* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download fulltext
Cognitive neuroscience of music wikipedia , lookup
Neuroeconomics wikipedia , lookup
Neuropsychopharmacology wikipedia , lookup
Eyeblink conditioning wikipedia , lookup
Holonomic brain theory wikipedia , lookup
Collective memory wikipedia , lookup
Visual extinction wikipedia , lookup
Eyewitness memory (child testimony) wikipedia , lookup
Exceptional memory wikipedia , lookup
Affective neuroscience wikipedia , lookup
Socioeconomic status and memory wikipedia , lookup
Prenatal memory wikipedia , lookup
Childhood memory wikipedia , lookup
Misattribution of memory wikipedia , lookup
State-dependent memory wikipedia , lookup
Emotional lateralization wikipedia , lookup
Emotion and memory wikipedia , lookup
Limbic system wikipedia , lookup
Reconstructive memory wikipedia , lookup
Digital Comprehensive Summaries of Uppsala Dissertations from the Faculty of Social Sciences 81 Erasing Fear Effect of Disrupting Fear Memory Reconsolidation on Central and Peripheral Nervous System Activity THOMAS ÅGREN ACTA UNIVERSITATIS UPSALIENSIS UPPSALA 2012 ISSN 1652-9030 ISBN 978-91-554-8455-2 urn:nbn:se:uu:diva-180202 Dissertation presented at Uppsala University to be publicly examined in Universitetshuset, Sal IX, Biskopsgatan 3, Uppsala, Friday, October 12, 2012 at 13:15 for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. The examination will be conducted in Swedish. Abstract Ågren, T. 2012. Erasing Fear: Effect of Disrupting Fear Memory Reconsolidation on Central and Peripheral Nervous System Activity. Acta Universitatis Upsaliensis. Digital Comprehensive Summaries of Uppsala Dissertations from the Faculty of Social Sciences 81. 62 pp. Uppsala. ISBN 978-91-554-8455-2. Fear memories, here defined as learned associations between a stimulus and a physiological fear reaction, are formed through fear conditioning. In animals, fear memories, present in the lateral amygdala, undergo reconsolidation after recall. Moreover, this reconsolidation process can be disrupted both pharmacologically and behaviourally, resulting in a reduced fear response to the stimulus. This thesis examines the attenuation of fear memories by disrupting reconsolidation in humans, using measures of both the central and peripheral nervous system activity. Serotonergic and dopaminergic genes have previously been tied to both fear conditioning and anxiety disorders, where fear conditioning mechanisms are important. In order to evaluate the possible role of fear memory reconsolidation mechanims in the effect on fear and anxiety by these genes, this thesis also compare the reconsolidation disruption effect between different serotonergic and dopaminergic genotypes. Study I examined the attentuation of fear memories by disrupting reconsolidation in humans using reacquisition as a measure of the return of fear. Moreover, study I investigated the impact of differences in serotonergic and dopaminergic alleles on this process. Study II examined the attentuation of fear memories by disrupting reconsolidation in humans using reinstatement as a measure of the return of fear. Study II also investigated the impact of differences in serotonergic and dopaminergic alleles on the process of fear memory reconsolidation. Study III used psychophysiology and fMRI to localize the functional neural activity mediating the fear memory reconsolidation disruption effect. In summary, this thesis provides evidence that fear memories are attenuated by reconsolidation disruption in humans and that serotonergic and dopaminergic alleles influence this process. Moreover, this thesis support that human fear memory reconsolidation is amygdaladependent, suggesting an evolutionary shared memory mechanism. Keywords: Memory reconsolidation; fear conditioning; serotonin; dopamine; polymorphism; gene; exposure therapy; fMRI; amygdala Thomas Ågren, Uppsala University, Department of Psychology, Box 1225, SE-751 42 Uppsala, Sweden. © Thomas Ågren 2012 ISSN 1652-9030 ISBN 978-91-554-8455-2 urn:nbn:se:uu:diva-180202 (http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:uu:diva-180202) List of Papers This thesis is based on the following papers, which are referred to in the text by their Roman numerals. I II III Agren, T., Furmark, T., Eriksson, E., & Fredrikson, M. (2012) Human fear reconsolidation and differences in serotonergic and dopaminergic genes. Translational Psychiatry, 2(2):e76 Agren, T., Furmark, T., Eriksson, E., & Fredrikson, M. Serotonergic and dopaminergic modulation of the prevention of return of fear using reconsolidation. (manuscript in preparation) Agren, T., Engman, J., Frick, A., Björkstrand, J., Larsson, EM., Furmark, T., & Fredrikson, M. Disruption of reconsolidation erases a fear memory trace in the human amygdala. (accepted for publication in Science) Reprints were made with permission from the respective publishers. Contents Abbreviations .................................................................................................. 7 Populärvetenskaplig sammanfattning på svenska ........................................... 9 Introduction ................................................................................................... 11 Fear conditioning .......................................................................................... 12 Spontaneous recovery .......................................................................... 12 Reinstatement ...................................................................................... 13 Renewal ............................................................................................... 13 Reacquisition ....................................................................................... 13 Extinction - new learning vs. unlearning.................................................. 13 Neurobiology of fear conditioning ................................................................ 15 Consolidation and reconsolidation ................................................................ 18 Consolidation ........................................................................................... 18 Reconsolidation. ....................................................................................... 20 Brain imaging of fear conditioning ............................................................... 23 Acquisition ............................................................................................... 23 Extinction ................................................................................................. 24 Extinction recall ....................................................................................... 25 Anxiety disorders and fear conditioning ....................................................... 27 Anxiety disorders and consolidation/reconsolidation ................................... 29 Genetics of fear conditioning ........................................................................ 30 5-HTTLPR ............................................................................................... 30 COMT ...................................................................................................... 31 Methods ........................................................................................................ 33 Psychophysiology- skin conductance ....................................................... 33 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI).................................... 34 Aim ............................................................................................................... 35 Study I: .......................................................................................................... 36 Aim and background ................................................................................ 36 Methods .................................................................................................... 36 Results ...................................................................................................... 37 Discussion ................................................................................................ 37 Study II: ........................................................................................................ 38 Aim and background ................................................................................ 38 Methods .................................................................................................... 38 Results ...................................................................................................... 39 Discussion ................................................................................................ 39 Study III: ....................................................................................................... 40 Aim and background ................................................................................ 40 Methods .................................................................................................... 40 Results ...................................................................................................... 41 Discussion ................................................................................................ 41 Results overview (study I, II & III)............................................................... 42 General Discussion ....................................................................................... 47 Main findings ........................................................................................... 47 Discussion ................................................................................................ 47 Limitations ............................................................................................... 50 Future directions ....................................................................................... 51 Acknowledgment .......................................................................................... 53 References ..................................................................................................... 54 Abbreviations 5-HTTLPR ACC BLA BOLD Ca CBT CE COMT CS CS+ CSDA EDA EPI GABA LA LTP NE NMDA NS MAPk OCD PET PTSD REM SCR SSRI UCS vlPAG vmPFC Serotonin transporter gene linked polymorphic region Anterior cingulate cortex Basolateral Amygdala Blood oxygen-level dependent Calcium Cognitive behaviour therapy Central amygdala Catechol-O-methyltransferase Conditioned stimulus Conditioned stimulus, reinforced Conditioned stimulus, not reinforced Dopamine Electrodermal activity Echo planar imaging Gamma-Aminobutyric acid Lateral Amygdala Long term potentiation Norepinephrine N-methyl d-aspartate Neutral stimulus Mitogenactivated protein kinase Obsessive compulsive disorder Positron emission tomography Post-traumatic stress syndrome Rapid eye movement Skin conductance response Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor Unconditioned stimulus ventrolateral periaqueductal grey ventromedial prefreontal cortex Populärvetenskaplig sammanfattning på svenska Rädslominnen har en central roll i alla ångeststörningar. Vid postraumatiskt stressyndrom kan t ex traumatiska minnen ge upphov till ångest och hos patienter med specifik fobi framkallar minnen av bestämda föremål eller situationer rädsla. Det går att skapa rädslominnen i experiment, genom att para ett neutralt med ett obehagligt stimulus. Sådan rädslobetingning kan användas för att studera under vilka förhållanden vi skapar, upprätthåller och förlorar minnen. Man talar förenklat om att vi först lagrar en upplevelse i korttidsminnet, som försvinner ganska snabbt, och efter en tid kodar den i ett bestående långtidsminne. Den process som gör minnen bestående över tid kallas för konsolidering. Redan sedan 1800-talet har det varit känt att patienter med skallskador ibland drabbas av minnesförlust av vad som hände några timmar innan skadan. Man tänker sig att anledningen till minnesförlusten är att konsolideringsprocessen blivit störd och därmed förhindrat att upplevelsen lagrats i långtidsminnet. Ett sätt att utnyttja detta i behandling av personer som varit med om traumatiska upplevelser vore att försöka störa ut konsolideringen av rädslan som är förknippad med händelsen för att på så sätt göra ett traumatiskt minne mer neutralt Då man drar sig något till minnes blir minnet återigen instabilt och genomgår en ny inkodningsprocess kallad rekonsolidering. Vi kan alltså efter att vi har framkallat ett minne under en period påverka det minnet innan det åter hamnar i långtidsminnet. Ett exempel på detta kan vara samtalsterapi där terapeuten får patienten att minnas något och sedan försöker påverka känsloinnehållet i minnet som sedan kodas om som neutralt innan det åter lagras i långtidsminnet. Den här avhandlingen har, med hjälp av experimentella rädslominnen, undersökt hur återbildning av minnen kan påverkas. Studierna visar att det är möjligt att påverka rädsloinnehållet i minnen genom att störa minnets återbildande. Genvarianter som styr serotonin och dopamin i hjärnan och är viktiga vid ångest har här relaterats till hur rädslominnen bildas och glöms bort. I en studie användes hjärnavbildning för att lokalisera var i hjärnan de funktionella förändringar sker, som leder till att rädslan minskar. I likhet med studier på andra djur framstod amygdala som en funktionellt central punkt i 9 nätverket som upprätthåller rädsla. Det nya är att även människor kan omforma rädslominnen till neutrala minnen och att rädslominnet i amygdala då försvinner. Rekonsolidering av rädslominnen är alltså en evolutionärt bevarad minnesmekanism som vi delar med många arter. 10 Introduction Fear memories cause problems for people suffering from situationally elicited fear reactions like phobias and post traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Hence, it is possible that individual differences in the encoding and retrieval processes of fear memories are part of the etiology of these disorders. Memories are made stable and permanent through a process called consolidation that stretches on for hours after the initial encoding. Consolidation is dependent on protein synthesis in building the endurable structures of memories. When a memory is recalled it is again rendered unstable and becomes the subject of a new stabilization process termed reconsolidation, also dependent on protein synthesis. During both the consolidation and reconsolidation processes some memories can be modified by the disruption of these processes. Fear memories, here defined as a learned association between a stimulus and a physiological fear reaction, can be formed through fear conditioning. In animals, these fear memories, present in the lateral amygdala, undergo reconsolidation after recall. Moreover, this reconsolidation process can be disrupted both pharmacologically and behaviourally, resulting in a reduced fear response to the stimulus. This attenuation of fear memories by means of disrupted reconsolidation has recently been demonstrated in humans and may represent a novel treatment strategy for fear and anxiety. This thesis examines the attenuation of fear memories in humans by means of reconsolidation disruption, using measures of both the central and peripheral nervous system. Also, a repeated cycle of recall and reconsolidation of fear memories represent a kind of fear memory maintenance. It is conceivable that individual differences in fear memory maintenance present a vulnerability trait for anxiety disorders. Therefore this thesis also compare the reconsolidation disruption effect between different genotypes of serotonergic and dopaminergic genes previously associated with both fear conditioning, which may be etiologically relevant in situationally elicited anxiety disorders, and anxiety. 11 Fear conditioning Conditioning is a process in which a neutral stimulus (NS), through pairing with a stimulus that elicits a physiological reaction (Unconditioned Stimuli; UCS), becomes a conditioned stimulus (CS) that in itself produces the physiological reaction. The process of conditioning was first reported by Ivan Pavlov in 1907 and is sometimes simply called Pavlovian conditioning (Pavlov, 1927). When the UCS involved elicits a physiological fear reaction, the process is called fear conditioning. Fear conditioning enables an organism to identify and remember what is dangerous and should be avoided, thus representing a crucial mechanism for an organism’s survival. The learning of an association between a CS and a UCS is called acquisition. Specifically, acquisition is the process in which a fear reaction to a CS is learned and a fear memory is formed. In single cue conditioning, a single NS is transformed to a CS. In differential conditioning, two CS are used, one that is paired with UCS and one that is not. The one that is paired with UCS is called CS+ and the other one, used as control for CS+, is called CS-. The use of CS- enables researchers to rule out the possibility that a subjects fear reactions are unspecific and not solely caused by the presence of CS+. A learned fear reaction to a CS can be reduced using extinction, a process in which the CS is repeatedly shown without pairing with the UCS. This leads to a weakened fear response to the CS and with a sufficient amount of presentations, no fear response at all. However, extinction does not permanently erase the fear reactions to a CS, because it is known that fear can return after extinction. Listed below are circumstances in which return of fear has been noted and can be measured. Spontaneous recovery Since Pavlov’s seminal work (Pavlov, 1927), it has been known that the fear reaction associated with a CS returns, simply with the passage of time. Spontaneous recovery of fear is tested by presenting the subject with the CS after a suitable passage of time and measure the fear reaction. 12 Reinstatement Reinstatement refers to the return of fear to a CS that occurs if subjects are presented with unpaired presentations of the UCS, for example if a subject who has acquired a fear association between a stimulus and shock is presented with only the shock. This has the ability to somehow strengthen or activate the dormant fear memory. In experiments using reinstatement, unpaired UCS presentations are presented before, or intermixed with CS presentations and the return of fear is measured by the reactions to the CS (Rescorla & Heth, 1975). Renewal Renewal refers to the return of fear to a CS that occurs if subjects are moved to a new context, different from the one in which acquisition or extinction took place. For example, if a subject has acquired a fear reaction to a CS and then had it extinguished in a certain room, the fear can return when presenting the CS in another room. The return of fear is measured by the reactions to the CS in the new context. Reacquisition Reacquisition is the relearning of an already learned association of a CS to UCS. It is noted that an acquired and extinguished fear association is relearned faster than the original acquisition, probably as a result of a remaining memory of the original learning (Bouton, 2002). Using reacquisition, return of fear is measured as the readiness for relearning, examining how lingering fear memories may affect future vulnerability to fear renewal (Kehoe & Macrae, 1998). Extinction - new learning vs. unlearning The return of fear observed in spontaneous recovery, reinstatement, renewal and reacquisition, strongly suggest that extinction does not completely remove a learned fear association, but simply inhibits it for a period of time. This is why it is generally believed that extinction is based on new learning consisting of an association between CS and the absence of UCS (Bouton, 2002; Myers & Davis, 2007). This new association will represent a kind of ‘safety memory’, signaling that the CS will be followed by the absence of UCS. This memory is thought to compete with the original fear memory that signals that CS will be followed by UCS. In these terms, spontaneous recovery is explained as follows: After extinction the safety memory dominates, but the fear memory will remain even after the safety memory fade, possibly 13 because the safety memory is more fragile (Pavlov, 1927) or context specific (Bouton, 2002). A competing theory to the one claiming new learning of a ‘safety memory’ is that the original association is simply unlearned. The main weakness of this theory is that it can’t easily account for the phenomena of ‘return of fear’ described above. However, recent advances in both neurobiological and behaviour studies provide evidence for such an unlearning mechanism (Kwapis, Jarome, Schiff, & Helmstetter, 2011; Monfils, Cowansage, Klann, & LeDoux, 2009; Myers, Ressler, & Davis, 2006; Schiller et al., 2010), which is the subject of study in this thesis, and suggests that extinction may consist of both unlearning and new learning (Pape & Pare, 2010). 14 Neurobiology of fear conditioning The literature concerning the neurobiology of fear conditioning is dominated by the amygdala, an almond shaped structure in the limbic cortex. Amygdala was tied to fear behaviour in the 1950s when it was established that amygdala lesions were crucial for the disappearance of several fear behaviours in monkeys (Weiskrantz, 1956) previously reported after temporal lobe lesions (Klüver & Bucy, 1939). Following this discovery, the dependence of amygdala in different inhibitory avoidance tasks, tasks in which an animal learns to avoid something that is paired with an aversive stimuli, were explored in several species of animals (Horvath, 1963; Robinson, 1963). In 1972, it was established that amygdala was necessary for the acquisition of fear conditioning, demonstrating that amygdala not only was vital for fear expression, but also for fear learning (Blanchard & Blanchard, 1972). Finally, in the 1990s it was shown in patients with lesions of the amygdala that also in humans, amygdala is a necessary structure for acquiring fear conditioning (Bechara et al., 1995; LaBar, LeDoux, Spencer, & Phelps, 1995). Furthermore, brain imaging has demonstrated amygdala activations to emotional stimuli, such as facial expressions of fear (Morris, Frith, Perrett, & Rowland, 1996), as well as fear conditioning (LaBar, Gatenby, Gore, LeDoux, & Phelps, 1998). In more detail, transmission of sensory information converges in the amygdala, particularly the lateral amygdala (LA), where inputs from auditory cortex, auditory thalamus, thalamic areas that receive information from the spinal-thalamic tract, cortical areas processing somatosensory stimuli, olfactory cortex and visual cortex provide all the information needed for associations between CS and UCS to be formed (LeDoux, 2000; Maren, 2001; McDonald, 1998). Lesions of LA as well as pharmacological inactivation of LA neurons prevent fear acquisition. Moreover, single cell recordings show that LA neurons fire in response to both CS and US stimuli, demonstrating that an association is present in the LA. For reviews, see (Johansen, Cain, Ostroff, & LeDoux, 2011; Maren, 2001). From amygdala, particularly from the central nucleus (CE) of amygdala, projections go to brainstem areas controlling expression of fear responses. These connections, together with the fact that lesions of the central nucleus prevent fear responses, make a strong case that the CE is necessary for fear responses. CE receives input from the LA and is generally believed to mediate and execute fear responses learned 15 in the LA (LeDoux, 2000; Maren, 2001). However, recent research argues that its role in fear conditioning also involves learning (Wilensky, Schafe, Kristensen, & LeDoux, 2006). On a molecular level, Hebbian mechanisms, mechanisms in which synaptic activity between two neurons increase the strength of the coupling between those two neurons, are important for fear learning in the LA. Important molecular functions for Hebbian mechanisms in the amygdala with a direct impact on fear conditioning are NMDA-type ionotropic glutamate receptors; a receptor known to detect pre- and postsynaptic synchronized depolarization, autophosphorylation of Ca2+/calmudulin-dependent protein kinase II; a process vital for memory formation in several types of learning, and inhibitory GABA-ergic neurotransmission (Johansen et al., 2011). Moreover, there is evidence that monoamine transmitters released in emotional situations, for example norepinephrine (NE) and dopamine (DA), regulate glutamatergic transmission and Hebbian plasticity (Rosenkranz & Grace, 1999; Tully, Li, Tsvetkov, & Bolshakov, 2007; de Oliveira et al., 2011). In addition to the above direct evidence of memory formation in LA there is also much indirect evidence simply showing that long term potentiation (LTP), an important function in forming memories, is taking place in LA (Clugnet & LeDoux, 1990; Maren, Fanselow, & Angeles, 1995; Rogan & LeDoux, 1995). In conclusion, there is much evidence that acquisition of fear conditioning takes place within the amygdala. The neurobiological underpinnings of extinction training are not as wellknown as those involved in acquisition. It is likely that extinction memory is distributed among a network of structures where extinction-related plasticity in different regions have different functions (Quirk & Mueller, 2008). As with acquisition, the amygdala is implicated also in extinction learning. Blocking mitogenactivated protein kinase (MAPk) activity, a way of measuring signaling activity, in basolateral amygdala (BLA) completely prevented extinction learning. Also, a higher level of MAPk activity was noted in BLA during extinction training. Moreover, blockade of glutamate receptors in BLA impair extinction learning (Pape & Pare, 2010; Quirk & Mueller, 2008). In addition to amygdala, blocking opioid receptors in the ventrolateral periaqueductal gray (vlPAG), a midbrain structure important for fear expression (De Oca, DeCola, Maren, & Fanselow, 1998) prevents extinction learning, implicating this structure in extinction (De Oca et al., 1998). There is much evidence that extinction is context-dependent and that this is the reason for the renewal effect (Bouton, 2002). It is generally believed that this context-dependency relies on hippocampus and its connection to amygdala (Kim & Fanselow, 1992; Phillips & LeDoux, 1992). Finally, the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) is often implicated in extinction train16 ing; albeit most in the consolidation of extinction training (consolidation will be discussed in the next section). Lesions of the vmPFC, as well as pharmacological inhibition of vmPFC activity, either by blocking glutamate or NE activity or by blocking protein synthesis, do not hinder extinction training, but impairs retrieval of extinction on following days (Quirk & Mueller, 2008). The vmPFC is also implicated in human brain imaging of extinction training (see Brain imaging of fear conditioning) providing evidence for at least a partly evolutionary shared mechanism of extinction in the mammalian brain. In conclusion, both fear and extinction learning appear to be amygdala dependent, but extinction is also dependent on other areas that modulate the fear response. 17 Consolidation and reconsolidation Consolidation Already in the 1800s it was noted that brain injuries could lead to a loss of recent memories. To explain these findings, it was proposed that memories became stable over time, that they consolidated, and that injuries to the brain would disrupt ongoing consolidation and hence, erase memories acquired shortly before the injury. In 1949, this kind of retrograde amnesia was experimentally induced in rats using electroconvulsive shocks (Duncan, 1949). These findings led to the development of a dual-trace memory theory in which one neural system or process was thought to underlie short term memory and that short term memory subsequently was transformed by consolidation to long term memory, possibly relying on another neural system or process (Hebb, 1949). Today, consolidation is a well-accepted concept, referring to a time-dependent stabilization process that fixates short term memory into a more permanent one. A wealth of research during the last 40 years have yielded much knowledge of the neurobiological underpinnings of consolidation, but it is still the target of intense study (McGaugh, 2000). Why is a consolidation process necessary when we know the brain almost instantly can form short term memory? It is argued that the consolidation process adds an adaptive function by enabling other systems to modulate the strength and character of the memory. For instance, adrenal stress hormones released in emotionally arousing situations facilitate long term memory by affecting consolidation (Liang, Juler, & McGaugh, 1986). There is a lot of evidence that this modulation is largely depending on the basolateral amygdala (BLA). Amygdala lesions block the effect of well-known modulators of consolidation, like the adrenal stress hormones. Moreover, blocking the activity of adrenal stress hormones in the BLA also block the modulating effect adrenal stress hormones have on consolidation. However, the BLA interacts with other brain systems to perform this modulation. For example, lesions of stria terminalis, a bundle of fibers containing many of the projections from amygdala, also block the modulating effects of intra-amygdala drug infusions (McGaugh, 2004). Moreover, consolidation following fear conditioning activates synaptic plasticity in brain areas other than the amygdala, notably, the hippocampus, striatum and cortex (Ressler, Paschall, Zhou, & Davis, 2002). Why would fear conditioning induce synaptic plasticity in 18 several brain areas if the memory is located in LA? The answer could be that LA stores a certain aspect of memory, the arousal components, and that other components, for example context memory or episodic memory, are situated elsewhere, and that all components together form a complete memory representation. Memory representations are in general thought to be dependent on long term potentiation (LTP). LTP, in short, is a mechanism by which increased firing between two neurons induces a long lasting enhancement in signal transmission between the active neurons. LTP, corresponding with the notion of memory consolidation, also have different temporal phases. There is an early phase of LTP, that lasts for minutes, and a late phase, that depend on protein synthesis, that can last for hours (Milner, Squire, & Kandel, 1998). As previously noted, rodent studies strongly suggest that the locus for fear conditioning memory is the lateral amygdala (LA) (see Neurobiology of fear conditioning). Consistently, inputs to amygdala, crucial for fear conditioning, induces LTP in the LA, further implicating the LA as a the site for fear conditioning memory, and if late phase LTP is tied to consolidation, also fear memory consolidation (Clugnet & LeDoux, 1990; Maren et al., 1995; Rogan & LeDoux, 1995). In animals, memories induced by fear conditioning have successfully been attenuated by disrupting consolidation following fear learning. This has been done both pharmacologically by preventing protein synthesis in the amygdala (Kwapis et al., 2011; Wilensky et al., 2006) and behaviourally by use of extinction training (Myers et al., 2006). Studies using protein synthesis inhibition suggest that the consolidation interval during which disruption is possible roughly starts a few minutes after training, the time it takes to synthesize new protein and transport it to a location where it can affect interneuronal communication, and ends around 3-6 hours after training. Also, protein synthesis inhibition must be initiated within 90 min after training to make an impact on memory retention, suggesting that consolidation disruption initiated later than 90 min after training probably stem from the disruption of other mechanisms than protein synthesis (Davis & Squire, 1984), for example the modulation of consolidation by adrenal stress hormones mentioned above. The pharmacological manipulations used on animals to disrupt consolidation are not safe for humans and the disruption of consolidation using behavioural manipulations have not been readily replicated in humans (Huff, Hernandez, Blanding, & LaBar, 2009; Norrholm et al., 2008). Still, consolidation of episodic memories are strengthened by arousal (Cahill & Alkire, 2003; Cahill, Gorski, & Le, 2003) and diminished by activity similar in character to the memory (Holmes, 19 James, Kilford, & Deeprose, 2010), suggesting that it may be possible to use consolidation disruption as a novel procedure in reducing fear. Reconsolidation. After recall, fear memories appear to again become destabilized and the target of a renewed consolidation process termed reconsolidation. This phenomenon was noted in rodent research in the late 1960s when a fear conditioned memory was reduced by inducing retrograde amnesia with an electroconvulsive shock during the reconsolidation interval after a fear memory reminder (Misanin, Miller, & Lewis, 1968). Several interesting studies examined what kind of cue that was needed for reactivation and during what circumstances reactivation of a memory occurred (Spear, Lewis, Mcgaugh, & Ralph, 1973). In essence, a memory can be reactivated by a presentation of at least one element of it, for example the stimuli itself, the context in which the stimuli was presented, or even an internal context like that of arousal. During the reconsolidation interval, but not after, memories can be enhanced (Rodriguez, Horne, & Padilla, 1999), impaired (Nader, Schafe, & Le Doux, 2000) or interfered with conflicting learning (Spear et al., 1973). Moreover, a reactivation with a following undisturbed reconsolidation seems to strengthen the memory (Inda, Muravieva, & Alberini, 2011). The term reconsolidation suggests that the process would consist of a second consolidation. However, research show that for certain memories, different brain areas are involved in consolidation and reconsolidation, although both processes seem to use the same molecules and mechanisms (Alberini, 2005). Because of the initial belief that reconsolidation was the same process as consolidation, research on reconsolidation have considered the reconsolidation interval to have the same duration as the consolidation interval. Considering that the neurobiological mechanisms underlying consolidation and reconsolidation seem to be the same, this may be true, but it is up to future research to examine possible differences in the duration of the reconsolidation interval and the consolidation interval. Fear conditioning is a well suited paradigm in which to study retrograde amnesia induced by reconsolidation disruption. The neural mechanisms of fear conditioning are well known in comparison with other types of learning and it has clinical relevance suggesting novel treatments for anxiety disorders (see section Anxiety disorders and consolidation). In rodents, fear memories can be reduced by disrupting the reconsolidation process following a fear memory reminder. Like consolidation, reconsolidation of fear conditioning memories has been tied to the BLA. Pharmacologically, reconsolidation processes can be disrupted, and thus, fear memories attenuated, by 20 inhibiting protein synthesis in BLA with anisomycin (Nader et al., 2000), possibly reflecting the late phase LTP dependency on protein synthesis. Moreover, blocking noradrenergic activity in the amygdala disrupt reconsolidation (Debiec & Ledoux, 2004) and enhancing noradrenergic activity in the amygdala enhances the reconsolidation process and lead to a stronger fear memory (Dębiec, Bush, & LeDoux, 2011). Recently, fear memories were successfully reduced using behavioural means, that is, by using extinction training during the reconsolidation interval (Monfils et al., 2009). New conflicting learning had been used for reconsolidation disruption earlier, but not by using extinction training (Gordon, Spear, 1973). Also in humans, recent studies report successfully modulated memories by disrupting reconsolidation. The first study to show an effect of disrupted reconsolidation in humans used a procedural finger-tapping task (Walker, Brakefield, & Hobson, 2003), but fear conditioned memories have also been successfully modulated. Kindt et al. (2009) used propranolol, a drug that reduces noradrenergic activity in both the peripheral and the central nervous system, to disrupt consolidation of human fear conditioning memory (Kindt, Soeter, & Vervliet, 2009). The same research group also demonstrated that enhancement of noradrenergic activity during reconsolidation enhanced the strength of the memory, although only for startle responses and not for skin conductance responses (Soeter & Kindt, 2011). In the same vein as the research on rodents by Monfils et al. (2009), fear conditioning reconsolidation has also been successfully disrupted by extinction training. In a study by Schiller et al. (2010), subjects underwent fear conditioning to induce an experimental fear memory. Twenty-four hours after the initial training, they received a fear memory reminder consisting of a presentation of the CS. One group of subjects then received extinction training 10 min later, hence during the reconsolidation interval, and one group received extinction training 6 hrs later, after reconsolidation was completed. Spontaneous recovery was tested another twenty-four hrs later using a second session of extinction. Return of fear was noted in the group that received extinction training outside of the reconsolidation interval and hence, after completed reconsolidation, while no return of fear was noted in the group which received reconsolidation disruption, that is extinction training during the reconsolidation interval (Schiller et al., 2010). Not all memories seem to be open for modulation through manipulation of the reconsolidation process. In the 1970s, Electroconvulsive therapy was used in an attempt to disrupt the reconsolidation of several declarative memories after a memory reminder, but no effect could be noted (Squire, Slater, & Chace, 1976). The authors of the study suggest that the memories that can be reduced with reconsolidation disruption may have to contain an element of arousal, since all rodent studies that successfully produced the phenome21 non had used fear or hunger memories. However, the reconsolidation disruption effect have also been produced using procedural memories with no apparent element of arousal (Walker et al., 2003). In conclusion, the disruption of consolidation and reconsolidation processes show some promise into attenuating the arousal component of memories, but without removing the episodic memory component of the events. 22 Brain imaging of fear conditioning Acquisition The first fear conditioning imaging studies was published in the middle of the 90s and used PET. The first study to image acquisition with the contrast CS+>CS- found heightened activity in the pulvinar and the thalamus (Morris, Friston, & Dolan, 1997). Amygdala was implicated only as an area that was positively correlated to the pulvinar activity. The advent of eventrelated fMRI studies introduced a way to image and compare the same time intervals of CS+ and CS- presentations that were traditionally analysed in psychophysiology. The two first event-related fMRI studies using the contrast CS+>CS- both noted stronger BOLD signal to CS+ in amygdala and the anterior cingulate cortex. Moreover, both studies noted that reactions to CS+ diminished over time (Büchel, Morris, Dolan, & Friston, 1998; LaBar et al., 1998). Since then, over fifty imaging studies of fear acquisition have been made and the gathered knowledge is starting to reveal a pattern. A recent review (Sehlmeyer et al., 2009) reports that the most common finding in fear acquisition is the amygdala, the ACC and the insular cortex. Drawing from rodent research on the neurobiological underpinnings of fear conditioning, amygdala is vital for acquisition (see Neurobiology of fear conditioning) which makes it a primary target for imaging of fear conditioning. However, fear memory activity in amygdala is no easy target for imaging. Animal experiments, using single cell recordings, note that the associative memory in amygdala is stored in a small number of sparsely distributed neurons which make them hard to register with neuroimaging methods (Bach, Weiskopf, & Dolan, 2011; Reijmers, Perkins, Matsuo, & Mayford, 2007). However, new methods of imaging and analyses are stepping up to the task (Bach et al., 2011; Visser, Scholte, & Kindt, 2011). In contrast to the amygdala, the function of the ACC and the insular cortex in fear conditioning are less known. The anterior cingulate cortex is known to be involved in attentional processes of emotional stimuli and may reflect the attention to the CS as an emotional stimuli (Lane, Fink, Chau, & Dolan, 1997). Both the anterior cingulate cortex and the insular cortex are known to be involved in control of cardiovascular (Oppenheimer, Gelb, Girvin, & Hachinsky, 1992) and electrodermal responses (Critchley, 2002), so their activation may reflect a control over activations of the sympathetic nervous 23 system. Moreover, both areas have connections to the amygdala and may modulate fear learning and expression. It is a task for combined connectivity and activation studies of fear conditioning to further substantiate the function of all implicated areas of the brain. Extinction Extinction is not nearly as common in imaging studies as acquisition. In spite of this, the very first imaging studies of fear conditioning were of extinction. These early studies, using PET, reported activations in the thalamus, hypothalamus and midbrain (Fredrikson, Wik, Fischer, & Andersson, 1995) consistent with the presence of fear reactions, as well as mainly rightsided activations in frontal and temporal cortices (Hugdahl et al., 1995). The first event-related fMRI study revealed amygdala activity, predominantly on the right side, along with activations in prefrontal cortex. Amygdala activity was notable only in the early phase of extinction and habituated over time (LaBar et al., 1998). Since then, studies have noted increased amygdala activity (Barrett & Armony, 2009; Gottfried & Dolan, 2004; Knight, Smith, Cheng, Stein, & Helmstetter, 2004; Milad et al., 2007), decreased amygdala activity (Phelps, Delgado, Nearing, & LeDoux, 2004) and no change in amygdala activity (Delgado, Nearing, Ledoux, & Phelps, 2008; Kalisch et al., 2006; Lang et al., 2009; Spoormaker et al., 2011; Yágüez et al., 2005). Research into the neurobiology of fear conditioning suggest that fear extinction is dependent on frontal areas to inhibit the fear response generated by the amygdala. Implicated from rodent research are especially the vmPFC (Milad & Quirk, 2002), although the translation from rodent brain areas to human brain areas is not trivial making the implications from rodent studies on human anatomy uncertain. Consistent with this theory the vmPFC has been implicated in several studies of extinction. Activity in the vmPFC have been found to be negatively correlated to SCRs reflecting extinction (Phelps et al., 2004) and to reflect decreasing aversive value of the CS+ (Gottfried & Dolan, 2004). Both the ACC (Barrett & Armony, 2009; Lang et al., 2009; Phelps et al., 2004; Spoormaker et al., 2011) and the insular cortex (Delgado et al., 2008; Gottfried & Dolan, 2004; Phelps et al., 2004; Spoormaker et al., 2011; Yágüez et al., 2005) are also frequently found activated during extinction. They both lack the evidence, as compared to the vmPFC, to be considered as inhibiting areas, but reflect perhaps attentional processes, cortical control over the sympathetic nervous system, or nodes in a distributed memory or control system in the human fear circuitry. These results taken together, imply that fear extinction involves a complex network of areas in humans, per24 haps reflecting the complexity of our cognitive abilities. Only two studies noted involvement of hippocampus (Knight et al., 2004; Phelps et al., 2004), a structure frequently implied in memory research (Eichenbaum, 1999). This implies that fear conditioning may be hippocampus independent, at least as long as it doesn’t involve contextual cues (Phillips & LeDoux, 1992). One could argue that there is reason to differentiate between extinction of fear memories that had a complete consolidation after acquisition and fear memories that were extinguished immediately upon acquisition. In the latter case, consolidation of acquisition and extinction training is simultaneous which may affect subsequent extinction, return of fear and extinction recall. The only study, to this authors knowledge, of brain correlates of extinction of fear memories that consolidated before the first extinction session found activations in thalamus, middle cingulate cortex and bilateral insular cortex (Spoormaker et al., 2011). The only indication of involvement of the amygdala was a group×stimulus interaction comparing two conditioned CS, one that was extinguished and one that was not extinguished, between REM sleep deprived and controls. Sleep deprived had “more” activity in the right amygdala as compared to controls. Extinction recall To study the lingering effect of extinction some time after the extinction training is called extinction recall. The term ‘extinction recall’ implies that a new ‘safety memory’ is learned during extinction and that this ‘safety memory’ can be recalled at a later time. Another way of putting it is that extinction recall studies the return of fear, possibly inhibited by a memory from the preceding extinction. There are both similarities and differences in findings between studies of extinction and extinction recall. As for extinction, extinction recall studies note activity in the vmPFC (Kalisch et al., 2006; Milad et al., 2007). Moreover, correlations of both vmPFC activity and vmPFC thickness with SCRs reflecting return of fear have been reported (Milad et al., 2005, 2007). Also, as in both acquisition and extinction, the ACC and insula are activated during extinction recall (Phelps et al., 2004). In contrast to studies of extinction, studies of extinction recall often implicate hippocampus with increased hippocampal activity correlating with both SCRs reflecting return of fear and the vmPFC (Kalisch et al., 2006; Milad et al., 2007). This may indicate that hippocampus is vital only for consolidated memories and/or the process of consolidation. Interestingly, amygdala activations have not been found during extinction recall. SCRs indicate a return of fear in subjects so an amygdala response would be expected (Phelps et al., 2004). Maybe, there is a fast degradation of the amygdala response, as have been found in both acquisition and extinction (Büchel et al., 1998; LaBar et 25 al., 1998), meaning that the amygdala mainly reacts to the first few trials. On the other hand, amygdala activity could be decreased or not involved at all. The studies cited above reflect extinction recall of fear memories that were extinguished immediately following acquisition, which as stated earlier may differ from fear memories with undisturbed consolidations. The one study using extinction of fear memories with an undisturbed consolidation process also studied extinction recall. It noted involvement of the same areas (thalamus, midcingulate cortex, supplementary motor area and insula) in extinction recall as in extinction (Spoormaker et al., 2011). 26 Anxiety disorders and fear conditioning Anxiety disorders have long been considered as reflecting possible deficits in the processes governing fear conditioning. Indeed, Pavlov himself, proposed that the development of anxiety occurred by means of fear conditioning (Pavlov, 1927). Today, there are several theories proposing a link between fear conditioning mechanisms and anxiety disorders. For instance, it has been proposed that anxiety disorders are caused by a failure to extinguish acquired fear (Eysenck, 1979), increased conditionability (Orr et al., 2000), and that they are a result of the inability to inhibit fear responses in the presence of ‘safety cues’ (Davis, Falls, & Gewirtz, 2000). Theories aside, the most convincing evidence that fear conditioning processes are tied to anxiety disorders come from clinical studies. Exposure therapy, a treatment based on the process of extinction, is highly effective in treating anxiety disorders, including PTSD and obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) (Foa, 2011; Franklin & Foa, 2011; Ougrin, 2011). A recent metaanalysis comparing cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT) and exposure treatment found the two treatments equally effective in most anxiety disorders, with the exception of social phobia, where CBT was more effective (Ougrin, 2011). Moreover, phenomena that follow extinction in fear conditioning experiments are also noticed in clinical populations after treatment with exposure therapy. For instance, fear reoccur after treatment as an effect of both spontaneous recovery (Rachman, 1989) and renewal (Mineka, Mystkowski, & Hladek, 1999; Mystkowski, Mineka, Vernon, & Zinbarg, 2003; B. Rodriguez, Craske, & Mineka, 1999). A meta-analysis, concentrating on studies of fear conditioning conducted on clinical populations, reported modest increases in fear acquisition and fear expression during extinction in anxiety patients. However, this difference was mainly a result of studies using single cue conditioning, that is, studies where no CS- was used. In studies of differential conditioning, where the difference of the reactions to CS+ and CS- is used as a measure of fear, the meta-analysis found no difference between patients and healthy controls (Lissek et al., 2005). Thus, it cannot be ruled out that the increased responsiveness of patients found in single cue conditioning studies reflect an increased sensitization to the situation as a whole and perhaps not specifically to the CS+, hence a fear memory process may not be involved. In line with this, and based on the results of several studies in the meta-analysis, Lissek and coworkers (2005) argued 27 that the lack of differences between patients and controls regarding differential conditioning may be because the patients reacted more to both CS+ and CS- and that this could indicate an inability in patients to inhibit their reaction to the CS- (Lissek et al., 2005). In addition, brain imaging of anxiety disorders often report elevated activations of the amygdala (Holzschneider & Mulert, 2011), a vital structure for fear conditioning (LeDoux, 2000). However, equating fear conditioning with anxiety is problematic. For instance, many patients suffering from specific phobia do not remember a specific learning instance from which the phobia originated, which would be expected from fear conditioning theory; see (Mineka & Oehlberg, 2008) for a discussion. Moreover, most studies find no link between anxiety-related traits (Chambers, Power, & Durham, 2004; Gershuny & Sher, 1998; Jorm et al., 2000) and fear conditioning (Davidson, Payne, & Sloane, 1964; Otto et al., 2007; Pineles, Vogt, & Orr, 2009). That is, there is no strong evidence that people at risk for developing anxiety disorders behave differently in fear conditioning as compared to people who are not. Recently it has been argued that perhaps the traditional experimental fear conditioning paradigm needs to incorporate other measures of fear than physiological, such as avoidance behavior, to expose the possible differences between anxiety patients and controls (Beckers, Krypotos, Boddez, Effting, & Kindt, 2012). Also, since phobias generally concerns a limited number of stimuli and situations it has been suggested that organisms are evolutionary prepared to fear certain stimuli (Seligman, 1972) and that it is in fear conditioning processes regarding these specific stimuli that anxiety patients differ from controls. This theory has gained some support in social phobia (Lissek et al., 2008), but have some obvious theoretical problems that must be dealt with. For instance, either fear conditioning to prepared stimuli is a qualitatively different neural process, which lacks evidence, or fear conditioning is modulated by an evolutionary old fear detection system other than the amygdala, still unknown to us. Fear conditioning and its relation to anxiety disorders is still, a hundred years after its discovery, a hot topic for study. As reported previously in this thesis, research has taken great strides to understand the neural processes underpinning fear conditioning. However, how these neural processes are ultimately involved in the development and maintenance of anxiety disorders and how they could be targeted for treatment remains largely unknown. 28 Anxiety disorders and consolidation/reconsolidation Treating anxiety by disrupting a memory’s consolidation processes is possible only if treatment follows within hours of the trauma leading to PTSD or phobia. Still, a few clever experimental studies show promising results. Arguing that humans have limited capacity for processing and consolidation of visual information, subjects performed different cognitive tasks after watching a stressful movie. Subjects then documented the frequency of involuntary memory recalls of the movie during the following week. Subjects that performed a visuo-spatial task had fewer episodes of involuntary memory recall than subjects who performed a verbal task, or no task (Deeprose, Zhang, Dejong, Dalgleish, & Holmes, 2012; Holmes, James, Coode-Bate, & Deeprose, 2009; Holmes et al., 2010). A more direct study tried to disrupt the consolidation process of fear memories in trauma patients by treating them with the noradrenergic antagonist propranolol shortly after initial trauma with some success (Pitman et al., 2002). Treating anxiety by disrupting reconsolidation processes have potentially a much wider clinical use, as compared to preventing consolidation, since the treatment does not have to follow immediately after the trauma. Also, if reconsolidation disruption can be used to influence fear and anxiety, other anxiety disorders that not necessarily are trauma dependent can be treatment candidates. In essence, many forms of therapy consist of re-imagining and re-framing memories by means of cognitive analysis of the events, leading to new perspectives, and less anxiety, in subsequent remembrance of the previous events. The first clinical study to use disruption of reconsolidation used a novel treatment approach with PTSD patients. Patients provided a written account of the event that triggered the PTSD, producing the written report served as a reactivation of the memory, and were administered propranolol to disrupt the following reconsolidation. Patients who were treated with propranolol reported large symptom improvements (Brunet et al., 2011). In conclusion, disruption of consolidation and reconsolidation processes holds clinical promise and hopefully, as we learn more of the phenomenon, this will lead to novel treatment strategies. 29 Genetics of fear conditioning This section is limited to the functional polymorphisms and genotypes of direct interest for the thesis. 5-HTTLPR The serotonin transporter gene linked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR) is a functional polymorphism where different alleles have different efficacy of the serotonin reuptake mechanism. This polymorphism directly impacts the mechanism that is targeted by selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs); arguably the most widely used medical treatment of anxiety. The polymorphism comes in two variants, simply called short (s) or long (l) allele, creating the possible genotypes, ss, sl, or ll. The short allele carriers have a more efficient reuptake mechanism leaving less serotonin in the synaptic cleft in vitro (Lesch et al., 1996). However, studies examining the availability of serotonin in vivo have yielded mixed results (Bah et al., 2008; Dyck & Malison, 2004; Shioe et al., 2003). Carriers of the short allele of 5HTTLPR have been reported to have elevated anxiety-related personality traits (Lesch et al., 1996) and increased risk for development of psychopathology (Caspi et al., 2003). Moreover, short allele carriers have enhanced reactivity of the amygdala (Hariri et al., 2002; Munafò, Brown, & Hariri, 2008), a key structure for fear conditioning (LeDoux, 2003). There is also evidence that 5-HTTLPR is associated more directly with fear conditioning. Short allele carriers have been associated with stronger acquisition and slower extinction as compared to long allele homozygotes (Crişan et al., 2009; Garpenstrand, Annas, Ekblom, Oreland, & Fredrikson, 2001; Lonsdorf et al., 2009). The target function of SSRIs is to block serotonin reuptake from the synaptic cleft and this is believed to increase the effectiveness of released serotonin. In rodents, a single dose of SSRIs facilitate fear acquisition (Burghardt, Sullivan, McEwen, Gorman, & LeDoux, 2004) and expression of earlier acquired fear memories (Burghardt, Bush, McEwen, & LeDoux, 2007). Moreover, a single dose SSRI treatment facilitate memory consolidation in 30 humans (Harmer, Bhagwagar, Cowen, & Goodwin, 2002). These results indicate that more serotonin means better fear conditioning, which is in line with the results reported above that implicate short allele carriers as having stronger acquisition and slower extinction. However, other differences between carriers of the long and short allele of 5-HTTLPR may be equally important. Short allele carriers, as compared to ll homozygotes, have been found to have altered functional connectivity between areas involved in emotion (Pezawas et al., 2005). It is still too early to draw any far reaching conclusions from this study, but serotonin is used as a neurotrophic factor during neural development, providing a plausible avenue for structural changes to depend on serotonergic genes (Nordquist & Oreland, 2010). In any case, the studies that reveal changes in fear conditioning after a single dose of SSRIs, further implicate serotonergic mechanisms as having an impact on fear memory learning and persistence. In conclusion, the study of 5HTTLPR’s impact on fear conditioning offer a possible link between fear memory processes, serotonergic neurotransmission and SSRI treatment. COMT The functional polymorphism val158met (rs4680) of the catechol-Omethyltransferease (COMT) enzyme has two alleles, valine and methionine, (val and met) that differ in the efficacy of the COMT enzyme, which is vital for the elimination of extracellular dopamine. The COMT enzyme is present throughout most of the brain (Hong, Shu-leong, Tao, & Lap-Ping, 1998). Functional analysis of the polymorphism report that val carriers have a higher activity of the COMT enzyme leading to the presence of less extracellular dopamine (Chen et al., 2004) particularly in val/val homozygotes, as reported by an in vivo imaging study (Slifstein et al., 2008). There are also reports claiming val158met COMT has an impact on D1-receptors and not D2-receptors, suggesting a possible receptor-specific mechanism of the polymorphism (Hirvonen et al., 2010; Slifstein et al., 2008). Functional polymorphisms affecting dopamine in the brain are interesting for a number of reasons. In anxiety research, this is mainly because of: 1) Dopamine is necessary in the amygdala for fear conditioning to take place (Darvas, Fadok, & Palmiter, 2011; de Oliveira et al., 2011). 2) Dopamine is necessary in BLA for long term fear memories to form (Fadok, Darvas, Dickerson, & Palmiter, 2010; Rossato, Bevilaqua, Izquierdo, Medina, & Cammarota, 2009) and basolateral amygdala is vital for the modulation of arousal memory consolidation (McGaugh, 2004). 3) Dopamine is a part of the arousal reaction and a candidate element of the mechanisms that enables the amygdala to form an association between CS and UCS (Fadok et al., 2010; Marinelli & Piazza, 2002; Rodrigues, LeDoux, & Sapolsky, 2009). 4) 31 Research in rodents demonstrate that dopamine affect the BLA by increasing the firing in inhibitory interneurons and decreasing the firing in projection neurons. Also dopamine receptor activity in the BLA reduce the impact of prefrontal and thalamic inputs to the amygdala (Rosenkranz & Grace, 1999). This means that dopaminergic signalling provide a possible mechanism for the modulatory role on other areas that the amygdala have in arousal memory consolidation (McGaugh, 2004). Hence, the val158met polymorphism is widely researched as a candidate gene for psychiatric disorders. Brain imaging studies have associated the val allele with psychiatric disorders (Domschke & Dannlowski, 2010; Lelli-Chiesa et al., 2011). However, association studies on large samples did not find any obvious link between COMT and anxiety disorders (Stein, Fallin, Schork, & Gelernter, 2005; Wray et al., 2008). Considering the above reported importance of dopamine in amygdala memory functions, it is likely that there should be some association between the COMT val158met polymorphism and fear conditioning. Two studies have examined fear conditioning and its relation to the COMT val158met genotype in humans, but with mixed findings. One, using startle response, reported slower extinction in met homozygotes (Lonsdorf et al., 2009) and the other, using learning rates based on a skin conductance signal model, reported stronger reacquisition in a val/met-group, but only in the presence of a certain dopamine transporter (DAT1) polymorphism (Raczka et al., 2011). More research is needed to evaluate these results and fit them into a bigger picture of how the COMT val158met polymorphism affect fear conditioning processes. 32 Methods Psychophysiology- skin conductance The arousal reaction is vital both for forming fear memories and fear expression in general. As part of the arousal reaction, the sympathetic subdivision of the autonomic nervous system trigger defensive responses such as an increase in heart rate, blood pressure and sweating as well as diversion of blood from digestion to limb musculature. The induced sweating lead to changes in skin conductance which can be measured. The study of skin conductance, or electrodermal activity (EDA), has been around since the 1840s, but the use for it in psychophysiology arose in the 1880s when two independent scientists, Féré in France and Tarchanoff in Russia, measured changes in skin conductance in response to sensory stimuli, imagination and expectation. During the second half of the 1900s, technological progress provided better equipment and the use of the skin conductance measurement became more widely used. In short, skin conductance uses a weak current that flows between two electrodes fastened on the skin using a conductive gel. When sweat glands open, for instance as a result of a fear reaction, this changes the conductivity of the skin, and a change in current can be noted (Boucsein, 1992). The measurement of skin conductance reactions is inherently event-related because there is no real baseline that can easily be measured. Simply put, the brainstem controls the activation of the sympathetic nervous system, but there is evidence that cortical areas in turn control the brainstem. For instance, direct stimulation of the amygdala, hippocampus, anterior cingulate cortex, lateral prefrontal cortex and middle temporal gyrus elicit changes in skin conductance. Moreover, an imaging study reported activity in the insular cortex and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex to vary with skin conductance (Critchley, 2002). In essence, all structures that we consider part of the emotion circuit of the brain (LeDoux, 2000) can produce or covariate with the production of skin conductance responses, which is encouraging for its use as a measurement of emotion reactions. 33 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) FMRI is presently the most widely used functional brain imaging method. An MRI-scanner can produce different signals (sequences) by shifting the magnetic fields in different ways. The most widely used MRI-sequence in psychological research is an echo planar imaging (EPI)-sequence measuring a blood oxygen level dependent signal, in short, the BOLD response. The functional principle behind a MRI-scanner is beyond the scope of this thesis and requires extensive knowledge of physics. Simply put, a magnetic field changes direction many times per second and measures a signal produced by the induced movement in blood molecules. It then uses the change in magnetization between oxygen-rich and oxygen-poor blood to produce the BOLD signal. Brain activation results in fMRI research are presented as contrasts between conditions and/or groups, that is, as differences between two conditions and/or groups. For example, the condition of watching a picture can be contrasted with the condition of looking at a blank screen. This contrast can then itself be contrasted between a group of patients and healthy controls. An experiment’s timeline will consist of many scans, for example one scan per three seconds. Every point in the brain is then usually modeled in a regression (y=β1x1+β2x2+…+βnXn+C), using all the scans as data points, where every condition is a separate regressor (β) and the scans between conditions become an additive element that serves as a baseline (C). A contrast between conditions is performed by subtracting the beta value for one condition from the beta value of another condition. These beta value differences are themselves normally distributed so it is possible to make a statistical test for each measured point in the brain. It is then possible to search for clusters of points where the activation differ with statistical significance between conditions or groups. These areas are often projected onto pictures of brains when presented as results (Logothetis, 2008). 34 Aim The general aim of this thesis was to examine the modulation of fear memories, achieved by disrupting the reconsolidation process that follow upon fear memory reactivation, using measures of peripheral (study I, II and III) and central nervous system activity (study III). The aim of the first study was to examine the attenuation of fear memories by reconsolidation disruption and to explore the impact of two functional polymorphisms previously associated with fear conditioning, the serotonin transporter gene linked polymorphic region and the val158met (rs4680) of the catechol-O-methyltransferease enzyme, on fear memory reconsolidation, using reacquisition as the memory probe. The second study aimed at attenuating fear memories by reconsolidation disruption and, using reinstatement as a memory probe, explore the impact of the 5-HTTLPR and val158met COMT polymorphisms on fear memory processes. Finally, study III aimed at testing the hypothesis that a fear memory trace localized in the amygdala is erased through reconsolidation disruption. 35 Study I: Aim and background Attenuation of fear memories by means of extinction during ongoing reconsolidation, that disrupts the reconsolidation processes, have recently been shown in rodents (Monfils et al., 2009) and humans (Schiller et al., 2010). Study I aimed to replicate this phenomenon by installing a fear memory with fear conditioning and then to disrupt the reconsolidation by means of extinction after a fear memory reminder. The strength of the fear memory was then tested with reinstatement and reacquisition. In addition, study I explored if the functional polymorphisms 5-HTTLPR and val158met COMT, implicated in fear conditioning (Garpenstrand et al., 2001; Lonsdorf et al., 2009) also affect fear memory reconsolidation processes. Methods Sixty-six participants (38 females) were recruited from public billboards around campus. Subjects visited the lab on three consecutive days. On day 1 they completed a session of fear conditioning. The subjects watched a computer screen with a photo of a lamp in a neutral environment. The lamp could be lit in either blue or red. One of the colours was paired with an electric shock and became CS+ and the other colour became CS-. Colours were counterbalanced between subjects. The session consisted of 16 presentations of CS+ and CS- each. CS+ was always paired with an unpleasant shock to the lower dorsal right arm. On day 2, twenty-four hours later, subjects returned for a fear memory activation. Following memory activation, one group underwent an extinction session 10 min after the memory activation (the 10 min group), and thus, during the ongoing reconsolidation. The other group underwent extinction 6 h after the memory activation (the 6 h group), and thus, after completed reconsolidation. Return of fear was tested on day 3, another twenty-four hours later, using reinstatement and reacquisition. After the final testing on day 3, subjects left a saliva sample that was used for gene analysis. Skin conductance responses were measured during all segments of the experiment. 36 Results The 6 h group discriminated between CS+ and CS- during reacquisition, demonstrating relearning, while the 10 min group did not. Moreover, reactions to CS+ were stronger in the 6 h group than in the 10 min group, while reactions to CS- did not differ between groups, demonstrating that the groups differed mainly in their reactions to the stimulus previously associated with shock. Thus, this study conceptually replicated the previously reported results (Schiller et al., 2010). In addition, gene analysis revealed that this effect was mainly found in short allele carriers of 5-HTTLPR and val/val homozygotes of the val158met COMT functional polymorphisms. Due to a programming error, most of the reinstatement data was corrupted. However, the data that was not corrupted indicated a trend towards stronger fear reactions in the 6 h group than the 10 min group during reinstatement. Discussion The present study demonstrates attenuation of fear memories by means of disrupting their reconsolidation and suggests that this effect is mainly found in certain genotypes. Reacquisition was mainly found in s carriers of 5HTTLPR and val/val homozygotes of the val158met COMT polymorphism and only when extinction was performed after an undisturbed reconsolidation. L/L homozygotes of 5-HTTLPR and met-carriers of COMT did not display reacquisition regardless of whether reconsolidation was disrupted or not, possibly because the reconsolidation process lasts longer in these genotypes making both groups reconsolidation processes disrupted. Also, due to technical reasons, most of the reinstatement data was corrupted making the replication of the reconsolidation effect measured by reinstatement unclear. 37 Study II: Aim and background The aim of Study II was to attenuate a fear memory by disrupting memory reconsolidation. Specifically, the aim was to install an experimental fear memory using fear conditioning and then disrupt the reconsolidation process after a memory activation using extinction training and finally, to test the strength of the fear memory using reinstatement. It could be argued that both reinstatement and reacquisition activate a previously formed, but now passive and dormant memory, and that reacquisition in addition restores and possibly creates an active memory. Thus, reinstatement examines fear expression of a previously acquired memory without the possible confound of new memory formation. In addition, study II investigated the effect of the functional polymorphisms 5-HTTLPR and val158met COMT enzyme on fear conditioning and fear memory reconsolidation processes. A cold pressor test was used prior to acquisition with the intention to boost initial memory encoding. Methods Fifty participants (32 females) were recruited by public advertisements. Subjects visited the lab on three consecutive days. On day 1, subjects went through a session of fear conditioning. Subjects watched a computer screen with a photo of a lamp in a neutral environment. The lamp was lit in either blue or red. One of the colours became CS+ and the other CS-, counterbalanced between subjects. The session consisted of 16 presentations of CS+ and CS- each. CS+ was always paired with an unpleasant shock to the lower dorsal right arm. On day 2, twenty-four hours later, subjects returned for a fear memory activation, consisting of a CS+ presentation. Following this memory activation, one group underwent an extinction session 10 min after the memory activation (the 10 min group), and thus, during the ongoing reconsolidation. The other group underwent extinction 6 h after the memory activation (the 6 h group), and thus, after completed reconsolidation. Return of fear was tested on day 3, another twenty-four hours later, using reinstatement. Skin conductance responses were measured during all segments of the 38 experiment. After the final testing on day 3, subjects left a saliva sample for gene analysis. Results This study partially replicated study I in that an effect of reconsolidation disruption was found, but only in s carriers of 5-HTTLPR and val/val homozygotes of the val158met COMT polymorphism. In addition, the effect was weaker, displaying a trend in val/val homozygotes and a significant difference in s carriers only in reactions to CS+. No main effect of reconsolidation disruption averaged over all genotypes was found. Discussion The group with disrupted reconsolidation (the 10 min group) showed weaker reactions to reinstatement than the group with no disrupted reconsolidation (the 6 h group). However, this was only noted as a trend in val/val homozygotes of the val158met COMT polymorphism and a significant difference in reactions to CS+ in s carriers of 5-HTTLPR. Unlike study I, no main effect of reconsolidation disruption irrespective of genotype was found. Numerically, it appears as the effect is there, but large variance between individuals contributes to the lack of statistical significance. Possibly the phenomenon is fragile or maybe the cold pressor test produced the larger variation between individuals. 39 Study III: Aim and background The attenuation of fear memories by disruption of reconsolidation has been localized to the amygdala in animals (Nader et al., 2000) using protein synthesis inhibition, but no such studies have been made in humans because the process of inhibiting protein synthesis used in animals are not safe. The aim of study III was to localize the processes of reconsolidation in humans by performing fMRI neuroimaging of BOLD signal changes reflecting fear memory strength linked to fear expression comparing subjects who had a disrupted reconsolidation with those who had not. Methods Thirty subjects (13 females) were recruited by public billboards and on underwent a session of fear conditioning (day 1). Subjects watched a computer screen projecting a picture of a lamp in a neutral environment that could be lit in either red or blue. One of the colours was always paired with a shock to the lower dorsal right arm and became the CS+ while the other colour became CS-. CS+ colour was balanced between subjects. The session consisted of 16 presentations of CS+ and CS- each. Twenty-four hours later (day 2), they returned for a two minute CS+ presentation serving as a fear memory activation. One group then got extinction after 10 minutes (the 10 min group) and hence, within the reconsolidation interval, and one group got extinction after 6 hours (the 6 h group) and hence, outside of the reconsolidation interval. Another twenty-four hours later (day 3) the subjects returned for a second extinction session, in order to image the renewal of fear in a fMRI scanner. Two days later (day 5), subjects returned for another test of return of fear using reinstatement. Skin conductance responses were measured in all phases of the experiment except during the extinction session performed in fMRI because of technical reasons. 40 Results The 6 h group displayed stronger return of fear, as defined as the difference between the last extinction and the first reinstatement trial, than the 10 min group. BOLD signal changes indicated the presence of a memory trace in the amygdala in the 6 h, but not the 10 min group. Moreover, BOLD activity in the 6 h group predicted return of fear the subsequent day and was correlated with fear expression the previous day. In addition, the area in amygdala in which the groups differed in BOLD activity overlapped with the area predicting return of fear and the area that correlated with fear expression in the 6 h group, further implicating the presence of a memory trace in the 6 h group. Also, fear circuit activity was more tightly linked to amygdala activity in the 6 h group as BOLD signal changes in the amygdala covaried with activity in the insula, ACC and hippocampus to a greater extent in the 6 h as compared to the 10 min group. Discussion The present study demonstrated attenuation of fear memories by reconsolidation disruption after a fear memory reminder. Also, using fMRI, it showed that the group with undisrupted reconsolidation, and hence, a more complete memory, had significantly more amygdala activation than the group with disrupted reconsolidation. Because the amygdala activation, indicating a difference in memory strength between groups, was functionally associated with return of fear and fear memory recall in the 6 h group, this suggests the presence of a spared memory in the 6 h group and in contrast, a disrupted or abolished memory in the 10 min group. Hence, there is reason to believe that in humans, as in rodents, fear memory reconsolidation is in part amygdala dependent. 41 Results overview (study I, II & III) Included here are figures intended to give an easily accessible overview of the results included in this thesis. Figure 1 presents the skin conductance results of study I, II and III. Figure 2 presents the skin conductance results of study I and study II regarding the examined genotypes. Figure 3 gives an overview of the sample sizes and the partitioning of genotypes on experimental group for study I and study II. Table 1 and 2 demonstrates the overlap between genotypes in the samples of study I and II. 42 Day 2 0 0,1 0,2 0,3 0,4 -0,1 -0,2 -0,1 -0,2 0,1 0,2 0,3 0,4 6h 10 min Reconsolidation interval -0,2 -0,1 0 0,1 0,2 0,3 0,4 -0,2 -0,2 0 0,1 0,2 0,3 0,4 -0,2 -0,1 0 0,1 0,2 0,3 0,4 -0,1 6h 10 min Reconsolidation interval 6h 10 min Reconsolidation interval Extinction Day 2 -0,1 0 0,1 0,2 0,3 0,4 -0,2 -0,1 Memory activation 0 6h 10 min Reconsolidation interval 6h 10 min Reconsolidation interval 6h 10 min Reconsolidation interval Fear conditioning Day 1 0 0,1 0,2 0,3 0,4 -0,2 -0,1 0 0,1 0,2 0,3 0,4 -0,2 -0,1 0 0,1 0,2 0,3 0,4 A SCR SCR SCR SCR SCR SCR SCR SCR SCR 10 min Reconsolidation interval 6h 6h 10 min Reconsolidation interval 6h 10 min Reconsolidation interval Return of fear Day 3 Experimental design and timeline (top). Mean differential skin conductance delta scores (CS+ minus CS-) for acquisition (Fear Conditioning), extinction and return of fear for study I (top panels), study II (middle panels) and study III (bottom panels) after extinction inside (10 min) and outside (6 h) the reconsolidation interval. Error bars are standard error of means. Title: Extinction during reconsolidation attenuates return of fear. Overview Figure 1. SCR SCR 10 min 6h 0 0,1 0,2 0,3 -0,2 met ll -0,2 Genotype Genotype -0,2 -0,1 0 0,1 0,2 0,3 -0,1 val/val s 10 min 6h -0,1 0 0,1 0,2 0,3 -0,2 -0,1 0 0,1 0,2 0,3 Study I SCR SCR val/val s Genotype Genotype Study II met ll 10 min 6h 10 min 6h Mean differential skin conductance delta scores (CS+ minus CS-) reflecting return of fear as measured by reacquisition (Study I, left panels) and reinstatement (Study II, right panels) after extinction inside (10 min) and outside (6 h) the reconsolidation interval in short allele carriers (s) and ll homozygotes of the 5HTTLPR polymorphism (top panels) and val/val homozygotes and met carriers of the val158met COMT polymorphism (bottom panels). Error bars are standard error of means. Title: The effect of reconsolidation disruption is modulated by genes. Overview Figure 2. met N = 10 val/val N=3 met N = 14 COMT COMT val/val N=5 5-HTTLPR ll s N=5 N = 13 5-HTTLPR ll s N=4 N = 10 val/val N=4 met N=9 COMT 5-HTTLPR ll s N=9 N=6 6 h: N = 16 val/val N=6 met N=9 COMT 5-HTTLPR ll s N=7 N=8 10 min: N = 15 Conditioned: N = 31 Conditioned: N = 33 10 min: N = 18 All subjects: N = 50 All subjects: N = 66 6 h: N = 15 Study II Study I Sample sizes and the partitioning of genotypes for conditioned subjects over experimental groups in study I and II. In study I, 5-HTTLPR genotyping failed for one of the subjects and COMT genotyping failed for another of the subjects. In study II, genotyping failed for two subjects regarding 5HTTLPR and for three subjects regarding COMT. Title: Sample sizes and the partitioning of genotypes over experimental groups. Overview Figure 3. Table 1: Genotype partitioning of conditioned subjects in study I 5HTTLPR COMT val/val COMT val/met COMT met/met fail 5-HTTLPR all SS 1 3 4 0 8 SL 6 7 1 1 15 LL 1 5 3 0 9 fail 0 0 1 0 1 COMT all 8 15 9 1 33 Table 2: Genotype partition of conditioned subjects in study II 5HTTLPR COMT val/val COMT val/met COMT met/met fail 5-HTTLPR all SS 2 1 2 0 5 SL 4 5 1 0 10 LL 4 9 3 0 16 fail 0 0 0 0 0 COMT all 10 15 6 0 31 46 General Discussion Main findings The main findings of the thesis is that attenuation of fear memories by reconsolidation disruption is a reproducible phenomenon and that the effect may be driven mainly by subjects with certain genotypes. Specifically, the phenomenon is present in short allele carriers of the 5-HTTLPR and val/val homozygotes of the val158met COMT polymorphisms. Moreover, fear reconsolidation disruption alters the activity and functionality of the amygdala at subsequent fear memory recall suggesting that reconsolidation processes are amygdala-dependent. Discussion All three studies demonstrate the attenuation of fear memories by reconsolidation disruption, although in varying degree. Taken together with previous results (Kindt et al., 2009; Schiller et al., 2010), this supports that the reconsolidation of fear memories occurs also in humans and present an evolutionary shared mechanism with other mammals such as rodents (Monfils et al., 2009). The evolutionary tie is strongly implicated by study III which finds functional differences depending on reconsolidation conditions in the amygdala, where reconsolidation also takes place in rodents (Nader et al., 2000). The finding that s carriers of 5-HTTLPR showed more return of fear is in line with other findings in fear conditioning implicating s carrier as having stronger acquisition and slower extinction (Garpenstrand et al., 2001; Lonsdorf et al., 2009). Moreover, since fear conditioning is an amygdaladependent process, this is also in line with the reported results of increased amygdala reactivity in s carriers (Hariri et al., 2002). The specific neurobiological mechanisms underlying these results (Garpenstrand et al., 2001; Hariri et al., 2002; Lonsdorf et al., 2009) are unknown, but studies using acute SSRI treatments produce stronger reactions in fear conditioning, implicating that an increased presence of extracellular serotonin in the synaptic cleft is involved (Burghardt et al., 2007; Harmer et al., 2002). However, although a stronger return of fear was noted in short 47 carriers in both study I and study II, neither study I nor study II found evidence for stronger acquisition in s carriers. The findings implicating the val/val homozygotes of the val158met COMT polymorphism as having increased return of fear doesn’t fit as easily with previous results in fear conditioning. It has been reported that met-carries have slower extinction (Lonsdorf et al., 2009) and that a val/met-group had stronger reacquisition (Raczka et al., 2011), which are results that are somewhat conflicting with the present results. However, imaging of emotional aversive stimuli have reported increased amygdala activity in val-carriers (Domschke et al., 2008, 2012), possibly affecting the amygdala-dependent fear conditioning processes. Study I and II of this thesis both implicate val/val homozygotes as functionally different from met-carriers in fear memory reconsolidation. This corresponds to results from in vivo imaging where val/val homozygotes differed from met-carriers in the amount of dopamine in cortico-limbic areas (Slifstein et al., 2008), validating the partioning of val/val homozygotes from met-carriers in the study of dopamine functionality. Val/val homozygotes had a higher activity of the COMT enzyme leading to the presence of less extracellular dopamine (Chen et al., 2004) throughout most of the brain (Hong et al., 1998). Basolateral amygdala appear vital for fear conditioning processes (LeDoux, 2000; Maren, 2001; Quirk & Mueller, 2008) and rodent studies suggest that dopamine mainly affect the BLA by increasing the firing in inhibitory interneurons and decreasing the firing in neurons projecting to the nucleus accumbens. In addition, dopamine receptor activity in the BLA seem to reduce the impact of prefrontal and thalamic inputs to the amygdala (Rosenkranz & Grace, 1999). It is possible that less dopamine leads to less inhibition and that this would lead to increased fear responses, in turn leading to better acquisition of fear memories and increased return of fear. However, one could also argue the reverse, since dopamine is necessary in the BLA for fear memory consolidation (Fadok et al., 2010) and that less dopamine could lead to weaker long term memories of fear. Although the specific mechanisms remain unclear, the result of this thesis suggests that val/val homozygotes maintain and update fear memories more effectively than met carriers. What do the results from study I and study II that fear memory reconsolidation disruption only seems to take place for specific genotypes mean? Met carriers of the val158met COMT polymorphism and the ll homozygotes of 5-HTTLPR displayed no return of fear, neither when extinction was performed after 10 min or 6 h after the memory reminder. Possibly, reconsolidation of fear memory arousal is simply weaker, or less effective, in these genotypes, so that the memory strength is comparable with the memories of disrupted reconsolidation in val/val homozygotes and s carriers. However, if 48 this was true, fear memories would have a more transient character in met carriers of COMT and ll homozygotes of 5-HTTLPR which would possibly be reflected in a lower propensity to anxiety disorders in these genotypes. Such a direct connection between these alleles and anxiety disorders has not been found (Samochowiec et al., 2004; Stein et al., 2005; Wray et al., 2008). An alternative is that the reconsolidation of met carriers of val158met COMT and ll homozygotes of 5-HTTLPR is more easily disrupted, in fact, disrupted by the learning of a new contingency. One could reason that the reminder is not only a reminder of the stimulus-shock contingency, but also learning of a new contingency of stimulus-no shock. This thesis have discussed that the removal of conditioned fear responses consist of both inhibition resulting from new learning (extinction) and unlearning of the original memory (reconsolidation disruption). It is possible that there in met carriers of COMT and ll homozygotes in 5-HTTLPR exist a direct interplay between new learning and reconsolidation disruption. However, a number of arguments can be raised against this view. It is well known from animal studies that a presentation consisting of a single element of a stimulus-shock contingency is enough to serve as a reminder of the stimulus-schock contingency without extinguishing the learned fear response, on the contrary, it strengthens the response. Also, the new contingency is supposed to be learned instantly, which our extinction data speak against. Moreover, the protein synthesis of reconsolidation need a few minutes to start, requiring the subjects to keep the presumably instantly learned new contingency in short term memory to subsequently affect reconsolidation. Furthermore, if an instant coupling between new learning and reconsolidation disruption exist in these genotypes, they would arguably have problems with generalizing fears from one situation to another, something that lends itself easily to experimentation. Perhaps a better alternative is that the met carriers of val158met COMT and ll homozygotes of 5-HTTLPR have a reconsolidation window that is open longer than val/val homozygotes and s carriers, rendering also the extinction that took place 6 h after the reminder as disruptive. However, it appears that protein synthesis inhibitors must be initiated 90 min after training to disrupt consolidation (Davis & Squire, 1984). This suggests that if a reconsolidation disruption can be initiated more than 6 h after a reminder, the disrupted mechanism is probably not protein synthesis. Indeed, injecting catecholamine synthesis inhibitors produces consolidation disruption in a different time-course and in different brain structures than protein synthesis inhibitors, suggesting a different reconsolidation interval for the modulation of reconsolidation by catecholamines than by protein synthesis inhibition (Davis & Squire, 1984). Future studies could establish if the reconsolidation interval differ between means of disruption or modulation such as protein synthesis 49 inhibitors or norepinephrine antagonists, both in timeframe, brain structures involved, and in different relevant genotypes. The demonstration of amygdala involvement in study III is in line with animal data on reconsolidation disruption (Nader et al., 2000). Molecular mechanisms are most easily studied in animals, but human studies give an important opportunity to study task-dependent whole brain connectivity in vivo using neuroimaging. Although the amygdala localized fear memory presence and reconsolidation may be an evolutionary shared mechanism the tighter coupling between amygdala and the so called fear circuitry (including hippocampus, insula and ACC) found in study III may represent speciesspecific memory- and cognitive processes. It is conceivable that what we colloquially mean with a fear memory consist of several aspects (such as cognitive, episodic and emotional) with different representations in our brains and that amygdala is responsible, through consolidation mechanisms, to update several of these representations with regards to emotional content (McGaugh, 2004). However, if the amygdala modulates memories in other brain structures with emotional content during the consolidation and reconsolidation process, it would not be seen in the imaging data of study III. Instead, the synchronized activation of insula, ACC and hippocampus may reflect different aspects of the memory activating in concert in their respective parts of the brain. On the other hand, these areas may be involved in fear expression or cognitive evaluation of the present fear memory. For instance, insular activity correlate with spontaneous fluctuation in skin conductance (Critchley, 2002) which may reflect a function in fear expression and the association between ACC and attentional processes (Lane et al., 1997) suggest that the activity in ACC may be the result of selective attention to the stimulus. However, with the present methodology it is almost impossible to separate the activation of fear memories from the production of linked fear expression without accompanying knowledge of the neurobiological processes involved. Study III, together with neurobiological studies on rodents placing both fear memories (Reijmers et al., 2007) and reconsolidation of fear memories (Nader et al., 2000) in the amygdala, strongly suggest that the disruption of reconsolidation reduces an amygdala localized fear memory also in humans. Limitations The studies contained in this thesis have some limitations. First, the programming error that corrupted most of the reinstatement data in study I. Second, study II would have benefited from an extra control group in order to be able to evaluate the evaluation of the stress manipulation thoroughly. Also, the sample sizes for the behavioural genetics results are to be consid50 ered small. However, the results are strengthened by the fact that the results were highly similar in the two different cohorts. Still, replications with larger sample sizes are warranted, especially considering the results that did not agree between study I and II. Moreover, the small sample sizes make it difficult to dissociate the effect of one polymorphism from another since subjects possess different combinations of the polymorphisms involved. The studies contained in this thesis have used ANCOVAs in an attempt to dissociate the effects of the different polymorphisms examined. Finally, technical limitations stopped us from using skin conductance measurements in the fMRIscanner, which would have provided an extra manipulation check. Future directions This thesis lends further support to the notion that attenuation of fear memories by reconsolidation disruption is possible in humans. It would prove interesting to further specify what kind of manipulations that can disrupt reconsolidation. For instance, there is some evidence that conflicting learning can be used to disrupt consolidation in humans (Holmes et al., 2010) and reconsolidation in rodents (Spear et al., 1973). To identify the methods that can be used for reconsolidation disruption limits the set of possible underlying mechanisms and also has clinical implications. Also, the timing conditions that enable reconsolidation disruption should be further investigated. For instance, is there a critical time window in which reconsolidation is most effective? Furthermore, what kind of memories can be modulated by reconsolidation disruption? Is it true that the memories must have an arousal component as previously speculated (Squire et al., 1976) ? Moreover, further imaging studies, using fMRI and PET, could perhaps clarify the findings implicating certain genotypes in this phenomenon by providing a picture of neural activity, neural structure, and neurochemical differences. For instance, do s carriers and ll homozygotes of the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism differ in serotonin receptor availability during encoding, extinction or recall? At the present time, the empirical justification for using fear conditioning as a model for anxiety disorders is mainly based on the presence of fear conditioning-related renewal phenomena in clinical patients (Mineka et al., 1999; Mystkowski et al., 2003; Rachman, 1989; Rodriguez, Craske, et al., 1999), modest increases in fear acquisition and extinction (Lissek et al., 2005) and the effectiveness of extinction therapy (Ougrin, 2011). It would be interesting, using the methodology proposed in this thesis, to investigate the fear memory processes of clinical populations to see if they differ from healthy controls. If a clinical population would differ from healthy controls in fear 51 memory maintenance, this would provide evidence to further justify fear conditioning as a model for anxiety disorders. The existing somewhat poor results associating anxiety disorders with enhanced fear conditioning may be a result of investigating elements of fear conditioning in which patients and controls do not differ, i.e. acquisition and extinction. Perhaps an investigation regarding the fear memory processes involved in fear conditioning would provide stronger results. If differences in fear memory processes between anxiety patients and controls exist, they could provide new target mechanisms for treatment. Indeed, the attenuation of fear memories using consolidation and reconsolidation disruption have already been addressed as a possible avenue for novel treatment strategies (Brunet et al., 2011; Holmes et al., 2010; Pitman et al., 2002). Finally, on a grand scale, the attenuation of fear memories using reconsolidation may provide a future neurobiological framework for many forms of therapy. Because, in essence, many forms of therapy consist of the re-imagining and re-framing of memories by means of cognitive analysis of an event, hopefully leading to less anxiety in subsequent remembrance. In summary, future research should clarify what manipulations that can disrupt reconsolidation, under what conditions reconsolidation can be disrupted, the neural mechanisms underlying reconsolidation disruption, and the possible clinical implications of the phenomenon. 52 Acknowledgment This work was supported by the Swedish Research Council, the Swedish Council for Working Life and Social Research, the Swedish Brain Foundation and Heumanska stiftelsen. I would also like to show my gratitude towards my supervisors, Mats Fredrikson and Tomas Furmark, for their generosity, time, immense knowledge, and patience. I learned so much from you about the value of good writing, simple designs, and scientific rigor. You are very talented and I am forever in your debt. Thanks to my fellow PhD-students in the group, from former titans to new recruits: Fredrik Åhs, perhaps the most generous man in science, Clas Linnman, perhaps the most conscientious man in science, Vanda Faria, perhaps the shortest and most violent woman in science, Åsa Michelsgård, perhaps the trickiest roborally player in science, Malin Gingngnell, perhaps the hardest woman to spell to in science, Jonas Engman, perhaps the neatest man in science, Andreas Frick, containing perhaps the best gui in science, Johannes Björkstrand, perhaps the most cross-training man in science and Iman Alaie, perhaps the most literate man in science. Thanks also to all the friends I made among other PhD-students through the years: the old gang, Simon L, Jessika K, Vanda (again, and yes, you are not only violent, but sweet) and Hällena, who introduced me to the champagne evenings, and so many more, too many to name. Thanks all for being such a fun-loving and friendly lot. Thank you Mats G for the brain gymnastics. We’ll get there. Thanks to mom and dad for telling us kids to go for it. Thank you Krister, Karin, Måns and Max. Thank you to all friends, old and new, who have endured the specific breed of spiritual taint that I bring to every situation. I don’t know where to start. I usually start with a salad or a soup before moving on to the main course, and this is no exception. Thank you students, audiences, writer friends, hedgehogs, moomins, consciousness, thank you for the music, the songs I’m singing, the whispers, the dreams, the madness, the colours out of space and time, H. P. Lovecraft, popcorn, but to make a long story unbareable… …finally there is, the Hanna, who makes this otherwise so wonderful life, so enjoyable. 53 References Alberini, C. M. (2005). Mechanisms of memory stabilization: are consolidation and reconsolidation similar or distinct processes? Trends in neurosciences, 28(1), 51–6. Bach, D. R., Weiskopf, N., & Dolan, R. J. (2011). A Stable Sparse Fear Memory Trace in Human Amygdala. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(25), 9383–9389. Bah, J., Lindström, M., Westberg, L., Mannerås, L., Ryding, E., Henningsson, S., Melke, J., et al. (2008). Serotonin transporter gene polymorphisms: effect on serotonin transporter availability in the brain of suicide attempters. Psychiatry Research, 162(3), 221–9. Barrett, J., & Armony, J. L. (2009). Influence of trait anxiety on brain activity during the acquisition and extinction of aversive conditioning. Psychological medicine, 39(2), 255–65. Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., Adolphs, R., Rockland, C., & Damasio, A. R. (1995). Double dissociation of conditioning and declarative knowledge relative to the amygdala and hippocampus in humans. Science, 269(5227), 1115. Beckers, T., Krypotos, A.-M., Boddez, Y., Effting, M., & Kindt, M. (2012). What’s wrong with fear conditioning? Biological psychology. In Press. Blanchard, D. C., & Blanchard, R. J. (1972). Innate and conditioned reactions to threat in rats with amygdaloid lesions. Journal of comparative and physiological psychology, 81(2), 281–90. Boucsein, W. (1992). Electrodermal activity. (pp. 96–98). New York: Plenum Press. Bouton, M. E. (2002). Context , Ambiguity , and Unlearning : Sources of Relapse after Behavioral Extinction Four Mechanisms of Relapse. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 976–986. Brunet, A., Poundja, J., Tremblay, J., Bui, E., Thomas, E., Orr, S. P., Azzoug, A., et al. (2011). Trauma reactivation under the influence of propranolol decreases posttraumatic stress symptoms and disorder: 3 open-label trials. Journal of clinical psychopharmacology, 31(4), 547–50. Burghardt, N. S., Bush, D. E., McEwen, B. S., & LeDoux, J. E. (2007). Acute selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors increase conditioned fear expression: blockade with a 5-HT(2C) receptor antagonist. Biological Psychiatry, 62(10), 1111–8. Burghardt, N. S., Sullivan, G. M., McEwen, B. S., Gorman, J. M., & LeDoux, J. E. (2004). The selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor citalopram increases fear after acute treatment but reduces fear with chronic treatment: a comparison with tianeptine. Biological Psychiatry, 55(12), 1171–8. Büchel, C., Morris, J., Dolan, R. J., & Friston, K. J. (1998). Brain systems mediating aversive conditioning: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron, 20(5), 947–57. Cahill, L., & Alkire, M. T. (2003). Epinephrine enhancement of human memory consolidation: interaction with arousal at encoding. Neurobiology of learning and memory, 79(2), 194–8. 54 Cahill, L., Gorski, L., & Le, K. (2003). Enhanced human memory consolidation with post-learning stress: interaction with the degree of arousal at encoding. Learning & Memory, 10(4), 270–4. Caspi, A., Sugden, K., Moffitt, T. E., Taylor, A., Craig, I. W., Harrington, H., McClay, J., et al. (2003). Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science, 301(5631), 386–9. Chambers, J. A., Power, K. G., & Durham, R. C. (2004). The relationship between trait vulnerability and anxiety and depressive diagnoses at long-term follow-up of Generalized Anxiety Disorder. Journal of anxiety disorders, 18(5), 587–607. Chen, J., Lipska, B. K., Halim, N., Ma, Q. D., Matsumoto, M., Melhem, S., Kolachana, B. S., et al. (2004). Functional analysis of genetic variation in catecholO-methyltransferase (COMT): effects on mRNA, protein, and enzyme activity in postmortem human brain. American Journal of Human Genetics, 75(5), 807– 21. Clugnet, M. C., & LeDoux, J. E. (1990). Synaptic plasticity in fear conditioning circuits: induction of LTP in the lateral nucleus of the amygdala by stimulation of the medial geniculate body. The Journal of neuroscience, 10(8), 2818–24. Critchley, H. D. (2002). Electrodermal Responses: What Happens in the Brain. The Neuroscientist, 8(2), 132–142. Crişan, L. G., Pana, S., Vulturar, R., Heilman, R. M., Szekely, R., Druğa, B., Dragoş, N., et al. (2009). Genetic contributions of the serotonin transporter to social learning of fear and economic decision making. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4(4), 399–408. Darvas, M., Fadok, J. P., & Palmiter, R. D. (2011). Requirement of dopamine signaling in the amygdala and striatum for learning and maintenance of a conditioned avoidance response. Learning & memory, 18(3), 136–43. Davidson, P. O., Payne, R. W., & Sloane, R. B. (1964). Introversion, Neuroticism, and Conditioning. Journal of abnormal psychology, 68(2), 136–43. Davis, H. P., & Squire, L. R. (1984). Protein synthesis and memory: a review. Psychological bulletin, 96(3), 518–59. Davis, M., Falls, W. A., & Gewirtz, J. (2000). Neural systems involved in fear inhibition: Extinction and conditioned inhibition. In M. Myslobodsky & I. Weiner (Eds.), Contemporary issues in modeling psychopathology (pp. 113–142). Kluwer Academic Publishers. De Oca, B. M., DeCola, J. P., Maren, S., & Fanselow, M. S. (1998). Distinct regions of the periaqueductal gray are involved in the acquisition and expression of defensive responses. The Journal of neuroscience, 18(9), 3426–32. de Oliveira, A. R., Reimer, A. E., de Macedo, C. E. A., de Carvalho, M. C., Silva, M. A. D. S., & Brandão, M. L. (2011). Conditioned fear is modulated by D2 receptor pathway connecting the ventral tegmental area and basolateral amygdala. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 95(1), 37–45. Elsevier Inc. Debiec, J., & Ledoux, J. E. (2004). Disruption of reconsolidation but not consolidation of auditory fear conditioning by noradrenergic blockade in the amygdala. Neuroscience, 129(2), 267–72. Deeprose, C., Zhang, S., Dejong, H., Dalgleish, T., & Holmes, E. A. (2012). Imagery in the aftermath of viewing a traumatic film: using cognitive tasks to modulate the development of involuntary memory. Journal of behavior therapy and experimental psychiatry, 43(2), 758–64. Elsevier Ltd. Delgado, M. R., Nearing, K. I., Ledoux, J. E., & Phelps, E. A. (2008). Neural circuitry underlying the regulation of conditioned fear and its relation to extinction. Neuron, 59(5), 829–38. 55 Domschke, K., Baune, B. T., Havlik, L., Stuhrmann, A., Suslow, T., Kugel, H., Zwanzger, P., et al. (2012). Catechol-O-methyltransferase gene variation: Impact on amygdala response to aversive stimuli. NeuroImage, 60(4), 2222–2229. Domschke, K., & Dannlowski, U. (2010). Imaging genetics of anxiety disorders. NeuroImage, 53(3), 822–31. Domschke, K., Ohrmann, P., Braun, M., Suslow, T., Bauer, J., Hohoff, C., Kersting, A., et al. (2008). Influence of the catechol-O-methyltransferase val158met genotype on amygdala and prefrontal cortex emotional processing in panic disorder. Psychiatry research, 163(1), 13–20. Duncan, C. P. (1949). The Retroactive Effect of Electroschock on Learning. The Journal of comparative and physiological psychology, 42(32), 32–44. Dyck, C. van, & Malison, R. (2004). Central Serotonin Transporter Availability Measured With [ 123 I ] β-CIT SPECT in Relation to Serotonin Transporter Genotype. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161(March), 525–531. Dębiec, J., Bush, D. E. A, & LeDoux, J. E. (2011). Noradrenergic enhancement of reconsolidation in the amygdala impairs extinction of conditioned fear in rats--a possible mechanism for the persistence of traumatic memories in PTSD. Depression and anxiety, 28(3), 186–93. Eichenbaum, H. (1999). The hippocampus and mechanisms of declarative memory. Behavioural brain research, 103(2), 123–33. Eysenck, L. J. L. (1979). The conditioning model of neurosis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2, 155–200. Fadok, J. P., Darvas, M., Dickerson, T. M. K., & Palmiter, R. D. (2010). Long-term memory for pavlovian fear conditioning requires dopamine in the nucleus accumbens and basolateral amygdala. PloS ONE, 5(9), e12751. Foa, E. B. (2011). Prolonged exposure therapy: past, present, and future. Depression and anxiety, 28(12), 1043–7. Franklin, M. E., & Foa, E. B. (2011). Treatment of obsessive compulsive disorder. Annual review of clinical psychology, 7, 229–43. Fredrikson, M., Wik, G., Fischer, H., & Andersson, J. (1995). Affective and attentive neural networks in humans: a PET study of Pavlovian conditioning. Neuroreport, 7, 97–105. Garpenstrand, H., Annas, P., Ekblom, J., Oreland, L., & Fredrikson, M. (2001). Human fear conditioning is related to dopaminergic and serotonergic biological markers. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115(2), 358–364. Gershuny, B. S., & Sher, K. J. (1998). The relation between personality and anxiety: findings from a 3-year prospective study. Journal of abnormal psychology, 107(2), 252–62. Gottfried, J. a, & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Human orbitofrontal cortex mediates extinction learning while accessing conditioned representations of value. Nature neuroscience, 7(10), 1144–52. Hariri, A. R., Mattay, V. S., Tessitore, A., Kolachana, B., Fera, F., Goldman, D., Egan, M. F., et al. (2002). Serotonin transporter genetic variation and the response of the human amygdala. Science, 297(5580), 400–3. Harmer, C. J., Bhagwagar, Z., Cowen, P. J., & Goodwin, G. M. (2002). Acute administration of citalopram facilitates memory consolidation in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology, 163(1), 106–10. Hebb, D. O. (1949). The Organization of Behavior. New York: Wiley. Hirvonen, M. M., Någren, K., Rinne, J. O., Pesonen, U., Vahlberg, T., Hagelberg, N., & Hietala, J. (2010). COMT Val158Met genotype does not alter cortical or striatal dopamine D2 receptor availability in vivo. Molecular Imaging and Biology, 12(2), 192–7. 56 Holmes, E. A, James, E. L., Coode-Bate, T., & Deeprose, C. (2009). Can playing the computer game “Tetris” reduce the build-up of flashbacks for trauma? A proposal from cognitive science. PloS one, 4(1), e4153. Holmes, E. A, James, E. L., Kilford, E. J., & Deeprose, C. (2010). Key steps in developing a cognitive vaccine against traumatic flashbacks: visuospatial Tetris versus verbal Pub Quiz. PloS one, 5(11), e13706. Holzschneider, K., & Mulert, C. (2011). Neuroimaging in anxiety disorders. Dialogues in clinical neuroscience, 13(4), 453–61. Springer. Hong, J., Shu-leong, H., Tao, X., & Lap-Ping, Y. (1998). Distribution of catechol-Omethyltransfearse in human central nervous system. Neuroreport, 9(12), 2861– 2864. Horvath, F. E. (1963). Effects of basolateral amygdalectomy on three types of avoidance behavior in cats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 56(2), 380. American Psychological Association. Huff, N. C., Hernandez, J. A., Blanding, N. Q., & LaBar, K. S. (2009). Delayed extinction attenuates conditioned fear renewal and spontaneous recovery in humans. Behavioral neuroscience, 123(4), 834. American Psychological Association. Hugdahl, K., Berardi, A., Thompson, W. L., Kosslyn, S. M., Macy, R., Baker, D. P., Alpert, N. M., et al. (1995). Brain mechanisms in human classical conditioning: a PET blood flow study. NeuroReport-International Journal for Rapid Communications of Research in Neuroscience, 6(13), 1723–1727. Oxford, UK: Rapid Communications of Oxford Ltd.,[1990-. Inda, M. C., Muravieva, E. V., & Alberini, C. M. (2011). Memory Retrieval and the Passage of Time: From Reconsolidation and Strengthening to Extinction. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(5), 1635–1643. Johansen, J. P., Cain, C. K., Ostroff, L. E., & LeDoux, J. E. (2011). Molecular Mechanisms of Fear Learning and Memory. Cell, 147(3), 509–524. Jorm, F., Christensen, H., Henderson, S., Jacomb, P., Korten, E., & Rodgers, B. (2000). Predicting anxiety and depression from personality: is there a synergistic effect of neuroticism and extraversion? Journal of abnormal psychology, 109(1), 145–9. Kalisch, R., Korenfeld, E., Stephan, K. E., Weiskopf, N., Seymour, B., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Context-dependent human extinction memory is mediated by a ventromedial prefrontal and hippocampal network. The Journal of neuroscience, 26(37), 9503–11. Kehoe, E., & Macrae, M. (1998). Savings in animal learning: Implications for relapse and maintenance after therapy. Behavior therapy, 28, 141–155. Kim, J. J., & Fanselow, M. S. (1992). Modality-specific retrograde amnesia of fear. Science, 256(5057), 675–7. Kindt, M., Soeter, M., & Vervliet, B. (2009). Beyond extinction: erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Nature Neuroscience, 12(3), 256–8. Klüver, H., & Bucy, P. B. (1939). Preliminary analysis of functions of the temporal lobes in monkeys. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 42(6), 979–1000. Knight, D. C., Smith, C. N., Cheng, D. T., Stein, E. A, & Helmstetter, F. J. (2004). Amygdala and hippocampal activity during acquisition and extinction of human fear conditioning. Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience, 4(3), 317–25. Kwapis, J. L., Jarome, T. J., Schiff, J. C., & Helmstetter, F. J. (2011). Memory consolidation in both trace and delay fear conditioning is disrupted by intraamygdala infusion of the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin. Learning & memory, 18(11), 728–32. 57 LaBar, K. S., Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., LeDoux, J. E., & Phelps, E. A. (1998). Human amygdala activation during conditioned fear acquisition and extinction: a mixed-trial fMRI study. Neuron, 20(5), 937–945. LaBar, K. S., LeDoux, J. E., Spencer, D. D., & Phelps, E. A. (1995). Impaired fear conditioning following unilateral temporal lobectomy in humans. The Journal of Neuroscience, 15(10), 6846–6855. Lane, R. D., Fink, G. R., Chau, P. M., & Dolan, R. J. (1997). Neural activation during selective attention to subjective emotional responses. Neuroreport, 8(18), 3969–72. Lang, S., Kroll, A., Lipinski, S. J., Wessa, M., Ridder, S., Christmann, C., Schad, L. R., et al. (2009). Context conditioning and extinction in humans: differential contribution of the hippocampus, amygdala and prefrontal cortex. The European journal of neuroscience, 29(4), 823–32. LeDoux, J. (2003). The emotional brain, fear, and the amygdala. Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology, 23(4-5), 727–38. LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion Circuits in the Brain. Annual review of neuroscience, 23, 155–184. Lelli-Chiesa, G., Kempton, M. J., Jogia, J., Tatarelli, R., Girardi, P., Powell, J., Collier, D. a, et al. (2011). The impact of the Val158Met catechol-Omethyltransferase genotype on neural correlates of sad facial affect processing in patients with bipolar disorder and their relatives. Psychological Medicine, 41(4), 779–88. Lesch, K. P., Bengel, D., Heils, A., Sabol, S. Z., Greenberg, B. D., Petri, S., Benjamin, J., et al. (1996). Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science, 274(5292), 1527– 31. Liang, K. C., Juler, R. G., & McGaugh, J. L. (1986). Modulating effects of posttraining epinephrine on memory: involvement of the amygdala noradrenergic system. Brain research, 368(1), 125–33. Lissek, S., Levenson, J., Biggs, A. L., Johnson, L. L., Ameli, R., Pine, D. S., & Grillon, C. (2008). Elevated fear conditioning to socially relevant unconditioned stimuli in social anxiety disorder. The American journal of psychiatry, 165(1), 124–32. Lissek, S., Powers, A. S., McClure, E. B., Phelps, E. a, Woldehawariat, G., Grillon, C., & Pine, D. S. (2005). Classical fear conditioning in the anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis. Behaviour research and therapy, 43(11), 1391–424. Logothetis, N. K. (2008). What we can do and what we cannot do with fMRI. Nature, 453(7197), 869–78. Lonsdorf, T. B., Weike, A. I., Nikamo, P., Schalling, M., Hamm, A. O., & Ohman, A. (2009). Genetic gating of human fear learning and extinction: possible implications for gene-environment interaction in anxiety disorder. Psychological Science, 20(2), 198–206. Maren, S. (2001). Neurobiology of Pavlovian fear conditioning. Annual review of neuroscience, 24, 897–931. Maren, Stephen, Fanselow, M. S., & Angeles, L. (1995). Synaptic Plasticity in the Basolateral Hippocampal Formation Stimulation Amygdala in viva Induced by. Journal of Neuroscience, 15(November). Marinelli, M., & Piazza, P. V. (2002). Interaction between glucocorticoid hormones, stress and psychostimulant drugs. European Journal of Neuroscience, 16(3), 387–394. McDonald, A. J. (1998). Cortical pathways to the mammalian amygdala. Progress in neurobiology, 55(3), 257–332. Elsevier. 58 McGaugh, J. L. (2000). Memory - a century of consolidation. Science, 287(5451), 248–51. McGaugh, J. L. (2004). The amygdala modulates the consolidation of memories of emotionally arousing experiences. Annual review of neuroscience, 27, 1–28. Milad, M. R., Quinn, B. T., Pitman, R. K., Orr, S. P., Fischl, B., & Rauch, S. L. (2005). Thickness of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in humans is correlated with extinction memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(30), 10706–11. Milad, M. R., & Quirk, G. J. (2002). Neurons in medial prefrontal cortex signal memory for fear extinction. Nature, 420(September), 713–717. Milad, M. R., Wright, C. I., Orr, S. P., Pitman, R. K., Quirk, G. J., & Rauch, S. L. (2007). Recall of fear extinction in humans activates the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and hippocampus in concert. Biological Psychiatry, 62(5), 446–54. Milner, B., Squire, L. R., & Kandel, E. R. (1998). Cognitive Neuroscience and the Study of Memory. Neuron, 20(3), 445–468. Mineka, S, Mystkowski, J., & Hladek, D. (1999). The effects of changing contexts on return of fear following exposure therapy for spider fear. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67(4), 599–604. Mineka, Susan, & Oehlberg, K. (2008). The relevance of recent developments in classical conditioning to understanding the etiology and maintenance of anxiety disorders. Acta psychologica, 127(3), 567–80. Misanin, J. R., Miller, R. R., & Lewis, D. J. (1968). Retrograde amnesia produced by electroconvulsive shock after reactivation of a consolidated memory trace. Science, 160(3827), 554–5. Monfils, M.-H., Cowansage, K. K., Klann, E., & LeDoux, J. E. (2009). Extinctionreconsolidation boundaries: key to persistent attenuation of fear memories. Science, 324(5929), 951–5. Morris, J., Frith, C., Perrett, D., & Rowland, D. (1996). A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions. Nature, (383), 812–815. Morris, J. S., Friston, K. J., & Dolan, R. J. (1997). Neural responses to salient visual stimuli. Proceedings. Biological sciences / The Royal Society, 264(1382), 769– 75. Munafò, M. R., Brown, S. M., & Hariri, A. R. (2008). Serotonin transporter (5HTTLPR) genotype and amygdala activation: a meta-analysis. Biological psychiatry, 63(9), 852–7. Myers, K M, & Davis, M. (2007). Mechanisms of fear extinction. Molecular psychiatry, 12(2), 120–50. Myers, Karyn M, Ressler, K. J., & Davis, M. (2006). Different mechanisms of fear extinction dependent on length of time since fear acquisition. Learning & memory, 13(2), 216–23. Mystkowski, J. L., Mineka, S., Vernon, L. L., & Zinbarg, R. E. (2003). Changes in caffeine states enhance return of fear in spider phobia. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71(2), 243–250. Nader, K., Schafe, G. E., & Le Doux, J. E. (2000). Fear memories require protein synthesis in the amygdala for reconsolidation after retrieval. Nature, 406(6797), 722–6. Nordquist, N., & Oreland, L. (2010). Serotonin, genetic variability, behaviour, and psychiatric disorders--a review. Upsala Journal of Medical Sciences, 115(1), 2– 10. 59 Norrholm, S. D., Vervliet, B., Jovanovic, T., Boshoven, W., Myers, K. M., Davis, M., Rothbaum, B., et al. (2008). Timing of extinction relative to acquisition: a parametric analysis of fear extinction in humans. Behavioral neuroscience, 122(5), 1016–30. Oppenheimer, S. M., Gelb, A., Girvin, J. P., & Hachinsky, V. C. (1992). Cardiovascular effects of human insular cortex stimulation. Neurology, 42, 1727–32. Orr, S. P., Metzger, L. J., Lasko, N. B., Macklin, M. L., Peri, T., & Pitman, R. K. (2000). De novo conditioning in trauma-exposed individuals with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109(2), 290– 298. Otto, M. W., Leyro, T. M., Christian, K., Deveney, C. M., Reese, H., Pollack, M. H., & Orr, S. P. (2007). Prediction of “fear” acquisition in healthy control participants in a De Novo fear-conditioning paradigm. Behavior modification, 31(1), 32–51. Ougrin, D. (2011). Efficacy of exposure versus cognitive therapy in anxiety disorders: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC psychiatry, 11(1), 200. Pape, H., & Pare, D. (2010). Plastic synaptic networks of the amygdala for the acquisition, expression, and extinction of conditioned fear. Physiological reviews, 90, 419–463. Pavlov, I. (1927). Conditioned reflexes: An investigation of the physiological activity of the cerebral cortex. London: Oxford University Press. Pezawas, L., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Drabant, E. M., Verchinski, B., Munoz, K. E., Kolachana, B. S., Egan, M. F., et al. (2005). 5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-amygdala interactions: a genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression. Nature Neuroscience, 8(6), 828–34. Phelps, E. A., Delgado, M. R., Nearing, K. I., & LeDoux, J. E. (2004). Extinction learning in humans: role of the amygdala and vmPFC. Neuron, 43(6), 897–905. Phillips, R. G., & LeDoux, J. E. (1992). Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus and its connection to activity in other brain structures. Behavioral neuroscience, 106, 274–285. Pineles, S. L., Vogt, D. S., & Orr, S. P. (2009). Personality and fear responses during conditioning: Beyond extraversion. Personality and individual differences, 46(1), 48–53. Pitman, R. K., Sanders, K. M., Zusman, R. M., Healy, A. R., Cheema, F., Lasko, N. B., Cahill, L., et al. (2002). Pilot study of secondary prevention of posttraumatic stress disorder with propranolol. Biological psychiatry, 51(2), 189–92. Quirk, G. J., & Mueller, D. (2008). Neural mechanisms of extinction learning and retrieval. Neuropsychopharmacology, 33(1), 56–72. Rachman, S. (1989). The return of fear: Review and prospect. Clinical Psychology Review, 9(2), 147–168. Raczka, K., Mechias, M.-L., Gartmann, N., Reif, A., Deckert, J., Pessiglione, M., & Kalisch, R. (2011). Empirical support for an involvement of the mesostriatal dopamine system in human fear extinction. Translational Psychiatry, 1(6), e12. Reijmers, L. G., Perkins, B. L., Matsuo, N., & Mayford, M. (2007). Localization of a stable neural correlate of associative memory. Science, 317(5842), 1230–3. Rescorla, R. a, & Heth, C. D. (1975). Reinstatement of fear to an extinguished conditioned stimulus. Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes, 1(1), 88–96. Ressler, K. J., Paschall, G., Zhou, X., & Davis, M. (2002). Regulation of synaptic plasticity genes during consolidation of fear conditioning. The Journal of neuroscience, 22(18), 7892–902. 60 Robinson, E. (1963). Effect of amygdalectomy on fear-motivated behavior in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 56(5), 814. Rodrigues, S. M., LeDoux, J. E., & Sapolsky, R. M. (2009). The influence of stress hormones on fear circuitry. Annual review of neuroscience, 32, 289–313. Rodriguez, B., Craske, M., & Mineka, S. (1999). Context-specificity of relapse : effects of therapist and environmental context on return of fear. Behaviour Research and, 37, 845–862. Rodriguez, W. A., Horne, C. A., & Padilla, J. L. (1999). Effects of glucose and fructose on recently reactivated and recently acquired memories. Progress in NeuroPsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 23(7), 1285–1317. Rogan, M. T., & LeDoux, J. E. (1995). LTP is accompanied by commensurate enhancement of auditory-evoked responses in a fear conditioning circuit. Neuron, 15(1), 127–36. Rosenkranz, J., & Grace, A. A. (1999). Modulation of basolateral amygdala neuronal firing and afferent drive by dopamine receptor activation in vivo. The Journal of Neuroscience, 19(24), 11027–39. Rossato, J. I., Bevilaqua, L. R. M., Izquierdo, I., Medina, J. H., & Cammarota, M. (2009). Dopamine controls persistence of long-term memory storage. Science, 325(5943), 1017–20. Samochowiec, J., Hajduk, A., Samochowiec, A., Horodnicki, J., Stepień, G., Grzywacz, A., & Kucharska-Mazur, J. (2004). Association studies of MAO-A, COMT, and 5-HTT genes polymorphisms in patients with anxiety disorders of the phobic spectrum. Psychiatry research, 128(1), 21–6. Schiller, D., Monfils, M.-H., Raio, C. M., Johnson, D. C., Ledoux, J. E., & Phelps, E. a. (2010). Preventing the return of fear in humans using reconsolidation update mechanisms. Nature, 463(7277), 49–53. Nature Publishing Group. Sehlmeyer, C., Schöning, S., Zwitserlood, P., Pfleiderer, B., Kircher, T., Arolt, V., & Konrad, C. (2009). Human fear conditioning and extinction in neuroimaging: a systematic review. PloS one, 4(6), e5865. Seligman, M. E. P. (1972). Phobias and preparedness. In M. E. P. Seligman & J. L. Hager (Eds.), Biological boundaries of learning (pp. 451–462). New York: Meredith corporation. Shioe, K., Ichimiya, T., Suhara, T., Takano, A., Sudo, Y., Yasuno, F., Hirano, M., et al. (2003). No association between genotype of the promoter region of serotonin transporter gene and serotonin transporter binding in human brain measured by PET. Synapse, 48(4), 184–8. Slifstein, M., Kolachana, B., Simpson, E. H., Tabares, P., Cheng, B., Duvall, M., Frankle, W. G., et al. (2008). COMT genotype predicts cortical-limbic D1 receptor availability measured with [11C]NNC112 and PET. Molecular Psychiatry, 13(8), 821–7. Soeter, M., & Kindt, M. (2011). Noradrenergic enhancement of associative fear memory in humans. Neurobiology of learning and memory. Elsevier Inc. Spear, N. E., Lewis, D. J., Mcgaugh, J. L., & Ralph, R. (1973). Retrieval of memory in animals. Psychological Review, 80(3), 163–194. Spoormaker, V. I., Schröter, M. S., Andrade, K. C., Dresler, M., Kiem, S. a, GoyaMaldonado, R., Wetter, T. C., et al. (2011). Effects of rapid eye movement sleep deprivation on fear extinction recall and prediction error signaling. Human brain mapping, 00(May), 1–15. Squire, L. R., Slater, P. C., & Chace, P. M. (1976). Reactivation of Recent or Remote Memory before Electroconvulsive Therapy Does Not Produce Retrograde Amnesia. Behavioral Biology, 343(6135), 335–343. 61 Stein, M. B., Fallin, M. D., Schork, N. J., & Gelernter, J. (2005). COMT polymorphisms and anxiety-related personality traits. Neuropsychopharmacology, 30(11), 2092–102. Tully, K., Li, Y., Tsvetkov, E., & Bolshakov, V. Y. (2007). Norepinephrine enables the induction of associative long-term potentiation at thalamo-amygdala synapses. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(35), 14146–50. Visser, R. M., Scholte, H. S., & Kindt, M. (2011). Associative Learning Increases Trial-by-Trial Similarity of BOLD-MRI Patterns. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(33), 12021–12028. Walker, M. P., Brakefield, T., & Hobson, J. A. (2003). Dissociable stages of human memory consolidation and reconsolidation. Nature, 425(October), 616–620. Weiskrantz, L. (1956). Behavioral changes associated with ablation of the amygdaloid complex in monkeys. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 49(4), 381. American Psychological Association. Wilensky, A. E., Schafe, G. E., Kristensen, M. P., & LeDoux, J. E. (2006). Rethinking the fear circuit: the central nucleus of the amygdala is required for the acquisition, consolidation, and expression of Pavlovian fear conditioning. The Journal of neuroscience, 26(48), 12387–96. Wray, N. R., James, M. R., Dumenil, T., Handoko, H. Y., Lind, P., Montgomery, G. W., & Martin, N. G. (2008). Association study of candidate variants of COMT with neuroticism, anxiety and depression. American journal of medical genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric genetics, 147B(7), 1314–8. Yágüez, L., Coen, S., Gregory, L. J., Amaro, E., Altman, C., Brammer, M. J., Bullmore, E. T., et al. (2005). Brain Response to Visceral Aversive Conditioning: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study. Gastroenterology, 128(7), 1819–1829. 62 Acta Universitatis Upsaliensis Digital Comprehensive Summaries of Uppsala Dissertations from the Faculty of Social Sciences 81 Editor: The Dean of the Faculty of Social Sciences A doctoral dissertation from the Faculty of Social Sciences, Uppsala University, is usually a summary of a number of papers. A few copies of the complete dissertation are kept at major Swedish research libraries, while the summary alone is distributed internationally through the series Digital Comprehensive Summaries of Uppsala Dissertations from the Faculty of Social Sciences. Distribution: publications.uu.se urn:nbn:se:uu:diva-180202 ACTA UNIVERSITATIS UPSALIENSIS UPPSALA 2012