria. The two patient groups did not differ significantly in length of disease duration, with a 95% confidence interval for the difference in means between groups (-15.1, 21.1) revealing no significant difference. Degree of disability was measured using the ALS Severity Scale (Hillel et al., 1989), which provides separate measures for bulbar and 2108 S. Abrahams et al. spinal function (see Table 2, lower scores represent greater functional impairment). There was no significant difference between the two patient groups in the separate bulbar and spinal function disability scores, although a non-significant trend emerged for the comparison of total disability scores [F(l,lO) =4.39, P = 0.06] which was significant using a 95% confidence interval (0.2, 5.4), with the ALSi group tending to show greater disability than the ALSu patients. This difference was more apparent on mean scores of bulbar function; however, the scores are computed from a maximum of 20, demonstrating that the ALSi group displayed some slight bulbar functional impairment, while the ALSu group was virtually unimpaired. A measurement of anxiety and depression was also obtained in order to exclude the possibility that abnormalities in blood flow may be associated with such psychological factors. This was obtained using the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (Zigmond and Snaith, 1983), which we adapted for use with both patients and controls. This alteration consisted of the removal of one statement ('I feel as if I am slowed down'), used to calculate depression scores, and which we felt would falsely exaggerate the measure of depression in ALS patients. The analysis of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale questionnaire scores (see Table 2) demonstrated no significant differences between the three groups on anxiety (P > 0.05), with a 95% confidence interval for the difference in means between ALSu and ALSi (-3.9, 2.7), controls and ALSu (-4.8, 3.4) and controls and ALSi (-5.3, 2.7). This was also the case for the modified depression scores (P > 0.05), with a 95% confidence interval for the difference in means between ALSu and ALSi (-1.6, 3.0), controls and ALSu (-3.6, 1.8), and controls and ALSi (-2.8, 2.4). This demonstrates that the three groups were matched in terms of anxiety and depression. Neuropsychological assessment A neuropsychological assessment was conducted on both patients and controls to determine whether the three groups differed in terms of neuropsychological profile. The assessment consisted of standard and experimental tests of frontal lobe and memory function. The tests included the National Adult Reading Test—Revised, to estimate premorbid full scale IQ (Nelson and Willison, 1991). In addition to the written verbal fluency procedure (see above), frontal lobe tests measuring executive functions included a computerized Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Grant and Berg, 1990) and a random movement joystick task (RMJT) (Deiber et al., 1991) where subjects have to generate random sequences of 50 movements with a four-way directional joystick. Memory tests included verbal Paired Associate Learning (Wechsler, 1987), the Recognition Memory Test (Warrington, 1984) using both words and faces, and the Kendrick Object Learning Test (Kendrick, 1985), in which a subject has to remember pictorial representations of common objects, printed on a card and presented to the subject for a limited time period. Structural imaging Wherever possible, patients underwent either an MRI scan, or a CT scan of the brain to exclude any significant cerebral atrophy or ventricular dilation, which may have affected the stereotactic normalization of the PET scans (see below). MRI images were acquired using a Picker 1.0 Tesla HPQ Vista MRI system. The scans consisted of a T r weighted 3D RF spoiled scan (TR = 21, TE = 6, flip angle 35%). The CT scans consisted of a series of axial images with a 10 mm inter-plane distance using a CT9800 whole body scanner (IGE Medical Systems Ltd, Slough, UK). Four of the patients studied underwent MRI and one CT, none of which showed any evidence of atrophy. In addition, an autopsy on a further patient was found to be normal. Of these six patients, four were from the ALSi cognitively impaired group. An MRI scan was performed on one control subject for the purposes of co-registration with his PET image. This was conducted in order to identify clearly and display optimally the regions of significant rCBF change produced by this word generation paradigm in a healthy individual. Coregistration of the PET and MRI was conducted using methods described previously (Watson et al., 1993; Woods et al, 1993). PET imaging Experimental design The PET activation paradigm was based on that developed by Frith et al. (1991a) and contrasted rCBF during two conditions: word generation and word repetition. In the present study, modifications to the procedure were required to accommodate an older patient group with possible speech impairment. The subjects had eight PET measurements of rCBF during a scanning session. Four measurements were made during each condition using an ABAB design. The rCBF measurements were performed with the subjects' eyes closed in a darkened room. During the word generation condition the subject was required to say aloud any word beginning with a given letter in response to the word 'next' presented aurally every 5 s. (The optimum rate of presentation was established prior to the scanning investigation in a rigorous pilot study of paced verbal fluency in a separate group of ALS patients, some with bulbar symptoms.) The subject was given a different letter (S, C, F, T) for each of the four word generation scans. On failure to generate an appropriate word, the subject was required to respond with the word 'pass', within the 5 s response time. This enabled the amount of speech output to be matched between the two conditions. During the four word repetition scans, random words (see Frith et al., 1991a) were presented verbally to the subject every 5 s and the subject was required to respond Frontal lobe activation in ALS by repeating the words aloud. Prior to the scanning session, all subjects underwent up to five practice word generation trials (with different letters), to familiarize them with the protocol. Data acquisition PET scans were performed using a Siemens 935b PET Scanner allowing 31 planes of data acquisition (CTI, Knoxville, Tenn., USA), whose physical characteristics have been described previously (Spinks et al., 1988). Scanning is performed in 3D mode with the septa retracted. At the start of each scanning session, a transmission scan was performed with an external ^Ge/^Ga ring source to correct for tissue attenuation of "/-irradiation. Regional cerebral blood flow measurements were obtained by recording regional levels of cerebral 15O activity following repeated infusions 15 mCi of H215O, administered through an antecubital vein, at a flow rate of 10 ml min~'. Prior to each rCBF measurement, a background scan lasting 30 s was performed. The rCBF scan lasted 165 s and followed the background scan. Five seconds before the start of the rCBF scan the subjects began performing the activating task, the entry of the radioactive water into the brain coinciding with performance of the activating task. A 10-min interscan interval was used to allow 15 O activity to decay to background levels. Each image was reconstructed in 31 axial planes, using a Hanning filter with a cut-off frequency of 0.5, giving an image resolution of 8.5X8.5X4 mm, full width at half maximum. Data analysis Data analysis was performed on a SUN Sparc2 workstation (Sun Microsystems Europe Inc., Surrey, UK) using display software (ANALYZE 6.0: Robb and Hanson, 1991). Image matrix calculations were performed using PRO MATLAB (Mathworks Inc., Sherborn, Mass., USA), and images were analysed using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM) software (MRC Cyclotron Unit, London, UK). The 31 planes in each image were initially interpolated to 43 planes to produce images with ~2 mm3 voxels. Correction for head movement between scans was conducted using a automated realignment program (Woods et al., 1992). The images were then standardized by resizing into stereotactic space, using the intercommissural line (AC-PC) as a reference plane for reslicing and reorientation (Friston et al., 1989). The images were smoothed using a Gaussian filter (10X10x6 mm, full width half maximum), to improve signal to noise ratio and accommodate variability in functional anatomy. This process generated a normalized set of 26 image planes, parallel to the AC-PC line, with an interplane distance of 4 mm corresponding to the axial brain sections of the atlas of Talairach and Tournoux (1988). Statistical image analysis Variations in global CBF between subjects and conditions were removed using a voxel based ANCOVA with global blood flow 2109 as the covariate (Friston et al, 1990). This process normalized global CBF to a value of 50 ml/100 g/min and produced a mean rCBF value and adjusted error variance for each voxel for each condition (word repetition and word generation). The normalized mean rCBF values for the two conditions in normals and patients were then compared with a within-group analysis using the t statistic for each voxel, and significant differences were displayed in three orthogonal projections as a statistical parametric map (SPM{f}; Friston et al., 1991a). The between-group analysis differences in mean rCBF distribution were first assessed with y} statistic using an omnibus threshold of P < 0.001 to avoid false positives due to multiple comparisons. Normalized mean rCBF differences were then again compared with the t statistic. Results Verbal fluency during scanning The number of different words produced (excluding passes) during each word generation scan was recorded as a measure of task performance. All subjects were clearly capable of conducting the word generation task successfully, following up to five practice trials, and, on average, produced five passes in 35 responses during scanning. A comparison of the scores between subject groups (ALSu, ALSi and controls) revealed no significant differences (group, P > 0.35; contrasts P > 0.15). The 95% confidence intervals for the difference in means between ALSu and ALSi (-5.3, 7.3), controls and ALSu (-1.4, 7.2) and controls and ALSi (-1.4, 9.3) are consistent with the results of the ANOVA revealing no significant differences between groups. This analysis demonstrates that the three subject groups were matched in terms of performance during scanning, and excludes the possibility that differences in rCBF between groups can be associated with this factor. rCBF changes in verbal fluency in healthy controls The regions of relative significant rCBF increase in healthy controls {n — 6), comparing word generation with word repetition, are represented in a within-group SPM, where only voxels with a t value corresponding to P < 0.001 are displayed (see Fig. 1). The areas of significant rCBF increase include extensive regions of the left prefrontal cortex, particularly of the DLPFC areas 46 and 9 with evidence of bilateral prefrontal and premotor activation. Regional cerebral blood flow increases were also found in the anterior cingulate gyrus and parietal lobes. The location of the regions of significant rCBF increase (including Brodmann areas) and the co-ordinates of voxels of peak activation within each region are displayed in Table 3. The PET-MRI co-registration of a single subject verifies the precise locations of the regions of rCBF activation produced by this verbal fluency paradigm, within a healthy individual (see Fig. 2). 2110 S. Abrahams et al. sogitfbl I V -10* 1H 1 coronal \ n V J L VAC 68 VPC VAC 0 P I PL } SPM projections transverse Fig. 1 Statistical parametric map (SPM) representing increases in rCBF produced in the comparison of word repetition and word generation in healthy controls (n = 6). Cortical and subcortical regions of significant (P < 0.001) rCBF increase are displayed in black. Left: sagittal, coronal and transverse projections of SPM displayed on a grid representing the stereotactic co-ordinate system of Talairach and Tournoux (1988). VAC and VPC are orientation lines transecting the anterior and posterior commissures. Right: medial and lateral view of left hemisphere (above); medial and lateral views of right hemisphere (below). The location of regions of significant change are displayed in Table 3. Table 3 Cortical regions of significant rCBF increase (P < 0.001) in controls, produced in the comparison of word generation with word repetition Region Region (mm above AC-PC) Coordinates Z score z) Left dorsal prefrontal cortex (areas 9 and 46) Right dorsal prefrontal cortex (area 9) Left inferior frontal gyms (areas 44 and 45) Left anterior prefrontal cortex (area 10) Right lateral premotor cortex (areas 6 and 8) Left lateral premotor cortex (areas 6 and 8) Left medial premotor cortex (area 6) Left anterior cingulate gyms (areas 32 and 24) Left insular cortex Right insular cortex Left inferior and superior parietal lobe (areas 40 and 7) Right inferior and superior parietal lobe (areas 40 and 7) Left precuneus (area 7) Right precuneus (area 7) Left cuneus (area 19) Left supramarginal gyms (area 40) Left superior occipital gyms (area 19) Left anterior thalamus (na and dm) Left posterior thalamus Right anterior thalamus (va) 16-36 24-36 20-28 20-24 40-64 40-64 44-60 24-44 8-12 4-20 40-48 36-48 44-52 36-44 36 36 28-32 18 8 8 -38 30 -40 -28 18 -24 0 -6 -30 28 -46 32 -14 6 -6 -34 -24 -6 0 12 24 32 12 44 0 2 16 14 20 12 -42 -64 -68 -74 -78 -46 -66 -14 -30 -6 28 32 24 24 60 52 44 40 12 8 44 44 48 40 36 36 32 16 8 8 5.67 5.93 4.62 5.73 4.82 5.50 5.30 6.12 5.40 4.43 4.62 4.38 4.22 3.64 3.96 4.45 3.79 3.12 3.40 4.03 Talairach and Tournoux coordinates of the most significant voxel within each region are displayed. AC-PC refers to the intercommissural line; 'area' refers to the cytoarchitectonic (Brodmann) area in which significant voxels were located. Z score refers to the specified coordinate, va = ventral anterior nucleus; na = anterior nucleus; dm = dorsal medial nucleus; p = pulvinar. The comparison of verbal fluency with repetition also showed relative decreases in rCBF in healthy controls. These regions of significant (P < 0.001) rCBF reduction (from word repetition to word generation) were found in posterior cingulate gyrus (areas 31, 23 and 30) and temporal lobes bilaterally; the left and right middle and inferior temporal gyri (areas 39, 21 and 37), and the left and right superior temporal gyri (areas 22 and 38). This is consistent with findings reported by Frith et al. (1991a). Comparison of rCBF activation between groups The ALSi group versus controls The ALSi group displayed significantly (P < 0.001) impaired activation in the DLPFC (areas 46 and 9), premotor cortex Frontal lobe activation in ALS 2111 "Fig. 2 Increases in rCBF in a single healthy control subject. Co-registered PET and MRI images. Left: transverse section 4 mm above AC-PC line, areas of rCBF increase include left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC; areas 46 and 45) Middle: transverse section 38 mm above AC-PC line, areas of rCBF increase include left and right DLPFC (area 9) anterior cingulate (area 32), left inferior parietal lobe (areas 40 and 19). Right: transverse section 55 mm above AC-PC line, areas of rCBF increase include left premotor cortex (areas 8 and 6) and right premotor cortex (area 8). sagittal i coronal A n =• ^v -"C VAC -10* VPCVAC 6B SPM projections transverse Fig. 3 Between-group SPM comparing the pattern of activation in ALSi and control groups. Cortical and subcortical regions showing significantly (P < 0.001) impaired activation in the ALSi group compared with controls, are displayed in black. Left: sagittal, coronal and transverse projections. Right: medial and lateral views of left hemisphere (above): medial and lateral views of right hemisphere {below). The location of the regions displayed are described in Table 4. (8 and 6), insular cortex (see Fig. 3.) and thalamus (see Table 4). Differences in activation arose as a result of increases in rCBF in controls relative to ALSi patients comparing word generation with word repetition. 40) and the left middle and superior temporal gyri (areas 39 and 22). The ALSi group versus the ALSu group The ALSu group versus controls A comparison of the ALSu group with controls at an omnibus significance level of P < 0.001 revealed minor differences in rCBF activation between the two groups (see Fig. 4). Areas of impaired activation included right dorsal prefrontal cortex (area 9), the left and right inferior parietal lobule (area The comparison of the two patient groups (ALSi versus ALSu) produced a similar pattern of rCBF activation differences as found in the comparison of the ALSi group with controls (see Fig. 5). The regions of significant (P < 0.001) difference included prefrontal cortex, lateral and medial premotor cortex, bilateral insular cortex, and thalamus (see Table 5). In general, these differences were S. Abrahams et al. 2112 Table 4 Regions of significant difference (P < 0.001) in activation in the comparison between the control and the ALSi (ALS impaired) groups Region Region (mm above AC-PC) Left dorsal prefrontal cortex (areas 46 and 9) Left anterior prefrontal cortex (area 10) Right medial prefrontal cortex (area 9) Right lateral prefrontal cortex (area 9) Right anterior medial prefrontal cortex (area 10) Left medial premotor cortex (area8) Left lateral premotor cortex (areas 6 and 8) Right lateral premotor cortex (area 8) Right primary motor cortex (area 4) Left primary motor cortex (area 4) Left anterior cingulate gyrus (area 24) Left posterior cingulate gyrus (area 23) Left insular cortex Right insular cortex Left precuneus (area 7) Left cuneus (area 19) Right cuneus (area 18) Left anterior thalamus (na and dm) Right anterior thalamus (va) Left posterior thalamus 24-32 24 36-44 28-36 20 36—44 44-52 40-48 52-56 44 24 28 16 8-16 48-52 32-36 24 12-16 16 8-12 Coordinates — (x -34 -22 4 28 12 -10 -36 26 36 -44 -8 -10 -6 30 -8 -12 10 -26 10 -4 38 52 48 32 50 44 2 30 12 -2 16 34 14 4 66 80 •82 10 -4 32 Z score z) % Changes in rCBF controls/ALSi 28 24 36 36 20 36 52 40 52 44 24 28 16 8 52 32 24 16 8 8 +3.4/-3.5 +2.11-2.1 —1.1/—6.6 +5.0/-0.4 +0.5/-2.6 +2.27-5.6 +2.8/-4.5 +4.3/-0.2 + 1.9/-3.0 +2.2/-3.3 +5.1/-0.5 +2.4/-3.5 + 1.9/-4.2 +2.3/-2.1 +3.1/-3.4 +2.9/-2.5 + 1.9/-2.8 + 1.7/-2.1 + 3.3/-2.8 +3.2/-2.2 3.83 3.18 3.33 4.17 3.18 3.25 4.35 3.25 3.30 3.70 3.21 3.17 3.12 3.76 3.75 3.46 3.11 3.38 4.03 3.31 % Changes in rCBF denotes the change in rCBF from word repetition to word generation (a positive sign denotes a relative activation and a negative sign a relative deactivation). Talairach and Tournoux coordinates (and corresponding Z score) of the most significant voxel within the region are displayed. See Table 3 legend for abbreviations. BO grtt d / a an al \ 72 V i 1 i— \ -•- 1 ' \ n 32JLil 66 -104 f r 1 VPC VAC * \ \ \ 0 projections \ s 64 SPM f Jy iransvans Fig. 4 Between-group SPM comparing the pattern of activation in ALSu and control groups. Cortical and subcortical regions showing significantly (P < 0.001) impaired activation in the ALSu group in comparison with controls are displayed in black. Left: sagittal, coronal and transverse projections. Right: medial and lateral views of left hemisphere {above); medial and lateral views of right hemisphere {below). due to increases in rCBF in the ALSu group relative to the ALSi group comparing word generation with word repetition {see Table 5). Neuropsychological tests The mean scores (and standard deviations) for the neuropsychological tests are reported in Table 6. The analysis of variance on National Adult Reading Test—Revised, IQ scores revealed no significant differences between the groups {P > 0.05). The 95% confidence intervals for the difference in means between ALSu and ALSi (-8.2, 20.8), controls and ALSu (-8.1, 12.5), controls and ALSi (-5.4, 21.7) are consistent with the results of the ANOVA, demonstrating that the three groups were matched in premorbid IQ. The WCST was measured in terms of four scores: number of categories achieved (maximum of six), total number of errors, trials to first criterion (number of trials taken to Frontal lobe activation in ALS * —< 1 sn \ i f -4 -T 1 -f i-* T V•L V J. -t(M If 2113 VPCVAC SPM / * projections * \ y j fi4 transverse Fig. 5 Between-group SPM comparing the pattern of activation in ALSi and ALSu. Cortical and subcortical regions of significantly (P < 0.001) impaired activation in the ALSi group in comparison with the ALSu group are displayed in black. Left: sagittal, coronal and transverse projections. Right: left hemisphere (above); right hemisphere (below). The locations of the regions displayed are described in Table 5. Table 5 Regions of significant difference (P < 0.001) in activation in the comparison between the ALSu (ALS unimpaired) and ALSi (ALS impaired) groups Region Left DPFC (area 46) Left DPFC (area 46) Right DPFC (areas 44 and 45) Right medial prefrontal cortex (area 9) Right lateral prefrontal cortex (area9) Right premotor cortex (area 8) Left medial premotor cortex (area 8) Left lateral premotor cortex (area 8) Left anterior medial premotor cortex (area 8) Right medial premotor cortex (area 8) Left paracentral lobule Left SMA (paracentral lobule) Right primary motor cortex (area 4) Left primary motor cortex (area 4) Left primary motor cortex (area 4) Right cingulate gyms (area 24) Left anterior cingulate gyrus (area 24) Right insular cortex Left insular cortex Left anterior thalamus (na and dm) Right posterior thaJamus (p) Region (mm above AC-PC) Coordinates 24-28 16 16-20 28 24-28 52-56 52 44-48 40 36-44 48 44-48 52 44-48 32-36 -36 -34 44 24 15 36^40 12 16-20 16 16 12 v) 12 -8 -46 -8 12 -16 -12 34 -46 -50 -10 -8 26 -22 -4 12 36 46 14 6 50 26 32 -6 44 40 •34 14 -4 -6 -8 12 32 16 8 14 •26 Z score •r) % Changes inrCBF ALSu/ALS 28 16 20 28 28 56 52 44 40 40 48 44 52 44 32 40 12 20 16 16 12 +2.6/-4.I +5.5/-1.1 +0.1/—3.9 +0.2/-4.0 + 1.0/-3.1 -0.4/-6.7 -2.0/-7.9 -0.1/-4.8 -2.0/-7.3 -0.5/-4.9 + I.3/-3.5 + 3Z7/-2.6 + 1.1/-4.0 + 3.7/-2.7 +3.0/-1.6 +3.0/-4.1 +2.5/-2.5 + 1.4/-4.0 + 1.9/-3.I + 1.5/-4.7 + 2.6/-3.0 3.52 3.44 3.22 3.13 3.20 3.29 3.10 3.45 3.23 3.26 3.18 3.18 3.17 3.74 3.18 4.02 3.10 3.10 3.11 3.07 3.10 SMA = supplementary motor area; DPFC = dorsal prefrontal cortex. See Table 3 legend for abbreviations. complete the first category) and perseverative errors (where the subject continues to use a previous and irrelevant rule). No significant differences emerged between groups on the category scores or on the total number of errors. The analysis of perseverative errors revealed a trend towards a significant difference in the comparison of the two patient groups [F(1,I3) = 3.84, P = 0.07], with the ALSi patients showing greater perseveration than the ALSu group. However, there was no overall difference between the three groups and the ALSi group were not significantly impaired relative to controls. In the analysis of the number of trials taken to reach the first criterion, a significant difference emerged between the three groups, [F(2,14) = 9.09, P < 0.005], with a clearly significant impairment in the ALSi patients as compared with the ALSu group [F( 1,14) = 14.04, P < 0.005] and controls [ALSi versus controls, F( 1,14) = 14.04, P < 0.005]. This result demonstrated a deficit in the ALSi group in the speed at which they learned to sort the cards and deduce the first rule. Performance on the RMJT was measured in terms of 2114 S. Abrahams et al. Table 6 Neuropsychological test scores Test Controls ALSu ALSi (/> < 0.05) Significant NART predicted full scale IQ Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Categories Total errors Trials to first criterion Perseverative errors (%) Random Movement Joystick Task Three string information statistic Decision time (s) Paired Associate Learning Easy Hard Total Recognition memory Words: raw score Words: age scaled score Faces: raw score Faces: age scaled score Kendrick object learning test total U5.5(7.5)/i = 6 111.3 (10.5)/i = 6 107.0 (14.7) n = 6 No 5.2 (1.0) n = 6 29.5 (10.4) n =6 14.0 (6.4) n = 6 12.9 (2.6) n = 6 4.2(1.8)/i = 6 33.3 (20.1)/i = 6 14.0 (3.7) n = 6 9.5 (3.4) n = 6 4.2 (2.2) n = 6 48.6(21.8)/! = 6 40.0(19.8)/! = 6 15.3 (7.1)/i = 6 No No Yes No 1.10(0.04)/i = 6 0.13 (0.14) n = 6 1.08 (0.12)/i = 4 0.16(0.18)/! = 4 0.95 (0.06) /i = 4 0.50(0.10)/! = 4 11.3(1.2) n = 6 7.2 (2.5) n = 6 18.5 (3.4) n = 6 11.2(1.3)/! = 6 6.5 (3.2) n = 6 17.7 (4.2)/i = 6 10.3 (1.2)/i = 6 2.5 (1.6)/i = 6 12.8 (2.4)/i = 6 47.5(1.6) 12.5(1.8) 40.8 (5.0) 8.0 (4.9) 50.3 (5.4) 47.0 (3.5) n 12.4(1.8)/i 43.8 (4.7) n 11.0 (4.1) n 52.2 (7.7) n 43.5 9.7 43.3 9.8 38.7 n n n n n = = = = = 6 6 6 6 6 = = = = = 5 5 5 5 6 (4.6) (3.5) (3.9) (4.2) (9.8) /i /i /i /i n = = = = = 6 6 6 6 6 Yes Yes No Yes Yes No No No No Yes Means (and standard deviations). NART = National Adult Reading test. 'randomness coefficients'. This was estimated by observing the sequence of 50 moves produced by each subject and comparing their frequency of occurrence with that predicted in a completely random set. The resulting measure of randomness was expressed as an 'information statistic' for strings of one, two, three and four movements (Frith and Done, 1983). The analysis of variance was conducted on each of the four information statistics. The results revealed a significant difference between the three groups on strings of three movements only [F(2,ll) = 6.51, P < 0.O25], with all three contrasts on this measure revealing a significant effect [ALSu + ALSi versus controls F(l,ll) = 5.45, P < 0.05; ALSu versus ALSi F(l,ll) = 7.57, P < 0.025; ALSi versus controls F(l,ll) = 11.97, P < 0.01]. Inspection of the information statistic for strings of three movements (Table 6) demonstrated that the ALSi group was clearly impaired on this measure of frontal lobe function in comparison with the ALSu group and the control group. The ALSu group and controls had comparable scores, despite the significant difference in the comparison of all ALS patients (ALSu + ALSi) and controls. Analysis of the RMJT decision items (time taken to decide in which direction to move the joystick) was also undertaken. This was calculated by comparing response times on the free selection task and a 'control' task, in which the direction of movement was specified by the computer. Analysis of this measure revealed a significant difference between the three groups [F(2,ll) = 9.07, P < 0.005], with all three contrasts demonstrating significant effects [ALSu + ALSi versus controls / r (l,ll) = 7.00, P < 0.025; ALSu versus ALSi F(l,ll) = 11.15, P < 0.01; ALSi versus controls F(l,ll) = 16.34, P < 0.005]. Inspection of the means (Table 6) showed that the ALSi group were impaired in comparison with both the ALSu and control groups. However, for the RMJT results the data were obtained from only eight patients. Overall, these tests of frontal lobe function demonstrated a pattern of deficits in the ALSi group alone, across tasks which included both verbal (VFI) and non-verbal (RMJT) procedures. The deficit found in the WCST was less striking than that on the verbal fluency task, since a significant impairment was revealed only in the number of trials to first criterion. However, when examining the mean scores for all of the WCST measures (Table 6), it was apparent that the patients in the ALSi group did not perform as well controls, despite differences being nonsignificant. With respect to measures of memory, the verbal Paired Associate Learning test revealed no significant differences between groups on the learning of the four 'easy' (semantically related) pairs. However, a significant difference was apparent between groups in learning the four 'hard' (semantically unrelated) pairs [F(2,15) = 5.98, P < 0.025]. The ALSi group showed a significant impairment relative to the ALSu group [F(l,15) =7.51, P < 0.025] and to controls [F(l,15) = 10.23, P < 0.01]. A similar pattern emerged for the total learning scores [F(2,15) = 4.80, P < 0.05], with the ALSi group displaying a significant deficit in comparison with the two other groups [ALSu versus ALSi F(l,15) = 5.98, P < 0.05; controls versus ALSi F(l, 15) = 8.23, P < 0.025]. In the Recognition Memory Test, no differences were found between groups for face recognition using raw or agescaled scores. In the word recognition test, the analysis of the raw scores revealed no significant differences between the three groups (P > 0.1), but a trend towards a significant difference emerged in the comparison of the ALSi group and controls [F(l,14) = 4.02, P = 0.06], Frontal lobe activation in ALS with the ALSi group displaying lower scores. For age scaled scores, a similar pattern was revealed with no evidence of an effect of group although contrasts demonstrated a trend towards a significant difference between the ALSi group and controls [F(l,14) = 3.74, P = 0.07]. The analysis of performance on the Kendrick object learning test was conducted on total scores, as the three groups were closely age-matched. The ANOVA revealed a significant group effect 17(2,15) = 5.21, P < 0.025] with the contrasts demonstrating a significant difference between ALSu and ALSi groups [F(l,15) = 8.86, P < 0.01] and between ALSi patients and controls [F(l,15) = 6.62, P < 0.025], in each case, with poorer performance by the ALSi group. Therefore the above tests revealed evidence of memory impairments in the ALSi group only, with significant deficits on the Kendrick object learning test and Paired Associate Learning Task. Overall, the findings of the neuropsychological assessment revealed a profile of cognitive dysfunction in the ALSi group, with deficits on frontal lobe and memory tasks, while cognitive functions in the ALSu patients appeared to be relatively unaffected. Discussion The findings of this study clearly illustrate that regional brain activation abnormalities, particularly in the frontal association areas, can be detected in a subgroup of ALS subjects, using a paced verbal fluency PET paradigm. Cognitively impaired ALS patients showed impaired activation in DLPFC (left areas 46, 9 and 10; right area 9), lateral and medial premotor cortex (areas 6 and 8) and frontal cortex (areas 8 and 9). They also displayed bilateral impaired activation of the insulae and anterior thalamic nuclei. In contrast, rCBF activation in ALS patients who performed normally on the written verbal fluency task (ALSu) was similar to healthy controls. This pattern of results was revealed despite matched performance between the three groups on the scanning paradigm, hence differences in rCBF activation cannot be associated with verbal output during scanning. Cerebral regions activated by verbal fluency paradigm The task, adapted from Frith et al. (1991a), activated regions of the frontal cortex, particularly the left DLPFC; areas 46 and 9, in a group of healthy subjects. The location of this region of activation was verified in a control subject, through the direct co-registration of rCBF increases onto the MRI scan. Activation of the left DLPFC is consistent with rCBF increases reported in previous studies of internal word generation (Friston et al., 1991; Frith etal., 1991a, b; CantorGraae et al., 1993). In contrast to the study by Frith et al. (1991a), the regions of cerebral activation revealed in the 2115 current task extended beyond the left DLPFC, as bilateral prefrontal rCBF increases were revealed. Such bilateral prefrontal activity has also been demonstrated in other tasks requiring volitional output such as internal generation of paced joystick movements, in freely chosen directions (Deiber et al., 1991) and freely chosen finger movements (Frith et al., 1991a). These nonverbal procedures are similar to verbal fluency in that responses are not wholly specified by the immediate situation and so decision making is central to the tasks. Our current study also revealed rCBF increases in the anterior cingulate gyms (areas 32 and 24) during word generation consistent with previous reports. This region is probably concerned with processes of response selection and attention (Petersen et al, 1988; Frith et al, 1991a). The regions of rCBF increase seen in healthy controls during word generation also encompassed areas known to be involved in language production, including regions of Broca's area (areas 44 and 45 and lateral premotor cortex 6; see Mesulam, 1990). The task was designed to ensure that die speech components of the word repetition and word generation conditions were matched, using a paced procedure in which the subject responded to every stimulus. However, differences may have occurred in the amount of subvocal speech each condition generated. Verbal fluency not only involves intrinsic generation, but also working memory. Letter-based fluency paradigms encourage the processing and manipulation of phonological information, which (in Baddeley's working memory model), is rehearsed through subvocalization within the articulatory loop (Baddeley, 1986). Functional studies have associated subvocal rehearsal and the covert retrieval of words with regions involved in the production of speech sounds, i.e. Broca's area (Wise et al, 1991; Demonet et al, 1992; Hinke et al, 1993; Paulesu et al, 1993). In the word repetition condition, fewer demands are placed on working memory and subvocalization is unnecessary, as the response is fully specified by the stimulus and generated immediately after the stimulus presentation. The paced verbal fluency paradigm employed also produced activation within the superior, inferior and medial aspects of the parietal lobes (areas 7 and 40). Connections between the prefrontal cortex (area 46) and superior parietal lobe have been described by Pandya and Barnes (1987). In addition, activation of the inferior parietal lobe was reported in covert word retrieval paradigms using verb generation (Warburton et al, 1996). Reports have also associated such regions of the parietal cortex to the processing of phonological information (Demonet et al, 1992, 1994; Zatorre et al, 1992). In neuropsychological investigations of brain-damaged patients (Shallice and Vallar, 1990), the left inferior parietal lobule was shown to be associated with the short-term phonological store. This was recently supported by the demonstartion of activation of the supramarginal gyms in a phonological short-term memory task (Paulesu et al. 1993) and is consistent with the intensive connectivity which is found between parietotemporal cortex and Broca's area (Mesulam, 1990). Paulesu et al, (1993) also showed 2116 S. Abrahams et al. association of bilateral increases in the insulae with such phonological processes. A similar pattern of activation was found in the current study. Hence, in the current paradigm, the pattern of rCBF change in healthy controls (in regions other than the DLPFC) may be related to demands on working memory. In orthographic verbal fluency, subvocal rehearsal and the short-term memory of phonological information is required to help cue wordretrieval and keep track of recent responses to avoid repetition (Jones-Gotman and Milner, 1977; Miller, 1984). The present paradigm may more reliably detect such areas of activation associated with verbal fluency, as a greater number of rCBF measurements were acquired (four measurements of each condition, in six subjects) than in previous studies. Cortical and subcortical dysfunction in ALS The results of the current investigation revealed frontal association area dysfunction in those ALS patients with cognitive impairment. Dysfunction of the DLPFC in ALS is consistent with reports from neuropsychological investigations of frontal lobe (executive) type cognitive deficits (Gallassi et al., 1985, 1989; David and Gillham, 1986; Ludolph et al., 1992; Kew et al, 1993ft). Verbal fluency deficits have been frequently related to lesions of the left prefrontal cortex (Benton, 1968; Miceli et al., 1981; Pendleton et al., 1982; Miller, 1984; Bornstein, 1986) and such impairments are repeatedly found in ALS using verbal and non-verbal tasks of the fluency-type (Gallassi et al., 1985, 1989; Ludolph et al., 1992; Kew et al., 1993ft). In addition, the results of our neuropsychological assessment of ALSi patients revealed evidence of executive dysfunction on verbal and nonverbal frontal lobe tests, with deficits on the RMJT and one measure of WCST, i.e. trials to first criterion. The RMJT was based on an activation paradigm reported by Deiber et al. (1991) which was associated with bilaterally increased rCBF in the DLPFC (as described above). The task involves the production of freely chosen sequences of movements and is similar to verbal fluency in terms of intrinsic generation; the ALSi subgroup was impaired in terms of its ability to generate a random string of movements. The neuropsychological profile displayed by the ALSi patients is therefore characterized by deficits in tests of verbal and non-verbal willed actions. Several studies have also reported an association between deficits on the WCST and damage of the DLPFC (Milner, 1963, 1971). The WCST has been employed as a functionally activating task and is also associated with rCBF increases in this region of the prefrontal cortex (Weinberger et al., 1986). Hence, the results of this neuropsychological investigation are in accordance with the pattern of rCBF abnormalities, in that ALS patients with frontal lobe executive-type deficits display corresponding dysfunction of the DLPFC. Cortical abnormalities in non-demented ALS patients have, until recently, not been well documented. However, the finding of frontal lobe involvement is supported by some brain imaging investigations. Ludolph et al. (1992) demonstrated reduced cortical glucose utilization in a group of 18 ALS patients. Neuropsychological assessment, conducted on some of these patients, revealed deficits in verbal and non-verbal fluency tests; however, no impairment was found on the WCST. Talbot et al. (1995) demonstrated pronounced reductions in rCBF in frontal and anterior temporal cortices in non-demented patients with classical ALS using single photon emission computed tomography. These patients showed evidence of subtle frontal lobe dysfunction with a significant deficit on a picture sequencing task, and trends towards reduced verbal fluency and category formation on the WCST. Structural imaging studies have typically shown atrophy restricted to the motor cortex and cortico-spinal tracts in non-demented ALS patients, using CT (Poloni et al., 1986; Gallassi et al., 1989) and MRI (Goodin et al., 1988; Luis et al., 1990; Abe et al., 1993; Ishikawa et al., 1993), with frontal lobe atrophy limited to ALS patients with dementia (Abe et al., 1993). Recently more sensitive analysis techniques using volumetric measurements have revealed atrophy of the underlying subcortical white matter in the anterior frontal cortex, but with no significant frontal cortical atrophy. In addition, in a longitudinal investigation of ALS patients, progressive atrophy of the frontal and temporal regions with serial CT and MRI was reported (Kato et al., 1993). This study demonstrated atrophy involving initially the frontal and anterior temporal lobes and progressing to pre- and then post-central gyrus, anterior cingulate and corpus callosum. Three of the patients studied were also demented; however, the fronto-temporal degeneration was not restricted to these patients. Unfortunately in neither of these structural imaging investigations were the findings related to neuropsychological status. The findings of our current investigation suggest that the deficit in verbal fluency revealed in several neuropsychological investigations is predominantly a result of dysfunction of the DLPFC. The pattern of impaired activation in the ALSi group did not include other regions which may be associated with the working memory component of the task (Broca's area and area 40 of the parietal cortex; see Paulesu et al., 1993). Bilateral abnormalities in rCBF were also revealed in lateral and medial premotor areas when comparing ALSi patients with controls. These regions, however, were located dorsal to those associated with Broca's area. In addition, the comparison of the two patient groups revealed a significant impairment of activation of the supplementary motor area in ALSi patients. In the study of shortterm memory and phonological processing conducted by Paulesu et al. (1993), activation of the supplementary motor area was demonstrated, even though the task was conducted using covert speech. This region may be involved in the motor aspect of vocal or subvocal speech preparation and execution, hence dysfunction could exaggerate the verbal fluency deficit. This pattern of rCBF abnormalities is supported by our previous findings of reduced rCBF in the lateral premotor cortex (area 6) bilaterally and in the supplementary Frontal lobe activation in ALS motor area in resting state scans of ALS patients (Kew et al, 1993a). The comparison of ALSi patients with controls also revealed impaired activation bilaterally in the anterior insular cortex. Unilateral dysfunction in this region has been reported previously in ALS patients, with reduced rCBF at rest, but increased activation during upper limb movement, as compared with healthy controls (Kew et al., 1993a). In the working memory task conducted by Paulesu et al. (1993), the insular cortex was bilaterally activated in control subjects, suggesting that this region is involved in phonological processing. In addition, the insulae also possess specific connections to areas 45 and 46 of the prefrontal cortex (Mesulam and Mufson, 1985). Hence dysfunction of the insular cortex could also contribute to the production of fluency-type deficits in ALS. The results of this investigation also support our previous findings of subcortical involvement in ALS patients with cognitive impairment. In our earlier investigations, impaired activation was revealed in some ALS patients, involving the medial prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate gyrus, parahippocampal gyrus and anterior thalamic nuclear complex (Kew et al., 1993a, b). Attenuation of rCBF along this pathway was associated with the presence of impaired written verbal fluency. This is supported by the present comparison of ALSi patients and controls which revealed reduced rCBF bilaterally in predominantly anterior, but also posterior thalamus. Fibres running through the anterior thalamic nuclei project to the medial prefrontal areas and to the lateral premotor cortices (Aggleton et al., 1986; Bachevalier and Mishkin, 1986; Walsh, 1994). Our findings also suggest impairment of activation of the dorsomedial thalamic nucleus which projects to the prefrontal cortex (Walsh, 1994). However, resolution using PET is not sufficient to determine involvement of specific nuclei. Thalamic degeneration has been shown to be associated with motor neuron disease in a number of investigations. Brownell et al. (1970) reported thalamic gliosis in 53% of 36 cases of non-demented patients with ALS. In addition, subcortical involvement in ALS is corroborated by Ludolph et al. (1992), who noted reduced resting glucose metabolism in the thalamus, although this reduction was not significant. Ludolph et al. (1992) suggested that the findings of cognitive deficits in ALS may be related to degeneration of subcortical structures. Thalamic abnormalities have been associated with severe cognitive changes resulting in dementia in a number of studies (Cambier and Graveleau, 1985; Moossy et al., 1987). Unilateral thalamic damage has been associated with memory loss and frontal signs (Speedie and Heilman, 1982, 1983). Thalamic dysphasia has also been observed with an accompanying verbal adynamia (see Walsh, 1994). This is characterized by a lack of spontaneous speech and is clearly associated with processes of intrinsic generation related to the DLPFC. The reduced thalamic activation and glucose metabolism currently seen may, however, simply reflect reduced afferent input and not intrinsic degeneration of this 2117 nucleus. Indeed, in view of the evidence for extra-motor cortical damage in ALS (Smith, 1960; Okamoto et al, 1992; Kato et al, 1993; Kiernan and Hudson, 1994; Leigh et al, 1994), subcortical degeneration is unlikely to be the dominant cause of neuropsychological impairments, although it is almost certainly contributory. Involvement of the hippocampal system in ALS reported in our previous investigation could not be verified in this study, since the verbal fluency paradigm employed does not activate this region in healthy controls. However, the neuropsychological profile of the ALSi patients also revealed evidence of memory impairments, which is consistent with dysfunction of the limbic system. Deficits were found on the Kendrick Object Learning Test and Paired Associate Learning test, supporting previous reports of impairments on visual recall (Gallassi et al, 1985, 1989; Kew et al, 1993/?) and verbal memory (Gallasi et al, 1985; David and Gillham, 1986; Iwasaki et al, 1990a, b). Pathological changes in limbic structures consisting of ubiquitin immunoreactive inclusions in the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus and associated entorhinal cortex and in the amygdala are present in some non-demented ALS subjects (Okamoto et al, 1991; Wightman et al, 1992; Leigh et al, 1994; Anderson et al, 1996). Altered function in extra-motor cortex in non-demented patients with cognitive impairment, closely parallels the regions most affected in patients with ALS with dementia (Neary et al, 1990; Kew and Leigh, 1992; Wightman et al, 1992; Talbot et al, 1995). The neuropsychological profile of ALS patients with dementia is also characterized by behavioural and cognitive problems reflecting gross frontal lobe dysfunction, with relative preservation of functions of the posterior association cortices (Neary et al, 1990). Behavioural features include disinhibition, apathy, and personality changes (Kew and Leigh, 1992). Neuropsychological test results are difficult to obtain. However, some studies have demonstrated gross deficits in verbal fluency, attention, and tests requiring the ability to shift from one line of thinking to another, similar to the WCST (Peavy et al, 1992; Kato et al, 1994; Talbot et al, 1995). Hence the findings of the current and earlier investigations demonstrates that some non-demented ALS patients display a lesser degree of the cognitive dysfunction found in ALS-dementia patients. This has led to the suggestion that a spectrum of extra-motor cortical and subcortical involvement exists in ALS (Leigh et al, 1994), although an alternative hypothesis is that an ALS subgroup exists, characterized by cognitive impairment. Interestingly ALS-related dementia frequently occurs in conjunction with bulbar pathology, which may be present in up to 85% of cases (Wikstrom et al, 1982; Mitsuyama, 1984; Kew and Leigh, 1992; Talbot et al, 1995). In this study, clinical features were similar in cognitively impaired and unimpaired groups, but there was a tendency for the ALSi group to show greater bulbar involvement than the ALSu patients. In addition there was a slightly higher incidence of pseudobulbar involvement in patients with 2118 S. Abrahams et al. cognitive impairment, suggesting the possibility of a subtle association. Only longitudinal studies will indicate whether the two groups are dissimilar from the outset, or whether subjects in the ALSu group progress to show the same cognitive impairments identified in the ALSi patients. In summary, this investigation has demonstrated that PET activation studies are an effective technique for detecting frontal lobe dysfunction in ALS subjects and has revealed evidence of impaired function in the prefrontal cortex in some non-demented patients with ALS, associated with selective cognitive deficits. It has also provided evidence that dorsolateral and not just medial prefrontal cortex may be impaired in some ALS patients. The PET findings are consistent with dysfunction of thalamo-cortical pathways, as suggested in our previous studies using a motor paradigm (Kew et al., 1993&). Future studies will need to define the temporal sequence of cognitive changes and the pathological correlates of frontal lobe involvement in ALS subjects. These observations will help to identify important phenotypic variations in ALS. Acknowledgements This project was supported by the Medical Research Council of Great Britain, and additional assistance was provided by the Tregaskis Bequest (University of London), the Wellcome Trust and the Guarantors of Brain. References Abe K, Fujimura H, Toyooka K, Hazama T, Hirono N, Yorifuji S, et al. Single-photon emission computed tomographic investigation of patients with motor neuron disease [see comments]. Neurology 1993; 43: 1569-73. Comment in: Neurology 1994; 44: 1186-7. Aggleton JP, Desimone R, Mishkin M. The origin, course, and termination of the hippocampothalamic projections in the macaque. J Comp Neurol 1986; 243: 409-21. Anderson VER, Caims N, Leigh PN. Involvement of the amygdala in motor neurone disease. J Neurol Sci 1996. In press. Bachevalier J, Mishkin M. Visual recognition impairment follows ventromedial but not dorsolateral prefrontal lesions in monkeys. Behav Brain Res 1986; 20: 249-61. Baddeley A. Working memory. Oxford' Clarendon Press, 1986. Benton AL. Differential behavioral effects in frontal lobe disease. Neuropsychologia 1968; 6: 53-60. Cantor-Graae E, Warkentin S, Franzen G, Risberg J. Frontal lobe challenge: a comparison of activation procedures during rCBF measurements in normal subjects Neuropsychiat Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 1993; 6: 83-92. David AS, Gillham RA. Neuropsychological study of motor neuron disease. Psychosomatics 1986; 27: 441-5. Deiber M-P, Passingham RE, Colebatch JG, Friston KJ, Nixon PD, Frackowiak RSJ. Cortical areas and the selection of movement: a study with positron emission tomography. Exp Brain Res 1991; 84: 393-402. Demonet J-F, Chollet F, Ramsay S, Cardebat D, Nespoulous JL, Wise R, et al. The anatomy of phonological and semantic processing in normal subjects. Brain 1992; 115: 1753-68. Demonet J-F, Price C, Wise R, Frackowiak RSJ. Differential activation of right and left posterior sylvian regions by semantic and phonological tasks: a positron-emission tomography study in normal human subjects. Neurosci Lett 1994; 182: 25-8. Friston KJ, Passingham RE, Nutt JG, Heather JD, Sawle GV, Frackowiak RSJ. Localisation in PET images: direct fitting of the intercommissural (AC-PC) line. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 1989; 9: 690-5. Friston KJ, Frith CD, Liddle PF, Dolan RJ, Lammertsma AA, Frackowiak RSJ. The relationship between global and local changes in PET scans. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 1990; 10: 458-66. Friston KJ, Frith CD, Liddle PF, Frackowiak RSJ. Comparing functional (PET) images: the assessment of significant change. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 1991; 11: 690-9. Frith CD, Done DJ. Stereotyped responding by schizophrenic patients on a two-choice guessing task. Psychol Med 1983; 13: 779-86. Frith CD, Fnston K, Liddle PF, Frackowiak RSJ. Willed action and the prefrontal cortex in man: a study with PET. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1991a; 244: 241-6. Fnth CD, Friston KJ, Liddle PF, Frackowiak RSJ. A PET study of word finding. Neuropsychologia 1991b; 29: 1137-48. GaJlassi R, Montagna P, Ciardulli C, Lorusso S, Mussuto V, Stracciari A. Cognitive impairment in motor neuron disease. Acta Neurol Scand 1985; 71: 480-4. Gallassi R, Montagna P, Morreale A, Lorusso S, Tinuper P, Daidone R, et al. Neuropsychological, electroencephalogram and brain computed tomography findings in motor neuron disease. Eur Neurol 1989; 29: 115-20. Goodin DS, Rowley HA, Olney RK. Magnetic resonance imaging in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Ann Neurol 1988; 23: 418-20. Bornstein RA. Contributions of various neuropsychological measures to detection of frontal lobe impairment. Int J Clin Neuropsychol 1986; 3: 18-22. Grant DA. Berg ED. Winconsin Card Sorting Test. Odessa (FL): Psychological Assessment Resources. 1990. Brownell B, Oppenheimer DR, Hughes JT. The central nervous system in motor neurone disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1970; 33: 338-57. Hillel AD, Miller RM, Yorkston K, McDonald E. Norris FH, Konikow N. Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis severity scale. Neuroepidemiology 1989; 8: 142-50. Cambier J, Graveleau T. Thalamic syndromes. In: Vinken PJ, Bruyn GW, Klawans HL, editors. Handbook of clinical neurology, Vol. 45. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1985: 87-98. Hinke RM, Hu X, Stillman AE, Kim S-G, Merkle H, Salmi R, et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of Broca's area during internal speech. Neuroreport 1993; 4: 675-8. Frontal lobe activation in ALS 2119 Hudson AJ. Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis and its association with dementia, parkinsonism and other neurological disorders: a review. [Review]. Brain 1981; 104: 217-47. Mesulam MM, Mufson EJ. The insula of Reil in man and monkey: architectonics, connectivity and function. In: Cerebral cortex, Peters A and Jones EG, editors. New York Press; 1985: 179-226. Ishikawa K, Nagura H, Yokota T, Yamanouchi H. Signal loss in the motor cortex on magnetic resonance images in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Ann Neurol 1993; 33: 218-22. Miceli G, Caltagirone C, Gainotti G, Masullo C, Silveri MC. Neuropsychological correlates of localized cerebral lesions in nonaphasic brain-damaged patients. J Clin Neuropsychol 1981; 3: 53-63. Iwasaki Y, Kinoshita M, Oceda K, Takamiya K, ShiojimaT. Cognitive impairment in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis and its relation to motor disabilities. Acta Neurol Scand 1990; 81: 141-3. Miller E. Verbal fluency as a function of a measure of verbal intelligence and in relation to different types of cerebral pathology. Br J Clin Psychol 1984; 23: 53-7. Jones-Gotman M, Milner B. Design fluency: the invention of nonsense drawings after focal cortical lesions. Neuropsychologia 1977; 15: 653-74. Milner B. Effects of different brain lesions on card sorting. Arch Neurol 1963; 9: 90-100. Kato S, Hayashi H, Yagishita A. Involvement of the frontotemporal lobe and limbic system in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: as assessed by serial computed tomography and magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurol Sci 1993; 116: 52-8. Kato S, Oda M, Hayashi H, Kawato A, Shimizu T. Participation of the limbic system and its associated areas in the dementia of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. J Neurol Sci 1994; 126: 62-9. Kendrick D. Cognitive Tests for the Elderly. Windsor: NFERNelson, 1985. Kew J, Leigh N. Dementia with motor neuron disease. In: Rosser MN, editor. Unusual dementias. Bailliere's clinical neurology, Vol. 1, No. 3. London: Bailhere Tindall, 1992: 611-26. Kew JJM, Leigh PN. Unusual forms of dementia. In: Burns A, Levy R, editors. Dementia. London: Chapman and Hall, 1994: 789-811. Kew JJM, Leigh PN, Playford ED, Passingham RE, Goldstein LH, Frackowiak RSJ, et al. Cortical function in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: a positron emission tomography study. Brain 1993a; 116: 655-80. Kew JJM, Goldstein LH, Leigh PN, Abrahams S, Cosgrave N, Passingham RE, et al. The relationship between abnormalities of cognitive function and cerebral activation in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: a neuropsychological and positron emission tomography study. Brain 1993b; 116: 1399^423. Kiernan JA, Hudson AJ. Frontal lobe atrophy in motor neuron diseases. Brain 1994; 117: 747-57. Leigh PN, Kew JJM, Goldstein LH, Brooks DJ. The cerebral lesions in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: new insights from pathology and functional brain imaging. In: Clifford Rose F, editor. Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis from Charcot to the present and into the future. London: Smith-Gordon, 1994: 191-209. Ludolph AC, Langen KJ, Regard M, Herzog H, Kemper B, Kuwert T, et al. Fronta] lobe function in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: a neuropsychological and positron emission tomography study. Acta Neurol Scand 1992; 85: 81-9. Luis ML, Hormigo A, Mauricio C, Alves MM, Serrao R. Magnetic resonance imaging in motor neuron disease [see comments]. J Neurol 1990; 237: 471^1. Comment in: J Neurol 1992; 239: 112-13. Mesulam MM. Large-scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory. [Review], Ann Neurol 1990; 28: 597-613. Milner B. Interhemispheric differences in the localization of psychological processes in man. [Review]. Br Med Bull 1971; 27: 272-7. Mitsuyama Y. Presenile dementia with motor neuron disease in Japan: clinico-pathological review of 26 cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1984; 47: 953-9. Moossy J, Martinez AJ, Hanin I, Rao G, Yonas H, Boiler F. Thalamic and subcortical gliosis with dementia. Arch Neurol 1987; 44: 510-13. Morita K, Kaiya H, Ikeda T, Namba M. Presenile dementia combined with amyotrophy: a review of 34 Japanese cases. [Review]. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 1987; 6: 263-77. Neary D, Snowden JS, Mann DMA, Northen B, Goulding PJ, Macdermott N. Frontal lobe dementia and motor neuron disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1990; 53: 23-32. Nelson HE, Willison JR. Restandardisation of the NART against the WAIS-R. Windsor NFER-Nelson, 1991. Okamoto K, Hirai S, Yamazaki T, Sun XY, Nakazato Y. New ubiquitin-positive intraneuronal inclusions in the extra-motor cortices in patients with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Neurosci Lett 1991; 129: 233-6. Okamoto K, Murakami N, Kusaka H, Yoshida M, Hashizume Y, Nakazato Y, et al. Ubiquitin-positive transneural inclusions in the extramotor cortices of presenile dementia patients with motor neuron disease. J Neurol 1992; 239: 426-30. Pandya DN, Barnes CL. Architecture and connections of the frontal lobes. In: Perecman E, editor. The frontal lobes revisited. New York: IRBN Press, 1987: 41-71. Paulesu E, Frith CD, Frackowiak RSJ. The neural correlates of the verbal component of working memory [see comments]. Nature 1993; 362: 342-5. Comment in: Nature 1993; 363: 583—4. Peavy GM, Herzog AG, Rubin NP, ' Mesulam M-M. Neuropsychological aspects of dementia of motor neuron disease: a report of two cases. Neurology 1992; 42: 1004-8. Pendleton MG, Heaton RK, Lehman RAW, Hulihan D. Diagnostic utility of the Thurstone Word Fluency Test in neuropsychological evaluations. J Clin Neuropsychol 1982; 4: 307-17. Petersen SE, Fox PT, Posner MI, Mintun M, Raichle ME. Positron emission tomographic studies of the cortical anatomy of singleword processing. Nature 1988; 331: 585-9. Poloni M, Capitani E, Mazzini L, Ceroni M. Neuropsychological 2120 S. Abrahams et al. measures in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis and their relationship with CT scan-assessed cerebral atrophy. Acta Neurol Scand 1986; 74: 257-60. Robb RA, Hanson DP. A software system for interactive and quantitative visualization of multidimensional biomedical images. Australas Phys Eng Sci Med 1991; 14: 9-30. Shallice T, Vallar G. In: Vallar G, Shallice T, editors, Neuropsychological impairments of short-term memory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1990: 11-53. Smith MC. Nerve fibre degeneration in the brain in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1960; 23: 269-82. Speedie LJ, Heilman KM. Amnesic disturbance following infarction of the left dorsomedial nucleus of the thalamus. Neuropsychologia 1982; 20: 597-604. Speedie LJ, Heilman KM. Anterograde memory deficits for visuospatial material after infarction of the right thalamus. Arch Neurol 1983; 40: 183-6. Spinks TJ, Jones T, Gilardi MC, Heather JD. Physical performance of the latest generation of commercial positron scanner. IEEE Trans Nucl Sci 1988; 35: 721-5. Swash M, Leigh N. Criteria for diagnosis of familial amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. [Review]. Neuromuscul Disord 1992; 2: 7-9. Talairach J, Tournoux P. Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. Stuttgart: Thieme, 1988. Warrington EK. Recognition Memory Test. Windsor: NFERNelson, 1984. Watson JDG, Myers R, Frackowiak RSK, Hajnal JV, Woods RP, Mazziotta JC, et al. Area V5 of the human brain: evidence from a combined study using positron emission tomography and magnetic resonance imaging. Cereb Cortex 1993; 3: 79-94. Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation, 1987. Weinberger DR, Berman KF, Zee RF. Physiologic dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia [see comments]. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986; 43: 114-24. Comment in: Arch Gen Psychiatry 1991; 48: 282-3. Wightman G, Anderson VER, Martin J, Swash M, Anderton BH, Neary D, et al. Hippocampal and neocortical ubiquitinimmunoreactive inclusions in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis with dementia. Neurosci Lett 1992; 139: 269-74. Wikstrom J, Paetau A, Palo J, Sulkava R, Haltia M. Classic amyotrophic lateral sclerosis with dementia. Arch Neurol 1982; 39: 681-3. Wise R, Chollet F, Hadar U, Fnston K, Hoffner E, Frackowiak R. Distribution of cortical neural networks involved in word comprehension and word retrieval. Brain 1991; 114: 1803-17. Woods RP, Cherry SR, Mazziotta JC. Rapid automated algorithm for aligning and reslicing PET images. J Comput Assist Tomogr 1992; 16: 620-33. Woods RP, Mazziotta JC, Cherry SR. MRI-PET registration with automated alogrithm. J Comput Assist Tomogr 1993; 17: 536-46. Talbot PR, Goulding PJ, Lloyd JJ, Snowden JS, Neary D, Testa HJ. Inter-relation between 'classic' motor neuron disease and frontotemporal dementia: neuropsychological and single photon emission computed tomography study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1995; 58: 541-7. World Federation of Neurology Research Group on Neuromuscular diseases. El Escorial World Federation of Neurology Criteria for the diagnosis of ALS. J Neurol Sci 1994: 124(Suppl): 96-107. Thurstone LL, Thurstone TG. Primary mental abilities. Chicago: Science Research Associates, 1962. Zatorre RJ, Evans AC, Meyer E, Gjedde A. Lateralization of phonetic and pitch discrimination in speech processing. Science 1992; 256: 846-9. Walsh KW. Neuropsychology: a clinical approach. 3rd ed. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone, 1994. Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1983; 67: 361-70. Warburton E, Wise RJS, Price CJ, Weiller C, Hadar U, Ramsay S, et al. Studies wiuS positron emission tomography of noun and verb retrieval by normal subjects. Brain 1996; 119: 159-79. Received August 14, 1996. Revised June 24, 1996. Accepted July 24, 1996