Download Check out figures to understand this tricky wiring pattern… After

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Neuroeconomics wikipedia , lookup

Embodied cognitive science wikipedia , lookup

Connectome wikipedia , lookup

Sensory cue wikipedia , lookup

Convolutional neural network wikipedia , lookup

Cognitive neuroscience of music wikipedia , lookup

Neuroplasticity wikipedia , lookup

Cortical cooling wikipedia , lookup

Time perception wikipedia , lookup

Lateralization of brain function wikipedia , lookup

Human brain wikipedia , lookup

Allochiria wikipedia , lookup

Emotional lateralization wikipedia , lookup

Visual search wikipedia , lookup

Transsaccadic memory wikipedia , lookup

Visual selective attention in dementia wikipedia , lookup

Neural correlates of consciousness wikipedia , lookup

Dual consciousness wikipedia , lookup

Visual memory wikipedia , lookup

Visual extinction wikipedia , lookup

Visual servoing wikipedia , lookup

Visual N1 wikipedia , lookup

Neuroesthetics wikipedia , lookup

Superior colliculus wikipedia , lookup

Feature detection (nervous system) wikipedia , lookup

Blindsight wikipedia , lookup

Inferior temporal gyrus wikipedia , lookup

P200 wikipedia , lookup

C1 and P1 (neuroscience) wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Check out figures to understand
this tricky wiring pattern…
After leaving the retina, the
outputs of each eye are split
• The nasal (toward the nose) half of each
eye's visual field crosses from one side to
the other at the optic chiasm
• The temporal half (towards the temple)
remains on the same side as its eye-oforigin
2/3/2006
1
– This splitting and crossing re-organizes the retinal
outputs so that the left hemisphere processes
information from the right visual field, and the right
hemisphere processes information from the left visual
field
2/3/2006
2
Visual processing beyond the retina
Visual Pathway…..
• Major Pathways
– optic chiasm
• medial fibers
cross over
• left visual field
processed in right
hemisphere
• right visual field
processed in left
hemisphere
2/3/2006
3
2/3/2006
4
• Retinal ganglion cells actually
come in (at least) 2 flavors:
– M (magnocellular)
– P (parvocellular)
2/3/2006
5
2/3/2006
6
•1
Thalamus contains…
Thalamus…..
• dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN)
– Left and right LGN
– 6 layers in each
• lower two –magnocellular
– M-cells - movement
• 4 upper layers – parvocellular
– P-cells – color, fine texture, depth
• Layers 1, 4, 6 from contralateral eye
• Layers 2,3,5 from ipsilateral
– Forms a “retinotopic map”
• Locations in LGN correspond to locations on the retina
– output of LGN forms the optic radiations
2/3/2006
7
2/3/2006
8
LGN
Occipital Lobe
primary visual cortex (“striate cortex” due to
“striped” appearance)
2/3/2006
9
2/3/2006
Primary Visual Cortex: V1
10
Primary visual cortex….
• V1 has a topographic/retinotopic map of the
visual world
• This means that there is a "neural image"
retaining the spatial layout of the pattern of
light that falls on the retina
• This map has several interesting
characteristics
2/3/2006
11
2/3/2006
12
•2
Characteristics of Retinotopic Map
Striate Cortex Anatomy/Function
• Remember that there are 2 V1s in each
person (left and right hemispheres)
– Each V1 has a representation of the
opposite half of the visual field (e.g., left
V1 has a map of the right visual field, and
vice versa)
– Each V1 does not simply receive input
from the opposite eye; the outputs of each
retina are split (left half/right half) and
then run through the LGN to the
appropriate V1
• six major “layers”
• mapped to contralateral half of visual field
„Cortical magnification devotes 25% to foveal
vision
• processes the “features” of visual stimuli
– higher-order (that is, does not merely respond to
“spots” of light
2/3/2006
13
Characteristics of Retinotopic Map
• Just as the image of the world is inverted
when projected onto the retina, the
retinotopic V1 map is upside down (and the
right hemisphere's V1 has a topographic
map of the left visual field, and vice versa)
• Cortical magnification
2/3/2006
14
3 main types of cells in primary
visual cortex
• Simple
• Complex
• End-stopped (formerly Hypercomplex)
– more cortical space is dedicated to the fovea
than the periphery (remember the higher
density of photoreceptors in the fovea, hence
clearer vision)
2/3/2006
15
2/3/2006
Simple
Complex
• Receptive fields often have a long, narrow
bar of light (ON) and flanking (OFF) parts
• Other types are the opposite (responding to
dark bars) or simply respond to a light/dark
edge
2/3/2006
16
• Bars of light must be oriented correctly, but can appear anywhere in
the receptive field
• Moving the bar through the field produces a sustained response
• Complex cells often show direction-selectivity:
– they fire more when the bar moves in one direction, and are
suppressed by motion in the opposite direction
17
2/3/2006
18
•3
End-stopped (formerly
Hypercomplex)
Characteristics of Retinotopic Map
• Many simple and complex cells exhibit length
summation
– if an appropriate bar is placed in the visual field, they
fire action potentials; if the bar is made longer, they fire
more, up to the extent of the full receptive field
• However, end-stopped cells increase their
responses with increases in bar length up to a limit
that is smaller than the receptive field
2/3/2006
19
• Remember that there are 2 V1s in each person (left and right
hemispheres)
– Each V1 has a representation of the opposite half of the visual field
(e.g., left V1 has a map of the right visual field, and vice versa)
– Each V1 does not simply receive input from the opposite eye; the
outputs of each retina are split (left half/right half) and then run
through the LGN to the appropriate V1
• Just as the image of the world is inverted when projected onto the
retina, the retinotopic V1 map is upside down (and the right
hemisphere's V1 has a topographic map of the left visual field, and
vice versa)
• Cortical magnification
– more cortical space is dedicated to the fovea than the periphery
(remember the higher density of photoreceptors in the fovea, hence
clearer vision)
2/3/2006
Ocular Dominance Columns
20
Architecture of V1
• Orientation columns:
– as you move perpendicular to the surface, the
preferred orientation of the cells changes
gradually from horizontal to vertical and back
again
2/3/2006
21
2/3/2006
22
2/3/2006
24
Orientation Columns combine
with Ocular Dominance Columns
2/3/2006
23
•4
Defining and Separating Different
Brain Areas
Secondary Visual Areas
• There are approximately 30 visual areas
after V1
• Brain areas can be differentiated according to 4 main
criteria:
– Function: physiology
• Neurons in different parts of the brain are responsive to different
aspects of the stimulus (= do different things).
– Architecture: microanatomy can differ widely across brain areas
• For example, V1 is also referred to as "striate cortex" because it has a
series of stripes that run parallel to the surface; these stripes end
abruptly at the end of V1.
– Connections:
– The functional specialization hypothesis drives
much of the research about these areas
– Some areas seem specialized for processing a
certain aspect of visual information (e.g., MT motion, V4 - color (?))
• different areas feed forward and also receive backward-reaching
connections from distinct areas.
– Topography: e.g., retinotopy
• Each distinct visual area has its own retinotopic map.
Remember 'FACT' as a mnemonic
2/3/2006
25
2/3/2006
26
Secondary Visual Areas
• Cortical areas dedicated to vision are
densely interconnected, and can seem quite
confusing at first glance
2/3/2006
27
2/3/2006
28
29
2/3/2006
30
Secondary Visual Areas
• However, a more general organization is evident in a pair
of parallel pathways
– What pathway
• Temporal lobe; recognition of objects
– Where pathway
• Parietal lobe; motion, spatial orientation,
localization
2/3/2006
•5
2/3/2006
31
•6