Download Chapter 14:

Document related concepts

Human brain wikipedia , lookup

Nervous system network models wikipedia , lookup

Perception of infrasound wikipedia , lookup

Cortical cooling wikipedia , lookup

Neuroeconomics wikipedia , lookup

Eyeblink conditioning wikipedia , lookup

Neuroesthetics wikipedia , lookup

Metastability in the brain wikipedia , lookup

Endocannabinoid system wikipedia , lookup

Psychophysics wikipedia , lookup

Embodied cognitive science wikipedia , lookup

Allochiria wikipedia , lookup

Aging brain wikipedia , lookup

Synaptic gating wikipedia , lookup

Sensory substitution wikipedia , lookup

Neuroplasticity wikipedia , lookup

Neural correlates of consciousness wikipedia , lookup

Perception wikipedia , lookup

Evoked potential wikipedia , lookup

Feature detection (nervous system) wikipedia , lookup

Neuropsychopharmacology wikipedia , lookup

Neurostimulation wikipedia , lookup

Clinical neurochemistry wikipedia , lookup

Microneurography wikipedia , lookup

Stimulus (physiology) wikipedia , lookup

Time perception wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Chapter 14:
The Cutaneous Senses
Overview of Questions
• Are there specialized receptors in the skin for
sensing different tactile qualities?
• What is the most sensitive part of the body?
• Is it possible to reduce pain with your
thoughts?
Somatosensory System
• There are three parts
– Cutaneous senses - perception of touch
and pain from stimulation of the skin
– Proprioception - ability to sense position of
the body and limbs
– Kinesthesis - ability to sense movement of
body and limbs
Cutaneous System
• Skin - heaviest organ in the body
– Protects the organism by keeping
damaging agents from penetrating the
body.
– Epidermis is the outer layer of the skin,
which is made up of dead skin cells.
– Dermis is below the epidermis and
contains mechanoreceptors that respond
to stimuli such as pressure, stretching, and
vibration.
Mechanoreceptors
• Two types located close to surface of the skin
– Merkel receptor fires continuously while
stimulus is present.
• Responsible for sensing fine details
– Meissner corpuscle fires only when a
stimulus is first applied and when it is
removed.
• Responsible for controlling hand-grip
Figure 14.1 A cross section of glabrous (without hairs or projections) skin, showing the layers of the skin
and the structure, firing properties and perceptions associated with the Merkel receptor and Meissner
corpuscle - two mechanoreceptors that are near the surface of the skin.
Mechanoreceptors - continued
• Two types located deeper in the skin
– Ruffini cylinder fires continuously to
stimulation
• Associated with perceiving stretching of
the skin
– Pacinian corpuscle fires only when a
stimulus is first applied and when it is
removed.
• Associated with sensing rapid vibrations
and fine texture
Figure 14.2 A cross section of glabrous skin, showing the structure, firing properties and perceptions
associated with the Ruffini cylinder and the Pacinian corpuscle - two mechanoreceptors that are deeper in
the skin.
Pathways from Skin to Cortex
• Nerve fibers travel in bundles (peripheral
nerves) to the spinal cord.
• Two major pathways in the spinal cord
– Medial lemniscal pathway consists of large
fibers that carry proprioceptive and touch
information.
– Spinothalamic pathway consists of smaller
fibers that carry temperature and pain
information.
– These cross over to the opposite side of
the body and synapse in the thalamus.
Figure 14.3 The pathway from receptors in the skin to the somatosensory receiving area of the cortex. The
fiber carrying signals from a receptor in the finger enters the spinal cord through the dorsal root and then
travels up the spinal cord in two pathways: the medial lemniscus and the spinothalamic tract. These
pathways synapse in the ventrolateral nucleus of the thalamus and then send fibers to the somatosensory
cortex in the parietal lobe.
Maps of the Body on the Cortex
• Signals travel from the thalamus to the
somatosensory receiving area (S1) and the
secondary receiving area (S2) in the parietal
lobe.
• Body map (homunculus) on the cortex in S1
and S2 shows more cortical space allocated
to parts of the body that are responsible for
detail.
• Plasticity in neural functioning leads to
multiple homunculi and changes in how
cortical cells are allocated to body parts.
Figure 14.4 (a) The sensory homunculus on the somatosensory cortex. Parts of the body with the highest
tactile acuity are represented by larger areas on the cortex. (b) The somatosensory cortex in the parietal
lobe. The primary somatosensory area, S1 (light shading), receives inputs from the ventrolateral nucleus of
the thalamus. The secondary somatosensory area, S2 (dark shading), is partially hidden behind the
temporal lobe. (Adapted from penfield & Rasmussen, 1950).
Figure 14.5 (a) Each numbered zone represents the area in the somatosensory cortex that represents one
of the monkey’s five fingers. The shaded area on the zone for finger 2 is the part of the cortex that
represents the small area on the tip of the finger shown in (b). (c) The shaded region shows how the area
representing the fingertip increased in size after this area was heavily stimulated over a 2-month period.
(From Merzenich et al., 1988)
Perceiving Details
• Measuring tactile acuity
– Two-point threshold - minimum separation
needed between two points to perceive
them as two units
– Grating acuity - placing a grooved stimulus
on the skin and asking the participant to
indicate the orientation of the grating
– Raised pattern identification - using such
patterns to determine the smallest size that
can be identified
Figure 14.7 Methods for determining tactile acuity (a) two-point threshold; (b) grating acuity.
Receptor Mechanisms for Tactile Acuity
• There is a high density of Merkel receptors in
the fingertips.
• Merkel receptors are densely packed on the
fingertips - similar to cones in the fovea.
• Both two-point thresholds and grating acuity
studies show these results.
Figure 14.8 (a) The firing of the fiber associated with a Merkel receptor to the grooved stimulus pattern. (b)
The firing of the fiber associated with a Pacinian corpuscle to the same grooved pattern. Results such as
these indicate that the Merkel receptor signals details (Johnson, 2002). (Adapted from Phillips & Johnson,
1981.)
Figure 14.9 Correlation between density of Merkel receptors density and tactile acuity. (From Craig & Lyte,
2002.)
Cortical Mechanisms for Tactile Acuity
• Body areas with high acuity have larger areas
of cortical tissue devoted to them.
• This parallels the “magnification factor” seen
in the visual cortex for the cones in the fovea.
• Areas with higher acuity also have smaller
receptive fields on the skin.
Figure 14.10 Two-point thresholds for males. Two-point thresholds for females follow the same pattern.
(From S. Weinstein, 1968.)
Figure 14.11 Receptive fields of monkey cortical neurons that fire (a) when the fingers are stimulated; (b)
when the hand is stimulated; and (c) when the arm is stimulated. (d) Stimulation of two nearby points on the
finger causes separated activation on the finger area of the cortex, but stimulation of two nearby points on
the arm causes overlapping activation in the arm area of the cortex. (From Kandel & Jessell, 1991 (a-c).
Perceiving Vibration
• Pacinian corpuscle (PC) is primarily
responsible for sensing vibration.
– Nerve fibers associated with PCs respond
best to high rates of vibration.
– The structure of the PC is responsible for
the response to vibration - fibers without
the PC only respond to continuous
pressure.
Figure 14.12 (a) When a vibrating pressure stimulus is applied to the Pacinian corpuscle, it transmits these
pressure vibrations to the nerve fiber. (b) When a continuous pressure stimulus is applied to the Pacinian
corpuscle, it does not transmit the continuous pressure to the fiber. (c) Lowenstein determined how the fiber
fired to stimulation of the corpuscle (at A), and to direct stimulation of the fiber (at B) (Adapted from
Lowenstein, 1960)
Perceiving Texture
• Katz (1925) proposed that perception of
texture depends on two cues
– Spatial cues are determined by the size,
shape, and distribution of surface
elements.
– Temporal cues are determined by the rate
of vibration as skin is moved across finely
textured surfaces.
• Two receptors may be responsible for this
process - called the duplex theory of texture
perception
Perceiving Texture - continued
• Past research showed support for the role of
spatial cues.
• Recent research by Hollins and Reisner
shows support for the role of temporal cues.
– In order to detect differences between fine
textures, participants needed to move their
fingers across the surface.
Figure 14.13 (a) Participants in Hollins and Reisner’s (2000) experiment perceived the roughness of two fine
surfaces to be essentially the same when felt with stationary fingers, but (b) could perceive the difference
between the two surfaces when they were allowed to move their fingers.
Experiment by Hollins et al.
• Adaptation experiment - participants’ skin was
adapted with either
– 10-Hz stimulus for six minutes to adapt the
Meissner corpuscle.
– 250-Hz stimulus for six minutes to adapt
the Pacinian corpuscle.
• Results showed that only the adaptation to
the 250-Hz stimulus affected the perception
of fine textures.
Figure 14.14 Eliminating the action of fibers associated with the Meissner corpuscle by adaptation to a 10Hz vibration had no effect on perception of a fine texture, but eliminating the action of the Pacinian
corpuscle by adapting to a 250-Hz vibration eliminated the ability to sense the fine textures. (Data from
Hollins, et al., 2001.)
Perceiving Objects
• Humans use active rather than passive touch
to interact with the environment.
• Haptic perception is the active exploration of
3-D objects with the hand.
– It uses three distinct systems
• Sensory system
• Motor system
• Cognitive system
Perceiving Objects - continued
• Psychophysical research shows that people
can identify objects haptically in one to two
seconds.
• Klatzky et al. have shown that people use
exploratory procedures (EPs)
– Lateral motion
– Contour following
– Pressure
– Enclosure
Figure 14.15 Some of the exploratory procedures (EPs) observed by Lederman and Klatzky as participants
identified objects. (From Lederman & Klatzky, 1987)
The Physiology of Tactile Object Perception
• The firing pattern of groups of
mechanoreceptors signals shape, such as
the curvature of an object
• Neurons further upstream become more
specialized
– Monkey’s thalamus shows cells that
respond to center-surround receptive
fields.
• Somatosensory cortex shows cells that
respond maximally to orientations and
direction of movement.
Figure 14.16 (a) Response of fibers in the fingertips to touching a high-curvature stimulus. The height of the
profile indicates the firing rate at different places across the fingertip. (b) The profile of firing to touching a
stimulus with more gentle curvature. (From Goodwin, 1998)
Figure 14.17 An excitatory-center, inhibitory-surround receptive field of a neuron in a monkey’s thalamus.
The Physiology of Tactile Object Perception
- continued
• Monkey’s somatosensory cortex also shows
neurons that respond best to
– grasping specific objects.
– paying attention to the task.
• Neurons may respond to stimulation of
the receptors, but attending to the task
increases the response.
Figure 14.18 Receptive fields of neurons in the monkey’s somatosensory cortex. (a) This neuron responds
best when a horizontally oriented edge is presented to the monkey’s hand. (b) This neuron responds best
when a stimulus moves across the fingertip from right to left. (From Hyvarinin & Poranen, 1978)
Figure 14.19 The response of a neuron in a monkey’s parietal cortex that fires when the monkey grasps a
ruler but that does not fire when the monkey grasps a cylinder. The monkey grasps the objects at time = 0.
(From Sakata & Iwamura, 1978)
Figure 14.20 Firing rate of a neuron in area S1 of a monkey’s cortex to a letter being rolled across the
fingertips. The neuron responds only when the monkey is paying attention to the tactile stimulus. (From
Hsiao, O’Shaughnessy, & Johnson, 1993)
Pain Perception
• Pain is a multimodal phenomenon containing
a sensory component and an affective or
emotional component.
• Three types of pain
– Nociceptive - signals impending damage to
the skin
• Types of nociceptors respond to heat,
chemicals, severe pressure, and cold.
• Threshold of eliciting receptor response
must be balanced to warn of damage,
but not be affected by normal activity.
Types of Pain
– Inflammatory pain - caused by damage to
tissues and joints or by tumor cells
– Neuropathic pain - caused by damage to
the central nervous system, such as
• Brain damage caused by stroke
• Repetitive movements which cause
conditions like carpal tunnel syndrome
Figure 14.21 Nociceptive pain is created by activation of nociceptors in the skin that respond to different
types of stimulation. Signals from the nociceptors are transmitted to the spinal cord and then from the
dorsal root of the spinal cord in pathways that lead to the brain.
Direct Pathway Model of Pain Perception
• Early model that stated nociceptors are
stimulated and send signals to the brain
• Problems with this model
– Pain can be affected by a person’s mental
state.
– Pain can occur when there is no
stimulation of the skin.
– Pain can be affected by a person’s
attention.
Gate Control Model of Pain Perception
• The “gate” consists of substantia gelatinosa
cells in the spinal cord (SG- and SG+).
• Input into the gate comes from
– Large diameter (L) fibers - information from
tactile stimuli
– Small diameter (S) fibers - information from
nociceptors
– Central control - information from cognitive
factors from the cortex
Figure 14.23 (a) Cross-section of the spinal cord showing fibers entering through the dorsal root and the
location of the substantia gelatinosa. (b) The circuit proposed by Melzack and Wall (1988) for their gatecontrol model of pain perception. See text for details.
Gate Control Model of Pain Perception continued
• Pain does not occur when the gate is closed
by stimulation into the SG- from central
control or L-fibers into the T-cell.
• Pain does occur from stimulation from the Sfibers into the SG+ into the T-cell.
• Actual mechanism is more complex than this
model suggests.
Cognitive and Experiential Aspects of Pain
• Expectation - when surgical patients are told
what to expect, they request less pain
medication and leave the hospital earlier
– Placebos can also be effective in reducing
pain.
• Shifting attention - virtual reality technology
has been used to keep patients’ attention on
other stimuli than the pain-inducing
stimulation
Cognitive and Experiential Aspects of Pain continued
• Content of emotional distraction - participants
could keep their hands in cold water longer
when pictures they were shown were positive
• Experiment by Derbyshire to investigate
hypnotically induced pain.
– Participants had a thermal stimulator
attached the to palm of their hand.
Experiment by Derbyshire et al. - continued
• Three conditions
– Physically induced pain
– Hypnotically induced pain
– Control group that imagined painful
stimulation
• Both subjective reports and fMRI scans
showed that hypnosis did produce pain
perception.
Figure 14.24 The results of deWied and Verbaten’s (2001) experiment showing that participants kept their
hands in cold water longer when looking at positive pictures than when looking at neutral or negative
pictures.
Brain Structures and Pain
• Subcortical areas including the
hypothalamus, limbic system, and the
thalamus.
• Cortical areas including S1 and S2 in the
somatosensory cortex, the insula, and the
anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortices.
• These cortical areas taken together are called
the pain matrix.
Figure 14.26 The perception of pain is accompanied by activation of a number of different areas of the
brain. All of these areas, taken together, are called the pain matrix.
Sensory and Affective Components of Pain
• Experiment by Hoffauer et al.
– Participants were presented with
potentially painful stimuli and asked
• To rate subjective pain intensity
• To rate the unpleasantness of the pain
– Brain activity was measured while they
placed their hands into hot water.
– Hypnosis was used to increase or
decrease the sensory and affective
components.
Experiment by Hoffauer et al. - continued
• Results showed that
– Suggestions to change the subjective
intensity led to changes in both ratings and
in activity in S1.
– Suggestions to change the unpleasantness
of pain did not affect the subjective ratings,
but did change
• Ratings of unpleasantness.
• Activation in the anterior cingulate
cortex.
Figure 14.27 Results of Hofbauer et al.’s (2001) experiment. Participants’ ratings of the intensity and the
unpleasantness of pain were affected by hypnosis. (a) Results of hypnotic suggestion to decrease or
increase the pain’s intensity. (b) Results of suggestion to decrease or increase the pain’s unpleasantness.
Opioids and Pain
• Brain tissue releases neurotransmitters called
endorphins.
– Evidence shows that endorphins reduce
pain.
• Injecting naloxone blocks the receptor
sites causing more pain.
• Naloxone also decreases the
effectiveness of placebos.
• People whose brains release more
endorphins can withstand higher pain
levels.
Figure 14.28 (a) Naloxone reduces the effect of heroin by occupying a receptor site normally stimulated by
heroin. (b) Stimulating sites in the brain that cause the release of endorphins can reduce the pain by
stimulating opiate receptor sites. (c) Naloxone decreases the pain reduction caused by endorphins, by
keeping the endorphins from reaching the receptor sites.
Pain in Social Situations
• Experiment by Eisenberger et al.
– Participants watched a computer game.
– Then, they were asked to play with two
other “players” who did not exist but were
part of the program.
– The “players” excluded the participant.
– fMRI data showed increased activity in the
anterior cingulate cortex and participants
reported feeling ignored and distressed.
Pain in Social Situations - continued
• Experiment by Singer et al.
– Romantically involved couples participated.
– The woman’s brain activity was measured
by fMRI.
– The woman either received shocks or she
watched while her partner received shocks.
– Similar brain areas were activated in both
conditions.