* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download `Am not I a fly like thee?` From genes in fruit flies to behavior in humans
Polycomb Group Proteins and Cancer wikipedia , lookup
Gene therapy of the human retina wikipedia , lookup
Behavioural genetics wikipedia , lookup
Epigenetics of cocaine addiction wikipedia , lookup
Gene therapy wikipedia , lookup
Genomic imprinting wikipedia , lookup
Gene nomenclature wikipedia , lookup
Epigenetics in learning and memory wikipedia , lookup
Ridge (biology) wikipedia , lookup
Medical genetics wikipedia , lookup
Genetic engineering wikipedia , lookup
Minimal genome wikipedia , lookup
Neuronal ceroid lipofuscinosis wikipedia , lookup
Quantitative trait locus wikipedia , lookup
Genome evolution wikipedia , lookup
Epigenetics of human development wikipedia , lookup
Site-specific recombinase technology wikipedia , lookup
History of genetic engineering wikipedia , lookup
Gene expression programming wikipedia , lookup
Human genetic variation wikipedia , lookup
Nutriepigenomics wikipedia , lookup
Artificial gene synthesis wikipedia , lookup
Biology and consumer behaviour wikipedia , lookup
Gene expression profiling wikipedia , lookup
Microevolution wikipedia , lookup
Public health genomics wikipedia , lookup
Epigenetics of neurodegenerative diseases wikipedia , lookup
Human Molecular Genetics, 2004, Vol. 13, Review Issue 2 doi:10.1093/hmg/ddh248 R267–R273 ‘Am not I a fly like thee?’ From genes in fruit flies to behavior in humans Ralph J. Greenspan* and Herman A. Dierick The Neurosciences Institute, 10640 John Jay Hopkins Drive, San Diego, CA 92118, USA Received June 14, 2004; Revised and Accepted July 26, 2004 The fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster has provided insight into the role of genes in behavior, some of which have relevant implications for humans. Mutants induced or engineered in the laboratory have contributed to our understanding of biological rhythms, learning, memory, neurodegenerative disease and drug response. Studies of naturally occurring genetic variation in behavior have advanced our understanding of what kinds of variants arise spontaneously and contribute to behavior. The Fly Little Fly, Thy summer’s play My thoughtless hand Has brushed away. Am not I A fly like thee? Or art not thou A man like me? For I dance, And drink, and sing, Till some blind hand Shall brush my wing. If thought is life And strength and breath, And the want Of thought is death; Then am I A happy fly. If I live, Or if I die. from ‘Songs of Experience’ by William Blake Charting the relationship between genes and behavior can be a perilous activity. The challenges are many: the difficulty of defining genetic and non-genetic factors, the difficulty of understanding the interactions among these many factors, the many steps that intervene between the expression of a gene and the manifestation of a behavior, and the inescapable fact that each individual is the unique product of a series of historical accidents. Historical uniqueness also applies to nonhuman biological individuals. It is a hallmark of the biological world due to the fact that no particular combination of genes and experiences is ever replicated exactly in nature, although this is not equally apparent for all behaviors. Moreover, all of these difficulties are compounded many times over by the limitations on what kinds of experimental approaches can be brought to bear on human behavioral traits. Studies of genes and behavior in model organisms offer one way out of this conundrum. To begin with, it is far easier to control environmental conditions and genetic heterogeneity in the laboratory, and such studies are generally undertaken keeping environmental conditions as uniform as possible and genetic makeup as well defined as possible. But this would be no help if the phenomena and principles are fundamentally different from those of humans. Fortunately, they appear to be similar by an ever increasing number of criteria. One of the most striking outcomes of modern biology and the sequencing of genomes is the similarity of genes between distantly related creatures. The fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster and humans share a great many of the same genes in common. Similarly, they share considerable conservation of metabolic and signaling pathways at the cellular level. Most strikingly, and most relevant for this discussion, there is growing evidence for conservation at the level of behavior and its molecular mechanisms. This has been shown most convincingly, thus far, for several behaviors in Drosophila: circadian rhythms (1), learning and memory (2) and most recently sleep (3). Thus, there is good reason to believe that the characteristics, interactions and *To whom correspondence should be addressed. Tel: þ1 8586262075; Fax: þ1 8586262099; Email: [email protected] Human Molecular Genetics, Vol. 13, Review Issue 2 # Oxford University Press 2004; all rights reserved R268 Human Molecular Genetics, 2004, Vol. 13, Review Issue 2 contributions of genes in the behavior of model organisms will provide important guidelines for how to think about the role of genes in human behavior. More recently, there has also been a movement towards creating Drosophila models for human diseases affecting the nervous system, such as Huntington’s Disease, Parkinson’s Disease and Alzheimer’s Disease. These have been created not only for the purpose of trying to understand underlying genetic and molecular mechanisms of the disease, but also as a platform for drug screening. Similarly, the fly’s responses to ethanol, cocaine and nicotine are also being studied as possible models for addiction. As a consequence, the fruit fly has taken on some surprising new roles as a potential model for human biology and genetics. All try to capitalize on the versatile genetic manipulations possible in the fly, and then look for transferability to humans either at the level of fundamental mechanism, or at the level of preliminary therapeutic screen. FUNDAMENTALLY CONSERVED MECHANISMS Circadian rhythms The clearest contribution fruit flies have made is in unraveling the cellular mechanism of the circadian clock. These discoveries began with the isolation of long-day, short-day and arrhythmic mutants in Drosophila, all of which proved to be alleles of the same gene, dubbed period (4). Through the isolation of additional mutants in flies, fungi and mice and the cloning of these genes, the cellular mechanism of the circadian clock was worked out and shown to be nearly universal in the biological world (1,5,6). Many of the actual genes involved, such as period, are conserved between flies and mammals (Fig. 1). From the discovery of the period gene, the quest progressed with the isolation and cloning of additional mutants: timeless, doubletime, Clock, cycle and cryptochrome (1). What has emerged is a picture of the circadian clock as a transcriptional regulatory loop in which each cell counts out its own 24 h period by means of an oscillating cycle of transcription and translation. Several other genes complete the basic cycling mechanism—per and tim repress their own transcription which requires activation by the transcription factors Clock and cycle, with the doubletime kinase regulating the degradation of period and timeless proteins (1). This serves as the central time-keeping mechanism for all circadian rhythmic activities. Aside from the very high degree of molecular conservation of this mechanism in mammals, including humans, there have also been two sleep syndromes linked to variants in human homologs of the period gene. A haplotype of the human period3 (hPer3 ) gene, one of the multiple human homologs of Drosophila period, has been associated with delayed sleep phase syndrome (7). A separate study of Finnish patients with the same sleep syndrome revealed a length polymorphism in the hPer3 gene, such that the long allele favored morning preference and the short allele favored evening preference (8). Another hereditary sleep disorder, familial advanced sleep phase syndrome is an autosomal dominant circadian rhythm variant in which affected individuals are ‘morning larks’ with a 4 h advance of the sleep, temperature and melatonin rhythms. Affected individuals have a serine to glycine mutation within the casein kinase Iepsilon (CKIepsilon)binding region of hPER2, which causes hypophosphorylation by CKIepsilon in vitro. Thus, a variant in human sleep behavior may be attributable to a missense mutation in a clock component, hPER2, which alters the circadian period (9). Further contributions to the understanding of human sleep are likely to emerge from studies in the new realm of fruit fly sleep (3). Learning and memory Learning and memory have many features common to flies, rodents and humans. Behaviorally, all are capable of associative conditioning, and the memory that is induced shows multiple phases that can be distinguished by pharmacology and by training regimen (2). In the fly, learning takes the form of distinguishing between two odors and avoiding the one that has previously been paired with electric shock. Long-term memory is produced by repeated training trials separated by suitable time intervals. Care has been taken in all searches for learning mutants to ascertain that these flies are normal in all other relevant faculties that are required for the learning task: olfaction, locomotor activity and shock reactivity. The original Drosophila learning mutant, dunce (10), encodes one of two forms of cAMP-phosphodiesterase. Mutations in other components of the cAMP signaling system, rutabaga in the synthetic enzyme (adenylate cyclase), and Dco and RI in the key enzyme regulated by cyclic-AMP (cAMP-dependent protein kinase), as well as the Creb (cAMP-response-element-binding) protein that regulates genes induced by cAMP, were subsequently shown also to affect learning and memory in the fly (Fig. 2) (2). Moreover, various of these components had selective effects on different phases of memory. The cyclic-AMP signaling system had already been implicated in neuronal plasticity as central to the mechanism that modifies efficacy at synapses in the circuitry underlying learning. The isolation of mutants affecting the system from unbiased, open-ended mutant screens and the demonstration that they affected classical conditioning in the intact animal gave universality to the finding. These findings not only placed the differential genetic effects on memory phases on firmer ground, they were also subsequently extrapolated to mice and rats (2). Subsequent mutant hunts for selective effects on long-term memory have also revealed the role of genes involved in mRNA localization and translational control (11). The relevance to humans has been borne out by the fact that there is a hererditary disorder, Rubinstein – Taybi syndrome (RTS), characterized by mental retardation and physical abnormalities including broad thumbs, big and broad toes, short stature, and craniofacial anomalies, that maps to chromosome 16p13.3, a genomic region containing CREB-binding protein (12). More significantly, many RTS patients are heterozygous for CBP mutations that yield truncations of the CBP C terminus. On the basis of the fly work, and its subsequent confirmation in mice, memory enhancement drugs are currently being tested in clinical trials that target cAMP phosphodiesterase (13). Human Molecular Genetics, 2004, Vol. 13, Review Issue 2 R269 Figure 1. Conservation of molecular mechanisms of the circadian clock between flies and mammals. PER, period gene: TIM, timeless gene and CKIe, casein kinase Ie gene. (Adapted with permission from 43. Copyright 2000 AAAS.) Validity of fly models for circadian rhythms and learning The fundamental biological principles that have been obtained from genetic studies of circadian rhythms and learning and memory in the fruit fly have proved to be widespread, if not universal, in the animal world. Most important for our discussion is that they have opened the way for understanding the human biology underlying these behavioral phenotypes. This is an incontrovertible argument for their relevance as models for humans. SYNTHETIC FLY MODELS OF HUMAN PHENOTYPES Neurodegenerative disease The completion of the Drosophila genome sequence has made it possible to identify genes in the fly that are homologous to human disease genes (14 –17). Approximately two-thirds of the known human disease genes have a counterpart in Drosophila (666 of 911 genes) (16,17) and 74 of these genes, roughly 10%, are involved in neurological diseases. It is in this area that the last few years have seen a veritable explosion of research using the fly as a model for human disease. Most of these models, however, have focused on three neurodegenerative diseases, Huntington’s, Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s disease involving mis-expression of the relevant mutant human genes for huntingtin, a-synuclein and Ab1 – 42, respectively. The strategies to study these and other diseases in flies cover the spectrum from basic biology to pragmatic pharmacotherapy. Consequently, some studies focus on the endogenous biological function of the homologous fly gene with little reference to human disease, whereas others try to cure a simulated human fly disorder with the hopeful expectation that it will translate to a successful treatment in humans. A fly model expressing the mutant human huntingtin, the human SCA3/MJD mutant gene or simple polyglutamine repeats in the compound eye causes neurodegeneration of photoreceptor neurons (18 –20). Screens for mutations in other genes that modify this degenerative eye phenotype have identified HSP70 and several other chaperones as protective components (18 –20). This seems significant, given the presence of HSP70 in the protein aggregates characteristic of the pathology of huntingtin –induced neurodegeneration, suggesting that protein folding affects aggregate formation. This protective effect seems to be conserved in other model systems as well (18 – 20). The TOR inhibitor rapamycin has also been shown to protect against polyglutamine-induced degeneration in the fly eye (21). In one instance, that of the SCA3/MJD mutant gene, the antiapoptotic gene bcl-2 was also shown to have a protective effect. Flies expressing this SCA3/MJD mutant gene have been tested behaviorally and shown to exhibit locomotor defects and anosmia (22). A fly model of Parkinson’s disease has been created through expression of wild-type or mutant forms of the human a-synuclein gene in the fly brain. Although flies have no endogenous a-synuclein gene, its expression can recapitulate many aspects of the disease in flies, including degeneration of dopaminergic cells (18 –20). The age-dependent loss of locomotor control seen in these engineered mutants can be ameliorated by some of the same drugs used to treat human Parkinson’s patients: L-DOPA and various dopamine agonists (including bromocryptine and SK&F 38393) ameliorated the deficits (23). A Drosophila version of another Parkinson’s disease gene, parkin, exists and a mutation at the fly locus causes mitochondrial swelling, muscle and locomotor defects, but does not lead to a loss of dopmanergic neurons (19,20). Over-expression of fly parkin partially suppresses the a-synuclein-induced degeneration phenotype (19,20,24). As in the Huntington’s fly, over-expression of HSP70 can also protect dopaminergic neurons against a-synuclein toxicity (18 –20). Flies have endogenous versions of APP (called APP-like or Appl ) and of Presenilin-1, both implicated in familial Alzheimer’s disease in humans. Fly Presenilin has been shown to play a role developmentally through its effect on cleavage of the NOTCH protein and work in the fly has added greatly to the understanding of APP processing (19,20). Fly Appl causes axonal vesicular accumulations when over-expressed or when mutated, suggesting that the normal protein plays a role in axonal transport (20,25). R270 Human Molecular Genetics, 2004, Vol. 13, Review Issue 2 Potential value of fly models for neurodegeneration Because the models for neurodegeneration are mainly synthetic, consisting of mutant human genes expressed in the fly nervous system, their study departs from the sort of basic biology described earlier. (Mutants such as spongecake, if proven to be bona fide, endogenous biological models for human neurodegeneration, would represent an exception.) The behavioral defects seen in these models only vaguely resemble the human symptoms. To the extent that the perturbations of the fly’s biology and the synthetic strains produced can be understood mechanistically, however, these models may hold promise for identifying interventional strategies to block or retard degeneration. The implication of chaperone proteins as neuroprotectors against polyglutamineinduced degeneration, and of certain kinases and phosphatases as neuroprotectors against tauopathy-induced degeneration provides the best example thus far of the sort of potentially useful information that such studies may yield. Addiction and drugs of abuse Figure 2. The cAMP signaling pathway in memory formation. AC, adenylate cyclase (rutabaga gene); PDE, cAMP phosphodiesterase (dunce gene); PKA, cAMP-dependent protein kinase (DCO and RI genes) and CREB, cAMPresponse-element-binding protein (dCreb gene). (Adapted from 44 with the kind permission of Nature Publishing Group, http://www.nature.com/.) Behavioral effects have been shown in the fly Appl knockout mutant and in flies expressing human Ab-peptides. The Appl mutant causes a general non-reactivity to stimuli that normally elicit phototaxis or chemotaxis, a phenotype that can be partially rescued by expression of a normal human APP gene (26). The strains expressing the Ab1 – 42 peptide exhibit Alzheimer’s-like degeneration and deficits in learning when the peptide is expressed in a part of the brain (the mushroom bodies) known to be required for associative conditioning (27). Tauopathy can also be produced transgenically in the fruit fly, most pronounced when a mutant human gene is used (28). A screen for genetic modifiers of its neurodegeneration yielded a set of kinases and phosphatases as prominent among the enhancing and suppressing genes, contrasting with the findings for polyglutamine-induced degeneration (see earlier). Over-expression study of normal tau protein in the mushroom bodies of the fly brain (29) produces learning deficits in the absence of degeneration, suggesting that behavioral deficits are a more sensitive indicator of malfunction than pathology. In addition to the biochemical and apparent behavioral similarities of these fly models to human biology, there are also similarities in histopathology between some fly neurodegeneration mutants and human disease. One such example is shown in Figure 3, comparing ultrastructural phenotype between the fly mutant spongecake and human Creutzfeldt – Jakob disease. The neurobiological effects of addictive drugs have been studied in numerous model systems, recently including Drosophila. Some aspects of drug abuse can be readily modeled in non-human organism, whereas others are much harder to assess (30). The behavioral and neurobiological consequences of acute and chronic exposure can be studied relatively easily and genetic analysis so far shows tantalizing similarities with some of the findings in mammalian model systems. Addiction per se, however, and its behavioral correlates such as craving, withdrawal, drug seeking and continued use despite physical harm are much harder to study in model organisms, notably the fruit fly. The work on drugs of abuse in Drosophila has so far mostly focused on ethanol, cocaine and nicotine. Flies show a biphasic response to acute ethanol exposure. After an initial increase in locomotor activity, they become uncoordinated and eventually pass out. This response is dosedependent and is similar to the response seen in rodents and humans. Concentrations that lead to increased locomotion in flies and rats lead to disinhibition and euphoria in humans. Concentrations that are sedative to flies are also sedative to humans (31). Genetic screens for altered ethanol sensitivity have implicated cAMP pathways in acute and chronic ethanol responses. The first fly mutant with an increased sensitivity to alcohol was named cheapdate and turned out to be an allele of the learning mutant amnesiac, encoding a pituitary adenylyl cyclase activating peptide (PACAP)-like neuropeptide with homology to the mammalian PACAP (31,32). This complex role of cAMP signaling in ethanol response has also been documented in rodents. As mentioned earlier the addictive properties of ethanol are more difficult to evaluate, although some interesting findings have been made in this area. Acute ethanol exposure leads to a release of dopamine in rodents and blocking this release blocks the locomotor effect as well as the rewarding properties of ethanol. In flies a similar reduction in locomotor activity levels has been observed, although a connection to reward cannot be made (31). Just as in humans, flies develop tolerance for ethanol in that repeated exposures delay the effects of Human Molecular Genetics, 2004, Vol. 13, Review Issue 2 R271 Figure 3. (A) Coalesced vacuolar structure in spongecake neurodegenerative mutant. (B) A similar membrane-bound vacuole in human brain neuropil associated with Creutzfeldt–Jakob disease, a prion disease (scale bars ¼ 2 mm). (Adapted from 45 with the kind permission of Elsevier.) impairment. Flies mutant for tyramine-b-hydroxylase, the enzyme that regulates octopamine synthesis, have lower octopamine levels and have a reduced tolerance to ethanol. This finding parallels the fact that mice require an intact noradrenergic system for ethanol tolerance as octopamine is thought to be functionally equivalent to vertebrate noradrenaline. Preference for ethanol in Drosophila seems closely related to their levels of alcohol dehydrogenase (Adh), and preference in adults increases after previous exposure, suggesting a parallel to the rewarding properties of ethanol in humans (31). Acute exposure to volatilized cocaine generates dosedependent behavioral responses in flies that are similar to those observed in rodents. Low doses lead to excessive grooming, moderate doses cause rapid rotation and sideways and backward walking and high doses cause tremor and paralysis (33). Molecularly, cocaine has been shown to affect the re-uptake transporters for catecholamines, so it is not surprising that the behavioral cocaine effects are diminished when dopamine levels are acutely reduced when flies are fed dopamine synthesis blockers. When dopaminergic and serotonergic neurons are blocked chronically throughout development, flies are more sensitive to the effects of cocaine, presumably because of compensatory adaptations that hypersensitize the response to dopamine (31). One property of cocaine response that relates to its addictive properties is behavioral sensitization or reverse tolerance. Repeated exposure to cocaine results in an increased sensitivity to the drug and this effect is long lasting. Flies with reduced tyramine levels do not sensitize, an effect that can be reversed by feeding flies tyramine. The enzyme that is required for tyramine synthesis through tyrosine decarboxylation, tyrosine decarboxylase (TDC), is upregulated in sensitized flies and the kinetics of this enzymatic induction parallel the behavioral sensitization kinetics (34). Suprisingly, the circadian gene period is required for sensitization, even though it is not a circadian behavior (35). Mutants with reduced cAMP-stimulated PKA activity are mostly resistant to acute cocaine effects and sensitization. Moreover, sensitization and cocaine sensitivity could be altered by changing the activity in the dopaminergic cells using stimulatory or inhibitory Ga subunits (36). Nicotine has been less studied in flies, though cholinergic transmission and the receptors, enzymes and transporter necessary for it have been identified (37). Flies exposed to volatilized nicotine are hyperactive at low doses and hypoactive to immobile at high doses similar to the effects described in rodents. Again, dopaminergic systems seem to be involved as dopamine synthesis blockers reduce nicotine-induced locomotion (31). Potential value of fly models for addiction Flies clearly have at least some of the neuronal machinery acted upon by substances of abuse such as ethanol, cocaine and nicotine, and show some of the same ability to modify their responses to these agents. To this extent, they offer useful biological models for understanding the mechanisms of action of the drugs. The missing phenotype, however, to make them full-fledged models for human addiction is addiction itself. This disparity may highlight interesting aspects of the evolution of dopaminergic and cholinergic systems. NATURAL VARIATION Traditionally, there have been two separate approaches to behavior genetics in model organisms: the isolation and manipulation of newly induced mutations (see earlier) and the measurement and manipulation of naturally occurring variation in laboratory or wild-caught strains. The latter assays the extent of relatively mild mutational variation that survives in the world and exerts effects on behavior. The molecular strategies that have made advances in these efforts possible in humans have also been applied to model organisms. R272 Human Molecular Genetics, 2004, Vol. 13, Review Issue 2 The resulting findings live up to the name ‘model’ inasmuch as they provide a detailed picture of what kinds of variation exist and what kinds of hurdles must be gotten over to obtain specific information on relevant genes. They also provide an interesting assay as to the overlap between those genes identified in mutant studies and those that vary naturally, as they affect a particular behavioral phenotype. One such case comes from a study of naturally occurring variation in the courtship song in Drosophila. This has more relevance to the foregoing discussion than is immediately obvious, due to the effect of the circadian period gene on a critical rhythmic parameter of the song (38). A survey of such variation and a QTL mapping analysis of recombinant lines from two variant strains identified three major QTLs, none of which coincided with any of the genes known from mutant studies of courtship song in the fly (39). These results underline the wide-ranging nature of the genes involved in any behavior. Many genes can affect a given behavior (discussed in 40). Another approach to the problem of identifying multi-genic traits has come with the advent of DNA microarray technology. A new approach to this analysis became possible, based on direct measurement of differences in transcriptional levels between divergent strains. This was first applied to strains in Drosophila selected for differences in gravity response (41). While not all such mRNA expression differences would be due to actual genetic polymorphisms in the affected genes, some would, and others could be indicative of relevant phenotypic effects by the polymorphic genes. Approximately 250 out of 8800 genes were differentially expressed between the two strains in RNA extracted from fly heads (42). Functional significance for several of these genes was verified by testing extant, more severe mutants in gravity response. In three cases, the predicted direction for a mutant corresponded to the same direction as the selected line with the lower expression level of that gene (42). Thus, for several of the loci, a severe, single-gene lesion could mimic the selected phenotype that was on the basis of a conglomerate of many genes. In no case, however, was a singlegene effect as strong as that of a selected line. Both of these examples highlight the multi-genic nature of natural variants, as well as the extent to which apparently distant genes can influence behavior. IMPLICATIONS FOR HUMANS The foregoing discussion briefly explores the relevance to humans of gene/behavior studies in the fruit fly. The main power of the fly models derives from the high level of conservation of function found for many of the relevant genes (e.g., period for circadian rhythms, Creb for long-term memory), and also for the high level of phenomenological conservation in phenotype for the behaviors they influence. The main caveat derives from the synthetic nature of some of the models, and the attendant problem of having taken the human gene out of context. The approach will validate itself to the extent that the findings from such synthetic studies show transferability back to the human context. REFERENCES 1. Hall, J.C. (2003) Genetics and molecular biology of rhythms in Drosophila and other insects. Adv. Genet., 48, 1–280. 2. Dubnau, J. (2004) Neurogenetic dissection of conditioned behavior: evolution by analogy or homology? J. Neurogenet., 17, 295 –326. 3. Greenspan, R.J., Tononi, G., Cirelli, C. and Shaw, P.J. (2001) Sleep and the fruit fly. Trends Neurosci., 24, 142 –145. 4. Konopka, R.J. and Benzer, S. (1971) Clock mutants of Drosophila melanogaster. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA, 68, 2112–2116. 5. Young, M.W. (1998) The molecular control of circadian behavioral rhythms and their entrainment in Drosophila. Ann. Rev. Biochem., 67, 135–152. 6. Lowrey, P. and Takahashi, J. (2004) Mammalian circadian biology: elucidating genome-wide levels of temporal organization. Annu. Rev. Genom. Hum. Genet., 5, 407 –441. 7. Ebisawa, T., Uchiyama, M., Kajimura, N., Mishima, K., Kamei, Y., Katoh, M., Watanabe, T., Sekimoto, M., Shibui, K., Kim, K. et al. (2001) Association of structural polymorphisms in the human period3 gene with delayed sleep phase syndrome. EMBO Rep., 2, 342 –346. 8. Archer, S.N., Robilliard, D.L., Skene, D.J., Smits, M., Williams, A., Arendt, J. and von Schantz, M. (2003) A length polymorphism in the circadian clock gene Per3 is linked to delayed sleep phase syndrome and extreme diurnal preference. Sleep, 26, 413 –415. 9. Toh, K.L., Jones, C.R., He, Y., Eide, E.J., Hinz, W.A., Virshup, D.M., Ptacek, L.J. and Fu, Y.H. (2001) An hPer2 phosphorylation site mutation in familial advanced sleep phase syndrome. Science, 291, 1040–1043. 10. Dudai, Y., Jan, Y.N., Byers, D., Quinn, W.G. and Benzer, S. (1976) dunce, a mutant of Drosophila deficient in learning. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA, 73, 1684–1688. 11. Dubnau, J., Chiang, A.S., Grady, L., Barditch, J., Gossweiler, S., McNeil, J., Smith, P., Buldoc, F., Scott, R., Certa, U. et al. (2003) The staufen/pumilio pathway is involved in Drosophila long-term memory. Curr. Biol., 13, 286–296. 12. Petrij, F., Dorsman, J.C., Dauwerse, H.G., Giles, R.H., Peeters, T., Hennekam, R.C., Breuning, M.H. and Peters, D.J. (2000) Rubinstein– Taybi syndrome caused by a de novo reciprocal translocation t(2;16)(q36.3;p13.3). Am. J. Med. Genet., 92, 47–52. 13. Bourtchouladze, R., Lidge, R., Catapano, R., Stanley, J., Gossweiler, S., Romashko, D., Scott, R. and Tully, T. (2003) A mouse model of Rubinstein –Taybi syndrome: defective long-term memory is ameliorated by inhibitors of phosphodiesterase 4. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA, 100, 10518–10522. 14. Rubin, G.M., Yandell, M.D., Wortman, J.R., Gabor Miklos, G.L., Nelson, C.R., Hariharan, I.K., Fortini, M.E., Li, P.W., Apweiler, R., Fleischmann, W. et al. (2000) Comparative genomics of the eukaryotes. Science, 287, 2204–2215. 15. Fortini, M., Skupski, M.P., Boguski, M.S. and Hariharan, I.K. (2000) A survey of human disease gene counterparts in the Drosophila genome. J. Cell Biol., 150, F23–F30. 16. Reiter L.T., Potocki, L., Chien, S. and Gribskov, M. and Bier, E. (2001) A systematic analysis of human disease associated gene sequences in Drosophila melanogaster. Genome Res., 11, 1114–1125. 17. Chien, S., Reiter, L.T., Bier, E. and Gribskov, M. (2002) Homophila: human disease gene cognates in Drosophila. Nucl. Acids Res., 30, 149–151. 18. Zoghbi, H.Y. and Botas, J. (2002) Mouse and fly models of neurodegeneration. Trends Genet., 18, 463–471. 19. Driscoll, M. and Gerstbrein, B. (2003) Dying for a cause: invertebrate genetics takes on human neurodegeneration. Nat. Rev. Genet., 4, 181–194. 20. Shulman, J.M., Shulman, L.M., Weiner, W.J. and Feany, M.B. (2003) From fruit fly to bedside: translating lessons from Drosophila models of neurodegenrative disease. Curr. Opin. Neurol., 16, 443–449. 21. Ravikumar, B., Vacher, C., Berger, Z., Davies, J.E., Luo, S., Oroz, L.G., Scaravilli, F., Easton, D.F., Duden, R., O’Kane, C.J. and Rubinsztein, D.C. (2004) Inhibition of mTOR induces autophagy and reduces toxicity of polyglutamine expansions in fly and mouse models of Huntington disease. Nat. Genet., 36, 585 –595. 22. Kim, Y.T., Shin, S.M., Lee, W.Y., Kim, G.M. and Jin, D.K. (2004) Expression of expanded polyglutamine protein induces behavioral changes in Drosophila. Cell Mol. Neurobiol., 24, 109–122. Human Molecular Genetics, 2004, Vol. 13, Review Issue 2 23. Pendleton, R.G., Parvez, F., Sayed, M. and Hillman, R. (2002) Effects of pharmacological agents upon a transgenic model of Parkinson’s disease in Drosophila melanogaster. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther., 300, 91– 96. 24. Giasson, I.B. and Lee, V.M-Y. (2003) Are ubiquitination pathways central to Parkinson’s disease? Cell, 114, 1–8. 25. Torroja, L.J., Hsin, C., Kotovsky, I. and White, K. (1999) Neuronal overexpression of APPL, the Drosophila homologue of the amyloid precursor protein (APP), disrupts axonal transport. Curr. Biol., 9, 489 –492. 26. Luo, L., Tully, T. and White, K. (1992) Human amyloid precursor protein ameliorates behavioral deficit of flies deleted for Appl gene. Neuron, 9, 595 –605. 27. Iijima, K., Liu, H.P., Chiang, A.S., Hearn, S.A., Konsolaki, M. and Zhong, Y. (2004) Dissecting the pathological effects of human Abeta40 and Abeta42 in Drosophila: a potential model for Alzheimer’s disease. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA, 101, 6623–6628. 28. Shulman, J.M. and Feany, M.B. (2003) Genetic modifiers of tauopathy in Drosophila. Genetics, 165, 1233–1242. 29. Mershin, A., Pavlopoulos, E., Fitch, O., Braden, B.C., Nanopoulos, D.V. and Skoulakis, E.M.C. (2004) Learning and memory deficits upon TAU accumulation in Drosophila mushroom body neurons. Learn. Mem., 11, 277 –287. 30. Tabakoff, B. and Hoffman, P.L. (2000) Animal models in alcohol research. Alcohol Res. Health, 24, 77– 84. 31. Wolf, F.W. and Heberlein, U. (2003) Invertebrate models of drugs of abuse. J. Neurobiol., 54, 161– 178. 32. Feany M.B. and Quinn W.G. (1995) A neuropeptide gene defined by the Drosophila memory mutant amnesiac. Science, 268, 869–873. R273 33. McClung, C. and Hirsh, J. (1998) Stereotypic behavioral responses to free-base cocaine and the development of behavioral sensitization in Drosophila. Curr. Biol., 8, 109– 112. 34. McClung, C. and Hirsh, J. (1999) The trace amine tyramine is essential for sensitization to cocaine in Drosophila. Curr. Biol., 9, 853– 860. 35. Andretic, R., Chaney, S. and Hirsh, J. (1999) Requirement of circadian genes for cocaine sensitization in Drosophila. Science, 285, 1066–1068. 36. Li, H., Chaney, S., Roberts, I.J., Forte, M. and Hirsh, J. (2000) Ectopic G-protein expression in dopamine and serotonin neurons blocks cocaine sensitization in Drosophila melanogaster. Curr. Biol., 10, 211–214. 37. Schafer, W.R. (2002) Genetic analysis of nicotinic signaling in worms and flies. J. Neurobiol., 53, 535 –541. 38. Kyriacou, C.P. and Hall, J.C. (1980) Circadian rhythm mutations in Drosophila melanogaster affect short-term fluctuations in the male’s courtship song. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA, 77, 6729–6733. 39. Gleason, J.M., Nuzhdin, S.V. and Ritchie, M.G. (2002) Quantitative trait loci affecting a courtship signal in Drosophila melanogaster. Heredity, 89, 1–6. 40. Greenspan, R.J. (2004) E pluribus unum, ex uno plura: quantitative- and single-gene perspectives on the study of behavior. Annu. Rev. Neurosci., 27, 79–105. 41. Ricker, J.P. and Hirsch, J. (1988) Genetic changes occurring over 500 generations in lines of Drosophila melanogaster selected divergently for geotaxis. Behav. Genet., 18, 13–25. 42. Toma, D.P., White, K.P., Hirsch, J. and Greenspan, R.J. (2002) Identification of genes involved in Drosophila melanogaster geotaxis, a complex behavioral trait. Nat. Genet., 31, 349 –353. 43. Young, M.W. (2000) Science, 288, 451–453. 44. Tully, T. (1998) Nat. Neurosci., 1, 543–545. 45. Min, K.-T. and Benzer, S. (1997) Curr. Biol., 7, 885 –888.