* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Heightened Interference on Implicit, but Not Explicit, Tests of
Visual selective attention in dementia wikipedia , lookup
Adaptive memory wikipedia , lookup
Priming (psychology) wikipedia , lookup
Dual process theory wikipedia , lookup
Sex differences in cognition wikipedia , lookup
Negative priming wikipedia , lookup
Mental chronometry wikipedia , lookup
Holonomic brain theory wikipedia , lookup
Collective memory wikipedia , lookup
Memory and aging wikipedia , lookup
Childhood memory wikipedia , lookup
Prenatal memory wikipedia , lookup
Eyewitness memory (child testimony) wikipedia , lookup
Source amnesia wikipedia , lookup
BRAIN AND COGNITION ARTICLE NO. 30, 44–58 (1996) 0004 Heightened Interference on Implicit, but Not Explicit, Tests of Negative Transfer: Evidence from Patients with Unilateral Temporal Lobe Lesions and Normal Old People GORDON WINOCUR Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Centre for Geriatric Care and Trent University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada MORRIS MOSCOVITCH Erindale College, University of Toronto, Ontario, Canada and Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Centre for Geriatric Care, Toronto, Ontario, Canada AND JOSEPH BRUNI Department of Neurology, Wellesley Hospital, Toronto, Ontario, Canada The present research investigated alternative explanations of heightened interference in AB-AC learning in individuals with memory loss related to medial temporal lobe dysfunction. In Experiment 1, patients with left or right temporal lobectomy and control subjects were administered the standard AB-AC test. Relative to the other groups, left temporal patients exhibited significant negative transfer that was characterized by large numbers of response intrusion errors. In Experiment 2, groups of community-dwelling old and young adults were administered the standard test and an implicit version in which, during AC testing, subjects were instructed to provide the first word that comes to mind in response to stimulus words. There were no differences between groups on either version. Of particular interest was that both groups made significantly more intrusion errors on the implicit test and did not differ on this measure. It was concluded that exaggerated interference in AB-AC learning, as reflected by response intrusion errors, is related to the use of This research was supported by MRC Grant (MA-6694) to G.W. and M.M. and by a Research Associateship to MM from the Ontario Mental Health Foundation. We thank Lorna Morris, Robin Green, Lee Smith, and Mary McCauley for their assistance. Address reprint requests to Gordon Winocur, Baycare Centre for Geriatric Care, the Rotman Research Institute, 3560 Bathurst Street, Toronto M6A 2E1, Canada. 44 0278-2626/96 $18.00 Copyright 1996 by Academic Press, Inc. All rights of reproduction in any form reserved. HEIGHTENED INTERFERENCE 45 implicit memory processes rather than a failure of inhibitory mechanisms. Memoryimpaired individuals, who have a selective loss of explicit memory, are vulnerable on this task because they rely excessively on implicit memory processes. 1996 Academic Press, Inc. The present research addresses the issue of interference effects in memory from both a structural and functional perspective. Over a period of years, we conducted several studies on interference in brain-damaged and aging populations using a variation of the A-B, A-C negative transfer paradigm (Winocur, 1982; Winocur & Weiskrantz, 1976; Winocur & Kinsbourne, 1978; Winocur & Moscovitch 1983). In this paradigm, subjects are presented with a list of semantically related pairs of words (e.g. soldier–battle) for acquisition during the initial A-B phase of the experiment. After a 20- to 30-min filled interval, they are taught a second A-C list in which a new, equally related response item, must be associated with the original stimulus word (e.g. soldier–army). Because memory-disabled individuals can learn the A-B list, this test provides an opportunity to assess interference effects in various populations against a background of comparable baseline performance. Our previous results showed that neurological patients with memory disorders and institutionalized old people were severely impaired in List 2 learning and, in the process, exhibited large numbers of intrusion errors from List 1. These results provide evidence that susceptibility to interference is a dominant feature of memory disorders in which there is some known damage or presumed impairment of particular brain regions. At the functional level, there are two possible interpretations of the interference effects, one relating to a loss of inhibition and the other to a loss of memory. According to the inhibition hypothesis, our impaired subjects were unable to suppress previously correct, but currently inappropriate, responses. The memory hypothesis is based on evidence that conscious recollection is impaired but non-conscious implicit memory processes are preserved in amnesia (Cohen & Squire, 1980; Moscovitch, Vriezen, & Goshen-Gottstein, 1993) and normal aging (Moscovitch, 1982; Howard, 1991). Because the stimulus and response items in our AB-AC paradigm are related, the paradigm can be considered as an implicit conceptual repetition priming test in which the stimulus items (A) can elicit the response items (B,C). As a result, performance by our elderly and amnesic subjects on the AB-AC task may have been driven more by preserved non-conscious implicit processes than by explicit memory processes, which were impaired (Mayes, Pickering, & Fairburn, 1987). By this view, it follows that heightened interference in amnesia and aging is linked to the pattern of lost and spared memory functions, rather than to deficits in inhibition. The brain region which has been most often implicated in both memory function and the control of interference is the medial temporal lobe which 46 WINOCUR, MOSCOVITCH, AND BRUNI includes the hippocampus. Severely amnesic patients, with lesions involving the medial temporal lobe, were impaired on A-B, A-C learning (Winocur & Weiskrantz, 1976) and on other tests with a strong interference component (Warrington and Weiskrantz, 1974). Similar experiments conducted on animals with brain-lesions provide converging evidence that the hippocampal formation is a critical structure in mediating performance on memory and high interference tests (Douglas, 1967; Jarrard, 1975; Kimble, 1968; Winocur, 1979). In this paper we report two experiments that address issues related to brain mechanisms and cognitive processes that control interference in negative transfer. In Experiment 1, we tested patients with unilateral temporal lobectomy to determine whether damage restricted to the medial temporal lobe is sufficient to produce exaggerated interference effects, comparable to those found in amnesic patients. Experiment 2 was concerned with the functional question of whether these interference effects are produced by impaired inhibition or are related to increased reliance on implicit memory processes. To answer this question, we devised an implicit version of the AB-AC test and administered it to groups of normal young and old people. Greater interference on the implicit than on the explicit version of the test in all our subjects would support the memory hypothesis. EXPERIMENT 1 In Experiment 1, we evaluate the importance of the medial temporal lobe in controlling interference by testing a group of patients with damage to this region on A-B, A-C learning. All the patients had unilateral surgical excision of the temporal lobe for control of epilepsy. Because A-B, A-C learning is a verbal test, it was predicted that only patients with left temporal lobectomy would be impaired. Method Subjects. Eight patients with left temporal lobectomy and 12 with right temporal lobectomy participated in the study. The characteristics of the patients are presented in Table 1. Only three patients were not strongly right-handed, as determined by Bryden’s (1977) questionnaire (2 right-temporal, 1 left-temporal), but all had speech represented in the left hemisphere as TABLE 1 Age of Temporal Lobe Patients and Their Scores on WAIS-R and WMS Tests Group n Age V1Q P1Q FS MQ Right temporal Left temporal 12 8 29.7 (18–54) 30.9 (26–37) 93.3 94.0 89.8 98.9 92.2 94.6 109.6 90.0 Note. V1Q 5 Verbal IQ, P1Q 5 Performance IQ, FS 5 Full Scale IQ, MQ 5 Memory Quotient. HEIGHTENED INTERFERENCE 47 determined by the sodium amytal test. Each patient underwent an en bloc resection of the temporal lobe that included large removal of the pes, parahippocampal gyrus, and the hippocampus. Depending on the patient, the resection extended 5.0 to 8.5 cm along the Sylvian fissure from the anterior tip. For one patient in the right-temporal group, the excision extended into the right inferior frontal gyrus. She was included in the study because her performance resembled that of the other right-temporal patients. The WAIS-R was administered to all patients. In five cases, absolute scores were not available but only the category (e.g., low normal). In such instances, we chose the middle of the category range as the score. Testing in all patients was conducted between 2 months and 5 years postsurgery. The control group consisted of nine normal young adults recruited from a subject pool of non-university, community volunteers. Their average age of 29.6 years (range 20–40) was not significantly different from that of the temporal lobectomy patients. Materials. Two lists of 12 paired–associate words were prepared with the same stimulus words but different response words. The lists were the same as those used in previous studies involving normal old people (e.g., Winocur & Moscovitch, 1983) and brain-damaged populations (e.g., Winocur & Weiskrantz, 1976). Each word was printed in large black letters on a blank 3 3 5″ index card. The two lists consisted of 12 triads of high frequency words drawn from the A or AA categories of the Thorndike-Lorge (1944) word counts. Each pair of words was closely linked on the basis of their common semantic characteristics (e.g., List 1: army– soldier: List 2: army–battle). Three versions of each list were prepared with the pairs arranged in different sequences to discourage rote order memorization. Procedure. The procedure was similar to that followed by Winocur and Moscovitch (1983). Subjects were given four study trials of List 1 with instructions to read aloud each pair and try to associate the words. They were told that, after four presentations of the list, they will be given the first word of each pair and asked to provide the second word. The pairs were presented at the rate of one pair every 2 sec, with 60 sec elapsing between versions. These periods were filled with conversation unrelated to the test. Recall of List 1 response words was tested 60 sec after the fourth presentation. For the recall test, each stimulus word (A) was presented for 10 sec, or until the person responded, after which the correct response (B) was always provided. Following List 1 recall, the subject was offered coffee and engaged in distracting conversation for approximately 20 min. The 12 pairs of List 2 words were presented once only followed by a series of trials in which subjects were to provide the new response words for the same stimuli as in List 1. No advance information was provided about the types of words or their relationship to List 1. During the study trial, the pairs were shown at a 2-sec rate, but in the test trials subjects had 10 sec to respond to each stimulus word (A), after which the correct response (C) was always provided. Testing was terminated when an errorless list trial was achieved or when four trials had been administered. Results On recall of list 1, the left temporal group made an average of 1.7 errors, as compared to 0.3 errors by the right temporal group, and 0.2 errors by the control group. It should be noted that of the total of 13 errors made by the left temporal group, five were committed by one patient. The group difference, although small in absolute terms, was statistically significant, F(2, 26) 5 5.96, p , .01. The results for List 2 performance in Experiment 1 are presented in Table 2. Patients with left temporal lobectomy, however, were markedly impaired on all aspects of negative transfer on List 2, whereas the performance of 48 WINOCUR, MOSCOVITCH, AND BRUNI TABLE 2 List 2 Performance as Measured by Mean Trials to Criterion and Mean Total Errors and Errors Categorized as Omission, Response Intrusion from List 1, and Extra-List Intrusion in Normal Control Subjects and in Patients with Unilateral Temporal-Lobe Lesions. Group n Trials to criterion Total errors Omission Response intrusion Extra-list intrusion Control Right temporal Left temporal 9 12 8 1.9 1.8 3.9 1.3 1.5 11.8 0.4 0.8 3.4 0.6 0.4 7.0 0.3 0.3 1.4 patients with right temporal lobectomy was indistinguishable from that of normal control subjects. This impression was confirmed by separate one factor analyses of variance on the various measures of List 2 learning. Significant differences were obtained in the total number of errors F(2, 26) 5 43.97, p , .0001; MS 5 308.65, and in trials to criterion F(2, 26) 5 22.85, p , .0001; MS 5 11.80. Of the eight left temporal patients, five failed to reach criterion and the remainder required the maximum four trials. All the right temporal lobectomy patients and normal control subjects reached criterion with only one right temporal patient requiring the allotted four trials. The errors were categorized into omission errors, response intrusions from List 1, and extra-list intrusions. Significant differences were found in omission errors, F(2, 26) 5 3.75, p , .04; MS 5 22.24; response intrusions, F(2, 26) 5 23.05, p 5 .0001; MS 5 123.33; and extra-list intrusions, F(2, 26) 5 20.98, p , .0001; MS 5 59.04. It is noteworthy that the extra-list intrusion errors were semantically related to the stimulus word. On all measures, the left temporal patients performed worse than the other two groups (Scheffe F test, p , .01 or greater), which did not differ significantly from each other. The number and type of errors made by the left temporal group during List 2 learning were unrelated to the group’s performance on List 1. If anything, there was a slight tendency for the five subjects who made zero or one error on List 1 to make more response intrusion and extra-list intrusion errors than the other three subjects. Discussion The present results clearly indicate that lesions of the left medial temporal cortex produced severe deficits on all measures of negative transfer. Right temporal lobe lesions produced no effect, indicating a striking functional asymmetry between the left and right temporal lobes, as predicted, given the verbal nature of our AB-AC task. This result is consistent with reports of similar asymmetries following temporal lobectomy on a variety of other verbal memory tests (Milner, 1974). The present findings indicate that lesions restricted to the left medial temporal lobe are sufficient to produce the type HEIGHTENED INTERFERENCE 49 of heightened interference previously observed in amnesic patients and institutionalized old people. It is important to emphasize that the results do not rule out the possibility that other brain regions, notably the frontal lobes, may also be implicated in controlling interference (Luria, 1971; Shimamura, in press). Having established that left medial temporal lobe lesions produce increased interference, we turn to a consideration of the processes that may have been disrupted. It was suggested in the Introduction that performance by normal subjects on the AB-AC test reflects a form of conceptual repetition priming in addition to conscious recollection of the learned associations. In standard tests of conceptual priming, subjects study a list of items which they later reproduce in response to a semantically related cue without conscious reference to the study episode. In our task, having studied the associated pairs in List 1, subjects then responded with a word that was conceptually related to the stimulus item at test. Unlike normal subjects, patients with impaired memory may have relied primarily on implicit processes in responding to the stimulus at test. When there were no competing responses, as in List 1 learning, conceptual repetition priming may have been sufficient to compensate for explicit memory loss and lead to normal performance in our patients. When there were strong competing responses, as in List 2 learning, reliance on conceptual priming was detrimental to performance. Patients often responded with the associated item that had been primed most strongly, namely, the List 1 item which was presented four times as compared to the List 2 item which had been presented only once. There may also have been an effect of prior entry, which may have contributed to the lasting dominance of List 1 over List 2 priming (Schacter, Moscovitch, Tulving, McLachlan, & Freedman, 1986). In contrast, normal people, with intact medial temporal lobes, could overcome the detrimental effects of priming by consciously recollecting the item they had previously studied rather than responding with the first word that came to mind. The data from Experiment 1 could also be interpreted in terms of an inhibitory hypothesis. According to such an hypothesis, the large number of List 1 intrusion errors made by left medial temporal patients resulted from a failure of inhibitory processes that normally suppress inappropriate responses. By this view, it is immaterial whether responses are tied to implicit or explicit memory. Rather, it emphasizes the role of a central inhibitory mechanism in controlling response selection across all domains (Shimamura et al., 1991; Moscovitch, 1992). To test the alternative hypotheses, for Experiment 2 we devised a more purely implicit version of A-B, A-C learning and administered it to normal young and old adults. If the memory interpretation is correct, the performance of both groups should resemble that of patients with left medial temporal lobe lesions. That is, the number of intrusion errors they make in the List 2 condition should increase and the number of List 2 responses decrease 50 WINOCUR, MOSCOVITCH, AND BRUNI in relation to performance on the nominally explicit version of the test. However, if the inhibition hypothesis is correct, the young subjects should perform normally but the older adults should show a disproportionate number of intrusion errors (Winocur, 1982; Hasher & Zacks, 1988). EXPERIMENT 2 For this experiment, we devised a version of A-B, A-C negative transfer that was similar to Mayes et al.’s (1987) test in emphasizing the implicit component. This was accomplished by converting a nominally explicit test to a nominally implicit conceptual repetition priming test. In the implicit version, the crucial change was introduced in the test phase of List 2. Subjects were asked to produce the first word that came to mind in response to the stimulus word rather than recollect the appropriate response word from List 2. We administered both the implicit and explicit versions of the test to groups of normal young and old people whose memory was equivalent to that of the control subjects in the previous study. The memory hypothesis, in contrast to the inhibition hypothesis, predicts strong interference effects only on the implicit version of the test, even in young people. Method Subjects. Forty-eight people, divided equally into young and old groups, participated in Experiment 2. The young subjects, were undergraduates at Erindale College, University of Toronto, and had an average age of 20.9 years and 14.5 years of education. The old adults, part of a subject pool of senior volunteers, had an average age of 71.6 years and 15.8 years of education. The mean scores of the elderly and young adult subjects on the Mill Hill Vocabulary Test were 17.0 and 14.4, respectively. Materials. Three equivalent lists of related word pairs were constructed. Each list consisted of 12 stimulus words each of which were paired with two associates (AB, AC). For each stimulus word, the two associates were chosen such that they had approximately equal response rates on word association norms (Postman & Keppel, 1970). Word pairs were moderately highly associated in the word norms (e.g., eat–drink, food; music–sound, note). Within each list, the associate with the higher response rate in the word pairs was the AB associate in half the pairs and the AC associate in the other half. Assignment to one of the three lists, as well as to the AB or AC portion of any single list, was random. The stimulus words of the other two lists appeared at test and served to provide baseline measures. With a couple of exceptions, both the stimulus and response words had a high frequency (A, AA) of occurrence according to Thorndike and Lorge (1944). Procedure. The young and old adults each were subdivided into groups of 12 and assigned to either the explicit or the implicit test condition. Within each group, subjects studied one of the three lists. The study phase was identical for the explicit and implicit tests. Pairs of words were presented at a rate of one pair every 2 sec, with 60 sec elapsing between list presentations. Subjects were told to read the words aloud and to attempt to remember them by associating the words with one another. To achieve equivalent baselines of near-perfect List 1 learning, the procedure was repeated twice for the young adults and three times for the old adults. The order of word pairs varied across presentations. After the final presentation, the subjects were engaged in distracting conversation for 1 min to prevent rehearsal. Memory for the AB pairs was then 51 HEIGHTENED INTERFERENCE tested by presenting an index card with the stimulus word and subjects had 10 sec to respond. The experimenter provided the correct response if the subject failed to produce any response or responded incorrectly. After a 20- to 30-min delay filled with irrelevant, non-verbal tasks, the AC word pairs were presented once and the subjects were again told to try to remember them. Following a 1-min delay filled with conversation, the test phase began. In the explicit test, subjects were again shown each of the 12 stimulus words (A) on a card, and they were asked to provide the new response word (C). In the implicit test, subjects were shown a list of 96 words in one of two orders randomized across subjects. Each word was presented separately on an index card, and subjects were asked to freely associate and supply the first word that came to mind. They were encouraged to respond quickly. To reduce further the possibility that subjects would intentionally retrieve the responses they had learned, they were led to believe that this task was merely a distractor prior to the memory test. They were told that information they supplied would serve as norms for another experiment. Two orders of presentation of the 96 words were used. Of these words, 12 were targets, 24 were stimulus words from the other two lists, and the remainder were lures that were unrelated to the targets. The subjects’ responses to the 24 stimulus words from the other lists provided baseline response rates against which learning and priming could be compared. Results There were no differences between groups in list 1 (AB) learning. Nine of the 12 old and 11/12 young subjects obtained perfect scores and the remainder made one error each. To determine baserates for the implicit test, responses to stimulus items from the two non-studied lists were scored as either B or C if they corresponded to the nominal response items on those lists. Baserates of B and C responses to each of list were analyzed separately by ANOVA for each response in young and old subjects. Because no significant differences were obtained in any of the tests, the values were collapsed across the three lists to provide one baseline measure for each associate, B or C, for each age group. The baseline rates are given in Table 3. As a correction for guessing, these baserates were then subtracted from the number of correct responses for each subject in each condition. These corrected values were then entered into subsequent analyses. TABLE 3 Mean Number of Correct (C) Responses (List 2) and Prior (List 1) Intrusions (B) Corrected for Guessing on Explicit and Implicit Versions of A-B, A-C Negative Transfer Explicit Implicit Baserates Group C (Correct) B (Intrusion) C (Correct) B (Intrusion) C B Young Old 9.83 9.00 1.58 2.08 4.33 4.83 3.83 2.92 1.61 1.00 1.40 1.13 Note. Baserates represent guessing rate in each condition. 52 WINOCUR, MOSCOVITCH, AND BRUNI As predicted by the memory hypothesis, List 1 intrusions in the test phase of List 2, were much greater in the implicit than in the explicit version of A-B, A-C learning (see Table 3). There were no noticeable differences between the performance of young and old subjects. These impressions were confirmed by ANOVA with group (old vs. young) and test-version (explicit vs. implicit) as between-subject factors and response-type (B vs C) as a within-subject factor. There were significant main effects of test-version, F(1, 44) 5 28.64, p , .0001; MS 5 65.01; response-type, F(1, 44) 5 27.17, p , .0001; MS 5 236.25; and a significant interaction between test-version by response-type, F(1, 44) 5 53.34, p , .0001; MS 5 463.76. The significant interaction is due to a larger number of correct (C) responses on the explicit than the implicit version and the greater number of intrusions (B) on the implicit version. Note that in the implicit tests significant priming effects above baseline performance were obtained for both B and C responses in young and old subjects on both lists. T-tests were conducted for each response type in each age group. In all cases, t(11) . 2.45 p , .01. Discussion When subjects were instructed to produce the first word that came to mind, significant priming effects for List 1 and List 2 responses were observed in the test phase of List 2. These results indicate that when the negative transfer task is modified to emphasize implicit processes, it becomes an effective test of conceptual repetition priming. Under such circumstances, interference effects in old and young people were increased over and above those observed in the standard version of the test. This confirms similar results obtained by Mayes et al. (1987) and provides clear support for the memory hypothesis. Our demonstration of increased interference effects in an implicit test of memory, in comparison to its explicit counterpart, appears to conflict with Graf and Schacter’s (1987) report of effects in the opposite direction. Although there are numerous differences between their study and ours that may account for the discrepancy, two in particular should be emphasized. One relates to the degree of association between the word pairs—ours were highly related, whereas theirs were randomly associated. Another crucial difference is that theirs was a repetition priming test in which the first three letters of the response word served as a perceptual cue to complete the word. In our test, the stimulus word served as a semantic cue for the primed response. Taken together, these points suggest that because the words were unrelated in Graf and Schacter’s test, the weight of the priming effect falls on the perceptual features of the response item rather than on the semantic association between the common stimulus word and the two competing responses. In our test, however, the stimulus word is highly related to both HEIGHTENED INTERFERENCE 53 competing responses, increasing the likelihood of interference effects. This is especially the case given that no perceptual cues were provided to direct the response. Our finding of heightened interference in the implicit version of the negative transfer test supports the hypothesis that on the standard version of the test, the implicit component contributes substantially to the interference effect. Normal people show little interference because of their ability to use explicit memory processes to differentiate between competing responses. When these explicit memory processes are impaired, as they are in people with memory disorders, performance reflects the greater contribution of the implicit component and leads to heightened interference. As our results show, comparable interference effects can be obtained in people with intact memory when they are directed to rely on the implicit component in the negative transfer test. These results are not consistent with the inhibitory hypothesis, which does not predict differences in interference between explicit and implicit versions of the AB-AC test. It is worth noting that the performance of the old people was statistically indistinguishable from that of the young people on both the explicit and implicit versions of the test. These results are consistent with our earlier report that high-functioning, community-dwelling old people performed well on the negative transfer test (Winocur & Moscovitch, 1983). If response disinhibition were the cause of interference effects, elderly subjects, whose inhibitory processes are believed to be deficient (Hasher & Zacks, 1988), would have shown more intrusions than young subjects on both versions. Interestingly, the performance of the community old people contrasts with that of some institutionalized elderly people who, despite normal intelligence, show heightened interference effects and severe memory impairment related to medial temporal lobe dysfunction (Winocur, 1982; Winocur & Moscovitch, 1983). GENERAL DISCUSSION The principal finding of the first experiment was that left temporal lobectomy that involves large removal of the hippocampus greatly exacerbates interference in a verbal, negative transfer test. The second experiment showed that comparable interference effects can be obtained in normal people on an implicit version of the negative transfer test. The latter result suggests that the increased interference that patients with medial temporal/hippocampal lesions experience on verbal memory tests is related to impaired explicit memory and greater reliance on implicit memory processes. The hypothesis that memory loss, rather than impaired inhibition, is the primary cause of interference effects following medial temporal/hippocampal lesions is consistent with other evidence from the human and animal literatures, which we now review briefly. 54 WINOCUR, MOSCOVITCH, AND BRUNI Increased interference effects have been widely associated with damage to the hippocampus, a critical structure in the medial temporal lobe. In humans, these effects are expressed in various forms, such as, intrusion errors in list learning, difficulties in reversal learning, deficits on verbal and nonverbal versions of the Brown-Peterson test, and susceptibility to distractors on recognition memory tests. Studies involving animals have also provided numerous examples of interference effects in a variety of paradigms (see Winocur, 1982; Smith, 1989). In both the human and animal literatures, these interference effects have often been attributed to a loss of behavioural inhibition (Douglas 1967; Gray 1982; Kimble, 1968). The hypothesis that a loss of inhibition is fundamental to the hippocampal deficit has been challenged by evidence that behaviour is normal in some tasks that require inhibition of inappropriate responses. In both human and animal studies, the primary problem seems to involve poor memory for the appropriate cue-response association rather than inhibition of the inappropriate response. When the appropriate salient retrieval cues are reinstated to serve as an aid to memory, there are numerous examples to indicate that interference is reduced or eliminated. In the animal literature, a paradigmatic example of a task that requires response inhibition is passive avoidance learning in a runway situation. In the standard test, rats learn to approach a goal box at the end of the runway for reward. When shock is later introduced in the goal box, normal rats readily learn to suppress the approach response. In contrast, rats with hippocampal lesions continue to approach the goal box. To test the idea that it is their poor memory for response consequences, rather than response inhibition per se, that accounts for their abnormal behavior, Winocur and Black (1975) provided rats with a reminder by reinstating the goal box cues prior to test. This reminder was sufficient to induce normal performance in hippocampal rats. If increased interference were caused by a damage to a hippocampal-inhibitory mechanism, the lesioned animals should have continued to emit inappropriate responses even in the presence of reminder cues. Similarly, our research with memory-impaired humans has shown that providing salient contextual cues on various tasks can reduce interference effects and improve the level of performance in amnesics and institutionalized old people with poor memory. For example, interference on the A-B, AC task was substantially reduced when contextual environmental cues were provided to remind subjects that one list is distinguishable from the other (Winocur & Moscovitch, 1983; Winocur & Kinsbourne, 1978; Kinsbourne & Winocur, 1980, see also Mayes, 1988). These results suggest that an important function of the hippocampus is to create distinctive memories that do not interfere with each other, as can happen with (implicit) memories mediated by non-hippocampal structures. The idea that the hippocampus is necessary for reducing interference in learning and memory has been incorporated into recent connectionist neural network HEIGHTENED INTERFERENCE 55 models. A number of investigators have shown that catastrophic interference results if sequential learning is dependent on only a single, connectionist network that allows for cross-talk, i.e., extensive cross-referencing due to multiple, common associations (McCloskey & Cohen, 1989; Ratcliffe, 1990). McClelland, McNaughton, and O’Reilly, (1995), however, have shown that this type of interference can be minimized by introducing a second network that forms distinctive independent memory representations whose information can be incorporated slowly into old, stored knowledge in the first network. McClelland et al. likened the second network to the hippocampus, which can form distinctive memories that are consolidated over time into cortex and stored as knowledge. It is not necessary to agree with McClelland et al.’s equating of memory with knowledge to appreciate that their model captures an important aspect of hippocampal function and its contribution to memory formation. It is important to emphasize that the memory interpretation does not rule out inhibition as part of the process of reducing interference. According to our view, inhibition of inappropriate responses may be initiated only when the relevant explicit memory has been recovered. The essential point is that an inhibitory mechanism is not necessarily compromised in individuals with medial temporal lobe damage. Rather, failure of inhibition is secondary to their loss of explicit memory. Although the focus of this paper is on the medial temporal lobe/hippocampal region, other brain structures have been implicated in reducing interference. Among these structures, the frontal lobes figure most prominently (Luria, 1971; Moscovitch, 1982). Evidence that frontal-lobe damage is associated with perseveration (Mishkin, 1964; Milner, 1964) as well as with distraction by irrelevant stimuli suggests that the frontal lobes are fundamentally involved in the inhibitory control of behaviour (Shimamura, Janowsky, & Squire, 1991; Stuss & Benson, 1986). The effects of frontal lobe lesions on AB-AC learning have been examined in two published studies. Shimamura et al. (1995) reported a deficit in List 2 learning in a group of patients with unilateral frontal lobe lesions (four left, two right). The deficits in these patients were milder than in our lefttemporal lobe patients, even though, on average, their patients were 35 years older than ours. In addition, response intrusions accounted for only 25% of the frontal lobe patients’ errors, as opposed to over 60% of the left temporal patients errors. In a second study, Van der Linden, Bruyer, Roland, and Schil (1993) examined A-B, A-C learning in patients with aneurysm of the anterior communicating artery (ACoA). Such patients frequently sustain damage to the ventromedial frontal cortex as well as to structures in the basal forebrain and striatum. Although Van der Linden et al.’s patients showed heightened interference effects on A-C learning, there were no significant correlations between performance on negative transfer and performance on standard tests 56 WINOCUR, MOSCOVITCH, AND BRUNI of frontal function (e.g., WCST, verbal fluency and Stroop). A possible interpretation of their results is that non-frontal structures are critical for AB-AC learning, whereas frontal-lobe involvement is minimal. An alternative interpretation is that the ventro-medial regions of the frontal lobes may be involved in AB-AC learning and that other frontal-lobe regions mediate performance on standard tests. Unpublished data from our laboratory on two patients with confirmed lesions in the ventro-medial frontal cortex argue against this interpretation. Both patients scored normally on all aspects of AB-AC learning, despite poor performance on WCST and verbal fluency. In addition to these patients, we have tested nine patients with unilateral and three with bilateral frontal lesions on the AB-AC test. The bilateral patients exhibited severe memory loss and poor List 2 learning. In contrast, the unilateral patients had minimal memory loss and, with one exception, experienced little difficulty with the task. Taken together, the evidence suggests some frontal lobe involvement in AB-AC learning. It would appear that unilateral right or left lesions produce a milder deficit and one involving fewer response intrusions than that seen following left medial temporal lobe lesions. The most severe deficits in List 2 learning tend to occur in patients with bilateral lesions and are frequently accompanied by global memory loss. Loss of inhibition in frontal-lobe patients may well be a factor in producing heightened interference on the ABAC task. However, the available evidence suggests that the effect of inhibition is small relative to that associated with memory loss following left medial temporal lobe damage. REFERENCES Bryden, M. P. 1977. Measuring handedness with questionnaires. Neuropsychologia, 15, 617– 624. Cohen, N. J., & Squire, L. R. 1980. Preserved learning and retention of pattern analyzing skill in amnesia: Dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. Science, 210, 207–210. Douglas, R. J. 1967. The hippocampus and behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 67, 416–442. Graf, P., & Schacter, D. L. 1987. Selective effects of interference on implicit and explicit memory for new associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 13, 45–53. Gray, J. A. 1982. The neuropsychology of anxiety. Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press. Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. 1988. Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation. New York: Academic Press. Vol. 22, pp. 193–225. Howard, D. V. 1991. Implicit memory: An expanding picture of cognitive aging. In K. W. Schaie (Ed.), Annual review of gerontology and geriatrics. New York: Springer. Volume 11, pp. 1–22. Jarrard, L. E. 1975. Role of interference in retention by rats with hippocampal lesions. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 89, 400–408. Kimble, D. P. 1968. Hippocampus and internal inhibition. Psychological Bulletin, 70, 285– 295. HEIGHTENED INTERFERENCE 57 Luria, A. R. 1971. The neuropsychology of memory. Washington: V. H. Winston & Sons. Kinsbourne, M., & Winocur, G. 1980. Response competition and interference effects in pairedassociate learning by Korsakoff amnesics. Neuropsychologia, 18, 541–548. Mayes, A. R. 1988. Human organic memory disorders. Cambridge, England: Cambridge Univ. Press. Mayes, A. R., Pickering, A. D., & Fairbairn, A. 1987. Amnesic sensitivity to proactive interference: its relationship to priming and the causes of amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 25, 211– 220. McClelland, J. L., McNaughton, B., & O’Reilly, R. C. 1995. Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychological Review, 102, 419– 457. McCloskey, M., & Cohen, N. J. 1989. Catastrophic interference in connectionist networks: The sequential learning problem. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation. New York: Academic Press. Milner, B. 1964. Some effects of frontal lobectomy in man. In J. M. Warren & K. Akert (Eds.), The frontal granular cortex and behavior. New York: McGraw–Hill. Pp. 313– 334. Milner, B. 1974. Hemispheric specialization: Scope and limits. In F. O. Schmitt & F. G. Worden (Eds.), The neurosciences: Third research program. Cambridge: MIT Press. Pp. 75–89. Mishkin, M. 1964. Perseveration of central sets after frontal lesions in monkeys. In J. M. Warren & K. Akert (Eds.), The frontal granular cortex and behavior. New York: McGraw–Hill. Pp. 219–241. Moscovitch, M. 1992. Memory and working-with-memory: A component process model based on modules and central systems. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 257–267. Moscovitch, M. 1982. Multiple dissociations of function in amnesia. In L. S. Cermak (Ed.), Human memory and amnesia. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Moscovitch, M. (1989). Confabulation and the frontal system: Strategic vs. associative retrieval in neuropsychological theories of memory. In H. L. Roediger III & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honor of Endel Tulving. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Moscovitch, M., & Winocur, G. 1992a. The neuropsychology of memory and aging. In T. A. Salthouse and F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Moscovitch, M., & Winocur, G. 1992b. Frontal lobes and memory. In L. R. Squire (Ed.), The encyclopedia of learning and memory: Neuropsychology (D. L. Schacter, section editor). New York: MacMillan & Co. Moscovitch, M., Vriezen, E., & Goshen-Gottstein, Y. 1993. Implicit tests of memory in patients with focal lesions and degenerative brain disorders. In H. Spinnler & F. Boller (Eds.), Handbook of Neuropsychology. Amsterdam: Elsevier. Vol. 8, pp. 133–173. Postman, L., & Keppel, G. (Eds.). 1970. Norms of word association. New York: Academic Press. Ratcliff, R. 1990. Connectionist models of recognition memory: constraints imposed by learning and forgetting functions. Psychological Review, 97, 285–308. Schacter, D. L. 1987. Memory, amnesia, and frontal lobe dysfunction. Psychobiology, 15, 21– 36. Schacter, D. L., Moscovitch, M., Tulving, E., McLachlan, D. R., & Freedman, M. 1986. Mnemonic precedence in amnesic patients: An analogue of the AB error in infants. Child Development, 57, 816–823. Shimamura, A. P., Janowsky, J. S., & Squire, L. R. 1991. What is the role of frontal lobe damage in memory disorders? In H. D. Levin, H. M. Eisenberg, & A. L. Benton (Eds.), 58 WINOCUR, MOSCOVITCH, AND BRUNI Frontal lobe functioning and dysfunction. New York: Oxford University Press. Pp. 173– 195. Shimamura, A. P., Jurica, P. J., Mangels, J. A., Gershberg, F. B., & Knight, R. T. 1995. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 144–152. Smith, M. L. 1989. Memory disorders associated with temporal-lobe lesions. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology. Volume 3, pp. 91–106. Stuss, D. T., & Benson, D. F. 1986. The Frontal Lobes. New York: Raven Press. Thorndike, E. L., & Lorge, I. 1944. The teacher’s word book of 30,000 words. New York: Teacher’s College Press. Van der Linden, M., Bruyer, R., Roland, J., & Schils, J. P. 1993. Proactive interference in patients with amnesia resulting from anterior communicating artery aneurysm. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, 525–536. Warrington, E., & Weiskrantz, L. 1974. The effect of prior learning on subsequent retention in amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia, 12, 419–428. Winocur, G. 1982. Learning and memory deficits in institutionalized and noninstitutionalized old people: an analysis of interference effects. In F. I. M. Craik & S. Trehub (Eds.), Aging and cognitive processes. New York: Plenum. Winocur, G., & Black, A. H. 1978. Cue-induced recall of a passive avoidance response by rats with hippocampal lesions. Physiology & Behavior, 21, 39–44. Winocur, G. 1979. Effects of interference on discrimination learning and recall by rats with hippocampal lesions. Physiology & Behavior, 22, 339–345. Winocur, G., & Weiskrantz, L. 1976. An investigation of paired-associate learning in amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia, 14, 97–110. Winocur, G., & Kinsbourne, M. 1978. Contextual cuing as an aid to Korsakoff amnesics. Neuropsychologia, 16, 671–682. Winocur, G., & Moscovitch, M. 1983. Paired-associate learning in institutionalized and noninstitutionalized old people: An analysis of interference and context effects. Journal of Gerontology, 38, 455–464. Winocur, G., Kinsbourne, M., & Moscovitch, M. 1981. The effect of cuing on release from proactive interference in Korsakoff amnesic patients. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Learning and Memory, 7, 56–65.