Download Impairment of episodic and semantic autobiographical memory in

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Visual selective attention in dementia wikipedia , lookup

Limbic system wikipedia , lookup

Holonomic brain theory wikipedia , lookup

Source amnesia wikipedia , lookup

Sex differences in cognition wikipedia , lookup

Effects of alcohol on memory wikipedia , lookup

Prenatal memory wikipedia , lookup

Socioeconomic status and memory wikipedia , lookup

Traumatic memories wikipedia , lookup

Metamemory wikipedia , lookup

Autobiographical memory wikipedia , lookup

Eyewitness memory (child testimony) wikipedia , lookup

State-dependent memory wikipedia , lookup

De novo protein synthesis theory of memory formation wikipedia , lookup

Collective memory wikipedia , lookup

Memory consolidation wikipedia , lookup

Exceptional memory wikipedia , lookup

Music-related memory wikipedia , lookup

Memory and aging wikipedia , lookup

Emotion and memory wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Neuropsychologia 47 (2009) 2464–2469
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Neuropsychologia
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia
Impairment of episodic and semantic autobiographical memory in patients
with mild cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer’s disease
Thomas Leyhe a,b,∗ , Stephan Müller a , Monika Milian a , Gerhard W. Eschweiler a,b , Ralf Saur a,c
a
b
c
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Tübingen, Osianderstraße 24, 72076 Tübingen, Germany
Geriatric Center at the University Hospital of Tübingen, Osianderstraße 24, 72076 Tübingen, Germany
Section of Experimental Magnetic Resonance of CNS, Department of Neuroradiology, University of Tübingen, Hoppe-Seyler-Straße 3, 72076 Tübingen, Germany
a r t i c l e
i n f o
Article history:
Received 24 November 2008
Received in revised form 21 April 2009
Accepted 22 April 2009
Available online 3 May 2009
Keywords:
Temporal gradient
Cognitive impairment
Dementia
Neurodegeneration
Cortical Reallocation Theory
Multiple Trace Theory
a b s t r a c t
Autobiographical memory includes the retrieval of personal semantic data and the remembrance of incident or episodic memories. In retrograde amnesias, it has been observed that recall of autobiographical
memories of recent events is poorer than recall of remote memories. Alzheimer’s disease (AD) may also
be associated with a temporal gradient (TG) in memory decline, though studies have yielded inconsistent
results on this point. They have also yielded inconsistent results on whether AD might differentially affect
semantic and episodic remembrance. Here, we compared autobiographical memory of childhood, early
adulthood, and recent life among healthy control (HC) subjects, patients with early AD, and patients with
amnesic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI). Both the aMCI and AD patients exhibited declines in recall
of autobiographical incidents and semantic information. In AD patients, both components of autobiographical memory had a clear TG, with better preservation of memories of childhood than those of early
adulthood and recent life. The TG of autobiographical memory decline in AD patients is more compatible
with the Cortical Reallocation Theory than with the Multiple Trace Theory of memory consolidation. In
contrast to AD patients, aMCI patients exhibited impaired recall of personal facts and autobiographical
incidents relating only to recent life. The significant decline in autobiographical memory for recent life
that occurred in aMCI patients suggests that deterioration of consolidation of personal facts and events
begins with commencement of functional impairment in the hippocampus.
© 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
Autobiographical memory is a mental representation of personal events and facts that allow for retrieval of both personal
semantic data and remembrance of incident or episodic memories. For example, in recounting the childhood, remembering an
autobiographical event, such as the time one first went to school,
is episodic, while remembering the name of the first grade teacher
is semantic. According to this phenomenological differentiation, it
is suggested that episodic and semantic autobiographical memory can be dissociated anatomically. Episodic memory primarily
involves structures of the medial temporal lobe, including the hippocampus, whereas semantic autobiographical memory mainly
involves the lateral temporal cortex, particularly of the left hemisphere (Gilboa et al., 2005; Kapur, Thompson, Kartsounis, & Abbott,
1999; Maguire, 2001; Moscovitch et al., 2005; Svoboda, McKinnon,
∗ Corresponding author at: Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Tübingen, Osianderstraße 24, 72076 Tübingen, Germany.
Tel.: +497071 2982311; fax: +497071 294141.
E-mail address: [email protected] (T. Leyhe).
0028-3932/$ – see front matter © 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.04.018
& Levine, 2006; Tulving, 2002; Wheeler, Stuss, & Tulving, 1997).
Medial temporal lobe structures seem to play a time-limited role in
semantic memory (Smith & Squire, 2009). Thus, semantic memory
that has been consolidated for a long time becomes independent
of the medial temporal lobe processes, while recently acquired
semantic memory would be expected to depend upon the medial
temporal lobe.
Clinical observations in patients with retrograde amnesias have
revealed that the retrieval of autobiographical memories of recent
events is often poorer than the retrieval of remote ones. This temporal gradient (TG) in memory decline, known as Ribot’s law (Ribot,
1881), has been explained along the lines of the main theories of
memory consolidation.
The Cortical Reallocation Theory (Alvarez & Squire, 1994; Meeter
& Murre, 2004; Squire, 2004) suggests a temporary role of the hippocampus for memory trace formation and consolidation for both
semantic and episodic information (declarative memory). Observations of a TG in memory performance resulting from hippocampal
damage or atrophy have led to the hypothesis that the hippocampus
participates only in the consolidation processes. At the completion
of these processes, the information itself is stored in the neocortex. Once memories have been stored in the neocortex, they
T. Leyhe et al. / Neuropsychologia 47 (2009) 2464–2469
become resistant to hippocampal disruption. Therefore, a decline in
the function of medial temporal lobe structures would affect only
recent memories, leading to the TG.
The Multiple Trace Theory (Moscovitch et al., 2005; Nadel &
Moscovitch, 1997) provides another explanation for the TG in amnesia. According to this theory, the hippocampal formation encodes all
information and forms memory traces that include both hippocampal and neocortical neurons. In this model, each time a memory is
retrieved, a new hippocampally mediated trace is created. Thus, frequently repeated remote memories are represented by more and
stronger hippocampal–neocortical traces than recent memories,
making them less susceptible to disruption by brain damage. As
the memory traces for autobiographical episodes are spatially distributed in the hippocampus, the extent and severity of retrograde
amnesia, as well as the slope of the TG, are related to the extent and
location of damage to the hippocampal system.
Impairment in autobiographical memory is often the most
apparent symptom of Alzheimer’s disease (AD). There have been
different findings concerning the effects of AD on the TG, as well
as on semantic and episodic remembrance in autobiographical
memory. Using the Autobiographical Memory Interview (AMI),
Kopelman and colleagues found significant deficits in AD patients.
These deficits were more pronounced for recent time periods than
for remote ones (Kopelman, Wilson, & Baddeley, 1989). Similar
results have been reported by other authors (Hou, Bruce, Kramer,
& Kramer, 2005; Nestor, Graham, Bozeat, Simons, & Hodges, 2002;
Snowden, Griffiths, & Neary, 1996). Greene, Hodges, and Baddeley
(1995) examined autobiographical memory in a cohort of patients
with early AD and found a slight TG in the incident component,
but not the personal semantic component of the AMI (Kopelman,
Wilson, & Baddeley, 1990). Ivanoiu et al. (2004) compared episodic
and semantic memory in AD patients, patients with semantic
dementia, and healthy elderly individuals, and they could not show
a clear TG for episodic autobiographical memory. There was, however, a modest gradient present for semantic autobiographical
memory in the AD group. In contrast, other studies of AD patients
have found no TG in autobiographical memory (Dall’Ora et al., 1989;
Meeter, Eijsackers, & Mulder, 2006).
Patients with amnesic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) have
memory deficits, and in some cases, these are present in combination with other cognitive impairments. The presence of aMCI
increases the risk for dementia, with rates of conversion to AD
of 10–15% per year (Gauthier et al., 2006; Petersen et al., 2001).
In a recent study, Murphy, Troyer, Levine, and Moscovitch (2008)
evaluated patients with aMCI for deficits in autobiographical memory. Using the autobiographical interview of Levine, Svoboda, Hay,
Winocur, and Moscovitch (2002), they found that episodic memory
was reduced, but semantic memory retrieval was increased when
compared to healthy controls. Moreover, a TG of autobiographical
memory could not be detected in these aMCI patients.
Here, we compared autobiographical memory among patients
with aMCI, patients with early AD, and healthy controls. We used
the AMI of Kopelman et al. (1990) to determine whether aMCI or
early AD impairs semantic or episodic autobiographical memory,
and whether these impairments exhibit a TG.
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
Sixty subjects (32 females and 28 males) with a mean age of 73.1 ± 6.6 years
participated in this study. Patients were recruited from the Memory Clinic of the
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy of the University Hospital of Tübingen. The study was approved by the Local Ethics Committee, and written informed
consent was obtained from each individual.
The study population was divided into healthy control (HC) subjects (N = 20),
patients with aMCI (N = 20), and patients with early AD (N = 20). The HC group consisted of relatives or friends of the patients. HC individuals did not have a history of
2465
neurological or psychiatric disease or any signs of cognitive decline, as confirmed
by a clinical interview. AD and aMCI patients underwent physical, neurological, and
psychiatric examinations. They also underwent electroencephalography and computed tomography or magnetic resonance imaging of the brain. Routine laboratory
tests included Lues (syphilis) serology as well as analysis of vitamin B12, folic acid,
and thyroid-stimulating hormone levels. In all patients, neuropsychological testing was performed using the “Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s
Disease” test battery (CERAD, Morris et al., 1989).
The diagnosis of aMCI was defined by the Mayo criteria (Petersen et al., 1999),
which includes the presence of a memory complaint (corroborated by an informant),
impaired memory function for age and education (delayed word recall score >1 standard deviation below normal age- and education-matched mean for older adults),
preserved general cognitive function, intact activities of daily living, and the absence
of dementia. The threshold of >1 standard deviation instead of >1.5 for impairment
was used to increase statistical power. In some aMCI patients, a slight decrease in
executive function was detected. No other cognitive impairments were detected
with neuropsychological testing.
All AD patients met the diagnostic criteria of probable AD according to the
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (American
Psychiatric Association, 1994), the ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural
Disorders (World Health Organization, 1992), and the criteria of the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke and the Alzheimer’s
Disease and Related Disorders Association (McKhann et al., 1984). All AD patients
had a score of four on the Global Deterioration Scale (Reisberg, Ferris, De Leon, &
Crook, 1982). A score of four implies clear-cut evidence of a memory deficit during an intensive interview with a clinician, including decreased memory of current
and recent events, decreased ability to travel or handle finances, and an inability to
perform complex tasks. The patient may also deny that there is any problem with
his/her memory, even though it is evident to friends and family.
All study participants had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity and sufficient hearing ability. None of the participants had a physical handicap that affected
their ability to perform the required tasks or any indication of neurological or psychiatric disorders unrelated to their diagnosis.
2.2. AMI
All subjects took part in the AMI (Kopelman et al., 1990). The AMI is a semistructured interview consisting of two parts that independently test recall for
the two components of autobiographical memory: autobiographical incidents and
personal semantic information. Questions about personal semantic content and
autobiographical incidents are chosen to evoke memories from three periods (childhood, early adult life, and recent life) and to expose the pattern of autobiographical
memory deficits. The personal semantic schedule requests facts from childhood (e.g.,
names of schools or teachers), early adult life (e.g., name of the first employer,
date and place of wedding), and recent years (e.g., holidays, journeys, and previous hospitalizations). For each time period, a maximum of 21 points can be
achieved.
The autobiographical incident questions contain items assessing the same three
time periods. Subjects are required to recall three incidents from childhood, three
incidents from early adult life, and three recent events. The amount of detail used
to describe an event is rated. Three points are awarded for a correct episodic autobiographical memory that is specific in time and place. Two points are given for a
personal but non-specific event or a specific event for which time and place are not
recalled. One point is awarded for a vague personal memory. Zero points are given
for no response or a response based on semantic memory. For each time period, the
maximum score is nine. Informants, such as spouses or life-long companions, were
used to verify the accuracy and details of the responses.
2.3. Data analysis
The SPSS-14 statistical package for Windows was used for data analysis. Differences in age, education, and MMSE score were assessed using a one-way analysis
of variance (ANOVA) followed by a post hoc Scheffé test. The chi-square test was
used to detect differences in gender distribution. Two-way ANOVAs were used to
examine the main effects of group and time periods as well as the interaction effect
on the performance of semantic and episodic memory recall. A group–time period
interaction effect is indicative of a TG when the memory recall of a patient group
becomes progressively worse with periods that are more recent, relative to that of
HCs. Subsequent ANOVAs were used to test for TG separately in the two patient
groups. Between-group differences within each time period were examined with
a one-way ANOVA followed by a post hoc Scheffé test. To test for a substantial
influence of age, two-way analyses of covariance (ANCOVA), with age as a covariate, were used to examine the main effects of group and time periods, as well as
an interaction effect on the performance of semantic and episodic memory recall.
To control for the influence of executive functions, correlations between the Trail
Making Test (TMT) from the CERAD (Morris et al., 1989) and the results of the
AMI in the patientsı̌ groups were evaluated using the Spearman-rank correlation
test.
2466
T. Leyhe et al. / Neuropsychologia 47 (2009) 2464–2469
Table 1
Demographic data and cognitive status.
Characteristic
Number
Female/male
Age (years): mean (SD)
Education (years): mean (SD)
MMSE (max. 30): mean (SD)
Subject group
HC
aMCI
AD
20
14/6
71.6 (6.5)
11.8 (2.8)
29.7 (0.5)
20
8/12
72.6 (6.8)
10.2 (5.2)
26.7 (3.3)
20
10/10
76.9 (5.5)
10.4 (3.8)
20.3 (5.9)
HC, healthy controls; aMCI, patients with amnesic mild cognitive impairment; AD,
patients with early Alzheimer’s disease; MMSE, Mini Mental State Examination;
max., maximum; SD, standard deviation.
3. Results
3.1. Characteristics of HCs, aMCI patients, and AD patients
The demographic data and the mean Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975) scores of the
three groups are shown in Table 1. A one-way ANOVA of the mean
age (F[2,57] = 3.601; P = .059) and education (F[2,57] = .954; P = .391)
did not show any significant group differences. The three groups
also did not differ in gender distribution (chi-square [3] = 2.67,
P = .606). As expected, a one-way ANOVA revealed highly significant differences for the MMSE (F[2,57] = 38.60; P < .001). HC subjects
had higher mean MMSE scores than aMCI (P < .05) and AD patients
(P < .001). Moreover, mean MMSE scores of aMCI patients were
higher than those of AD patients (P < .001).
3.2. Autobiographical semantic memories
An analysis of semantic information retrieval revealed a significant main effect for group (F[2,57] = 19.63; P < .001). Post hoc
Scheffé tests showed that retrieval was poorer in AD patients than
in HC subjects (P < .001) and aMCI patients (P < .001). No differences in retrieval were detected between the HC and aMCI groups
(P = .072). The other main factor, time period, was also significant
(F[2,56] = 18.092; P < .001). To explore whether a TG existed in the
recollection of memories, we tested the interaction between time
period and group. We found that the interaction was significant
(F[4,114] = 5.232; P < .001), indicating the presence of a TG. Fig. 1
shows semantic autobiographical memory performance of the individual groups.
To control for a possible influence of age, we performed a twoway ANCOVA, with age as a covariate. We again found a significant
main effect of group (F[2,56] = 15.56; P < .001) and a significant
interaction effect between time period and group (F[4,112] = 5.288;
P < .001). There was no main effect of the covariate factor age
(F[1,57] = 1.26; P = .275).
We also examined the possibility that executive function confounded the results. We correlated the results of the TMT from
the CERAD (Morris et al., 1989) with the results of autobiographical semantic memory in the patient groups. We did not find any
significant correlations (r scores ranged from −0.295 to 0.247; all
P > .1) between autobiographical semantic memory and either TMT
A or TMT B. As TMT B scores reflect not only executive function but
also attention, we computed TMT B–A scores by subtracting TMT A
scores (indicating attentional speed) from TMT B scores (indicating
the more executive aspect of divided attention). There were no significant correlations between autobiographical semantic memory
and TMT B–A scores in the patient groups (r scores ranged from
−0.152 to 0.359; all P > .5).
Subsequent analyses showed a TG over all time periods in AD
patients compared to HC subjects (F[2,37] = 11.568; P < .001). AD
patients performed worse than the HC group in recall of autobiographical semantic information from childhood (P < .05), early
Fig. 1. Recall of personal semantic memories across life periods in healthy controls (HC), patients with amnesic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI), and early
Alzheimer’s disease (AD) patients. Means and standard errors of means (SEM) are
given. Max. = maximum.
adulthood (P < .001), and recent life (P < .001). Facts from childhood
were recalled better than facts from early adulthood (P < .01) or
recent life (P < .001). Moreover, facts from early adulthood were
retrieved better than facts from recent life (P < .01).
No significant TG was found in the aMCI group compared to
HC subjects, although a stable trend was observed (F[2,37] = 3.020;
P = .06). Retrieval of facts from recent life was significantly worse in
aMCI patients than HC subjects (P < .05). However, these groups did
not differ in the recall of childhood (P = .66) and early adulthood
facts (P = .15). The aMCI patients performed better than the AD
patients in recalling autobiographical semantic information from
early adulthood (P < .001) and recent life (P < .01), but not from
childhood (P = .27). In aMCI patients, recall of facts from childhood
was similar to recall of those from early adulthood (P < .40). In contrast, these patients had more difficulty retrieving facts from recent
life than from either childhood (P < .01) or early adulthood (P < .01).
In the HC group, recall of personal semantic information did not
vary by time period. Table 2 summarizes AMI scores for autobiographical semantic memory.
3.3. Autobiographical incident (episodic) memories
Incident memory recall varied significantly according to group
(F[2,57] = 31.65; P < .001). Post hoc Scheffé tests showed that the HC
group performed better in the recall of incident memories than the
AD group (P < .001) or the aMCI group (P < .05). In addition, recall
was better in aMCI patients than AD patients (P < .001). Both time
period (F[2,56] = 18.905; P < .001) and the group–time period interaction were also significant (F[4,114] = 4.002; P < .01), indicating the
presence of TG. Fig. 2 shows the incident autobiographical memory
performance of the individual groups.
To exclude a confounding influence of age, we performed a
two-way ANCOVA with age as a covariate. There was a significant main effect of group (F[2,56] = 26.53; P < .001) and a significant
interaction effect between time period and group (F[4,112] = 4.74;
P < .001), but there was no main effect of the covariate factor age
(F[1,57] = 0.31; P = .581).
We also examined the potentially confounding effect of executive function by performing a correlation analysis between
autobiographical episodic memory scores and data from the TMT
T. Leyhe et al. / Neuropsychologia 47 (2009) 2464–2469
2467
Table 2
Scores for autobiographical semantic memory, as assessed using the Autobiographical Memory Interview (Kopelman et al., 1990).
Subject group
Life period
Significant differences between time periods
Childhood
Early adulthood
Recent life
HC
20.00 (1.86)
20.45 (.94)
19.70 (1.86)
n.s.
aMCI
19.30 (1.49)
18.90 (1.97)
16.65 (3.86)
AD
18.05 (3.45)
15.35 (3.64)
12.75 (4.85)
Childhood > Recent life**
Early adulthood > Recent life**
Childhood > Early adulthood**
Childhood > Recent life***
Early adulthood > Recent life**
Significant group differences
AD < HC*
AD < HC*** AD < aMCI***
AD < HC*** AD < aMCI** aMCI < HC*
Note: scores are expressed as mean (standard deviation). Maximum score = 21. HC, healthy controls; aMCI, patients with amnesic mild cognitive impairment; AD, patients
with early Alzheimer’s disease.
*
P < .05.
**
P < .01.
***
P < .001.
of the CERAD (Morris et al., 1989) in the patient groups. Neither
for TMT A, nor for TMT B was there any significant correlation
(r from −0.262 to 0.299; all P > .1) with autobiographical episodic
memory. Since TMT B scores reflect not only executive function but
also attention, we computed TMT B–A scores by subtracting TMT
A scores (indicating attentional speed) from TMT B scores (indicating the more executive aspect of divided attention). There were no
significant correlations between performances in autobiographical
episodic memory and TMT B–A scores in the patient groups (r from
−0.224 to 0.205; all P > .3).
The AD group showed a TG over all time periods relative to
HC subjects (F[2,37] = 13.11; P < .001). AD patients performed worse
than HC subjects in recall of autobiographical incident memories
from childhood (P < .001), early adulthood (P < .001), and recent life
(P < .001). Recall of incidents from childhood was better than recall
of those from early adulthood (P < .05) or recent life (P < .001). Moreover, recall of incidents from early adulthood was better than recall
of those from recent life (P < .001).
A memory decline could also be observed in the aMCI group
(F[2,37] = 3.602; p < .05). Recall of autobiographical incidents from
childhood (P = .77) and early adulthood (P = .42) was similar
between the aMCI and HC subjects. However, recall of recent life
Fig. 2. Recall of personal incident memories across life periods in healthy controls (HC), patients with amnesic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI), and early
Alzheimer’s disease (AD) patients. Means and standard errors of means (SEM) are
given. Max. = maximum.
events was significantly worse in aMCI patients (P < .05). For all time
periods, aMCI patients scored higher than their AD counterparts
(childhood P < .05, early adulthood P < .001, recent life P < .001).
Although aMCI patients recalled incidents from childhood and early
adulthood equally well (P = .19), retrieval of incidents from recent
life was poor compared to retrieval of either childhood (P < .01) or
early adulthood incidents (P < .05).
In HC subjects, personal incident recall did not differ by time
period. Table 3 summarizes the AMI scores for autobiographical
episodic memory.
4. Discussion
The data presented here are in agreement with those from previous studies showing that autobiographical memory is impaired in
AD patients (Eustache et al., 2004; Meeter et al., 2006; Piolino et al.,
2003; Starkstein, Boller, & Garau, 2005) and MCI patients (Murphy
et al., 2008). We have found that AD-related declines in the memory
of autobiographical incidents and semantic information have a clear
TG, wherein remote memories are better preserved than ones that
are more recent. These results were not significantly confounded
by age or impairments in executive function.
These findings are in accordance with the Cortical Reallocation Theory (Alvarez & Squire, 1994; Meeter and Murre, 2004;
Squire, 2004). As memory consolidation depends on hippocampal
integrity (Maguire, 2001; Svoboda et al., 2006), disturbances in the
consolidation of autobiographical incidents can occur at the beginning of hippocampal degeneration, which is associated with early
AD (deToledo-Morrell, Goncharova, Dickerson, Wilson, & Bennett,
2000). As the disease typically starts late in life, degradation of
autobiographical incident memory is most pronounced for recent
events. According to the Cortical Reallocation Theory, episodic
memory of early adulthood and childhood is better preserved,
as these memories have been consolidated and stored in the
neocortex, and have become independent of the hippocampus.
However, our AD patients showed a slightly impaired ability to
remember remote incidents. This finding might be due in part to
the spreading of the neurodegenerative process to the neocortex,
as had been shown in previous studies in early AD (Scheff &
Price, 2006). Possibly retrieval deficits contribute to AD patients’
problems. As the disease develops gradually, they may retrieve
recent memories less often and thereby forget them more. This
could indirectly affect consolidation.
Our data are less compatible with the Multiple Trace Theory
(Moscovitch et al., 2005; Nadel & Moscovitch, 1997), as a more balanced impairment of autobiographical incident memory over all
life periods would be expected with that theory. Explaining our
results in accordance with the Multiple Trace Theory would mean
2468
T. Leyhe et al. / Neuropsychologia 47 (2009) 2464–2469
Table 3
Scores for autobiographical incident memory, as assessed using the Autobiographical Memory Interview (Kopelman et al., 1990).
Subject group
Life period
Significant differences between time periods
Childhood
Early adulthood
Recent life
HC
8.30 (1.03)
8.05 (1.05)
8.06 (1.31)
n.s.
aMCI
7.90 (1.25)
7.30 (1.45)
5.65 (2.81)
Childhood > Recent life**
Early adulthood > Recent life*
AD
6.15 (2.51)
5.05 (2.52)
2.60 (2.11)
Childhood > Early adulthood*
Childhood > Recent life***
Early adulthood > Recent life***
Significant group differences
AD < HC*** AD < aMCI*
AD < HC*** AD < aMCI***
AD < HC*** AD < aMCI*** aMCI < HC*
Note: scores are expressed as mean (standard deviation). Maximum score = 9. HC, healthy controls; aMCI, patients with amnesic mild cognitive impairment; AD, patients with
early Alzheimer’s disease.
*
P < .05.
**
P < .01.
***
P < .001.
that older hippocampal–neocortical traces are generally stronger
and thus less susceptible to partial hippocampal damage. The weakest traces of recent life are disrupted first, followed by the stronger
traces of early adulthood and the strongest traces of childhood.
The pattern of autobiographical semantic memory decline
resembled that of autobiographical incident memory decline in our
study. Given that the lateral temporal cortex (mainly on the left) is
particularly involved in the retrieval of semantic, autobiographical
memory (Fujii, Moscovitch, & Nadel, 2000; Gilboa et al., 2005), the
initial hippocampal damage associated with AD should not affect
the memory of personal semantic facts. One possible explanation
for our findings is an early spread of the disease to the lateral temporal cortex, as has been shown with neuroimaging (Whitwell et
al., 2007). However, a more reasonable explanation would be that
the initial consolidation of personal semantic facts depends on the
hippocampus (Nadel & Moscovitch, 1997; Squire, 1992; Squire &
Alvarez, 1995) and, thus, is also susceptible to early hippocampal
damage.
Inconsistent results from previous studies with regard to TG
as well as semantic and episodic memory in the autobiographical memory of AD patients might be attributable to the study of
patients with varying stages of AD (Dall’Ora et al., 1989; Greene
et al., 1995; Hou et al., 2005; Ivanoiu, Cooper, Shanks, & Venneri,
2004; Kopelman et al., 1989; Meeter et al., 2006; Nestor et al.,
2002; Snowden et al., 1996). With progression of AD, atrophy of
the hippocampus progresses (deToledo-Morrell et al., 2000), and
the neurodegenerative process affects larger portions of the neocortex (Braak & Braak, 1999). Thus, as both the Cortical Reallocation
Theory and Multiple Trace Theory would predict, patients with
advanced cases of AD should exhibit no TG in autobiographical
memory, because neuronal correlates of remote memory are also
disrupted at this stage. Similarly, differences between semantic and
episodic memory should disappear.
Previous studies have shown that the posterior cingulate cortex,
a region anatomically connected with medial temporal cortex, plays
a role in autobiographical memory retrieval (Maddock, Garrett,
& Buonocore, 2001). PET imaging studies have found that amyloid accumulates in the posterior cingulate in early AD (Rowe et
al., 2007). Functional imaging studies have detected the earliest
metabolic changes of AD in this region (Salmon, Lekeu, Bastin,
Garraux, & Collette, 2008). Thus, neuropathological changes in the
posterior cingulate cortex may contribute to impaired autobiographical memory retrieval in patients with early AD.
While AD patients performed worse than HC subjects in remembering both remote and recent time periods, the aMCI group
differed significantly from HC subjects only for recent memory.
This decline occurred in the memory of both personal semantic
information and incidents. In contrast, Murphy et al. (2008) did not
detect any impairment in autobiographical semantic memory in
aMCI patients. In this study, aMCI patients performed worse than
HC subjects only in terms of incident recall. Moreover, fewer details
were provided for earlier life periods than for later life periods
in both patients and healthy controls. Differences between these
previous findings and the findings of this study may be due to differences in the patient populations studied. Impairment of our aMCI
group was possibly more pronounced, as we found that global cognitive function (assessed with the MMSE) was significantly lower in
aMCI patients than in HC subjects. The aMCI and HC subject groups
in the study by Murphy and colleagues were comparable in this
measure.
Another reason for contradictory results between this study and
previous reports may be that different memory tests were used.
Murphy’s group used the autobiographical interview of Levine et
al. (2002). In that interview method, one narrative for each of five
life periods is given. These narratives are used to evaluate both
episodic and semantic autobiographical memory. In contrast, the
AMI of Kopelman et al. (1990) uses separate items to assess memory
for episodic events and personal semantics. In this method, evaluation of autobiographical incident memory is based upon recall of
three incidents from childhood, three from early adult life, and three
recent events. Therefore, recall of only one event per time period, as
requested in the autobiographical interview of Levine et al. (2002),
might be easier. This event was presumably the most accessible and,
therefore, the most likely to yield detailed recollection. Additional
events may have been less accessible for recall. Differences between
the results of Murphy et al. (2008) and the present study may also
be due to the more complex scoring system used by Murphy et al.
Nevertheless, our findings in the aMCI group suggest that a
decline in autobiographical memory starts as soon as consolidation of autobiographical information is disturbed by hippocampal
damage. Previous studies have shown altered activation patterns
in the hippocampus and medial temporal lobes in people without memory impairment who have an increased genetic risk of
developing AD (Wermke, Sorg, Wohlschläger, & Drzezga, 2008).
Thus, functional impairment of the hippocampus may precede the
appearance of MCI. It can be hypothesized that the time of diminution of autobiographical memory in this group could mark the
beginning of functional impairment in the medial temporal lobe.
A limitation of this study is the lack of neuroimaging results.
Future investigations of autobiographical memory should involve
an examination of the association between memory and a volumetric analysis of anatomical brain regions.
In summary, the present study shows that autobiographical
memory is impaired in AD and aMCI patients. Deterioration was
found for both autobiographical incident remembrance and autobiographical semantic remembrance. In AD patients, a TG was
T. Leyhe et al. / Neuropsychologia 47 (2009) 2464–2469
detected, while in aMCI patients, a significant decline occurred
only in autobiographical memory of recent life. This likely indicates
that deterioration of consolidation of personal facts and events
begins with the commencement of functional impairment in the
hippocampus.
References
Alvarez, R., & Squire, L. R. (1994). Memory consolidation and the medial temporal
lobe: A simple network model. Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences, 91,
7041–7045.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (4th ed.). Washington: American Psychiatric Association.
Braak, H., & Braak, E. (1999). Temporal sequence of Alzheimer’s disease-related
pathology. In A. Peters, & J. H. Morrison (Eds.), Cerebral cortex, neurodegenerative and age-related changes in structure and function of the cerebral cortex (pp.
475–512). New York: Kluwer Academic Plenum Publishers.
Dall’Ora, P., della Sala, S., & Spinnler, H. (1989). Autobiographical memory: Its impairment in amnesic syndromes. Cortex, 25, 197–217.
deToledo-Morrell, L., Goncharova, I., Dickerson, B., Wilson, R. S., & Bennett, D. A.
(2000). From health aging to early Alzheimer’s disease: In vivo detection of
entorhinal cortex atrophy. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 911,
240–253.
Eustache, F., Piolino, P., Giffard, B., Viader, F., DeLa Sayette, V., Baron, J.-C., et al. (2004).
‘In the course of time’: A PET study of the cerebral substrates of autobiographical
amnesia in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, 127, 1549–1560.
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental state. A practical
method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of
Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198.
Fujii, T., Moscovitch, M., & Nadel, L. (2000). Consolidation, retrograde amnesia, and
the temporal lobe. In F. Boller, J. Grafman, & L. S. Cermak (Eds.), The handbook
of neuropsychology (2nd ed., Vol. 4, pp. 223–250). Amsterdam, The Netherlands:
Elsevier.
Gauthier, S., Reisberg, B., Zaudig, M., Petersen, R. C., Ritchie, K., Broich, K., et al. (2006).
Mild cognitive impairment. Lancet, 367, 1262–1270.
Gilboa, A., Ramirez, J., Köhler, S., Westmacott, R., Black, S. E., & Moscovitch, M. (2005).
Retrieval of autobiographical memory in Alzheimer’s disease: Relation to volumes of medial temporal lobe and other structures. Hippocampus, 15, 535–550.
Greene, J. D. W., Hodges, J. R., & Baddeley, A. D. (1995). Autobiographical memory
and executive function in early dementia of Alzheimer type. Neuropsychologia,
33, 1647–1670.
Hou, C. E., Bruce, L., Kramer, M., & Kramer, H. (2005). Patterns of autobiographical memory loss in dementia. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 20,
809–815.
Ivanoiu, A., Cooper, J. M., Shanks, M. F., & Venneri, A. (2004). Retrieval of episodic and
semantic autobiographical memories in early Alzheimer’s disease and semantic
dementia. Cortex, 40, 173–175.
Kapur, N., Thompson, P., Kartsounis, L. D., & Abbott, P. (1999). Retrograde amnesia:
Clinical and methodological caveats. Neuropsychologia, 37(1), 27–30.
Kopelman, M. D., Wilson, B. A., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). The autobiographical memory
interview. Bury St. Edmunds: Thames Valley Test.
Kopelman, M. D., Wilson, B. A., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). The autobiographical memory interview: A new assessment of autobiographical and personal semantic
memory in amnesic patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 11, 724–744.
Levine, B., Svoboda, E., Hay, J. F., Winocur, G., & Moscovitch, M. (2002). Aging and autobiographical memory: Dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval. Psychology
and Aging, 17, 677–689.
Maddock, R. J., Garrett, A. S., & Buonocore, M. H. (2001). Remembering familiar
people: the posterior cingulate cortex and autobiographical memory retrieval.
Neuroscience, 104(3), 667–676.
Maguire, E. A. (2001). Neuroimaging studies of autobiographical event memory.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 356, 1441–1451.
McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R., Price, D., & Stadlan, E. M. (1984).
Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s Disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA work
group under the auspices of the Department of Health and Human Services task
force on Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 34, 939–944.
Meeter, M., Eijsackers, E. V., & Mulder, J. L. (2006). Retrograde amnesia for autobiographical memories and public events in mild and moderate Alzheimer’s disease.
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychiatry, 28, 914–927.
Meeter, M., & Murre, J. M. J. (2004). Consolidation of long-term memory: Evidence
and alternatives. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 843–857.
2469
Morris, J. C., Heyman, A., Mohs, R. C., Hughes, J. P., van Belle, G., Fillenbaum, G., et al.
(1989). The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERADNP) Part 1. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease.
Neurology, 39, 1159–1165.
Moscovitch, M., Rosenbaum, R. S., Gilboa, A., Addis, D. R., Westmacott, R., Grady, C.,
et al. (2005). Functional neuroanatomy of remote episodic, semantic and spatial
memory: A unified account based on multiple trace theory. Journal of Anatomy,
207(1), 35–66.
Murphy, K. J., Troyer, A. K., Levine, B., & Moscovitch, M. (2008). Episodic, but
not semantic, autobiographical memory is reduced in amnestic mild cognitive
impairment. Neuropsychologia, 46, 3116–3123.
Nadel, L., & Moscovitch, M. (1997). Memory consolidation, retrograde amnesia and
the hippocampal complex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 7, 217–227.
Nestor, P. J., Graham, K. S., Bozeat, S., Simons, J. S., & Hodges, J. R. (2002). Memory
consolidation and the hippocampus: Further evidence from studies of autobiographical memory in semantic dementia and frontal variant frontotemporal
dementia. Neuropsychologia, 40, 633–654.
Petersen, R. C., Doody, R., Kurz, A., Mohs, R. C., Morris, J. C., Rabins, P. V., et al.
(2001). Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Archives of Neurology,
58, 1985–1992.
Petersen, R. C., Smith, G. E., Waring, S. C., Ivnik, R. J., Tangalos, E. G., & Kokmen,
E. (1999). Mild cognitive impairment: Clinical characterization and outcome.
Archives of Neurology, 56, 303–308.
Piolino, P., Desgranges, B., Belliard, S., Matuszewski, V., Lalevée, C., De La Sayette,
V., et al. (2003). Autobiographical memory and autonoetic consciousness: Triple
dissociation in neurodegenerative diseases. Brain, 126, 2203–2219.
Reisberg, B., Ferris, S. H., De Leon, M. J., & Crook, T. (1982). The Global Deterioration Scale for assessment of primary degenerative dementia. American Journal
of Psychiatry, 139, 1136–1139.
Ribot, T. (1881). Diseases of memory. New York: Appleton.
Rowe, C. C., Ng, S., Ackermann, U., Gong, S. J., Pike, K., Savage, G., et al. (2007).
Imaging beta-amyloid burden in aging and dementia. Neurology, 68(20), 1718–
1725.
Salmon, E., Lekeu, F., Bastin, C., Garraux, G., & Collette, F. (2008). Functional imaging of cognition in Alzheimer’s disease using positron emission tomography.
Neuropsychologia, 46, 1613–1623.
Scheff, S. W., & Price, D. A. (2006). Alzheimer’s disease-related alterations in synaptic density: Neocortex and hippocampus. Journal of Alzheimers Disease., 9(3),
101–115.
Smith, C. N., & Squire, L. R. (2009). Medial temporal lobe activity during retrieval of
semantic memory is related to the age of the memory. Journal of Neuroscience,
29(4), 930–938.
Snowden, J. S., Griffiths, H. L., & Neary, D. (1996). Semantic–episodic memory interactions in semantic dementia: Implications for retrograde memory function.
Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13, 1101–1137.
Squire, L. R. (1992). Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with
rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychological Review, 99, 195–231.
Squire, L. R. (2004). Memory systems of the brain: A brief history and current perspective. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 82, 171–177.
Squire, L. R., & Alvarez, P. (1995). Retrograde amnesia and memory consolidation: A
neurobiological perspective. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 5, 169–177.
Starkstein, S. E., Boller, F., & Garau, L. (2005). A two-year follow-up study of remote
memory in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 17, 336–341.
Svoboda, E., McKinnon, M. C., & Levine, B. (2006). The functional neuroanatomy of
autobiographical memory: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2189–2208.
Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 1–25.
Wheeler, M. A., Stuss, D. T., & Tulving, E. (1997). Toward a theory of episodic memory: The frontal lobes and autonoetic consciousness. Psychological Bulletin, 121,
331–354.
Wermke, M., Sorg, C., Wohlschläger, A. M., & Drzezga, A. (2008). A new integrative model of cerebral activation, deactivation and default mode function in
Alzheimerı̌s disease. European Journal of Nuclear Medicine and Molecular Imaging,
35(Suppl. 1), 812–824.
Whitwell, J. L., Przybelski, S. A., Weigand, S. D., Knopman, D. S., Boeve, B. F., Petersen, R.
C., et al. (2007). 3D maps from multiple MRI illustrate changing atrophy patterns
as subjects progress from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease.
Brain, 130, 1777–1786.
World Health Organization. (1992). Mental and behavioural disorders (including disorders of psychological development) (Chapter 5) (F). In: Clinical descriptions and
diagnostic guidelines. Tenth revision of the International Classification of Diseases. Geneva: World Health Organzization.