Download Effects of Reversible Inactivation of the Primate Mesencephalic

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Clinical neurochemistry wikipedia , lookup

Development of the nervous system wikipedia , lookup

Biological neuron model wikipedia , lookup

Neural oscillation wikipedia , lookup

Feature detection (nervous system) wikipedia , lookup

Neuroscience in space wikipedia , lookup

Neuroanatomy wikipedia , lookup

Nervous system network models wikipedia , lookup

Synaptic gating wikipedia , lookup

Pre-Bötzinger complex wikipedia , lookup

Neuropsychopharmacology wikipedia , lookup

Optogenetics wikipedia , lookup

Central pattern generator wikipedia , lookup

Metastability in the brain wikipedia , lookup

Neural correlates of consciousness wikipedia , lookup

Channelrhodopsin wikipedia , lookup

Premovement neuronal activity wikipedia , lookup

Process tracing wikipedia , lookup

Eye tracking wikipedia , lookup

Superior colliculus wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Effects of Reversible Inactivation of the Primate Mesencephalic
Reticular Formation. I. Hypermetric Goal-Directed Saccades
DAVID M. WAITZMAN, VALENTINE L. SILAKOV, STACY DEPALMA-BOWLES, AND AMANDA S. AYERS
Department of Neurology, The University of Connecticut Health Center, Farmington, Connecticut 06030
Waitzman, David M., Valentine L. Silakov, Stacy DePalmaBowles, and Amanda S. Ayers. Effects of reversible inactivation of
the primate mesencephalic reticular formation. I. Hypermetric goaldirected saccades. J. Neurophysiol. 83: 2260 –2284, 2000. Singleneuron recording and electrical microstimulation suggest three roles
for the mesencephalic reticular formation (MRF) in oculomotor control: 1) saccade triggering, 2) computation of the horizontal component of saccade amplitude (a feed-forward function), and 3) feedback
of an eye velocity signal from the paramedian zone of the pontine
reticular formation (PPRF) to higher structures. These ideas were
tested using reversible inactivation of the MRF with pressure microinjection of muscimol, a GABAA agonist, in four rhesus monkeys
prepared for chronic single-neuron and eye movement recording.
Reversible inactivation revealed two subregions of the MRF: ventralcaudal and rostral. The ventral-caudal region, which corresponds to
the central MRF, the cMRF, or nucleus subcuneiformis, is the focus
of this paper and is located lateral to the oculomotor nucleus and
caudal to the posterior commissure (PC). Inactivation of the cMRF
produced contraversive, upward saccade hypermetria. In three of eight
injections, the velocity of hypermetric saccades was too fast for a
given saccade amplitude, and saccade duration was shorter. The
latency for initiation of most contraversive saccades was markedly
reduced. Fixation was also destabilized with the development of
macrosaccadic square-wave jerks that were directed toward a contraversive goal in the hypermetric direction. Spontaneous saccades collected in total darkness were also directed toward the same orbital
goal, up and to the contraversive side. Three of eight muscimol
injections were associated with a shift in the initial position of the
eyes. A contralateral head tilt was also observed in 5 out of 8 caudal
injections. All ventral-caudal injections with head tilt showed no
evidence of vertical postsaccadic drift. This suggested that the observed changes in head movement and posture resulted from inactivation of the caudal MRF and not spread of the muscimol to the
interstitial nucleus of Cajal (INC). Evidence of hypermetria strongly
supports the idea that the ventral-caudal MRF participates in the
feedback control of saccade accuracy. However, development of
goal-directed eye movements, as well as a shift in the initial position
following some of the cMRF injections, suggest that this region also
contributes to the generation of an estimate of target or eye position
coded in craniotopic coordinates. Last, the observed reduction in
contraversive saccade latency and development of macrosaccadic
square-wave jerks supports a role of the MRF in saccade triggering.
INTRODUCTION
Three possible oculomotor roles have been suggested for
the central mesencephalic reticular formation (cMRF)
The costs of publication of this article were defrayed in part by the payment
of page charges. The article must therefore be hereby marked “advertisement”
in accordance with 18 U.S.C. Section 1734 solely to indicate this fact.
2260
(Waitzman et al. 1996). Subthreshold low-frequency electrical microstimulation and single neuron recording of a
low-frequency, long-latency (15–100 ms) discharge before
saccades support the idea that the cMRF participates in
saccade triggering (Cohen et al. 1985, 1986; Handel and
Glimcher 1997; Waitzman 1982, 1992; Waitzman et al.
1996). Second, existence of cMRF neurons with contralateral movement fields that increase their discharge with the
horizontal but not the vertical component of movement
suggests that these cells could serve as a spatial filter extracting the horizontal component of movements from the
superior colliculus (SC) output (Sparks 1986; Sparks and
Mays 1990; Waitzman et al. 1996). Cells in the rostral
portion of the MRF (see accompanying paper) may participate in the generation of the vertical component of saccadic
eye movement (Handel and Glimcher 1997). Third, by virtue of a burst of activity that peaks just before and during
saccades and dynamics of the neural discharge that correlate
closely with either eye velocity and/or displacement, we
have hypothesized that cMRF neurons could participate in
the feedback control of saccades (Waitzman et al. 1996).
The impact of each of these hypotheses on saccade generation is illustrated with the help of two feedback models of
oculomotor control (Fig. 1). The eye position model shown in
Fig. 1A was based on the original, local-feedback model of
Robinson (1975). Current eye position in space (Eye) was
subtracted from target position in space (Targ) by the retina, to
produce a retinal error signal (Rerr). Robinson’s major contribution was to suggest that retinal error was added to an internal
copy of eye position (i.e., efference copy, or corollary discharge, E⬘) in craniotopic coordinates to create an estimate of
target position with respect to the head not the retina (Tarest).
In a subsequent step, efference copy (E⬘) was subtracted from
a delayed copy of target position to generate a motor error
signal (em) used to drive the burst neurons in the pontine
reticular formation (B). Integration of the velocity output of the
burst neurons (Vc) by the neural integrator (NI) produced an
eye position signal used to drive the ocular motoneurons. Two
unique properties emerged from this model. First, by virtue of
local feedback, burst output continued for as long as necessary
to get the eyes onto the target and explained many aspects of
the relationship between saccade amplitude and duration. Second, the input to the oculomotor system was a target position
with respect to the head signal. This property in particular
made it easy to incorporate vestibular inputs (Robinson 1975).
However, since its proposal, a number of objections have been
raised to this model and question its applicability to the ocu-
0022-3077/00 $5.00 Copyright © 2000 The American Physiological Society
CMRF
AND GOAL-DIRECTED SACCADES
2261
FIG. 1. Two local feedback models for generation of saccadic eye movements. A: an eye position model modified from
Robinson (1975). Target position in space is compared with eye position in space by the retina generating retinal error (Rerr) at
summing junction 1 (SJ1). Retinal error is then added to an efference copy of current eye position (E⬘) at SJ2 to produce an estimate
of target position with respect to the head (Tarest). After a delay of ⬃250 ms (i.e., central processing of visual information including
target selection), current eye position (Eⴕ), is now subtracted from desired eye position (ed) at SJ3 to generate a motor error (em).
Motor error, which represents how far the eyes must move to point toward the target, drives the burst neurons in the pontine
reticular formation (PPRF) through a switch (trigger) thought to represent the omnipause neurons. The “velocity command” [Vc;
output of the medium lead burst neurons (MLBs), B] is applied to the neural integrator (NI) to generate current eye position (an
internal representation of eye position) and to the oculomotor plant to produce the actual eye movement. Reductions in desired eye
position will lead to hypometric saccades, whereas interruption in the generation of the current eye position (i.e., loss of feedback)
will generate hypermetric saccades. B: the local feedback portion of the above model (dashed rectangle) is recast into “change in
eye position” coordinates (Jurgens and Becker 1981). The input to the local feedback loop is now a desired eye displacement (⌬E)
that is compared with current eye displacement to generate motor error (em). Again em drives the MLBs through a switch. However,
now the output of the MLBs, Vc, is fed back through a resettable integrator (RI) to generate current eye displacement (⌬E⬘). Vc
is also applied to the NI to hold the eyes steady at a new location following each saccade, eye position (E⬘). This diagram illustrates
that an increase in the input (⌬E) to the feedback loop can lead to saccade hypermetria (Hyper #1). Damage to the RI or feedback
path itself could also lead to saccade hypermetria (Hyper #2). Reduction in the trigger threshold will initiate saccades earlier (i.e.,
reduced saccade latency). Increased delays in the 2nd feedback pathway (Hyper #3) will lead to macrosaccadic square-wave jerks
and in some instances, hypermetria (see DISCUSSION).
lomotor system. One primary concern has been that few regions of the brain contain eye position activity [i.e., nucleus
prepositus hypoglossi (NPH) and the interstitial nucleus of
Cajal (INC)]. More importantly these regions do not project
back to areas such as the SC, which should receive feedback of
this efference copy of eye position.
The eye displacement model shown in Fig. 1B addressed
these issues by placing a resettable integrator (RI) into the local
feedback pathway. This modification transformed the inputs of
the model into retinotopic coordinates (Jurgens et al. 1981; see
Waitzman et al. 1991, 1996 for further discussion of this
model). Briefly, the input to the model, desired eye displacement (⌬E), was thought to arise from the frontal eye fields
(FEF) and dorsomedial frontal cortex (DMFC). The desired
displacement was compared with current eye displacement
(⌬Eⴕ) to produce a motor error (em) that was thought to reside
in the long lead burst neurons (LLBNs) of the paramedian zone
of the pontine reticular formation (PPRF). This motor error
signal was then relayed through a switch (controlled by a
trigger signal) to the medium-lead burst neurons (B) in the
2262
WAITZMAN, SILAKOV, DEPALMA-BOWLES, AND AYERS
PPRF. The output of the burst neurons was a velocity command (Vc) that was directed to both the NI and a RI (⌬E⬘ or the
efference copy) whose output was reset to zero at the end of
each saccade. The purpose of the NI was to hold the eyes
steady following the occurrence of each saccade while the
output of the RI was used to update higher structures of the
current displacement of the eyes. The NI for the horizontal
saccade component is generated in the NPH (Cannon and
Robinson 1987), and the NI for the vertical component of
saccades is thought to originate from the INC (Crawford et al.
1991). The source of the trigger signal used to initiate saccades
is thought to be the omnipause neurons located in the nucleus
raphe interpositus (RIP).
Predictions about the specific oculomotor deficits, which
may occur after inactivation of brain stem structures, are easier
to understand by reference to these models. Shifts in the input
to either model, that is a more distant orbital position (EP
model), or larger eye displacement (ED model), would result in
saccades that overshoot the goal (Fig. 1, A and B, Hyper #1).
A shift in input could occur if cMRF neurons performed a
spatial filter role for the SC and FEF output (Sparks 1986;
Sparks and Mays 1990; Waitzman et al. 1996). Simulations of
these various aspects of the models are presented in the DISCUSSION.
Reduction or damage to the pathways within the feedback
loop would eventually produce a reduction in either the current
eye position (EP model) or eye displacement (ED model)
feedback signals. This reduction would increase the duration of
the motor error signal (em), and the eyes would continue to
move beyond their goal, albeit at a slower velocity (Fig. 1, A
and B, Hyper #2). Thus, in the ED model if the reticulotectal,
long lead burst neurons (RTLLBNs) of the cMRF provide a
conduit for a velocity signal from the PPRF to the SC, or
participate in the process of integrating eye velocity (i.e., the
RI), loss of these cells should produce saccade hypermetria.
This result would correlate well with the feedback hypothesis
(Waitzman et al. 1996). However, damage to the feedback
mechanism of the ED model could not produce a change in
initial eye position or generate a saccade goal.
Reduction of feedback or damage to the neural integrator
itself in the EP model would also produce hypermetric, slow
saccades (Fig. 1A, Hyper #2). However, in this instance, shifts
in initial position and generation of a saccade goal relative to
the head could result. Moreover, damage in the second portion
of the feedback pathway of the EP model (Fig. 1A, Hyper #3)
could increase delays in the generation of the Tarest and cause
repeated saccades to a virtual target that continues to reappear
(see DISCUSSION). Finally, making the saccade trigger easier to
flip from opened to closed and vice versa could make saccade
latency shorter. This might occur if excitatory activity from
cMRF neurons important for maintaining the tonic firing of
omnipause neurons was removed (i.e., the triggering hypothesis) (Cohen et al. 1985; Hepp and Henn 1982, 1983; Waitzman
et al. 1996). Providing clear neurophysiological evidence to
support each of these hypotheses of MRF participation in
oculomotor control has proven difficult. The midbrain tegmentum contains both cells and fibers in passage from the superior
colliculus and other structures. As a result, the destruction or
activation of the collicular output may have biased previous
electrolytic lesion and electrical microstimulation experiments
(Cohen et al. 1982, 1985, 1986; Komatsuzaki et al. 1972).
The current group of experiments has been designed to
circumvent some of these difficulties. Following electrical microstimulation and single and multiunit identification of the
MRF, we have made microinjections of muscimol, a GABAA
agonist. We demonstrate that the MRF can be divided into two
separate regions. Inactivation of a ventral-caudal region, which
corresponds to the nucleus subcuneiformis (the cMRF), leads
to oblique (contraversive and up) saccade hypermetria, higher
saccade velocity, reduced saccade duration, and marked instability in fixation with the development of macrosaccadic
square-wave jerks to a specific goal in the orbit (the current
paper). Inactivation of the rostral portion of the MRF results in
severe hypometria primarily of vertical, but not horizontal
saccades (see accompanying paper, Waitzman et al. 2000). The
implications of these findings are discussed with reference to
the two models and three possible hypotheses for cMRF function just presented. Abstracts of these findings have appeared
previously (Silakov and Waitzman 1996; Waitzman and Silakov 1994; Waitzman et al. 1997).
METHODS
The methods for recording eye movements and single neurons,
electrical microstimulation, and data analysis in awake behaving
primates in these experiments are essentially the same as those described in detail elsewhere (Waitzman et al. 1991, 1996). All procedures were approved by the University Animal Care and Use Committee.
Injection and recording procedures
In brief, four male rhesus monkeys (G, C, K, and T) were surgically
prepared under isoflurane inhalational anesthesia with two eye coils
(Judge et al. 1980), a head restraining device, and two stainless steel
chambers to allow separate access to the MRF and the SC. The MRF
cylinder was positioned over the posterior portion of the cerebral
cortex tilted 15° off the sagittal plane (Waitzman et al. 1996). The
MRF, located just lateral to the oculomotor nuclei, was identified by
the characteristic features of single neurons that discharge with contraversive saccadic eye movements and electrical microstimulation
that elicited contraversive, conjugate saccades at short latency (Silakov et al. 1995; Waitzman et al. 1996). Eye movements were recorded
using the magnetic search coil technique and were accurate to 0.1°
(Judge et al. 1980). In two monkeys, a series of guide tubes were
placed parallel to each other and sampled the rostral, mid, and caudal
portions of the MRF. The tubes were semipermanently positioned
using a grid (spacing of 1 mm) fixed within the stainless steel
recording chamber. In the third and fourth monkeys, only the caudal
portion of the MRF was sampled. The arrangement of a rostral-caudal
orientation of the guide tubes allowed for repeated testing and subsequent permanent identification of the sites of muscimol injections. A
customized microinjection/recording needle (Crist et al. 1988) attached to a Hamilton syringe allowed for physiological confirmation
of neuronal activity related to saccades before an injection and monitoring of neuronal activity after the injection. In monkey T, a picospritzer apparatus was substituted for the Hamilton syringe (Dias and
Segraves 1997).
Behavioral paradigms
Figure 2A shows the fixation paradigm, and Fig. 2, B–D, illustrates
the visually guided saccade (VGS) paradigm. Visual targets were
positioned at eight different directions [0° (position 0), 45° (1), 90°
(2), 135° (3), 180° (4), 225° (5), 270° (6), and 315° and/or ⫺45° (7)]
and five amplitudes (5, 10, 15, 20, and 25°) along each of these
CMRF
AND GOAL-DIRECTED SACCADES
2263
FIG. 2. Behavioral paradigms and control experiment. A: fixation paradigm. With fixation point onset the monkey makes a saccade
to orient the fovea toward a small (⬍1°) visual stimulus. Note maintenance of fixation for ⱖ400 ms. Traces are horizontal (H) and vertical
(V) eye position and fixation point appearance (FP). Saccades to the right are indicated as upward deflections. B: visually guided saccade
(VGS) paradigm. Trial begins with fixation point onset toward which the monkey directs his eyes (similar to A). At a random interval
later the FP disappears and a peripheral target appears (Tar). After a variable delay period of 200 – 400 ms, the monkey makes a visually
guided saccade to look at the peripheral target. Calibration in A applies to B. C: control trajectories of visually guided saccades from
primary position (straight ahead) to 8 different target positions (0 –7) before injection (monkey K). Zero degrees was defined as saccades
to the right and 180° as saccades to the left. Saccades down and to the right (i.e., 315°) were relabeled for ease of presentation as ⫺45°
(Fig. 11). D: 20° visually guided saccades to the 8 peripheral targets after the injection of 1 ␮l of saline in the rostral MRF of monkey
G (for location see Fig. 1 of Waitzman et al. 2000). ●, average final eye position for ten 20° control saccades to each of these targets
(g0217b.1).
directions for a total of 40 different target locations. The monkey was
trained to fixate a central light-emitting diode. After a variable interval
of 200 – 400 ms, the light was extinguished, and a new target light
appeared that was the cue for the monkey to shift his eyes and fixate
the new visual target (15° saccades are shown in Fig. 2B and 20° in
Fig. 2C). The monkey was rewarded for moving the eyes to within
⫾2° window of the visual target. After the injections this window was
relaxed to ⫾7° and in some cases ⫾12° so that all attempted saccades
to the visual target would be collected. The trajectories in each
direction of Fig. 2C show five repetitions. Note the regularity, accuracy, and straightness of the trajectories. Following a control injection
of saline in another monkey, the trajectories of the saccades were
unchanged from baseline (compare Fig. 2D, saline, to Fig. 4A, same
monkey 25° saccades, no injection). Filled circles show the average of
all endpoints of control saccades to the same visual target. Saccades
of five different amplitudes (5–25°; 8 randomized directions ⫻ 5
repetitions of each saccade ⫹ errors) were collected into separate files
for each injection. Each file took ⬃6 –15 min for the monkey to
complete. A “complete” set of data covering all 40 positions was
comprised of 5 files (1 for each amplitude, total collection time of
30 –50 min). The amplitudes sampled during the first two or three files
were repeated at the end of the sequence to document changes that
occurred while the drug had diffused.
Data analysis
Following each experiment raw eye movement records were processed by software that identified the beginning and end of each eye
movement using a template matching algorithm (Waitzman et al.
1991). Each trial was visually inspected, and marks indicating the
2264
WAITZMAN, SILAKOV, DEPALMA-BOWLES, AND AYERS
beginning and end of horizontal and vertical components of each
saccade were corrected as needed. Corrective saccades following the
primary movement were specifically excluded from the current analysis. Variations in saccade amplitude and direction following the
injections were evaluated in a number of ways. One analytic technique
calculated the fractional change in saccade amplitude and direction
following the injection (Fig. 4D). A “difference coefficient” for each
of these metrics was calculated by taking the difference between postand pre-parameters and then expressing this as a fraction of the
prevalue. This technique effectively normalized the data so that
changes in eye movements of different amplitudes or directions could
be compared. A negative value for the difference coefficient for
amplitude (Diff. Amp.) indicated saccade hypometria, and a positive
value reflected saccade hypermetria. Difference coefficients were
plotted against target direction. In the analysis of changes following
an injection, we also tested the “null” hypothesis that saccades in a
particular direction were not deviated from their normal trajectory
(Fig. 4E). If direction was not modified, then the direction at saccade
end should be no different from target direction. The absolute difference between the angle at saccade end and target direction was the
saccade deviation that was plotted as a function of target direction. If
the deviation was positive (i.e., the postinjection angle was larger than
target direction), then by definition this was plotted as a counterclockwise deviation (CCW), and if the deviation was negative (i.e., postinjection angle smaller than target direction), then this was scored as a
clockwise (CW) deviation.
Two midbrain structures could be influenced by inactivation of the
MRF by muscimol: 1) the nuclei of the optic tract (NOT) and 2) the
INC. Contraversive slow phases of nystagmus develop after inactivation of the NOT, and position-dependent vertical postsaccadic drift
occurs after inactivation of the INC (Cohen et al. 1992; Crawford and
Vilis 1993; Crawford et al. 1991). To calculate the slow-phase eye
velocity, instantaneous eye velocity was averaged from the end of the
current saccade to just before the beginning of the subsequent saccade.
This was done for the horizontal component of all spontaneous
saccades (in total darkness) that occurred just before the paradigm
began (including control injections of saline). The horizontal slowphase eye velocity plotted for a single time point represented the
average of all intersaccadic intervals for a particular file (⬃70 –100
movements per file spanning 5–10 min). Time points were collected
starting just before the injection and for each subsequent file following
each injection until recording ended.
Drift amplitude [the amplitude of slow movement from saccade
offset to the end of the drift as per Crawford and Vilis (1993)] was
measured for at least 10 spontaneous eye movements occurring in
each file. A running average (Student’s t-test) was used to decide
when significant vertical drift had occurred.
Duration is directly proportional to vectorial amplitude for pure
horizontal saccades (Fuchs 1967). However, for oblique saccades
component stretching occurs to produce saccade trajectories that are
TABLE
straight. As a result, component (horizontal or vertical) duration is
proportional to vectorial amplitude (King et al. 1986) and is used to
display the duration data here. Comparison of the slopes of saccade
duration versus vectorial amplitude was made by t-test to determine
whether a change in component duration had occurred after muscimol
injections. In a similar fashion, the log relationship between vectorial
amplitude and velocity (Fuchs 1967; King et al. 1986) was compared
before and after muscimol to decide whether saccades had been
displaced off this main sequence.
Histology
Once all data were collected and the most productive eye movement regions identified, a pressure injection of 1–2 ␮l of fluorescent
labeled microspheres (green and red, LumaFluor, ⬃0.05 ␮m diam;
blue, Polyscience, BB19773, 0.05 ␮m diam) was made to positively
localize the sites of microinjection in three monkeys. The location of
the electrode tracks in one monkey was identified by placement of a
small electrolytic lesion. At the conclusion of the experiments, monkeys were deeply anesthetized with pentobarbital sodium and perfused. The brains were removed, and 50 ␮m vibratome sections were
made through the brain stem. Unstained sections (with fluorescent
beads) were mounted wet and photographed under both white and
fluorescent light. Alternating sections were stained with thionin and
drawn onto paper using an inverted microscope. Drawings were then
scanned into the computer and traced to produce the final anatomic
representation of injection sites.
RESULTS
Neuronal effects of muscimol: areas of inactivation
Eight injections of the GABAA agonist muscimol were made
in four monkeys at sites in the MRF where eye movement–
related cells were recorded (Table 1). Of the eight injections,
seven were made in head-fixed animals, and these injections
were used to summarize the effects of muscimol inactivation.
The eighth injection was done in the head-free animal to
demonstrate the interaction between head and eye initial position shift. Besides these eight injections, two injections of
inactive muscimol (determined empirically) produced no
changes in eye movements and were used as controls. Changes
in eye movements were noted as early as 5 min after a 1.0 ␮g
injection of muscimol (Sigma, 0.5 ␮g/␮l in sterile NaCl) into
the MRF and could last for up to 7 h. Typically, electrical
silence was noted at 20 –30 min, and thus early time points
were repeated after this initial inactivation period. Data collec-
1. Caudal cMRF muscimol injections
Injection
g021794
k032995
k033195
k040395
c041696
c041996
c052196
t091798
(head free)
Amount, ␮l
Concentration, ␮g/␮l
Side
Head Tilt
Nystagmus Onset
1
1
1
1
1
0.5
1
0.5
1
1
0.5
0.5
0.6
0.5
0.5
1
1
1
1
1
Left
Left
Left
Right
Left
Left
Left
Right
Right
Right
Yes/right
Yes/right
Yes/right
No
No
Yes/right
Yes/right
Yes/left
Yes/left
Yes/left
P ⬎ 0.05
5 min; hypermetric; up and right
P ⬎ 0.05
P ⬎ 0.05
P ⬎ 0.05
P ⬎ 0.05
No effects
50 min; spontaneous; up and left
30 min; spontaneous; up and right
44 min; hypermetric; up and right
0.675
1
Right
Yes/left
Onset of Effects
33 min, P ⬍ 0.05
59 min; hypermetric; up and left
127 min, P ⬍ 0.05
93 min; hypermetric; up and left
21 min; head and eye shift
Eight muscimol injections made in 4 monkeys. Rows without labels indicate a repeated dose of the given amount at the same injection site.
CMRF
AND GOAL-DIRECTED SACCADES
tion began with the start of the injection and continued for as
long as the monkey could perform the behavioral tasks.
We made parallel tracks 1 and 2 mm away from a 1.0 ␮g/␮l
muscimol injection in one monkey. Data from these tracks
showed that the blocked region (electrical silence) extended no
greater than 1.5 mm laterally from the site of injection. Cell
activity 2 mm lateral to the injection was normal. Monitoring of
activity above and below this site demonstrated a vertically
blocked region of 2.5 mm above the site of the injection. No eye
movements could be elicited from within the blocked region using
electrical microstimulation at 3 times threshold, but eye movements could be elicited below the blocked region. Twenty-four
hours later, neuronal activity in the blocked area had recovered,
electrical microstimulation could elicit saccades, and eye movements had returned to normal. These experiments suggested that
an injection of 1.0 ␮g/␮l of muscimol inactivated an ellipsoid
portion of the brain stem 2.2 mm in diameter and 1.5–2.5 mm in
2265
length. After a control injection of saline, neuronal activity was
suppressed for ⬃3 min (Fig. 2D, monkey G), but returned to
normal levels within 5–10 min. Following this control injection,
saccades to the eight different target positions located 20° from
primary position were straight, accurate, and thus unaffected by
the injection (Fig. 2D).
Our initial hypothesis was that the MRF [corresponding to
nucleus cuneiformis and nucleus subcuneiformis of Olszewski
and Baxter (1954)] was physiologically a homogeneous region.
As our experiments progressed, it was clear that some division of
the “MRF” was necessary, because the effects on eye movements
were quite different if injections were made rostral or caudal to the
posterior commissure. Specifically, an analysis of caudal injection
sites showed that oblique, upward, contraversive saccades became
hypermetric (Fig. 3, all caudal injections), whereas vertical saccades became hypometric after rostral injections (Fig. 4A of
accompanying paper, Waitzman et al. 2000).
FIG. 3. One representative section of the
caudal brain stem showing the locations of
all 8 muscimol injections made in 4 monkeys (多). All of these injections produced
saccade hypermetria, and their results are
presented in the current paper. Abbreviations for all anatomic sections: III, 3rd
nerve nucleus; IV, trochlear nucleus; aq,
aqueduct of sylvius; BC, brachium conjunctivum; BCdec, decussation of the brachium
conjunctivum; BIC, brachium of the inferior colliculus; BSC, brachium of the superior colliculus; CG, central gray; HBL, medial habenular nucleus; inc, interstitial
nucleus of Cajal; LL, lateral lemniscus;
ML, medial lemniscus; MLF, medial longitudinal fasciculus; NOT, N. of the optic
tract; NPC, N. of posterior commissure; PB,
N. parabigeminus; PC, posterior commissure; riMLF, rostral interstitial nucleus of
the MLF; RPC or NRPC, N. reticularis
ponto caudalis; RTP or NRTP, N. reticularis tegmenti pontis.
2266
WAITZMAN, SILAKOV, DEPALMA-BOWLES, AND AYERS
Inactivation of the ventral-caudal MRF (the cMRF):
synopsis
Muscimol inactivation of the ventral-caudal MRF produced
seven primary oculomotor effects in the monkey: 1) hypermetria of contraversive oblique saccades; 2) reduced saccade
latency; 3) a moderate reduction in saccade duration, with an
increase in saccade velocity following many injections; 4)
repetitive macrosaccadic square-wave jerks to a specific goal in
the orbit; 5) spontaneous saccades in the dark directed toward
the same specific goal in space (relative to the head); 6) straight
trajectories of saccades directed toward the orbital goal and
curved trajectories of saccades directed toward adjacent locations to the goal; and 7) contraversive head-tilt following five
of eight ventral-caudal injections.
These effects were consistent across monkeys and did not
occur after inactivation of adjacent locations in the brain stem.
The effects on saccade metrics (duration, velocity, and latency)
will be illustrated for seven injections. The eighth injection was
made in a monkey free to move its head, and thus the data for
saccade metrics were not comparable to the head-fixed case.
Careful examination of saccade duration will point to which
portions of the oculomotor models could account for the observed changes. No change in saccade duration would suggest
the input to the local feedback loop had shifted, whereas
increased duration would suggest loss of feedback. Shorter
duration and higher saccade velocity suggest a combination of
effects on model parameters.
Analysis of square-wave jerks and the goal-directed nature
of postinjection saccades are presented for all injections made
in head-fixed animals. Each of these injections had a different
goal to which spontaneous saccades were directed repeatedly.
Data from two injections will be presented in detail, one in the
left and the other in the right MRF. The rest of the data are
presented in summary format to illustrate the range of effects
observed.
Inactivation of the cMRF: changes in saccade metrics
Seven injections (c0416, c0419, c0521, g0217, k0329, k0331,
and k0403) placed into the caudal MRF of three head-fixed
monkeys produced hypermetric saccades. The results of one muscimol injection (1 ␮g/2 ␮l) placed at the site of cMRF long-lead
burst neurons that discharged before contraversive (rightward)
saccades is shown in Fig. 4. Multiunit contraversive eye movement related activity was registered through the recording syringe
at a similar depth at which the single cells of Fig. 4F (movement
fields) had previously been recorded. Electrical microstimulation
was not performed at this site. Within 5 min after the end of the
injection (duration of 20 min), 25° saccades up and to the contraversive side became hypermetric (Fig. 4B, positions 1 and 2; F,
averaged endpoints of control saccades). During the next hour of
observation (5–30 min shown) all visually guided saccades up and
to the right became hypermetric (Fig. 4, B–D). This hypermetria
affected the vertical more than the horizontal component of movement (Fig. 4D). For a 15° oblique saccade the horizontal component of movement was increased by 20%, whereas the increment
in the vertical component approached 50% (Fig. 4D, compare E
with 䊐).
There was a counterclockwise, upward deviation of the
endpoints of contraversive, horizontal (position 0), and
oblique, upward saccades (position 1, 45°) after this injection.
The endpoints of pure upward movements (position 2, 90°)
were deviated downward (i.e., negative direction, clockwise in
Fig. 4E). A similar, albeit smaller reversal of saccade endpoint
deviation occurred between positions 5 (225°) and 6 (270°;
Fig. 4E). These reversals of saccade deviation correspond with
zero crossings from counterclockwise to clockwise (Fig. 4E,
arrows). This defined a plane tilted ⬃25° from the vertical
toward which saccade endpoints were deviated (Fig. 4B). This
plane also influenced the trajectory of the saccade. Saccade
trajectories close to the plane remained almost straight,
whereas saccade trajectories in other directions became curved.
For example, the trajectories of upward vertical saccades to
position 2 were bowed away from this plane (but their endpoints were closer to the plane), whereas the trajectories of
oblique saccades to position 1 and those of horizontal saccades
were bowed upward toward this plane. Similarly, downward
saccades to position 6 were bowed away from the tilted vertical
plane (Fig. 4B). Such curvature suggests discoordination in the
generation of the horizontal or vertical components of the
saccade such that the vertical component reached peak velocity
before the horizontal component.
Details of the upward saccade hypermetria following this injection (g0217) are shown in Fig. 5. Hypermetric oblique saccades
(position 1) had an increase in peak velocity compared with
preinjection movements (Fig. 5A, horizontal; Fig. 5B, vertical).
Saccades in the opposite direction (down and to the left, position
5) were only slightly hypermetric (Fig. 5D). In both cases (position 1 and 5), the amplitude and velocity of the vertical component
was affected more than the horizontal component. In fact, horizontal component amplitude for ipsiversive saccades (position 5)
was slightly hypometric (Fig. 5C, solid line). The overall increases
in vectorial peak velocity for both directions (positions 1 and 5)
were matched by a commensurate increase in saccade amplitude
and duration. As a result, these postinjection saccades remained
on the amplitude versus peak velocity main sequence (Fig. 5E,
Table 2, P ⬎ 0.05).
The increased amplitude of postinjection saccades was matched
by a commensurate increase in horizontal saccade duration. This
maintained the same linear amplitude-duration relationship as
before injection [Fig. 6A, slopes (m) not different]. However,
duration of the vertical saccade component was longer than the
associated vectorial amplitude would have required (Fig. 6B),
while the slope of postinjection vertical duration versus amplitude
relationship rose. The difference in slope did not reach statistical
significance (see Fig. 10C). On the other hand, the latency to onset
for saccades of all amplitudes was significantly reduced following
this injection. Contraversive, upward saccades were initiated the
fastest and some latencies (150 ms) approached that of express
saccades (Fig. 6C).
To determine whether muscimol had spread to include the
NOT, dorsal to the MRF, horizontal slow-phase eye velocity
(slow movements between saccades) was calculated after the
injection (see METHODS) (Cohen et al. 1992). Control preinjection files demonstrated ⬍3°/s of contraversive, slow-phase eye
velocity (Fig. 6D, E). Following this muscimol injection, no
contraversive horizontal nystagmus was found (Fig. 6D, F).
These results suggested that the changes in saccade amplitude,
velocity, and latency could not be accounted for by spread of
the muscimol to involve the nucleus of the optic tract.
CMRF
AND GOAL-DIRECTED SACCADES
FIG. 4. Hypermetric saccades developed after a muscimol injection in the caudal MRF of monkey G (g0217). A: control
trajectories before injection. B: VGS trajectories 5 min following the injection of 1 ␮g (2 ␮l of 0.5 ␮g/␮l) of muscimol into the
left MRF. Note contraversive (rightward) saccade hypermetria to target positions 1 and 2. C: large almost 25° hypermetric saccades
generated in direction 1 to a visual target that was located 10° up and to the right. Times indicate the onset of eye movement
recording after muscimol injection. F, averages of all preinjection control eye positions endpoints. D: vectorial difference
coefficients for 4 different amplitude VGS in each of 8 target directions (●, ■, and Œ). Horizontal and vertical difference coefficients
for 15° VGS in each of 8 directions (E and 䊐). Note that both the horizontal and vertical components are modified by the injection.
Different symbols indicate which amplitude was affected by the injection. E: deviation of the endpoints of saccades occurred after
the muscimol injection. Key in D applies to E. CW, clockwise rotation of saccade endpoints are shown along the ordinate
(negative); CCW, counterclockwise rotation of saccade endpoints are shown along the ordinate (positive). F: 2-dimensional
cartesian plots of the contraversive (right) movement fields of 3 neurons recorded from the left MRF at the site of this muscimol
injection. G: site of this injection (g0217) and a control saline injection (g0223). Abbreviations as in Fig. 3.
2267
2268
WAITZMAN, SILAKOV, DEPALMA-BOWLES, AND AYERS
FIG. 5. Changes in saccade dynamics and
metrics that followed the left caudal MRF injection (g0217) of Fig. 4. A and B: eye position
and velocity trajectories of 10° VGS directed
up and to the contraversive (right) side (A and
B: position 1, same movements as Fig. 4C) and
down and to the ipsiversive side (C and D:
position 5). Traces from top to bottom: horizontal eye position and eye velocity (A and C);
vertical eye position and velocity (B and D;
right and up are positive). Control saccades are
indicated by dotted lines, and solid lines show
postinjection saccades. Note that the vertical
component of postinjection saccades toward
the 10° target at position 1 had significant
variability in saccade amplitude and velocities
of up to 750°/s (B). Postinjection saccades to
the ipsilateral side had slightly lower horizontal velocity (C), but higher vertical velocity
compared with control (D). E: complementary
changes in saccade velocity and amplitude
produced saccades that fell on the main sequence relating vectorial amplitude and velocity. E, preinjection saccades; ●, postinjection
movements; 䡠 䡠 䡠, control regression; —, postinjection regression.
Inactivation of the cMRF: square-wave jerks and changes in
initial position
At the end of 25 min after the injection, the monkey developed pronounced contraversive, upward macrosaccadic
square-wave jerks (Fig. 7). The requirement of this particular
paradigm was for the monkey to maintain stable fixation (Fig.
7, control, dotted line: see also Fig. 2A). After the injection the
monkey made repeated saccades that were in the same direction as the previously described hypermetria. Each eye movement was separated by a minimal intersaccadic time interval of
150 –200 ms. These movements were very stereotypic and
brought the eye to a specific location in the orbit (Fig. 8A).
Although changes of visually guided saccades with shifts in
initial position were not specifically studied in this monkey,
spontaneous saccades made in total darkness were collected
just after this fixation file. The trajectories of the spontaneous
saccades (whose vectors are shown in Fig. 8B) demonstrate
that the eyes were directed to a specific goal in the orbit located
13° to the right and 19° up (error bars are ⫾1 SD). We
compared an average of the endpoints of the macrosaccadic
square-wave jerks from the fixation paradigm with the location
CMRF
TABLE
g0217
c0416
c0419
c0521
k0329
k0331
k0403
AND GOAL-DIRECTED SACCADES
2. Main sequence for caudal injections
Control
Experimental
Control
Experimental
Control
Experimental
Control
Experimental
Control
Experimental
Control
Experimental
Control
Experimental
log r 2
P Value
0.9
0.82
0.82
0.9
0.82
0.78
0.82
0.62
0.87
0.89
0.87
0.85
0.93
0.94
⬎0.05
⬍0.001
⬍0.001
2269
tion 3) were displaced above the main sequence relating vectorial amplitude and velocity. However, the slope of the log
regression for all postinjection saccades while higher did not
reach statistical significance compared with preinjection control (Table 2). This result would not be expected from displacement of the saccade input (Hyper #1, Fig. 1), or interruption of
the feedback loop (Hyper #2, Fig. 1), which would have left
saccade velocity normal or lower, respectively.
⬎0.05
⬎0.05
⬎0.05
⬎0.05
Table of log regression coefficients for the amplitude-velocity (main sequence) relationship following each injection. r 2 (log) is the correlation coefficient for the log model.
of the endpoints of all of the spontaneous saccades and found
an extensive overlap (compare rectangular boxes in Fig. 8, A
and B). The dependence of postinjection spontaneous saccades
on initial eye position was assessed by calculation of an “orbital perturbation index.” This reflects the slope of the regression line relating component saccade amplitude with initial
position (Russo and Bruce 1993). The indexes for horizontal
and vertical saccade components were markedly elevated, supporting a strong effect of initial eye position on saccade amplitude. In summary, this injection demonstrated that inactivation of the cMRF was critical for the generation of saccade
hypermetria. Within 1 h of this injection the monkey generated
repetitive macrosaccadic square-wave jerks that brought the
eyes to a specific goal in the orbit. Two hours after the
injection, the monkey’s head was released and a contraversive
head tilt was noted.
Six other cMRF sites in two additional monkeys produced
essentially the same results but to varying degrees. The results
of an injection on the opposite side of the brain stem of another
monkey are shown in Fig. 9. The first visually guided saccades
were collected 88 min after the injection (Fig. 9B). The monkey could still make 25° saccades in all directions; however,
the initial position of the eyes was displaced up and slightly to
the contraversive side. Downward saccades were hypometric
missing the target, even when the shift in initial eye position
was taken into account. Contraversive (left) upward saccades
were displaced clockwise toward the earth vertical. Thirty
minutes later (127 min postinjection), saccades intended for the
5° contraversive target position (position 3) were markedly
hypermetric and drawn toward a specific position in the orbit
(Fig. 9C). Other contraversive movements were either very
hypermetric or could not be generated. Ipsiversive 5° saccades
were normal. Fixation was persistently interrupted by macrosaccadic square-wave jerks toward a contraversive upward
goal (x ⫽ 11.4°; y ⫽ 19°, Fig. 9D). One hundred forty-two
minutes after the end of the injection, spontaneous saccades
were directed toward this same location, 11.8° left and 19.4° up
(Fig. 9E). Slopes of the regression of initial eye position on
component saccade amplitude were markedly elevated. This
confirmed a strong effect of initial position on the amplitude
and direction of spontaneous saccades (Kh ⫽ ⫺0.61; Kv ⫽
⫺1.6). Last, postinjection saccades (particularly those to posi-
Summary of results: caudal injections
All but one muscimol injection showed a significant reduction in saccade latency (Fig. 10). The higher control latency for
monkey C was most likely the result of difficulty with down
gaze in the right eye (the recorded eye) following surgical
procedures to correct tethering of the eye-coil in this eye.
However, the reduction in latency was significant, and control
latency returned back to 600 ms the day following the c0416,
c0419, and c0521 injections. Taken together, these latency
results suggest a role for the ventral-caudal MRF in saccade
initiation and maintenance of fixation.
Because of the differential effect on saccade velocity following the two injections shown in detail, we also examined
the duration of the saccades for each of the seven injections.
Note that in six of seven injections, horizontal saccade duration
was reduced and in five of the seven injections, vertical saccade duration was reduced compared with control. These
trends reached significance in only two of the horizontal and
one of the vertical measurements (Fig. 10, B and C). This
corresponded closely to an increase in saccade velocity that
was higher than expected for amplitude and positioned these
movements above the main sequence. These results suggest
that inactivation of the ventral-caudal MRF could influence
portions of the saccade burst generator.
Effects of muscimol inactivation of the caudal MRF on the
amplitude of postinjection saccades are shown in Fig. 11. To
compare the degree of hypermetria of one injection to that of
another, the difference coefficient data were plotted so that the
direction of hypermetria was up and to the right (i.e., 45°).
Thus all injections were displayed as if they had occurred on
the left side of the brain stem. Difference coefficients for the
time point for which the monkey demonstrated the greatest
hypermetria but was still capable of making saccades in all
other directions are illustrated. All MRF injections caudal to
the posterior commissure produced contraversive saccade hypermetria, albeit to a small degree in two injections (k0329 and
k0403). The direction of saccade hypermetria was primarily up
and to the contraversive side. In one case, horizontal saccades
were hypermetric (c0419, E), but most often oblique upward
saccades were hypermetric. This family of curves illustrates
two points. First, the saccade hypermetria following ventralcaudal MRF inactivation ranged from ⬃5% of the control
value to ⬎50%. Second, there was a small secondary peak in
saccade hypermetria 180° in the opposite direction of the
primary hypermetria. This occurred for all injections except
k0403, which had a small degree of hypometria in the opposite
direction.
A final aspect of the hypermetria was that it directed saccades toward a specific region in the orbit regardless of initial
eye position. This was demonstrated by analyzing the direction
and final endpoints of files of spontaneous saccades recorded
2270
WAITZMAN, SILAKOV, DEPALMA-BOWLES, AND AYERS
FIG. 6. Changes in saccade duration, latency, and nystagmus following the muscimol injection of Fig. 4. A: horizontal
component saccade duration (ordinate) was not different from control following this injection. Vectorial amplitude is shown
on the abscissa. B: vertical component duration (ordinate) was longer after the injection, although the slopes were not
statistically different. Key is the same as in A. C: postinjection latency (●, 30 min postinjection) was shorter than control (E)
for saccades of all amplitudes. D: horizontal slow-phase eye velocity following this same muscimol injection was not
different (P ⬎ 0.5) from control (only contraversive slow phases are shown). Each symbol represents a different time point
collected up to 60 min after the injection. Note that ipsiversive slow phases were also analyzed and were also not different
from control (P ⬎ 0.5).
toward the end of the monkey’s ability to generate visually
guided saccades. The different regions to which spontaneous
saccades were directed are summarized in Fig. 12 for seven
injections and two control days. Regions determined by rightsided MRF injections are shown by open symbols, and those
following left-sided injections are filled. Note that all regions
were located in the top half plane of movement and most (6 of
8) were contraversive. This trend of spontaneous saccades
toward a specific goal in the orbit was confirmed by calculating
an orbital perturbation index for both the horizontal and ver-
tical components of spontaneous saccades. A clear effect (P ⬍
0.05) of initial eye position on the vertical component of
spontaneous saccades was found in three of seven injections
(data not shown). The horizontal orbital perturbation indexes
were increased over control (5 of 7) but did not reach statistical
significance.
Head tilt and shift of initial eye position
Three of eight injections in the ventral-caudal MRF were
associated with a shift in initial eye position (Fig. 13). The
CMRF
AND GOAL-DIRECTED SACCADES
2271
FIG. 7. Macrosaccadic square-wave jerks developing 20 min after a muscimol injection in the
left ventral-caudal MRF (g0217). A and B: horizontal and vertical eye position, respectively, following the injection. . . ., control fixation; —,
postinjection fixation. Note the ⬎25° repetitive up
and to the right (contraversive) saccades generated
with barely one (200 ms) intersaccadic delay.
shift in initial eye position increased over time, and typically
the monkey made no attempt to compensate for the shift.
The shift was contraversive in two injections (c0419 and
c0521) and ipsiversive and up after one injection (c0416).
One key to a better understanding of the changes noted in
initial eye position came from studying the contralateral head
tilt generated after the ventral-caudal muscimol injections. One
possibility was that the shifts in initial position noted with the
head fixed were compensatory for an attempted head movement in the opposite direction. Another possibility was that the
MRF contributed to maintenance of initial eye position via its
connections to the omnipause region of the PPRF. Loss of a
fixation signal would produce destabilization of fixation similar
to the macrosaccadic square-wave jerks shown above (Fig. 7).
To examine whether the shift in initial eye position was compensatory, we measured the head tilt in one monkey free to
move its head following a muscimol injection in the right
ventral-caudal MRF.
With the use of an additional coil fixed to the head in the
coronal plane, horizontal and vertical, but not torsional displacement of the head could be recorded (only 2 coils, 1 eye,
and 1 head could be monitored). An almost immediate contralateral head roll of ⬃30° was confirmed visually and via
photographs. The coronal coil demonstrated a contraversive
and downward head displacement (Fig. 14, D, E, and G). This
was associated with a compensatory shift of the initial position
of the eyes up and to the ipsilateral (right) side (Fig. 14D, F;
F, horizontal, ■; H, vertical, ■). This combination of head tilt
and shift in eye position resulted in gaze (combination of head
and eyes) being directed toward the center of the screen (Fig.
14B). This injection was performed using ⬍0.5 ␮l of muscimol
from a picospritzer apparatus, limiting spread of muscimol to
adjacent structures. This suggests that the shift of initial eye
position seen after the muscimol injections could have been
compensatory for an intended head tilt.
DISCUSSION
To better characterize the oculomotor function of neurons in the
MRF, injections of the GABAA agonist, muscimol, were placed at
the sites of midbrain neurons that discharged with, or where
electrical microstimulation induced, contraversive, conjugate saccades (Cohen et al. 1985; Handel and Glimcher 1997; Waitzman
et al. 1996). Two previous, careful studies of single-neuron activity in behaving monkeys have revealed only a gross topographic
arrangement of the oculomotor functions in this region. In particular, cMRF neurons, adjacent to the oculomotor nuclei, began to
discharge 150 ms and peaked 8 –10 ms before saccades with a
contraversive horizontal or downward oblique component of
movement (Waitzman et al. 1996). More rostrally located MRF
neurons, adjacent to the INC also had long-lead activity, but were
most sensitive to contraversive oblique and vertical saccades
(Handel and Glimcher 1997). The movement fields for both
groups of neurons were large and could extend for up to 40°.
The primary findings of this study were that inactivation of
the ventral-caudal MRF 1) generated conjugate, contraversive,
upward saccade hypermetria; 2) reduced saccade latency; and
3) produced a moderate increase in saccade velocity accompanied by a moderate reduction in saccade duration. Many ventral-caudal injections also produced macrosaccadic squarewave jerks that repetitively brought the eyes to a fixed place in
the orbit. Similarly, spontaneous saccades executed in total
darkness were directed toward the same specific orbital position. The distribution of the orbital goals was across the contralateral upper field of movement. Interestingly, three of the
muscimol injections induced a displacement of the initial position of the eyes. These findings suggested a number of
possible roles for cells in the cMRF: 1) participation in feedback of an eye position or displacement signal, 2) stabilization/
maintenance of fixation, and 3) activation of the saccade burst
generator. These results are discussed in light of the various
anatomic connections of the MRF and how these cells could
2272
WAITZMAN, SILAKOV, DEPALMA-BOWLES, AND AYERS
FIG. 8. Trajectories of macrosaccadic square-wave jerks and spontaneous saccades. A: Cartesian x-y plot of macrosaccadic
square-wave jerks that interrupted fixation (see Fig. 7) were directed up and to the contraversive side. The middle of the box here
and in B represents the average of the endpoints of all saccades, the edges of the box are ⫾1 SD around the mean. B: vectorial
representation of amplitude and direction of spontaneous saccades made in total darkness 60 min after a left-sided muscimol
injection (g0217). C and D: the slopes of the component horizontal and vertical amplitude vs. initial position plots formed the
perturbation indexes for the spontaneous saccades of B. Note the effect of initial position on saccade amplitude.
participate in the circuitry needed for the control of saccades
and stabilization of fixation. Simulations of the two models
presented in the INTRODUCTION are used to demonstrate that an
eye position and not eye displacement model can best explain
the current observations.
Localization of muscimol inactivation
GABA containing interneurons have been localized to the
MRF (Nagai et al. 1983). Our basic assumption was that
muscimol activation of GABAergic synapses on saccade related long-lead burst neurons in the MRF produced the observed oculomotor effects and left fibers in passage (i.e., the
central tegmental tract) unaffected (Andrews and Johnston
1979; Krogsgaard-Larsen et al. 1979; Ritchie 1979). Singleunit recordings through and around the region blocked by
muscimol demonstrated no neural activity in a sphere of radius
1.1 mm centered on the site of injection and support this view.
Saline injections produced no oculomotor effects, thus eliminating a mechanical pressure gradient as the source of our
findings (Fig. 2). Inadvertent inactivation of a number of oculomotor structures adjacent to the MRF including the nucleus
reticularis tegmenti pontis (NRTP), the SC, and the NOT,
could color the above interpretation of our results. However,
muscimol inactivation of these regions has produced little or no
saccade hypermetria (Aizawa and Wurtz 1998; Hikosaka and
Wurtz 1985a; Lee et al. 1988; Munoz and Wurtz 1993; Quaia
et al. 1998; van Opstal et al. 1996). Destabilization of fixation
has occurred following rostral SC inactivation, but macrosaccadic square-wave jerks directed toward a specific location in
the orbit were not generated (Munoz and Wurtz 1993). Possible inactivation of the NOT could produce horizontal, contraversive nystagmus (Cohen et al. 1992). In five of seven injections, no nystagmus was found (Table 1). In one of the other
two remaining injections, onset of hypermetria occurred before
contraversive nystagmus developed. Taken together this con-
CMRF
AND GOAL-DIRECTED SACCADES
2273
FIG. 9. Generation of repetitive macrosaccadic square-wave jerks following a 2nd muscimol injection (1.0 ␮l of 1.0 ␮g/␮l) in
the right caudal MRF (c0521). A: control saccade trajectories. B: trajectories of saccades made to eight 25° targets 88 min after a
muscimol injection in the right, caudal MRF. Note moderate saccade deviation and hypermetria of saccades to position 3. ●, average
location of control saccades. C: a similar file of 5° visually guided saccades collected 127 min after the muscimol injection. Note
the marked hypermetria of contraversive saccades made to a 5° target at position 3. D: macrosaccadic square-wave jerks that
interrupted steady fixation at a target located at the center of the tangent screen. The center of the rectangular box corresponds to
the average, and its edges are ⫾1 SD of final eye position for the entire group of square-wave jerks. E: spontaneous saccades made
in total darkness 142 min after this muscimol injection. Note that the saccades were directed toward a goal whose center was located
11.8° to the left and 18.4° up. The rectangular box around these endpoints is determined as in D. Note extensive overlap with
endpoints of saccades in D.
stellation of findings suggests that inactivation of the MRF and
not adjacent structures produced saccade hypermetria.
Oculomotor functions of cMRF: anatomic connections
Intracellular filling of MRF neurons has shown them to be of
at least three types (Scudder et al. 1996). RTLLBNs located
within the central MRF (i.e., the nucleus subcuneiformis) direct their axons toward the ipsilateral SC where they arborize
within the intermediate and deep layers. These cells provide a
collateral branch that crosses the intracollicular commissure to
innervate the contralateral SC (Moschovakis et al. 1988). This
group of neurons could influence the generation of saccades in
the SC. A second group of MRF LLBNs [probably reticulospi-
nal LLBNs (RSLLBNs)] is located lateral to the INC, rostral to
the RTLLBNs just described (Scudder et al. 1996). The
RTLLBNs have contralateral movement fields, whereas the
RSLLBNs have vertical movement fields (Handel and Glimcher 1997; Scudder et al. 1996; Waitzman et al. 2000). These
cells have axons that descend toward the pons to innervate the
raphe nuclei (raphe pontis, nucleus RIP, raphe obscuris) and
the medullary reticular formation (primarily the inhibitory
burst neuron region caudal to the abducens nucleus) (Scudder
et al. 1996). The RSLLBNs could interact with both saccade
and head generation networks in the pons and spinal cord (see
accompanying paper, Waitzman et al. 2000). Scudder and
colleagues (1996) have also described a third group of saccaderelated neurons whose cell bodies are probably located within
2274
WAITZMAN, SILAKOV, DEPALMA-BOWLES, AND AYERS
FIG. 10. Summary of changes in saccade latency and duration following muscimol inactivation of the caudal MRF. Data from
7 muscimol injections are included of which 4 were performed on the left side and 3 were placed in the right MRF (c0419, c0521,
k0331). A: note the reduction in latency of all saccades with the exception of one experiment (k0331). The high preinjection
latencies for monkey C were noted after an eye coil repair that slowed downward saccades. The postinjection reduction in saccade
latency for the 3 injections in this monkey was still significant when downward saccades were excluded from the analysis. B: slopes
of the component duration vs. vectorial amplitude graphs as shown in Fig. 6B (horizontal, B, and vertical, C) before and after
injection. Horizontal saccade duration was reduced for all injections with exception of k0403, but reached significance for c0416
and c0419. C: a similar reduction in vertical component duration for all injections with the exception of c0521 and g0217, both of
which showed moderate increases. Only the reduced duration of postinjection saccades of c0416 reached statistical significance.
CMRF
AND GOAL-DIRECTED SACCADES
2275
FIG. 11. Summary of difference coefficients for
the 7 muscimol injections performed in the caudal
portion of the MRF of 3 monkeys. The last difference
coefficient curve for which the monkey could make
saccades in all directions is displayed. ●, ■, Œ, and ,
left-sided injections; E, 䊐, ‚, right-sided injections.
The 3 caudal MRF injections (g0217, c0416, and
c0419) associated with prominent contraversive hypermetria (25–50%) were also associated with a
smaller but significant degree of ipsiversive hypermetria, directed 180° in the opposite direction of the
largest contraversive point. Control injection of saline
is shown by the bold solid line with opened circles.
The data are plotted so that direction of largest hypermetria was located at 45°. This demonstrates 2 points.
First, there was moderate hypermetria in the opposite
direction for 3 injections. For example g0217 showed
contraversive hypermetria at 45° (up and to the right)
and ipsiversive hypermetria at 225° (down and to the
left). Second, the degree of hypermetria ranged from
5% (k0403) to ⬎50% (g0217).
the caudal MRF (i.e., again nucleus subcuneiformis) and
whose discharge is similar to the RTLLBNs. However, the
axons of these cells (cRSLLBNs) were directed caudally
within the predorsal bundle and innervated the NRTP, RIP, the
nucleus reticularis pontis oralis and caudalis (NRPo-NRPc;
including the excitatory and inhibitory burst neurons) and sent
a descending axon to cervical levels of the spinal cord. An
ipsilateral projection from the cMRF and cuneate reticular
nucleus to the ventral horn of the cervical spinal cord, separate
from that of the INC, has been confirmed repeatedly in both cat
(Castiglioni et al. 1978) and monkey (Crawford and Villis
1993; Fukushima 1987; Fukushima et al. 1987; Kokkoroyannis
et al. 1996; Robinson et al. 1994; Scudder et al. 1996). Moreover, the descending MRF projections were more numerous
than the projections from the INC to the cervical spinal cord
(Robinson et al. 1994; Scudder et al. 1996). Furthermore, the
MRF also receives head-related proprioception via direct afferents from the cervical spinal cord and dorsal column nuclei
(Bjorkland and Boivie 1984; Pechura and Liu 1986). In sum,
this pattern of connectivity suggests that caudal MRF neurons
could participate in the generation of combined head and eye
movements. The output of cRSLLBNs to the omnipause neurons in the RIP and the precerebellar saccade generating machinery in the NRTP and NRPo-NRPc could account for the
marked reduction in saccade latency found after cMRF inactivation (J. Buttner, personal communication; Buttner-Ennever
and Buttner 1988; Horn et al. 1994; Scudder et al. 1996). In the
normal state, MRF activity could enhance the tonic firing rate
of omnipause neurons thereby suppressing unwanted saccades
as has been suggested by the results of electrical microstimu-
FIG. 12. Evidence for goal-directed saccades
following muscimol injections. Plot showing the
locations of the saccade goals of the spontaneous
saccades for each of 7 muscimol injections made
in the caudal MRF. All goals were located in the
top half of the contraversive field of movement
with the exception of 2 (k0329 and k0403). These
goals corresponded closely with the location of the
endpoints of square-wave jerks during fixation.
Open symbols are right-sided injections, and filled
symbols are left-sided injections.
2276
WAITZMAN, SILAKOV, DEPALMA-BOWLES, AND AYERS
FIG. 13. Changes in initial position following all injections in 3 monkeys. The initial position was measured
for successive time points and plotted in x-y coordinates.
Note a small but consistent shift in the initial position
following 3 injections (c0521, c0419, and c0416). ●, ■, ,
and °, injections made in the left side; ‚, {, and E,
injections made on the right side. Two injections (c0419
and c0521) produced shifts to the contraversive side (E
and ‚). One injection produced a shift up and to the
ipsiversive side (■).
lation (Cohen et al. 1985). A likely candidate for this function
would be high background cMRF neurons described previously (Waitzman et al. 1996). Loss of this excitation would
lead to fixation instability.
However, loss of fixation would not necessarily generate
macrosaccadic square-wave jerks per se. More likely, the muscimol inactivation produced two possible effects. First, it could
produce a loss of a suppression signal (i.e., reduced excitation)
to the omnipause neurons, permitting more frequent and longer
pauses. Second, during saccades, the MRF could provide a
feedback signal (i.e., of current eye displacement or eye position) needed to stop the saccade accurately. A reduction or
complete loss of this feedback signal would cause a persistent
motor error that the saccadic system would try to correct, thus
generating the repeated macrosaccadic jerks at short latency.
The interesting part of the square-wave jerks observed here
were that they brought the eyes to a particular position in the
orbit that was similar to the final positions of many of the
spontaneous saccades made in total darkness. This could not be
accomplished by feedback of an eye displacement signal.
Caudal MRF: craniotopic not retinotopic organization
Five findings in the current study suggest that MRF neurons
are organized in a spatial not a retinotopic frame of reference.
First, electrical stimulation at sites where muscimol was injected generated contraversive saccades. Recent results have
demonstrated that electrical stimulation and single-unit recording at similar sites elicited goal-directed saccades (Waitzman et
al. 1998). Muscimol inactivation of this region left horizontal
saccades for the most part intact. The endpoints of horizontal
saccades were deviated upward and in a few instances, the
horizontal component of movement was modestly hypermetric.
At the same time, contraversive upward, oblique saccades were
markedly hypermetric. If the ventral-caudal MRF were retinotopically coded, then horizontal saccades should have been
rendered hypometric and the vertical component of saccades
should have been unaffected. This would be similar to the
results of muscimol inactivation of the retinotopic map found
in the superior colliculus (Aizawa and Wurtz 1998; Hikosaka
and Wurtz 1985a; Lee et al. 1988; Munoz and Wurtz 1995a,b;
Quaia et al. 1998).
A second finding following MRF inactivation was that spontaneous saccades were not executed to random locations in the
orbit. Instead, saccade endpoints defined a specific “goal” in
the orbit. This goal was typically displaced upward from the
pure horizontal locus defined by prior electrical stimulation and
single-unit recording (see Fig. 15). Third, an analysis of the
endpoints of macrosaccadic square-wave jerks during fixation
showed that the eyes were directed to the same locus defined
by spontaneous saccades executed in total darkness. The amplitude and direction of these repetitive movements varied in a
predictable way with shifts in the initial position of the eyes,
such that their endpoints coincided with the same region of the
orbit delineated by the final positions of spontaneous saccades.
Fourth, a moderate shift in the initial position of saccades was
noted after three of eight injections. Last, a contraversive head
tilt was observed after nearly all the caudal MRF injections.
Electrical stimulation in the MRF of dogs and monkeys has
produced contraversive saccades in association with head
movements (Bender and Shanzer 1964; Silakov et al. 1999;
Szentagothai 1943). Electrolytic lesions in the MRF produced
an ipsilateral gaze preference, in which monkeys did not make
gaze movements (head and eyes) to visual stimuli on the
contralateral side (Komatsuzaki et al. 1972). Taken together,
these data are most consistent with the idea that MRF neurons
participate in the generation of a final eye or target position in
the orbit (craniotopic, Eye Position Model), rather than an eye
displacement signal (retinotopic, Eye Displacement Model).
The concept that the MRF specifies final orbital position
would fit with a number of other physiological and anatomic
findings. The cMRF receives projections from both cortical
and subcortical regions where goal-directed saccades have
CMRF
AND GOAL-DIRECTED SACCADES
FIG. 14. Head tilt and initial position changes 21 min after a muscimol injection in the right MRF of a head-free monkey
(t0917). A and C: preinjection initial gaze, eye, and head positions. Gaze is centered. Head and eye positions are distributed around
initial eye position. D: 30 min after muscimol injection there was a downward and small contraversive shift in initial head position
(open circles) measured by a head coil in the coronal plane. Initial eye position (gray circles) was shifted upward and to the
ipsiversive side. Gaze (filled circles, B) showed no change. E–H: histograms of initial head (E, horizontal, mean 1.86 ⫾ 3.56; G,
vertical, mean 4.50 ⫾ 4.47) and eye position (F, horizontal, mean ⫺2.46 ⫾ 2.68; H, vertical, mean ⫺2.82 ⫾ 4.31) before (open
bars) and initial head (E, horizontal, mean ⫺0.47 ⫾ 4.34; G, vertical, mean ⫺8.75 ⫾ 3.062) and eye position (F, horizontal, mean
1.75 ⫾ 3.04; H, vertical, mean 8.15 ⫾ 3.14) after (filled bars) injection. These histograms show the significant difference in initial
head and eye positions that exactly compensated for each other. The postinjection means of both initial eye and head positions were
significantly different from control at the P ⬍ 0.05 level.
2277
2278
WAITZMAN, SILAKOV, DEPALMA-BOWLES, AND AYERS
in the MRF (Schall 1991). Recent data suggest that the discharge of some SEF neurons is object oriented and initial eye
position can have a moderate effect on discharge (Olson and
Gettner 1995; Russo and Bruce 1993). Microstimulation of the
SEF produces contraversive saccades that bring the eyes to a
“termination zone” (Russo and Bruce 1993; Tehovnik et al.
1994). This is similar to the variable amplitude and/or goaldirected saccades generated after MRF microstimulation (Cohen et al. 1985; Silakov et al. 1995; Waitzman et al. 1998). In
conclusion, we suggest that muscimol inactivation of the
cMRF uncovered an underlying craniotopic organization of
neurons that contributes to the generation of an estimate of a
final eye or target position in the orbit signal.
Generation of vertical saccade components from a
“horizontal” region
FIG. 15. Changes of averaged cMRF neural activity before and after muscimol inactivation. A: averaged activity of 9 cMRF neurons recorded from
monkey G before muscimol injection. Target position in the horizontal and
vertical direction is shown on the x-y plane. Neuronal activation, normalized to
individual peak activity and combined across all cells is shown along the
z-axis. A high level of spontaneous background activity of MRF neurons is
apparent from the shelf shown across the contraversive portion of the x-y plane.
Note a large contraversive peak of activity for primarily horizontal saccades of
40 – 60° final position. B: large “relative” peak of activity appears after hypothetical suppression of MRF activity related to down and contraversive saccades (cutoffs were set at ⬍10° up and 0° horizontal). This suggests that
resultant saccades would be directed up and to the contraversive side (site of
peak activity). cMRF neurons and their associated best saccade vectors used
for the plot in A were g1203, 14.5°; g1207, 1.1°; g1209, ⫺10.9°; g1210, 14.4°;
g1221, ⫺67.9°; g0309, ⫺56°; g0310, ⫺54°; g0310.2, ⫺65.6°; g0328, ⫺8.9°
(negative angles relative to 0° right).
been elicited. Electrical microstimulation and single-neuron
recording in the SC and the FEF, both of which provide
projections to the cMRF, have demonstrated a moderate effect
of initial eye position on the size of elicited saccades (Cowie
and Robinson 1994; Segraves and Goldberg 1991). Probably
more important are the large numbers of projections from the
supplementary eye fields (SEF) (Huerta and Kaas 1990; Shook
et al. 1990) and posterior parietal cortex (Kunzle and Akert
1977; Leichnetz and Goldberg 1988) to the MRF. Neurons in
the SEF discharge in response to purposeful movements and
have open and closed movement fields similar to those found
Single-unit recordings and microstimulation have strongly
implied that neurons in the caudal MRF participate in the
generation of the horizontal component of saccades (Cohen et
al. 1985; Waitzman et al. 1996). Our expectation was that
inactivation of this caudal region would have generated hypometric horizontal not the hypermetric oblique saccades observed. This prediction was based on two primary assumptions.
First, we hypothesized that the MRF was coded in retinotopic
coordinates, and, second, we thought MRF neurons provided
signals for generation of eye movements alone, not a combination of head and eye movements. The data just reviewed
support the idea that MRF neurons are organized in craniotopic
not retinotopic coordinates. We now present one example of
how inactivation of a brain stem region that specifies final
horizontal position of the eyes in the orbit could produce
oblique saccades.
An averaged representation of the movement fields from
nine caudal MRF neurons from one monkey was constructed
by normalizing all neuronal activity to the peak firing of each
cell (see also Fig. 9C of Waitzman et al. 1996). The peak
activity of this averaged movement field was greatest for
contraversive eye positions (Fig. 15, positive horizontal axis is
contraversive). However, contraversive movements, with vertical components were also associated with increased (albeit
lower than peak) neuronal discharge (note elevated shelf of
neural activity on the right half of the movement field). Moreover, there was no clear cutoff of activity for saccades of very
large amplitude (i.e., the movement field was open). Ipsiversive movements were associated with little or no activity.
Unfortunately these MRF neurons were not studied during
shifts of initial position, but for the purposes of this discussion
we assume that changes in initial position would lead to activation of cells when the eye reached a particular final position.
We also made the assumption that muscimol inactivation
would eliminate neural activity in this averaged “movement
field” of all saccades with final positions, which landed on the
contraversive side 10° up to 40° down. The resultant averaged
activity is shown in Fig. 15B. Note that a relative peak activity
of the grouped MRF activity appears up and to the contraversive side. We further assume that, as more of the map is
inactivated, final eye position will be shifted further upward
(e.g., arrow moving the mountain toward the upward region).
This idea would explain the upward displacement of the final
CMRF
AND GOAL-DIRECTED SACCADES
position of spontaneous and visually guided eye movements
observed after the injection of muscimol.
Evidence of a contraversive head tilt following MRF inactivation suggests that interaction of the MRF with either the
vestibular or head movement control system could produce
vertical saccade components. Interactions with the vestibular
system would have to occur via indirect pathways because
there are no direct projections from the MRF to the vestibular
nuclei or vestibular cerebellum (flocculus). On the other hand,
projections from the MRF to regions of the brain stem and
spinal cord that participate in head movements have been
described. Two regions could have particular importance. First,
the loss of MRF input to the cervical spinal cord (Castiglioni et
al. 1978; Robinson et al. 1994) could produce an intended
contraversive head tilt (i.e., the monkeys were head fixed). This
could generate secondary changes of proprioceptive inputs to
the vestibular cerebellum and would result in a reduction of
floccular output. This scenario would result in the generation of
a compensatory upward vertical output signal. A more direct
route might target the NRTP, which also receives significant
projections from the MRF as well as the SC and cerebellar
nuclei (Brodal 1980; Gerrits and Voogd 1986; Harting 1978;
Scudder et al. 1996; Thielert and Their 1993). Inactivation of
the caudal portion of NRTP would produce a loss of torsional
eye control and could result in the generation of an upward eye
movement component (van Opstal et al. 1996). A similar
mechanism has been proposed for the upward deviation of
saccade trajectories observed after inactivation of the SC
(Aizawa and Wurtz 1998; Quaia et al. 1998, 1999). In sum,
generation of the vertical saccade component after muscimol
inactivation would be better understood in head-free monkeys
whose head and eye movements were recorded in three dimensions.
Generation of saccade hypermetria: models
What physiological mechanisms are responsible for saccade
hypermetria, increased saccade velocity, and goal-directed
macrosaccadic square-wave jerks observed after muscimol inactivation of the MRF? Simulation has demonstrated that
changes at three sites (Hyper #1, #2, and #3) in the Eye
Position model (Fig. 1A) and two sites in the Eye Displacement
model (Fig. 1B) could generate saccade hypermetria. Manipulation of two locations (Hyper #1 and #2), the model input and
the local feedback pathway, could generate saccade hypermetria in both models (Hyper #1 and #2). A shift in the input to
either model (i.e., a new eye displacement, or desired final eye
position) to a higher value resulted in saccade hypermetria
(Fig. 16A, new input).
In the one-dimensional case shown here, this would be a
switch from a 12° to a 24° eye displacement site or a similar
shift in final orbital position. Such a shift might occur via
inactivation of a particular portion of the saccade map in the
MRF that projects to either the SC or the NRTP.
To utilize the better understood SC saccade map, assume
that “quasi-visual” and “build-up” neurons of the SC provide a
desired displacement signal (⌬E) to the medium lead burst
neurons in the PPRF (Mays and Sparks 1980; Munoz and
Wurtz 1995a,b). Then, MRF RTLLBNs with presumed inhibitory projections to a region of the SC that mediates large,
upward movements would be inactivated. This loss of inhibi-
2279
tion would shift the distribution of activity on the collicular
map toward the medial (upward eye movement) and caudal
portion of the SC, thus generating upward, hypermetric saccades.
An alternative route for shifting model input utilizes projections from the MRF to the NRTP (Crandall and Keller 1985;
Edwards 1975; Edwards and Olmos 1976; Lefevre et al. 1998;
Suzuki et al. 1994). Inactivation of the fastigial nuclei, to
which the NRTP projects, generates a significant degree of
ipsilateral hypermetria and contralateral hypometria (Robinson et al. 1993; van Opstal et al. 1996). Although no changes
in latency were observed after muscimol inactivation of the
fastigial nuclei, the inaccuracies of saccade endpoints and
curvature of saccade trajectories were similar to those found
after caudal MRF inactivation. This suggests that inactivation
of the caudal MRF RSLLBNs could shift activity in a NRTP 3
Cerebellum (fastigial nuclei) 3 PPRF and/or 3 SC loop in a
similar way as in the proposed MRF 3 SC pathway (Lefevre
et al. 1998; Quaia et al. 1999). In either scenario, changes in
model inputs would preserve the relationships between amplitude and duration, as well as amplitude and velocity (Fig. 16A,
new input) (Robinson et al. 1993). Hypermetric saccades with
a normal amplitude/velocity relationship but longer duration
for a given amplitude were noted after one injection (g0217,
Figs. 4 and 10). Longer saccade duration would not be observed after a shift in model input. Moreover, shifts in saccade
inputs could not account for the goal-directed nature of postinjection spontaneous saccades.
Inactivation in the feedback pathway of either model would
also produce saccade hypermetria (Hyper #2). In this schema
we propose that the build-up neurons in the intermediate and
deep layers of the SC in conjunction with MRF RTLLBNS
directly participate in the local feedback pathway (i.e., are in
the feedback loop) (Waitzman et al. 1991, 1996). We hypothesize that via reciprocal inhibitory connections between the
MRF and the SC, a spatial integration of eye velocity could
occur (RI, Fig. 1B). Although the idea that the SC is involved
in the spatial integration of a velocity signal has been suggested
previously (Lefevre and Galiana 1992; Optican 1994), recent
data do not support a role for the SC alone as the resettable
integrator for the displacement model. Muscimol inactivation
of the SC produces curved saccade trajectories and hypometria,
but not saccade hypermetria (Aizawa and Wurtz 1994, 1998;
Quaia et al. 1998). Alternatively, MRF neurons could be a part
of a velocity to position integrator that is not reset at the end of
each saccade (i.e., part of the NI in the Eye Position model,
Fig. 1A). In the latter case, MRF neurons should be sensitive to
initial eye position. The eye velocity command signal (Vc)
from the medium lead burst neurons in the PPRF would be
channeled to MRF neurons, where it would be integrated to
produce eye displacement (i.e., the Eye displacement model) or
eye position (Eye position model) (Benevento et al. 1977;
Buttner-Ennever and Buttner 1988; Edwards 1975; Horn et al.
1994; Scudder et al. 1996). Loss of the resettable integrator or
damage to the velocity to position integrator of Robinson
results in modest lengthening of saccade duration, commensurate reduction in the velocity of postinjection saccades, and
increased saccade amplitude (Fig. 16A, reduced feedback). In
fact, saccade duration was moderately increased after the muscimol injection of g0217, with preservation of the amplitude/
velocity relationship (Figs. 5, A and C, and 10C). Thus effects
2280
WAITZMAN, SILAKOV, DEPALMA-BOWLES, AND AYERS
FIG. 16. Simulation of changes in the
feed-foward or feedback pathways for eye
movement control. A: 2 conditions are
shown. A shift in the input from a small
(12°) to a large (24°) saccade region (Hyper
#1, – – –). Inactivation of the feedback pathway in either the eye position or eye displacement models produces hypermetric saccades (Hyper #2). A reduction of 50% in the
gain (top panel) of the feedback path is illustrated (—). Note that saccade peak velocity (bottom panel) is unchanged, but saccade
duration is increased. This was the case for
injection g0217 (see Fig. 10C). B: inactivation of the feed-foward pathway that normally provides inhibition to the burst neurons in the PPRF is hypothesized. This
would cause an increase in the gain of PPRF
excitatory burst neurons. As a result there is
a marked increase in saccade velocity (bottom panel) without a change in saccade amplitude (top panel). C: a combination of the
above 2 effects produces a marked saccade
hypermetria (left panel ), a moderate increase
in saccade velocity (right panel ) and intermediate effects on saccade duration (i.e., it
remains the same or slightly reduced). These
were the effects observed in injections
c0416, c0521, and c0419.
on saccades following injection g0217 could be explained by a
reduction in the gain of the local feedback pathway of either
model (Hyper #2, in Figs. 1, A and B, and 16A, reduced
feedback, solid line).
However, for three other injections, saccade hypermetria
was associated with normal or shorter saccade duration and
either normal or higher saccade velocity (c0521, c0419, and
c0416). Explanation of increased saccade velocity (over control) necessitated modulation of another model parameter. Increasing the gain of the burst generator causes saccades to be
executed faster while accurately reaching their goal (Fig. 16B,
i.e., the burst generator is in the local feedback loop). This
condition was not observed in our data. In short, we were able
to replicate saccade hypermetria and increased velocity in both
the Eye Position and Eye Displacement models only by simultaneously increasing the gain of the burst generator while
reducing the feedback of current eye position or displacement
(Fig. 16C). Note that the simulated saccade amplitude was
hypermetric and its velocity was higher, but saccade duration
was slightly reduced compared with the amplitude matched
control movement. We hypothesize that a combination of
saccade hypermetria and increased velocity might occur if
muscimol inactivation affected both RTLLBNs (feedback) and
caudal RSLLBNs (feed-foward) in the MRF. Loss of the
RTLLBNs would severely impair the feedback of current eye
position or displacement activity to the SC. Impaired
RSLLBNs output could remove inhibition on NRPo and NRPc
neurons, thus increasing the number of spikes produced by
medium lead burst neurons for a given amplitude saccade.
In summary, reduced feedback would account for the increased amplitude of saccades seen after all injections. However, we must hypothesize an increase in the gain of the
pontine burst generator in the PPRF to account for the increased saccade velocity demonstrated in three other injections.
Mechanisms for the generation of square-wave jerks
Two questions arise regarding the generation of the repetitive macrosaccadic square-wave jerks. First, how do the two
CMRF
AND GOAL-DIRECTED SACCADES
2281
FIG. 17. Simulation of macrosaccadic squarewave jerks. A: a decrease in the gain (50%) of the
local feedback pathway (Hyper #2, Fig. 1) in
either the Eye Displacement or the Eye Position
models results in saccade hypermetria, and then
persistent oscillation of the eyes around the intended final eye position (– – –). A small increment in this gain (60%) in each model allowed the
error to decay over time so that the eyes eventually
reached the goal (—). A large reduction in gain
(25% of normal) resulted in unstable oscillations
(– 䡠 䡠 –). A reduction in feedback gain at the Hyper
#2 location of either model could not simulate
orbital position effects. B: placement of a small
delay (110 ms) in the portion of the feedback
pathway of the Robinson model leading to SJ2
(Hyper #3) also produce macrosaccadic squarewave jerks (—). However, different from the simulation in A, these movements would often reach a
specific orbital position after a small overshoot
(saccade 3). Open loop conditions (delay of 50 s)
produced hypermetric, unstable oscillations (– 䡠 䡠 –,
saccades 5 and 6). – – –, target; 䡠 䡠 䡠, normal, control
eye movements.
models generate these characteristic movements? Second, can
either of the models produce goal-directed movements? The
answer to the first question turns out to be straightforward.
Generation of saccade hypermetria from a reduction in the gain
of feedback in each model leads to an unresolved motor error
(em). If the model is stable, then oscillations around the final
position should slowly die away and the eye will come to rest
at the desired, eccentric location (⌬E or ed). We observed this
behavior for both models with the proper selection of feedback
gain (i.e., ⬃60% of normal gain for both models Fig. 17A, 0.6
gain). When feedback was reduced by ⬃50% in the eye dis-
placement model, the eye continued to oscillate around the
final desired eye position without any decay (Fig. 17A, 0.5
gain). With further reductions in feedback gain, both models
could be made very underdamped, in which case the oscillations increased, completely destabilizing the system (Fig. 17A,
0.25 gain).
The answer to the second question (goal-directed saccades)
was addressed by reducing feedback (i.e., at the Hyper #2
location) by 50% and asking each model to execute saccades
from different initial positions to the same final position. Neither model could generate goal-directed saccades in this
2282
WAITZMAN, SILAKOV, DEPALMA-BOWLES, AND AYERS
scheme (data not shown). Rather, each produced saccades that
oscillated around the final position without actually reaching
the final position. With appropriate choices of feedback gain,
the oscillations for each model would eventually die away, and
the final position would be achieved (similar to the 0.6 gain
shown in Fig. 17A), but these are not goal-directed movements.
On the other hand, goal-directed saccades could be produced in
some circumstances by careful manipulation of the Eye Position model (Fig. 1A). By placement of a moderate delay (110
ms) in the second limb of the feedback loop directed to
summing junction 2 (SJ2, Fig. 1B, Hyper #3, del) macrosaccadic, hypermetric, square-wave jerks could be generated (Fig.
17B, Hyper #3, solid line). Feedback gain in the portion of the
feedback loop reaching SJ3 was left intact (i.e., it was 1.0). The
interpretation of this result was that the local feedback loop
(SJ3) would control execution of the proper initial saccade to
the desired orbital position (Fig. 17B, 1). In the meantime,
without proper feedback of current eye position to SJ2, retinal
error (Rerr) would fall to zero without a commensurate rise in
E⬘. This would result in a sudden drop in ed as if a desired eye
position of zero was needed, and a saccade in the opposite
direction would be generated (Fig. 17B, 2). However, 110 ms
after this “return” saccade ended, the original current eye
position signal (E⬘) would suddenly arrive at SJ2. This new
“current eye position” would be summed with the residual
retinal error (Rerr) remaining after the return saccade. Again a
new desired orbital position (ed) would be produced, initiating
a third movement (after a normal refractory period) of similar
amplitude and direction (⫹ a small error) toward the original
goal (Fig. 17B, 3). The small “overshoot” seen at the end of
this movement (Fig. 17B, 4) is the result of the previous retinal
error, which disappears quickly as the new residual retinal
error goes to zero. Note the difference between this tracing and
that obtained with changes at Hyper #2 (Fig. 17A). Oscillations
around the goal (with the exception of the overshoot) were
abolished. These movements, albeit one dimensional in the
model, are very similar to the repetitive goal-directed, macrosaccadic square-wave jerks we observed after inactivation of
the caudal MRF. In open loop conditions (i.e., via placement of
a long delay in the feedback loop), repetitive saccades were
generated, but they did not land on the orbital position goal and
became progressively larger in amplitude (Fig. 17B, dasheddotted line, 5 and 6). Again similar, but not exactly like the
square-wave jerks observed experimentally. In conclusion,
both models can generate hypermetric, macrosaccadic squarewave jerks, but only the Robinson model could be adapted to
generate repetitive goal-directed movements. Clearly new
models of higher dimensionality and increased sophistication
are needed to more fully characterize the oculomotor behavior
found following muscimol inactivation of the caudal MRF.
The authors thank Dr. Douglas Oliver for the use of the computing and
laboratory facilities, as well as for expertise with fluorescent bead injections,
neuroanatomy, and histology. The comments of Drs. Wolfson and Duckrow
were instrumental in editing an early version of this manuscript. Numerous
discussions with Drs. Robert Wurtz, Lance Optican, and postdoctoral members
of the Laboratory of Sensorimotor Research, National Eye Institute (NEI),
have been invaluable in framing the idea of damage to the resettable integrator.
The comments of three anonymous reviewers were critical for focusing and
simplifying the manuscript.
This work was supported by NEI Grant EY-09481 and a Research Advisory
Group grant from the Office of Medical Research, Department of Veterans
Affairs.
Address for reprint requests: D. M. Waitzman, Dept. of Neurology, MC
3974, The University of Connecticut Health Center, 263 Farmington Ave.,
Farmington, CT 06030.
Received 9 August 1999; accepted in final form 8 October 1999.
REFERENCES
AIZAWA, H. AND WURTZ, R. H. Control of trajectory of saccadic eye movements by monkey superior colliculus. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 20: 141, 1994.
AIZAWA, H. AND WURTZ, R. H. Reversible inactivation of monkey superior
colliculus. I. Curvature of saccade trajectory. J. Neurophysiol. 79: 2082–
2096, 1998.
ANDREWS, P. R. AND JOHNSTON, G.A.R. GABA agonists and antagonists.
Biochem. Pharmacol. 28: 2697–2701, 1979.
BENDER, M. B. AND SHANZER, S. Oculomotor pathways defined by electrical
stimulation and lesion in the brain stem of monkey. In: The Oculomotor
System, edited by M. B. Bender. New York: Harper and Row, 1964, p.
81–140.
BENEVENTO, L. A., REZAK, M., AND SANTOS-ANDERSON, R. An autoradiographic study of the projections of the pretectum in the rhesus monkey
(Macaca mulatta): evidence for sensorimotor links to the thalamus and
oculomotor nuclei. Brain Res. 127: 197–218, 1977.
BJORKLAND, M. AND BOIVIE, J. An anatomical study of the projections from the
dorsal column nuclei to the midbrain in the cat. Anat. Embriol. (Berl.) 170:
29 – 43, 1984.
BRODAL, P. The projection from the nucleus reticularis tegmenti pontis to the
cerebellum in the rhesus monkey. Exp. Brain Res. 38: 29 –36, 1980.
BUTTNER-ENNEVER, J. A. AND BUTTNER, U. The reticular formation. In: Neuroanatomy of the Oculomotor System. Reviews of Oculomotor Research,
edited by J. A. Buttner-Ennever. New York: Elsevier, 1988, vol. II, p.
273–333.
CANNON, S. C. AND ROBINSON, D. A. Loss of neural integrator of the oculomotor system from brain stem lesions in the monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 57:
1383–1409, 1987.
CASTIGLIONI, A. J., GALLWAY, M. C., AND COULTER, J. D. Spinal projections
from midbrain in monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 178: 329 –346, 1978.
COHEN, B., MATSUO, V., FRADIN, J., AND RAPHAN, T. Horizontal saccades
induced by stimulation of the central mesencephalic reticular formation.
Exp. Brain Res. 57: 605– 616, 1985.
COHEN, B., MATSUO, V., RAPHAN, T., WAITZMAN, D. M., AND FRADIN, J.
Horizontal saccades induced by stimulation of the mesencephalic reticular
formation (MRF). In: Physiological and Pathological Aspects of Eye Movements, edited by A. Roucoux and M. Crommelinck. The Hague: D. W. Junk,
1982, p. 325–335.
COHEN, B., REISINE, H., YOKOTA, J., AND RAPHAN, T. The nucleus of the optic
tract. Its function in gaze stabilization and control of the visual-vestibular
interaction. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 656: 277–296, 1992.
COHEN, B., WAITZMAN, D. M., BUTTNER-ENNEVER, J. A., AND MATSUO, V.
Horizontal saccades and the central mesencephalic reticular formation. In:
Progress in Brain Research. The Oculomotor and Skeletalmotor Systems:
Differences and Similarities, edited by H.-J. Freund, U. Buttner, B. Cohen,
and J. Noth. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1986, vol. 64, p. 243–256.
COWIE, R. J. AND ROBINSON, D. L. Subcortical contribution to head movements
in macaques. I. Contrasting effects of electrical stimulation of a medial
pontomedullary region and the superior colliculus. J. Neurophysiol. 72:
2648 –2664, 1994.
CRANDALL, W. F. AND KELLER, E. L. Visual and oculomotor signals in nucleus
reticularis tegmenti pontis in alert monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 54: 1326 –1345,
1985.
CRAWFORD, J. D. The oculomotor neural integrator uses a behavior-related
coordinate system. J. Neurosci. 14: 6911– 6923, 1994.
CRAWFORD, J. D., CADERA, W., AND VILIS, T. Generation of torsional and
vertical eye position signals by the interstitial nucleus of Cajal. Science 252:
1551–1553, 1991.
CRAWFORD, J. D. AND VILIS, T. Symmetry of oculomotor burst neuron coordinates about Listing’s plane. J. Neurophysiol. 68: 432– 448, 1992.
CRAWFORD, J. D. AND VILIS, T. Modularity and parallel processing in the
oculomotor integrator. Exp. Brain Res. 96: 443– 456, 1993.
CRIST, C. F., YAMASAKI, D.S.G., KOMATSU, H., AND WURTZ, R. H. A grid
system and a microsyringe for single cell recording. J. Neurosci. Methods
26: 117–122, 1988.
DIAS, E. C. AND SEGRAVES, M. A. A pressure system for microinjection of
substances into the brain of awake monkeys. J. Neurosci. Methods 72:
43– 47, 1997.
CMRF
AND GOAL-DIRECTED SACCADES
EDELMAN, J. A. AND KELLER, E. L. Activity of visuomotor burst neurons in the
superior colliculus accompanying express saccades. J. Neurophysiol. 76:
908 –926, 1996.
EDWARDS, S. B. Autoradiographic studies of the projections of the midbrain
reticular formation: descending projections of nucleus cuneiformis. J. Comp.
Neurol. 161: 341–358, 1975.
EDWARDS, S. B. AND DE OLMOS, J. S. Autoradiographic studies of the projections of the midbrain reticular formation: ascending projections of nucleus
cuneiformis. J. Comp. Neurol. 165: 417– 432, 1976.
FUCHS, A. F. Saccadic and smooth pursuit eye movements in the monkey.
J. Physiol. (Lond.) 191: 609 – 631, 1967.
FUKUSHIMA, K. The interstitial nucleus of Cajal and its role in the control of
movements of head and eyes. Prog. Neurobiol. 29: 107–192, 1987.
FUKUSHIMA, K., FUKUSHIMA, J., AND TERASHIMA, T. The pathways responsible
for the characteristic head posture produced by lesions of the interstitial
nucleus of Cajal. Exp. Brain Res. 68: 88 –102, 1987.
GERRITS, N. M. AND VOOGD, J. The nucleus reticularis tegmenti pontis and the
adjacent rostral paramedian pontine reticular formation: differential projections to the cerebellum and the caudal brainstem. Exp. Brain Res. 62:
29 – 45, 1986.
GOOSSENS, H. H. AND VAN OPSTAL, A. J. Human eye-head coordination in two
dimensions under different sensorimotor conditions. Exp. Brain Res. 114:
542–560, 1997.
HANDEL, A. AND GLIMCHER, P. W. Reponse properties of saccade-related burst
neurons in the central mesencephalic reticular formation. J. Neurophysiol.
78: 2164 –2175, 1997.
HARTING, J. K. Descending pathways from the superior colliculus: an autoradiographic analysis in the rhesus monkey (Maccaca mulatta). J. Comp.
Neurol. 173: 583– 612, 1978.
HEPP, K. AND HENN, V. Spatio-temporal recoding in the generation of rapid eye
movements. In: Physiological and Pathological Aspects of Eye Movements,
edited by A. Roucoux and M. Crommelinck. The Hague: W. Junk, 1982, p.
319 –323.
HEPP, K. AND HENN, V. Spatio-temporal recoding of rapid eye movement
signals in the monkey paramedian pontine reticular formation (PPRF). Exp.
Brain Res. 52: 105–120, 1983.
HIKOSAKA, O. AND WURTZ, R. H. Modification of saccadic eye movements by
GABA-related substances. I. Effect of muscimol and bicuculline in monkey
superior colliculus. J. Neurophysiol. 53: 266 –291, 1985a.
HIKOSAKA, O. AND WURTZ, R. H. Modification of saccadic eye movements by
GABA-related substances. II. Effects of muscimol in monkey substantia
nigra pars reticulata. J. Neurophysiol. 53: 292–308, 1985b.
HORN, A., BUTTNER-ENNEVER, J. A., WAHLE, P., AND REICHENBERGER, I.
Neurotransmitter profile of saccadic omnipause neurons in nucleus raphe
interpositus. J. Neurosci. 14: 2032–2046, 1994.
HUERTA, M. F. AND HARTING, J. K. The mammalian superior colliculus: studies
of its morphology and connections. In: Comparative Neurology of the Optic
Tectum, edited by H. Vanegas. New York: Plenum, 1984, p. 687–773.
HUERTA, M. F. AND KAAS, J. H. Supplementary eye field as defined by
intracortical microstimulation: connections in macaques. J. Comp. Neurol.
293: 299 –330, 1990.
JUDGE, S. J., RICHMOND, B. J., AND CHU, F. C. Implantation of magnetic search
coils for measurement of eye position: an improved method. Vision Res. 20:
535–538, 1980.
JURGENS, R., BECKER, W., AND KORNHUBER, H. H. Natural and drug-induced
variations of velocity and duration of human saccadic eye movements:
evidence for a control of the neural pulse generator by local feedback. Biol.
Cybern. 39: 87–96, 1981.
KING, W. M., LISBERGER, S. G., AND FUCHS, A. F. Oblique saccadic eye
movements of primates. J. Neurophysiol. 56: 769 –784, 1986.
KOKKOROYANNIS, T., SCUDDER, C. A., BALABAN, C. D., HIGHSTEIN, S. M., AND
MOSCHOVAKIS, A. K. Anatomy and physiology of the primate interstitial
nucleus of Cajal I. Efferent projections. J. Neurophysiol. 75: 725–739, 1996.
KOMATSUZAKI, A., ALPERT, J., HARRIS, H. E., AND COHEN, B. Effects of
mesencephalic reticular formation lesions on optokinetic nystagmus. Exp.
Neurol. 34: 522–534, 1972.
KROGSGAARD-LARSEN, P., HANORE, T., AND TJYSSEN, K. GABA receptor
agonists: design and structure activity studies. In: GABA-Neurotransmitters,
edited by P. Krogsgaard-Larsen, J. Scheel-Kruger, and H. Kofod. Copenhagen: Munksgaard, 1979, p. 201–216.
KUNZLE, H. AND AKERT, K. Efferent connections of cortical area 8 (frontal eye
field) in Macaca fascicularis. A reinvestigation using the autoradiographic
technique. J. Comp. Neurol. 173: 147–164, 1977.
2283
LEE, C., ROHRER, W. H., AND SPARKS, D. L. Population coding of saccadic eye
movements by neurons in the superior colliculus. Nature 332: 357–360,
1988.
LEFEVRE, P. AND GALIANA, H. L. Dynamic feedback to the superior colliculus
in a neural network model of the gaze control system. Neural Networks 5:
871– 890, 1992.
LEFEVRE, P., QUAIA, C., AND OPTICAN, L. Distributed model of control of
saccades by superior colliculus and cerebellum. Neural Networks 11: 1175–
1190, 1998.
LEICHNETZ, G. R. AND GOLDBERG, M. E. Higher centers concerned with eye
movement and visual attention: cerebral cortex and thalamus. In: Neuroanatomy of the Oculomotor System, edited by J. A. Buttner-Ennever. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1988, p. 365– 429.
MAYS, L. E. AND SPARKS, D. L. Dissociation of visual and saccade-related
responses in superior colliculus neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 43: 207–232,
1980.
MOSCHOVAKIS, A. K., KARABELAS, A. B., AND HIGHSTEIN, S. M. Structurefunction relationships in the primate superior colliculus. II. Morphological
identity of presaccadic neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 60: 263–302, 1988.
MOSCHOVAKIS, A. K., SCUDDER, C. A., AND HIGHSTEIN, S. M. The microscopic
anatomy and physiology of the mammalian saccadic system. Prog. Neurobiol. 50: 133–254, 1996.
MUNOZ, D. P. AND WURTZ, R. H. Role of the rostral superior colliculus in
active visual fixation and execution of express saccades. J. Neurophysiol.
67: 1000 –1002, 1992.
MUNOZ, D. P. AND WURTZ, R. H. Fixation cells in monkey superior colliculus
II. Reversible activation and deactivation. J. Neurophysiol. 70: 576 –589,
1993.
MUNOZ, D. P. AND WURTZ, R. H. Saccade-related activity in monkey superior
colliculus. I. Characteristics of burst and buildup cells. J. Neurophysiol. 73:
2313–2333, 1995a.
MUNOZ, D. P. AND WURTZ, R. H. Saccade-related activity in monkey superior
colliculus. II. Spread of activity during saccades. J. Neurophysiol. 73:
2334 –2348, 1995b.
NAGAI, T., MCGEER, P. L., AND MCGEER, E. G. Distribution of GABA-Tintensive neurons in the rat forebrain and midbrain. J. Comp. Neurol. 218:
220 –238, 1983.
OLSON, C. R. AND GETTNER, S. N. Object-centered direction selectivity in the
macaque supplementary eye field. Science 269: 985–988, 1995.
OLSZEWSKI, J. AND BAXTER, D. Cytoarchitecture of the Human Brain Stem.
Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott, 1954.
OPTICAN, L. M. Control of saccade trajectory by the superior colliculus. In:
Contemporary Oculomotor and Vestibular Research: A Tribute to David A.
Robinson, edited by A. Fuchs, T. Brandt, U. Buttner, and D. S. Zee. New
York: Georg Thieme Verlag, 1994, p. 98 –105.
PECHURA, C. M. AND LIU, R. P. Spinal motoneurons which project to the
periaqueductal grey and the medullary reticular formation via axon collaterals: a double-label fluorescence study in the rat. Brain Res. 374: 357–361,
1986.
QUAIA, C., AIZAWA, H., OPTICAN, L. M., AND WURTZ, R. H. Reversible
inactivation of monkey superior colliculus. II. Maps of saccadic deficits.
J. Neurophysiol. 79: 2097–2110, 1998.
QUAIA, C., LEFEVRE, P., AND OPTICAN, L. Model of the control of saccades by
superior colliculus and cerebellum. J. Neurophysiol. 82: 999 –1018, 1999.
RITCHIE, J. M. A pharmacological approach to the structure of sodium channels
in myelinated axons. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2: 341–362, 1979.
ROBINSON, D. A. Oculomotor control signals. In: Basic Mechanisms of Ocular
Motility and Their Clinical Implications, edited by P. Bach-y-Rita and G.
Lennerstrand. Oxford, UK: Pergamon, 1975, p. 337–375.
ROBINSON, F. R., PHILLIPS, J. O., AND FUCHS, A. F. Coordination of gaze shifts
in primates: brainstem inputs to neck and extraocular motoneuron pools.
J. Comp. Neurol. 346: 43– 62, 1994.
ROBINSON, F. R., STRAUBE, A., AND FUCHS, A. F. Role of caudal fastigial
nucleus in saccade generation. II. Effects of muscimol inactivation. J. Neurophysiol. 70: 1741–1758, 1993.
RUSSO, G. S. AND BRUCE, C. J. Effect of eye position within the orbit on
electrically elicited saccadic eye movements: a comparison of the macaque
monkey’s frontal and supplementary eye fields. J. Neurophysiol. 69: 800 –
818, 1993.
SCHALL, J. D. Neuronal activity related to visually guided saccades in the
frontal eye fields of rhesus monkeys: comparison with supplementary eye
fields. J. Neurophysiol. 66: 559 –579, 1991.
2284
WAITZMAN, SILAKOV, DEPALMA-BOWLES, AND AYERS
SCUDDER, C. A., MOSCHOVAKIS, A. K., KARABELAS, A. B., AND HIGHSTEIN,
S. M. Anatomy and physiology of saccadic long-lead burst neurons recorded
in the alert squirrel monkey. I. Descending projections from the mesencephalon. J. Neurophysiol. 76: 332–352, 1996.
SEGRAVES, M. A. AND GOLDBERG, M. E. Properties of eye and head movements
evoked by electrical stimulation of the monkey superior colliculus. In: The
Head-Neck Sensory-Motor System, edited by A. Berthoz, W. Graf, and P. P.
Vidal. New York: Oxford, 1991, p. 292–295.
SHOOK, B. L., SCHLAG-REY, M., AND SCHLAG, J. Primate supplementary eye
field. I. Comparative aspects of mesencephalic and pontine connections.
J. Comp. Neurol. 301: 618 – 642, 1990.
SILAKOV, V. L. AND WAITZMAN, D. M. Changes of remembered saccades and
fixation during temporary inactivation of the mesencephalic reticular formation (MRF). Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 22: 665, 1996.
SILAKOV, V. L., WAITZMAN, D. M., AND DEPALMA, S. R. Combined head and
eye movements evoked by microstimulation of the primate mesencephalic
reticular formation (MRF). Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 25: 1651, 1999.
SILAKOV, V. L., WAITZMAN, D. M., AND TINTURIN, S. J. Effects of mesencephalic reticular formation (MRF) electrical microstimulation upon eye
movements in primates. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 21: 1915, 1995.
SPARKS, D. L. Translation of sensory signals into commands for control of
saccadic eye movements: role of primate superior colliculus. Physiol. Rev.
66: 118 –171, 1986.
SPARKS, D. L. AND MAYS, L. E. Signal transformations required for the generation
of saccadic eye movements. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 13: 309 –336, 1990.
SUZUKI, D. A., YAMADA, T., BETELAK, K., AND YEE, R. D. Ocular motor
functions of monkey nucleus reticularis tegmenti pontis. In: Contemporary
Oculomotor and Vestibular Research: A Tribute to David A. Robinson,
edited by A. Fuchs, T. Brandt, U. Buttner, and D. S. Zee. New York: Georg
Thieme Verlag, 1994, p. 244 –246.
SZENTAGOTHAI, J. Die zentrale Innervation der Augenbewegungen. Arch. Psychiat. Nervenk. 116: 721–760, 1943.
TEHOVNIK, E. J., LEE, K., AND SCHILLER, P. H. Stimulation-evoked saccades
from the dorsomedial frontal cortex of the rhesus monkey following lesions
of the frontal eye fields and superior colliculus. Exp. Brain Res. 98: 179 –
190, 1994.
THIELERT, C.-D. AND THEIR, P. Patterns of projections from the pontine nuclei
and the nucleus reticularis tegmenti pontis to the posterior vermis in the
rhesus monkey: a study using retrograde tracers. J. Comp. Neurol. 337:
113–126, 1993.
VAN OPSTAL, J., HEPP, K., SUZUKI, Y., AND HENN, V. Role of monkey nucleus
reticularis tegmenti pontis in the stabilization of Listing’s plane. J. Neurosci.
16: 7284 –7296, 1996.
WAITZMAN, D. M. Burst Neurons in the Mesencephalic Reticular Formation
(MRF) of the Rhesus Monkey Associated With Saccadic Eye Movements
(PhD thesis). New York: City University of New York, 1982.
WAITZMAN, D. M. Saccade-related burst cells of the central mesencephalic
reticular formation (cMRF) associated with dynamic motor error. Soc.
Neurosci. Abstr. 18: 700, 1992.
WAITZMAN, D. M., MA, T. P., OPTICAN, L. M., AND WURTZ, R. H. Superior
colliculus neurons mediate dynamic characteristics of saccades. J. Neurophysiol. 66: 1716 –1737, 1991.
WAITZMAN, D. M. AND SILAKOV, V. L. Effects of reversible lesions of the
central mesencephalic reticular formation (cMRF) on primate saccadic eye
movements. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 20: 1399, 1994.
WAITZMAN, D. M., SILAKOV, V. L., AND COHEN, B. Central mesencephalic
reticular formation (cMRF) neurons discharging before and during eye
movements. J. Neurophysiol. 75: 1546 –1572, 1996.
WAITZMAN, D. M., SILAKOV, V. L., AND DEPALMA, S. R. Paralysis of vertical
gaze following unilateral inactivation of the mesencephalic reticular formation (MRF). Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 23: 2368, 1997.
WAITZMAN, D. M., SILAKOV, V. L., AND DEPALMA, S. R. Goal directed
movements following electrical microstimulation of the mesencephalic reticular formation (MRF) in primates. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 24: 145, 1998.
WAITZMAN, D. M., SILAKOV, V. L., DEPALMA, S. R., AND AYERS, A. S. Effects
of reversible inactivation of the primate mesencephalic reticular formation.
II. Hypometric vertical saccades. J. Neurophysiol. 83: 2285–2299, 2000.