* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Isolated Retrograde Amnesia
Adaptive memory wikipedia , lookup
Cognitive neuroscience of music wikipedia , lookup
Holonomic brain theory wikipedia , lookup
Prenatal memory wikipedia , lookup
Sex differences in cognition wikipedia , lookup
Memory and aging wikipedia , lookup
False memory wikipedia , lookup
Exceptional memory wikipedia , lookup
Eyewitness memory (child testimony) wikipedia , lookup
Collective memory wikipedia , lookup
Misattribution of memory wikipedia , lookup
Childhood memory wikipedia , lookup
Neurocase (2001) Vol. 7, pp. 269–282 © Oxford University Press 2001 PREVIOUS CASES Isolated Retrograde Amnesia Kristina Fast and Esther Fujiwara Department of Physiological Psychology, University of Bielefeld, PO Box 100131, D-33501 Bielefeld, Germany The syndrome of isolated retrograde amnesia refers to a neuropathological state of impaired memory for information acquired prior to the onset of brain damage (retrograde memory) contrasted with normal or near-normal learning of new information after the onset of the disease (anterograde memory). Whereas a more extensive impairment of anterograde memory together with minimal retrograde amnesia is often described in patients with (medial) temporal lobe damage or diencephalic lesions (e.g. Alvarez and Squire, 1995), disproportionate retrograde memory impairment can be found less frequently in patients with more heterogeneous lesions. Isolated retrograde amnesia is also known as ‘focal retrograde amnesia’ (e.g. Kapur, 1993, 2000; Evans et al., 1996; Carlesimo et al., 1998a, b; Kopelman, 2000a), ‘pure retrograde amnesia’ (e.g. Lucchelli et al., 1998), or ‘selective retrograde amnesia’ (e.g. Andrews et al., 1982). Although the aetiology and even the existence of the syndrome of retrograde amnesia is controversial [see Kapur (2000), Kopelman (2000a, b)], the principal symptom is consistently described as a severe loss of (mainly and mostly episodic) retrograde memory, combined with largely preserved anterograde memory ability. [See Tulving and Markowitsch (1998) for a recent definition of episodic memory.] A wide variety of aetiologies has been observed in different cases suffering from disproportionate retrograde amnesia. As organic aetiologies, the following were noted most consistently: mild brain injury (Stracciari et al., 1994; De Renzi et al., 1995; Starkstein et al., 1997), severe traumatic brain injury (Goldberg et al., 1981; Rousseaux et al., 1984; Kapur et al., 1992; Markowitsch et al., 1993a, b; Hunkin et al., 1995; Kapur et al., 1996), encephalitis (Eslinger and Cermak, 1988; Stuss and Guzman, 1988; O’Connor et al., 1992, 1997; Yoneda et al., 1992; Eslinger et al., 1993, 1996; Hokkanen et al., 1995; Calabrese et al., 1996; Carlesimo et al., 1998a; Levine et al., 1998), hypoxia (De Renzi and Lucchelli, 1993), vascular pathology (Andrews et al., 1982; Evans et al., 1996; Reinvang and Gjerstad, 1998), epilepsy (transient epileptic amnesia; TEA) (Kapur et al., 1989) and transient global amnesia (TGA) (Roman-Campos et al., 1980; Evans et al., 1993), suggesting that multifocal lesions produce isolated retrograde amnesia. Patients with more restricted organic brain damage most commonly demonstrate combined bilateral temporal lobe and pre-frontal damage (Damasio, 1989; Markowitsch et al., 1993a, b; Markowitsch, 1995; Kapur, 1997; Kroll et al., 1997; Markowitsch and Ewald, 1997; Levine et al., 1998). Sometimes damage is centred at both anterior temporal lobes (Tanaka et al., 1999), the medial temporal lobe structures of both hemispheres (including parts of the hippocampus and amygdala) (Fujii et al., 1999), or right parieto-occipital and left occipital regions (Hunkin et al., 1995). A patient with isolated retrograde amnesia and organic pathology was described by Tanaka et al. (1999). Following herpes simplex encephalitis, patient HK suffered from a persistent isolated retrograde amnesia after damage to the bilateral temporal poles and anterior parts of the inferotemporal lobes with some extensions to anterior parts of the medial temporal lobes as revealed by magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). The lesions were more severe on the left hemisphere, and no pathology was found in the frontal lobes. Standard neuropsychological assessment revealed no impairments in anterograde memory and other cognitive domains. The patient’s low verbal IQ was attributed to reduced performance in the information subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS; Wechsler, 1981) and was explained by a general semantic knowledge deficit. Her remote memory was severely deteriorated and affected mainly autobiographical events, while both personal and impersonal semantic memories were less impaired. An advantage of recognition performance over performance in free recall was observed in tests of impersonal semantic memory, while retrieval mode had no effect on autobiographical memory. It was suggested that the anterior temporal poles play a critical role in evoking remote memories and Correspondence to: Kristina Fast, Department of Physiological Psychology, University of Bielefeld, P.O. Box 100131, D-33501 Bielefeld, Germany. Tel: ⫹49 521 1064484; Fax: ⫹49 521 1066049; e-mail: [email protected] 270 Previous cases: Isolated retrograde amnesia possibly contain codes necessary for retrieval processes. Alternatively, Markowitsch (1995) suggested the anterior temporal lobes as a critical relay between the pre-frontal lobes and the posterior association cortices. Retrieval processes, therefore, are assigned to pre-frontal regions. These then are connected via anterior temporal lobes with formerly stored information, situated in posterior association areas. Predominantly frontal lesions were described in patient ML (Levine et al., 1998). He sustained a severe head injury with small right frontal lobe and left inferior posterior temporal contusions, a mild diffuse oedema and small bifrontal subdural hygromas [computer tomographic (CT) results 6 days post-trauma]. More than 1 year later, a MRI scan revealed damage in the right ventral frontal cortex, the underlying white matter, as well as in a few other smaller foci. Neuropsychological deficits included minor visuomotor and visuoperceptual problems, possibly caused by a subtle right upper quadrant anopsia, and a low score on an experimental task of strategy application. His disproportionate retrograde amnesia included a general retrieval deficit for semantic knowledge (personal and impersonal) and autobiographical episodes. Additionally, in an experimental task series, the ‘remember–know paradigm’ (Tulving, 1985) was applied to test different levels of consciousness according to two memory systems: episodic and semantic memory. Levine et al. (1998) suggested the remember–know technique as a sensitive indicator for different memory qualities in patients with (isolated) retrograde amnesia. It is hypothesized that episodic information is processed autonoetically (being aware of one’s self across time) and can be measured by ‘remember’ judgements, while semantic information is processed noetically (being aware of more general, impersonal facts) and can be assessed by ‘know’ judgements. A clear dissociation of remember and know judgements was found in ML, indicating preserved noetic awareness while autonoetic consciousness, on the other hand, was extremely poor. With regard to prior event-related potential and positron emission tomography (PET) findings, Levine et al. (1998) explained ML’s impairment in remember responses and autonoetic awareness by his right frontotemporal disconnection. More posterior lesions were observed in patient DH (Hunkin et al., 1995) following a closed head injury. Structural MRI revealed lesions in the right parieto-occipital and left occipital lobes. Temporal, frontal or diencephalic lesions could not be identified. Neuropsychological assessment indicated a severe loss of personal and public remote memory accompanied by visual anterograde memory impairments. DH’s pattern of deficits can be explained by a model of Damasio (1989). He proposed that episodic memory is represented in a multimodal fashion, and assumed that different aspects of an episode are stored unimodally in the sensory cortices in which they were originally registered. Recalling of an episode therefore requires similar neural activity patterns to those which occurred when the event was initially perceived. It is suggested that DH’s lesions disrupted transmission of information either from visual activity in the posterior cortex to multimodal convergence zones or vice versa, resulting in a change in neural activity patterns and exacerbating recall. As exemplified above, multiple lesions are associated with isolated retrograde amnesia. Accordingly, it is possible that different brain regions contribute to different aspects of remote memory processes resulting in similar observable deterioration. From a theoretical point of view, bilateral hippocampal lesions are also considered likely to evoke autobiographical memory loss. In the multiple trace model of Nadel and Moscovitch (1997) [see also Moscovitch and Nadel (1999)] it is assumed that, through close interaction of the hippocampal complex with other cortical structures, the experience of an event is mediated and by this process multiple traces are formed over time. Nadel and Moscovitch (1997) proposed that older memories require more memory traces compared with recent memories and that successful retrieval is facilitated by a higher quantity of memory traces. Moreover, they assumed that episodic memories remain dependent upon continuing interactions between the hippocampal complex and neocortex. Long-term consolidation processes depend on the hippocampal complex. Therefore, Moscovitch and Nadel (1998) supposed extensive bilateral hippocampal lesions to be necessary to evoke dense autobiographical amnesia. However, as shown above, in some cases with isolated retrograde amnesia, various lesion sites could not be entirely explained by this model. Another hypothesis centres on frontotemporal networks sparing medial temporal regions as critical for remote memory access (e.g. Kroll et al., 1997). In comparing the neuropsychological performance of two patients with combined bilateral temporopolar and pre-frontal damage with a patient suffering solely from bilateral prefrontal damage, Kroll et al. (1997) observed disproportionate retrograde memory deficits only in the patients with combined lesions. Emphasizing this regional combination (Kapur et al., 1992; Markowitsch et al., 1993a, b; Markowitsch and Ewald, 1997), in light of corresponding results of functional neuroimaging studies in non-brain-damaged subjects (Fink et al., 1996), the authors considered the temporofrontal junction area (e.g. fasciculus uncinatus) to be critical for retrieving old memories. The syndrome of isolated retrograde amnesia can also be caused by psychogenic factors, such as psychic traumata and stress, and becomes even more complex when these psychological factors are considered. Within this group of patients, those with a clear psychogenic causation (Schacter et al., 1982; Bremner et al., 1995; Campodonico and Rediess, 1996; Markowitsch et al., 1997a, b; Costello et al., 1998) can be differentiated from others with possible or likely psychological aetiologies, combined with additional (manifest) organic brain injury. If neither psychogenic causation nor organic causation can be clearly identified or separated, the term ‘functional retrograde amnesia’ is often used Previous cases: Isolated retrograde amnesia 271 to indicate metabolic changes in brain regions contributing to memory processes [see also De Renzi et al. (1997) and Markowitsch (1999)]. A frequently observed combination of psychogenic and organic aetiologies for isolated retrograde amnesia is represented by patients with initial brain pathology but without enduring organic lesions (Stuss, 1993; Binder, 1994; Kopelman et al., 1994; Lucchelli et al., 1995). Disproportionate and lasting retrograde amnesia is frequently described in mild head trauma patients without pathological signs in static neuroimaging (Barbarotto et al., 1996; De Renzi et al., 1997; Lucchelli et al., 1998; Mackenzie Ross, 2000). For instance, patient PA (Barbarotto et al., 1996) sustained a minor head trauma followed by isolated retrograde amnesia. Psychogenic and organic causation as well as deliberate simulation were discussed by the authors. CT and single photon emission computer tomography revealed no pathological signs and psychiatric evaluation gave no evidence of present or previous psychiatric disorders. Post-incidental clinical observations led to the assumption of a histrionic personality disorder and hysterical personality structures. Besides her retrograde amnesia, only multimodal short-term memory deficits were reported, accompanied by an intact anterograde long-term memory. PA complained of a loss of both semantic and episodic memories. However, neuropsychological assessment only revealed evidence of a severe loss of autobiographical memories. Semantic memory was also very poor, but indications for an implicit use of semantic knowledge were observed. The authors concluded that the presence of minor brain damage together with several psychogenic factors, a hysterical personality profile, emotional stress, possible secondary gain deriving from disease and intact implicit metamemory may all have contributed to the isolated retrograde amnesia. A case with a more transient isolated retrograde amnesia was described by Papagno (1998). Patient CL sustained a minor head injury caused by falling from a scaffold. CT and MRI were normal, EEG data showed irritative components in the left temporal region. Psychiatric evaluation revealed no psychiatric diagnoses, whereas in a clinical interview several episodes of emotional stress, including separation from his girlfriend 6 months prior to the accident, were ascertained. Besides his profound retrograde amnesia, his neuropsychological deficits included category-specific naming difficulties (living versus non-living things) and impaired verbal fluency. Learning ability, retrieval of postincident learned information, and the ability to relearn autobiographical data, were spared. The transient retrograde amnesia was defined by a total loss of semantic and autobiographical memories characterized by an absence of a clearcut temporal gradient. All aspects of retrograde amnesia and language impairment recovered spontaneously 10 days after the incident. Psychogenic and organic aetiology were discussed with respect to the idea of a mnestic block syndrome, proposed by Markowitsch (1996). In his revised model of psychogenic and organic amnesia, Kopelman (2000a) described the contributions of organic and psychogenic/functional factors to cognitive disorders as interactive and of considerable variation. Additional external and internal influencing factors acting on memory-relevant brain systems were established. These factors are comprised of learning experiences, current emotional state, personal semantic belief system, severe precipitating stress and ‘normal’ environmental input. The importance of careful assessment of all these factors is emphasized to provide a more comprehensive classification of patients with isolated retrograde amnesia on the continuum of organic and psychogenic/functional (namely hysterical/ unconscious and malingering/simulation) amnesia. In conclusion, the nature of the syndrome of isolated retrograde amnesia has to be considered as multifaceted and heterogeneous in aetiology, brain lesion or pathology, and neuropsychological impairment. These conditions lead to marked difficulties in differentiating aetiologies, structural and functional neuropathologies, as well as in specifying a theoretical model capable of elucidating all contributing aspects. References Alvarez P, Squire LR. Memory consolidation and the medial temporal lobe: a simple network model. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 1995; 91: 7041–5. Andrews E, Poser CM, Kessler M. Retrograde amnesia for forty years. Cortex 1982; 18: 441–58. Barbarotto R, Laiacona M, Cocchini G. A case of simulated, psychogenic or focal pure retrograde amnesia: did an entire life become unconscious? Neuropsychologia 1996; 34: 575–85. Binder LM. Psychogenic mechanisms of prolonged autobiographical retrograde amnesia. Clinical Neuropsychologist 1994; 8: 439–50. Bremner JD, Krystal JH, Southwick SM, Charney DS. Functional neuroanatomical correlates of the effects of stress on memory. Journal of Traumatic Stress 1995; 8: 527–53. Calabrese P, Markowitsch HJ, Durwen HF, Widlitzek H, Haupts M, Holinka B et al. Right fronto-temporal cortex as a critical locus for the ecphory of old episodic memories. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 1996; 61: 304–10. Campodonico JR, Rediess S. Dissociation of implicit and explicit knowledge in a case of psychogenic retrograde amnesia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 1996; 2: 146–58. Carlesimo GA, Sabbadini M, Loasses A, Caltagirone C. Analysis of the memory impairment in a post-encephalitic patient with focal retrograde amnesia. Cortex 1998a; 34: 449–60. Carlesimo GA, Sabbadini M, Bombardi P, Di Porto E, Loasses A, Caltagirone C. Retrograde memory deficits in severe closed-head injury patients. Cortex 1998b; 34: 1–23. Costello A, Fletcher PC, Dolan RJ, Frith CD, Shallice T. The origins of forgetting in a case of isolated retrograde amnesia following a hemorrhage: evidence from functional imaging. Neurocase 1998; 4: 437–46. Damasio AR. Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: a systems level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition. Cognition 1989; 33: 25–62. De Renzi E, Lucchelli F. Dense retrograde amnesia, intact learning capability and abnormal forgetting rate: a consolidation deficit? Cortex 1993; 29: 449–66. De Renzi E, Lucchelli F, Muggia S, Spinnler H. Persistent retrograde amnesia following a minor trauma. Cortex 1995; 31: 531–42. De Renzi E, Lucchelli F, Muggia S, Spinnler H. Is memory loss without anatomical damage tantamount to a psychogenic deficit? The case of pure retrograde amnesia. Neuropsychologia 1997; 35: 781–94. Eslinger PJ, Cermak LS. Anatomic correlates of retrograde amnesia. Social Neuroscience Abstracts 1988; 14: 1289. 272 Previous cases: Isolated retrograde amnesia Eslinger PJ, Damasio H, Damasio AR, Butters N. Nonverbal amnesia and asymmetric cerebral lesions following encephalitis. Brain and Cognition 1993; 21: 140–52. Eslinger PJ, Easton A, Grattan LM, Van Hoesen GW. Distinct forms of partial retrograde amnesia after asymmetric temporal lobe lesions: possible role of the occipitotemporal gyri in memory. Cerebral Cortex 1996; 6: 530–9. Evans JJ, Wilson BA, Wraight EP, Hodges JR. Neuropsychological and SPECT scan findings during and after transient global amnesia: evidence for the differential assessment of remote episodic memory. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 1993; 56: 1227–30. Evans JJ, Breen EK, Antoun N, Hodges JR. Focal retrograde amnesia for autobiographical events following cerebral vasculitis: a connectionist account. Neurocase 1996; 2: 1–11. Fink G, Markowitsch HJ, Reinkemeier M, Bruckbauer T, Kessler J, Heiss WD. A PET-study of autobiographical memory recognition. Journal of Neuroscience 1996; 16: 4275–82. Fujii T, Yamadori A, Endo K, Suzuki K, Fukatsu R. Disproportionate retrograde amnesia in a patient with herpes simplex encephalitis. Cortex 1999; 35: 599–614. Goldberg E, Antin SP, Bilder RM, Gerstman LJ, Hughes JEO, Mattis S. Retrograde amnesia: possible role of mesencephalic reticular activation in long-term memory. Science 1981; 213: 1392–4. Hokkanen L, Launes R, Vataja R, Vallanne L, Iivanainen M. Isolated retrograde amnesia for autobiographical material associated with acute left temporal lobe encephalitis. Psychological Medicine 1995; 25: 203–8. Hunkin NM, Parkin AJ, Bradley VA, Burrows EH, Aldrich FK, Jansari A et al. Focal retrograde amnesia following closed head injury: a case study and theoretical account. Neuropsychologia 1995; 33: 509–23. Kapur N. Focal retrograde amnesia: a critical review. Cortex 1993; 29: 217–34. Kapur N. How can we best explain retrograde amnesia in human memory disorder. Memory 1997; 5: 115–29. Kapur N. Focal retrograde amnesia and the attribution of causality: an exceptionally benign commentary. Cognitive Neuropsychology 2000; 17: 623–37. Kapur N, Young A, Bateman D, Kennedy P. Focal retrograde amnesia: a long term clinical and neuropsychological follow-up. Cortex 1989; 25: 387–402. Kapur N, Ellison D, Smith M, McLellan L, Burrows EH. Focal retrograde amnesia following bilateral temporal lobe pathology: a neuropsychological and magnetic resonance study. Brain 1992; 116: 73–86. Kapur N, Scholey K, Moore E, Barker S, Brice J, Thompson S et al. Longterm retention deficits in two cases of disproportionate retrograde amnesia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 1996; 8: 416–34. Kopelman MD. Focal retrograde amnesia and the attribution of causality: an exceptionally critical review. Cognitive Neuropsychology 2000a; 17: 585–621. Kopelman MD. Comments on focal retrograde amnesia and the attribution of causality: an exceptionally benign commentary by Narinder Kapur. Cognitive Neuropsychology 2000b; 17: 639–40. Kopelman MD, Christensen H, Puffett A, Stanhope N. The great escape: a neuropsychological study of psychogenic amnesia. Neuropsychologia 1994; 32: 675–91. Kroll NEA, Markowitsch HJ, Knight RT, von Cramon DY. Retrieval of old memories: The temporo-frontal hypothesis. Brain 1997; 120: 1377–99. Levine B, Black SE, Cabeza R, Sinden M, Mcintosh AR, Toth JP et al. Episodic memory and the self in a case of isolated retrograde amnesia. Brain 1998; 121: 1951–73. Lucchelli F, Muggia S, Spinnler H. The ‘Petites Madeleines’ phenomenon in two amnesic patients. Sudden recovery of forgotten memories. Brain 1995; 118: 167–83. Lucchelli F, Muggia S, Spinnler H. The syndrome of pure retrograde amnesia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 1998; 3: 91–118. Mackenzie Ross S. Profound retrograde amnesia following mild head injury: organic or functional? Cortex 2000; 36: 521–37. Markowitsch HJ. Which brain regions are critically involved in the retrieval of old episodic memory? Brain Research Reviews 1995; 21: 117–27. Markowitsch HJ. Organic and psychogenic retrograde amnesia: two sides of the same coin? Neurocase 1996; 2: 357–71. Markowitsch HJ. Gedächtnisstörungen. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer. 1999. Markowitsch HJ, Ewald K. Right-hemispheric fronto-temporal injury leading to severe autobiographical retrograde and moderate anterograde episodic amnesia. Neurology, Psychiatry and Brain Research 1997; 5: 71–8. Markowitsch HJ, Calabrese P, Haupts M, Durwen HF, Liess J, Gehlen W. Searching for the anatomical basis of retrograde amnesia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 1993a; 15: 947–67. Markowitsch HJ, Calabrese P, Haupts M, Durwen HF, Niess J, Gehlen W. Retrograde amnesia after traumatic injury of the fronto-temporal cortex. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 1993b; 56: 988–92. Markowitsch HJ, Calabrese P, Fink GR, Durwen HF, Kessler J, Härting C et al. Impaired episodic memory retrieval in a case of probable psychogenic amnesia. Psychiatry Research 1997a; 74: 119–26. Markowitsch HJ, Fink GR, Thöne A, Kessler J, Heiss WD. A PET-study of persistent psychogenic amnesia covering the whole life span. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 1997b; 2: 135–58. Moscovitch M, Nadel L. Consolidation and the hippocampal complex revisited: In defense of the multple-trace model. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 1998; 8: 297–300. Moscovitch M, Nadel L. Multiple-trace theory and semantic dementa: A reply to K.S. Graham. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 1999; 3: 87–9. Nadel L, Moscovitch M. Memory consolidation, retrograde amnesia and the hippocampal complex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 1997; 7: 217–27. O’Connor M, Butters N, Miliotis P, Eslinger P, Cermak LS. The dissociation of anterograde and retrograde amnesia in a patient with herpes encephalitis. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 1992; 14: 159–78. O’Connor M, Sieggreen MA, Ahern G, Schomer D, Mesulam M. Accelerated forgetting in association with temporal lobe epilepsy and paraneoplastic encephalitis. Brain and Cognition 1997; 35: 71–84. Papagno C. Transient retrograde amnesia associated with impaired naming of living categories. Cortex 1998; 34: 111–21. Reinvang I, Gjerstad L. Focal retrograde amnesia associated with vascular headache. Neuropsychologia 1998; 36: 1335–41. Roman-Campos G, Poser CM, Wood FB. Persistent retrograde memory deficit after transient global amnesia. Cortex 1980; 16: 509–18. Rousseaux M, Delafosse A, Cabaret M, Lesoin F, Jomin M. Amnésie retrograde post traumatique. Cortex 1984; 20: 575–93. Schacter DL, Wang PL, Tulving E, Freedman M. Functional retrograde amnesia: a quantitative case study. Neuropsychologia 1982; 20: 523–32. Squire LR, Alvarez P. Retrograde amnesia and memory consolidation: A neurobiological perspective. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 1995; 5: 169–77. Starkstein SE, Sabe L, Dorrego MF. Severe retrograde amnesia after a mild injury. Neurocase 1997; 3: 105–9. Stracciari A, Ghidoni E, Guarino M, Poletti M, Pazzaglia P. Post-traumatic retrograde amnesia with selective impairment of autobiographical memory. Cortex 1994; 30: 459–68. Stuss DT. An unusual case of retrograde amnesia: fact or fake? Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 1993; 15: 389. Stuss DT, Guzman DA. Severe remote memory loss with minimal anterograde amnesia: a clinical note. Brain and Cognition 1988; 8: 21–30. Tanaka Y, Miyazawa Y, Hashimoto R, Nakano I, Obayashi T. Postencephalitic focal retrograde amnesia after bilateral anterior temporal lobe damage. Neurology 1999; 53: 344–50. Tulving E. Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology 1985; 26: 1–12. Tulving E, Markowitsch HJ. Episodic and declarative memory: role of the hippocampus. Hippocampus 1998; 8: 198–204. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation, 1981. Yoneda Y, Yamadori A, Mori E, Yamashita H. Isolated prolonged retrograde amnesia. European Neurology 1992; 32: 340–2. Received on 26 March, 2001; accepted on 12 April, 2001 Previous cases: Isolated retrograde amnesia 273 A traumatic temporal lobe pathology and autobiographical memory A case of simulated, psychogenic or focal pure retrograde amnesia: did the entire life become unconscious? R. Babinsky, B. Maier, P. Calabrese, H. J. Markowitsch and W. Gehlen R. Barbarotto, M. Laiacona and G. Cochini Abstract Abstract A patient with isolated autobiographical episodic memory is presented. The patient (male; 64 years old) was admitted to hospital suffering from double images and headache. CT examination revealed an arachnoid cyst at the anterior part of the left temporal lobe. In neuropsychological examination, a clear dissociation between subnormal autobiographical episodic memory and intact anterograde memory as well as unimpaired remote memory for personal and public facts was found. Regarding limited lesion sites in this patient, it is hypothesized that left anterior temporal lobe regions may play a critical role in effortful processing of episodic autobiographical memories. A patient (PA; female; 38 years old) developed a pure retrograde amnesia covering her whole life-span following a mild head injury. CT and SPECT gave no evidence for brain pathology. Psychogenic and organic aspects of the amnesia were taken into account to explain the patient’s profile as well as the possibility of simulated amnesia. Her implicit use of remote information was not impaired, supporting the interpretation of a psychogenic block in a person with a hysterical personality structure instead of deliberate simulation. Journal European Neurology 1994; 34: 290–1 Neurocase Reference Number: P814 Primary diagnosis of interest Isolated (focal) autobiographical retrograde amnesia Author’s designation of case Not stated Key theoretical issue d Journal Neuropsychologia 1996; 34: 575–85 Neurocase Reference Number: P815 Primary diagnosis of interest Isolated (focal) retrograde amnesia Author’s designation of case PA Key theoretical issue d In a patient a dissociation between unimpaired anterograde memory performance and retrograde autobiographical amnesia is attributed to a selective left temporal pole lesion A patient developed a pure retrograde amnesia without temporal gradient following a mild head trauma with no persistent brain pathology. Postmorbid information was normally learned and retrieved. The case was discussed as a form of psychogenic amnesia caused by mnestic block or by unconscious simulation Key words: amnesia; retrograde; arachnoid cysts; anterior temporal lobe Key words: retrograde amnesia; psychogenic amnesia Scan, EEG and related measures Scan, EEG and related measures EEG, CT 1993: CT 1994: SPECT Standardized assessment Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (modified version: Reduzierter WechslerIntelligenztest WIP), Concept Comprehension Task, Concentration Endurance Test d2, Zahlen-Verbindungs-Test (German version of Trail Making Test), Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised, Aachener Aphasie Test (German language test), Word Stem Completion Tasks, Famous Events Test, Autobiographical Memory Interview, German personality questionnaire Other assessment None Lesion location d d EEG: normal CT: arachnoid cyst at the left temporal pole filled with fluid, possibly cerebrospinal fluid Standardized assessment Mini-Mental-Status Test, Raven’s Progressive Matrices, Token Test, Naming of Objects and Details, Behavioural Memory Rivermead Test, Digit Span, Disyllable Word Span, Spatial Span, Verbal Learning, Story Recall, Spatial Learning, Semantic Memory Questionnaire, Public Events and Famous People Questionnaires, Fame Decision Task, Autobiographical Memory Questionnaire, Procedural Memory Task Other assessment Psychodiagnostic evaluation: clinical interview, Rorschach Test, Blacky Test Lesion location d d CT: normal SPECT: normal Lesion type Lesion type Arachnoid cyst Mild head injury Language Language English English 274 Previous cases: Isolated retrograde amnesia Analysis of the memory impairment in a post-encephalitic patient with focal retrograde amnesia G. A. Carlesimo, M. Sabbadini, A. Loasses and C. Caltagirone Abstract Data from a post-encephalitic patient (AV; female; 29 years old) with focal retrograde amnesia is presented. MRI demonstrated widespread abnormal intensity areas encroaching upon the white matter of temporal, parietal and occipital lobes but sparing temporal poles and orbito-frontal cortex. The quality of her memory impairment is extensively studied. AV’s memory deficit encompassed autobiographical data, public events and famous people. In a public events questionnaire, a negative temporal gradient with good recollection of events that occurred about 15 years before the disease, but poor memory of more recent events was found. Lexicality judgement for words and familiarity judgement for proper names was normal, but she was poor in familiarity judgement for famous faces (prosopagnosia) and in accessing the meaning of words or specific information about people. Visual input lexicon and semantic description system are supposed to normally function through close associations between the two systems, e.g. by means of corresponding nodes. Diffuse damage of associative areas of posterior-temporal and parietal lobes in patient AV is assumed to account for her remote memory deficits. Journal Remembering and knowing the past: a case study of isolated retrograde amnesia G. Dalla Barba, M. C. Mantovan, E. Ferruza and G. Denes Abstract A patient (RM; female; 17 years old) suffered from a sudden selective retrograde amnesia for personal episodes with mostly spared retrograde memory for semantics. General intellectual and learning abilities were normal. Neuroimaging data gave no hints for brain damage. Although the patient complained of occipital headache, back pain and nausea, these signs were too unspecific and gave no sufficient support for an organic pathogenesis. Psychogenic aetiology could not be excluded, but history and clinical observations did not fit with known cases of psychogenic amnesia. There was no evidence for simulation. The dissociation between episodic and semantic memory in RM gave support for the distinction between remembering and knowing. Journal Cortex 1997; 33: 143–54 Neurocase Reference Number: P817 Cortex 1998; 34: 449–60 Primary diagnosis of interest Neurocase Reference Number: Isolated (focal) retrograde amnesia P816 Author’s designation of case Primary diagnosis of interest RM Isolated (focal) retrograde amnesia Author’s designation of case AV Key theoretical issue d Key theoretical issue d In this patient diffuse bilateral damage of associative areas in the posteriortemporal and parietal lobes led to isolated retrograde amnesia. On the neuropsychological level, dissociation across different pre-semantic structural description systems and between the visuo-verbal description system and the semantic system were observed A case of spontaneously developed retrograde amnesia involving solely personal memory was described. No clear evidence for organic brain damage, psychogenic aetiology or simulation was found. The case gave additional support for the distinction between episodic and semantic memory Key words: amnesia; retrograde amnesia Scan, EEG and related measures Key words: memory disorders; retrograde amnesia; herpes virus encephalitis CT, MRI, PET Scan, EEG and related measures Standardized assessment MRI, CSF examination, visual field testing Standardized assessment Token Test, Boston Naming Test, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Raven’s Progressive Matrices, Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised, Corsi Span, Rey– Osterrieth Complex Figure Test B, semantically related word list, semantically unrelated word list, short story, supraspan, copying line drawings, test for prosopagnosia, Famous Events Recognition Test, Lexical Decision Task, Semantic Decision Task, Famous Faces Test, Famous Names Test Other assessment Verbal autobiographical memory testing (main life episodes derived by AV’s mother), visual autobiographical memory testing (photographs provided by AV’s mother) Lesion location d Widespread abnormal intensity areas diffusing to the white matter of temporal, parietal and occipital lobes and periventricular regions of both hemispheres, predominantly on the left side Lesion type (Presumed) herpes encephalitis Language English Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised, Wechsler Memory Scale, Crovitz Cueing Procedure, Warrington Recognition Memory Test, Verbal Fluency, Famous Faces Test, Famous Person Test, Famous Events Questionnaire, Semantic Association Test based on items adapted from the Pyramid and Palm Tree Test, unspecified tests on perception, language, praxis, visual attention and ‘frontal’ functions Other assessment Rorschach Test, Persons and Places Sorting Out Test (based on items given by the parents), recognition of personal past episodes (remember/knowing paradigm), recognition of personal photographs (based on photographs given by the parents), name–activity incongruous association (paired-associate learning) Lesion location d CT, MRI, PET: normal Lesion type None Language English Previous cases: Isolated retrograde amnesia 275 Is memory loss without anatomical damage tantamount to a psychogenic deficit? The case of pure retrograde amnesia Other assessment Tests of writing, computational abilities and motor skills Lesion location d CT, EEG, MRI, SPECT: normal Lesion type Presumed mild head trauma E. De Renzi, F. Lucchelli, S. Muggia and H. Spinnler Abstract Following a car accident, a patient (Andrea; male; 58 years old) remained unconscious for approximately 20 min and confused for a few hours. After the incident he developed a retrograde amnesia, whereby organic or psychological aetiology remained unclear. Neither signs of brain damage were detectable in the neurological examination and on CT, MRI and SPECT, nor evidence for psychiatric history, psychological stress or emotional precipitants was found. The retrograde amnesia covered his whole life and affected autobiographical events as well as famous facts and encyclopaedic knowledge. It also partially involved the verbal and visual lexicon in a way that reading, writing and counting were no longer possible. The retrograde amnesia contrasted with the preservation of anterograde memory which permitted the patient to reacquire some autobiographical information, but in a detached manner. In a 4 year follow-up, retrograde amnesia persisted. The authors propose that cases of focal retrograde amnesia, similar to the present one, might be classified separately from organic and psychogenic forms under the label of ‘functional’ retrograde amnesia. In functional retrograde amnesia the threshold of activation of pre-morbid memories is assumed to be abnormally raised by the trauma, leaving the encoding and retrieval of new memories unaffected. Journal Neuropsychologia 1997; 35: 781–94 Neurocase Reference Number: P818 Primary diagnosis of interest Isolated (focal) retrograde amnesia Author’s designation of case Andrea Key theoretical issue d A dissociation between partially preserved anterograde memory and profound retrograde memory loss in combination with additional neuropsychological impairments was observed in a patient without neurological pathology. Despite successfully reacquiring autobiographical and contextual information, the patient remained impaired in several procedural domains for at least 2 years. The amnesic profile is interpreted by a trauma-raised threshold of patterned matrices of facilitation and inhibition allocated to pre-incidentally encoded engrams, leaving the engrams themselves and storage/retrieval processes undamaged Key words: memory disorders; retrograde amnesia; functional amnesia Scan, EEG and related measures CT, ECG, EEG, MRI, SPECT Standardized assessment Raven’s Progressive Matrices, Weigl Colour Form Sorting Test, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Visual Form Discrimination, Unknown Face Recognition, Token Test, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Boston Naming Test, Verbal Fluency, Semantic Fluency (colours, animals, fruits, cities), Word Repetition, Lexical Decision (auditory), Famous Face Recognition Tests, Autobiographical Memory Questionnaire, Story Recall, Paired Associate Learning, BuschkeFuld 10-word Learning (selective reminding), Corsi Supra-Span Spatial Learning, Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test, Name–Activity Association Learning Test Language English 276 Previous cases: Isolated retrograde amnesia Disproportionate retrograde amnesia in a patient with herpes simplex encephalitis Focal retrograde amnesia following closed head injury: a case study and theoretical account T. Fujii, A. Yamadori, K. Endo, K. Suzuki and R. Fukatsu N. M. Hunkin, A. J. Parkin, V. A. Bradley, E. H. Burrows, F. K. Aldrich, A. Jansari and C. Burdon-Cooper Abstract A patient (female; 51 years old) developed a severe retrograde amnesia extending back for about 10 years associated with a relatively mild anterograde amnesia following herpes simplex encephalitis. Neuroimaging revealed a bilateral medial temporal lobe pathology. The retrograde amnesia included autobiographical memory as well as knowledge about famous people and public events. Abstract Not stated Data from a closed head injury patient (DH; male; 40 years old) are presented who suffered from focal retrograde amnesia. Structural MRI revealed posterior brain lesions in parietal and occipital regions. The patient was unable to recollect any autobiographical incidents from the pre-morbid period, although he provided personal and public information from this period. The data are discussed in relation to other reported instances of focal retrograde amnesia and a theoretical account is considered. The systems-level model of recall and recognition is applied to the patient’s neuropsychological impairments. The model proposes that episodic memory comprises multimodal representation and it assumes that different aspects of episodes are stored unimodally in the sensory cortices in which they were originally registered. Recalling an episode is supposed to require similar neural activity patterns compared to those occurred, when the event was initially perceived. It is suggested by the authors that DH’s lesions disrupted transmission of information either from visual activity in the posterior cortex to multimodal convergence zones or vice versa, thus resulting in changed neural activity patterns and deteriorated recall. Key theoretical issue Journal Journal Cortex 1999; 35: 599–614 Neurocase Reference Number: P819 Primary diagnosis of interest Isolated (focal) retrograde amnesia Author’s designation of case d A patient suffered from a severe but temporally limited retrograde amnesia coupled with a relatively mild anterograde amnesia following herpes simplex encephalitis Neuropsychologia 1995; 33: 509–23 Neurocase Reference Number: P820 Key words: retrograde amnesia; herpes simplex encephalitis; herpes virus Primary diagnosis of interest Scan, EEG and related measures Isolated (focal) retrograde amnesia MRI, SPECT Author’s designation of case Standardized assessment DH Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Mini-Mental-State Test, Token Test, Raven’s Progressive Matrices, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Verbal Fluency, Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, Rey– Osterrieth Complex Figure Test, Kohs Cube Test, Memory for Public Events, Dead or Alive Test Key theoretical issue Other assessment Clinical assessment of anterograde autobiographical memory, structured interview of autobiographical memory based on information of the patient’s closest sister Lesion location d d MRI: abnormal high intensity areas in bilateral medial temporal lobes (including two thirds of the hippocampal formation, hippocampus, dendate gyrus, subiculum) and the posterior part of the amygdala SPECT: diminution of regional blood flow in bilateral anterior medial temporal lobe regions Lesion type Herpes simplex encephalitis Language English d The dissociation between preserved anterograde memory functioning and impaired remote memory access is explained by means of disrupted transmission between unimodal sensory cortices and multimodal convergence zones. According to the occipital lesions in the patient, impaired visual processing might account for the inability to evoke similar visual activity patterns compared with those of initially experienced episodes, thus leading to a recall deficit of remote memories Key words: memory; amnesia; retrograde Scan, EEG and related measures MRI Standardized assessment Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised, National Adult Reading Test, Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, Rey– Osterrieth Complex Figure Test, Warrington Recognition Memory Test, Cognitive Estimation, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Verbal Fluency, Public Events Test, Famous Faces Test, Famous Names Test, Autobiographical Memory Interview, autobiographical cueing procedure Other assessment None Previous cases: Isolated retrograde amnesia 277 Lesion location d Structural lesions in the right parieto-occipital and left occipital lobes. The right hemispheric lesions were associated with focal atrophy of cortex and underlying white matter, the left occipital lesion involved damage to a discrete area of grey matter on the inferior surface of the occipital lobe. Additional damage involved the medial surface of both occipital lobes associated with some enlargement of the body of the right lateral ventricle and gross enlargement of the posterior horns bilaterally Amnesia can facilitate memory performance: evidence from a patient with dissociated retrograde amnesia N. Kapur, P. Heath, P. Meudell and P. Kennedy Lesion type Abstract Closed head injury A patient (ED; male; 70 years old) suffered from transient attacks of retrograde amnesia lasting 15 min to several hours during the last 6 years. No precipitating factors were revealed. The attacks produced an impairment in the ability to recall past events and facts especially for verbal material without corresponding anterograde impairments. The findings gave support for fractionation of memory in different components of a single aspect of memory and indicated that in certain situations specific types of amnesia can produce a facilitation effect compared to the performance of healthy controls. Language English Journal Neuropsychologia 1986; 24: 215–21 Neurocase Reference Number: P821 Primary diagnosis of interest Isolated (focal) retrograde amnesia Author’s designation of case ED Key theoretical issue d A case of dissociation in memory was presented which indicated that a ‘functional facilitation paradigm’ might be useful to explore the scope of facilitation effects in amnesic patients Key words: retrograde amnesia; memory Scan, EEG and related measures CT, EEG Standardized assessment Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, National Adult Reading Test, Wechsler Memory Scale, Benton Visual Retention Test, Faces Matching Task, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Verbal Fluency, Famous Personalities Test, Public Events Test of Retrograde Amnesia, Voices Recognition Test of Famous Personalities Other assessment Paired Associate Learning Test Lesion location d d CT: normal EEG: normal Lesion type Not stated Language English 278 Previous cases: Isolated retrograde amnesia Long-term retention deficits in two cases of disproportionate retrograde amnesia N. Kapur, K. Sholey, E. Moore, S. Barker, J. Brice, S. Thompson, A. Shiel, R. Carn, P. Abbott and J. Fleming Other assessment 2. Long-term retention test of stimuli used in the Wechsler Memory ScaleRevised (recall and recognition versions), retention test of events recorded from the patient’s diary by using graded retention intervals (1–5 weeks) Lesion location d Abstract Data from two patients with disproportionate retrograde amnesia are presented. Both patients suffered from severe closed head injury. The first patient (GR; female; 40 years old) had residual autobiographical amnesia for pre-incidental events, mild anterograde memory deficits and significantly impaired longterm knowledge acquisition. The second patient (SP; female; 50 years old) was left with marked retrograde amnesia for events, mild anterograde memory deficits and faster rate of forgetting over a period of 6 weeks compared to control subjects. In both cases there was major pathology in the region of the left temporal lobe, with lateral structures being more affected than medial structures. Evidence for the independence of certain anterograde and retrograde memory mechanisms is provided and a link of retrograde amnesia to impairments in very long-term retention is assumed. It is hypothesized that lesions to intrahemispheric fasciculi or similar association fibres in combination with lesions to critical anterior or posterior cerebral structures may lead to disconnection of visual–semantic and other associations from verbal representations of such associations, resulting in the observed neuropsychological impairment. Journal Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 1996; 8: 416–34 Neurocase Reference Number: P822 Primary diagnosis of interest Disproportionate retrograde amnesia Author’s designation of case 1. GR 2. SP Key theoretical issue d In two patients a relationship between retrograde amnesia and long-term retention deficits is found. The possibility is raised that some forms of disproportionate retrograde amnesia might result from more than one site of brain pathology with intrahemispheric connections of anterior temporal lobes with posterior brain regions being critically involved in remote memory access Key words: closed head injury; autobiographical memory; retrograde amnesia; long-term follow-up; consolidation; medial temporal lobe Scan, EEG and related measures 1. EEG, MRI, PET 2. MRI Standardized assessment 1. National Adult Reading Test, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised, modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Verbal Fluency, Graded Naming Test, Facial Recognition Test, Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised, Recognition Memory Test, Autobiographical Memory Interview, structured interview of autobiographical memory, Dead-or-Alive-Test, Famous Faces Test, Famous Names Test, Knowledge of Terms Test, Knowledge of Abbreviations Test 2. National Adult Reading Test, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised, modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Verbal Fluency, Graded Naming Test, Facial Recognition Test, Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test, Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised, Recognition Memory Test, Autobiographical Memory Interview, structured interview of autobiographical memory, Public Events Test d d d 1. MRI: marked abnormality of the left inferolateral temporal lobe with gliosis in the remaining parenchyma and some involvement of the left parietal lobe. Focal dilatation of the left temporal horn. The right temporal lobe showed a discrete area of gliosis in the inferior temporal gyrus. Additionally, atrophy of the left posterior hippocampus, marked atrophy of the posterolateral aspect of the left frontal lobe, mild atrophy of the left fornix, marked atrophy of the left anterior perforated substance were shown and dilatation of the lateral and third ventricles reflected mild generalized atrophy. PET: area of abnormality in the left temporoparietal region. EEG: focal transient abnormality around the right mid-temporal and right parietal electrodes. 2. Major atrophy of the left temporal lobe, particularly involving the temporal pole and the anterior portions of the three temporal gyri. Dilatation of the left temporal horn. Some involvement of the left hippocampus, especially in the anterior portion, and part of the right uncus/hippocampus. Lesion type Closed head injury Language English Previous cases: Isolated retrograde amnesia 279 Episodic memory and the self in a case of isolated retrograde amnesia Lesion location d d B. Levine, S. E. Black, R. Cabeza, M. Sinden, A. R. Mcintosh, J. P. Toth, E. Tulving and D. T. Stuss Abstract A patient (ML; male) suffered from isolated retrograde amnesia contrasting with normal anterograde memory performance following a severe traumatic head injury. MRI gave evidence for damage to the right frontal cortex and underlying white matter. Initial SPECT showed left superior medial parietal hypoperfusion which resolved 1 year later. The mnemonic deficits and the impaired self-regulation measured by remember/know judgements were discussed as produced by impaired autonoetic awareness affecting behaviour across the time dimension. The findings suggest that the patient has impaired autonoetic awareness attributable to the right ventral frontal lobe injury including the right frontotemporal connections. Journal Brain 1998; 121: 1951–73 Neurocase Reference Number: P823 Primary diagnosis of interest Isolated (focal) retrograde amnesia Author’s designation of case ML Key theoretical issue d Structural and functional neuroimaging as well as neuropsychological findings converge on the hypothesis that the clinical syndrome of isolated retrograde amnesia can be related to a right frontal lesion affecting the ability to re-experience past experiences. This effect can also extend across the time continuum into anterograde learning and self Key words: functional reorganization; amnesia; autonoetic awareness; MRI; PET Scan, EEG and related measures 1993: CT, MRI 1994/1995: SPECT, PET Standardized assessment Galveston Orientation and Amnesia Test, Wechsler Adult Intelligence ScaleRevised, National Adult Reading Test-Revised, Wechsler Memory ScaleRevised, Trail Making Test, Stroop Task, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Benton Visual Retention Test, Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test, Selective Reminding Test, Autobiographical Memory Interview, Crovitz Cueing Procedure, Six Elements Task, Boston Naming Test, Word List Learning, Verbal Fluency, Procedural Learning, Conditional Associative Learning, Concept Generation, Strategy Application Other assessment Anterograde cued recall and recognition/remember/knowing judgements, amytal interview d d d CT/1993: subdural haematoma along the falx and right tentorium, small left inferior posterior temporal contusions, small right frontal lobe contusions, mild diffuse oedema, small bifrontal hygromas MRI/1993: right ventral frontal cortex and underlying white matter (uncinate fasciculus, a frontotemporal band of fibres) SPECT/1994: left superior medial parietal hypoperfusion SPECT/1995: resolved PET/1994: normal Lesion type Severe closed head injury Language English 280 Previous cases: Isolated retrograde amnesia The syndrome of pure retrograde amnesia Transient retrograde amnesia associated with impaired naming of living categories F. Lucchelli, S. Muggia and H. Spinnler Abstract C. Papagno Three cases of pure retrograde amnesia (PRA) are reported (CDA, AF, GC; male; aged 15, 20, and 38 years old). The sudden onset of PRA occurred after minor head trauma in two cases (CDA and AF) and after emotional stress in one case (GC). Neuroimaging revealed no structural brain damage in any of the three patients. Evidence of a temporal gradient was found. A progressive recovery occurred in all cases. Issues concerning the definition of PRA are addressed. A unitary, aetiology-independent mechanism linking of heterogeneous triggering conditions to the isolated outcome of retrograde amnesia is suggested. Neuropsychological aspects of such mechanisms are envisaged. Abstract Journal A patient (CL; male; 26 years old) suffered from a pure transient retrograde amnesia for remembering personal and semantic memory contrasted with normal learning ability following a mild head injury. The patient showed a deficit in verbal fluency and naming with a living/non-living dissociation during the amnesic period and recovered 10 days after the incident. CT and MRI data gave no hints for brain damage, though EEG showed irritating components in the left temporal region. The simultaneous presence of naming impairment with living/non-living dissociation and impaired verbal fluency was discussed as a functional inhibition which involves also less ‘psychogenic’ aspects of mind. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 1998; 3: 91–118 Journal Neurocase Reference Number: Cortex 1998; 34: 111–21 P824 Neurocase Reference Number: Primary diagnosis of interest P825 Isolated (focal) retrograde amnesia Primary diagnosis of interest Author’s designation of case Isolated (focal) retrograde amnesia CDA, AF, GC Author’s designation of case Key theoretical issue CL d Pure retrograde amnesia after minor head trauma and emotional stress are presented in three cases. The contributions of PRA to neuropsychological research are discussed. In particular, the implicit versus explicit availability of past information, the architecture of remote memory archives, and the dissociation of anterograde and retrograde recall are considered Key theoretical issue d Key words: pure retrograde amnesia A case of transient retrograde amnesia following a mild head injury was described. The simultaneous presence of a naming impairment with living/ non-living dissociation and impaired verbal fluency gave support for a ‘functional’ inhibition which also involves less ‘psychogenic’ but more cognitive aspects of mind Scan, EEG and related measures Key words: retrograde amnesia; closed head injury CDA: EEG, CT, MRI GC: EEG, CT, MRI, SPECT AF: EEG, CT Scan, EEG and related measures Standardized assessment Standardized assessment CT, MRI, EEG CDA, CG, AF: Buschke Selective Reminding, Corsi Supra Span Learning, Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test, Verbal Span, Spatial Paired Associates CDA: Famous Events Questionnaire GC: Standardized Autobiographical Memory Questionnaire, Famous Events Questionnaire Raven’s Coloured Matrices, Token Test, Digit Span, Spatial Span, Story Recall, Word List Learning, Supra Span Spatial Learning, Verbal Fluency (phonological cues and categories), Picture Naming, Benton’s Line Orientation, Digit Cancellation Test, Semantic Memory Battery (living/non-living categories), Famous Faces Test, Famous Events Questionnaire Other assessment Other assessment CDA: autobiographical memory interview provided with the help of relatives and friends, psychiatric evaluation (several times) GC: psychiatric evaluation (brief) AF: informal assessment of autobiographical memory and semantic memory Lesion location Lesion location d d d CDA: EEG, CT, MRI: normal GC: EEG, CT, MRI, SPECT: normal AF: EEG, CT: normal Lesion type CDA: head trauma GC: none AF: head trauma Language English Psychiatric evaluation, structured autobiographical interview based on information provided by the mother d d EEG: left temporal irritations CT/MRI: normal Lesion type Mild head injury Language English Previous cases: Isolated retrograde amnesia 281 Focal retrograde amnesia associated with vascular headache Post-encephalitic focal retrograde amnesia after bilateral anterior temporal lobe damage I. Reinvang and L. Gjerstad Abstract A patient (DH; male; 42 years old) with repeated attacks of headache was initially affected by a dense retrograde amnesia for public and private facts and episodes comprising a period of 15–20 years. His anterograde memory was spared and neuropsychological assessment showed only mild deficits of other cognitive functions. There were hints for focal left frontotemporal changes in EEG. CT, MRI and SPECT were normal. In a post-incidental reassessment after 5 years he showed no neuropsychological deficits despite his persisting dense retrograde amnesia with no abnormalities in PET. Journal Neuropsychologia 1998; 36: 1335–41 Neurocase Reference Number: P826 Primary diagnosis of interest Isolated (focal) retrograde amnesia Author’s designation of case DH Abstract A patient (HK; female; 43 years old) showed a severe, persistent retrograde amnesia extending back to her childhood in contrast to very limited anterograde deficits following a herpes simplex encephalitis. MRI revealed bilateral damage of the temporal lobes and anterior inferotemporal lobes in the absence of frontal lobe pathologies. The personal memory was more severely impaired than the ability to recall factual knowledge about her past. Referring to her impairments in recalling public memories, her scores in forced-choice recognition tasks were nearly normal showing only mild impairment for the last decade before the incident. Journal Neurology 1999; 53: 344–50 Neurocase Reference Number: P827 Primary diagnosis of interest Isolated (focal) retrograde amnesia Key theoretical issue d Y. Tanaka, Y. Miyazawa, R. Hashimoto, I. Nakano and T. Obayashi A patient showed a persistent loss of retrograde memories for at least 15– 20 years following repeated attacks of headache. This was in contrast with his anterograde memory and learning capability Author’s designation of case HK Key theoretical issue d Key words: retrograde amnesia; vascular headache Scan, EEG and related measures 1990: 1991: 1992: 1995: 1996: 1997: EEG/CT MRI/SPECT EEG/MRI SPECT EEG/MRI PET Standardized assessment Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, subtests of the Wechsler Memory Scale: Designs, Paired Associate Learning, Paired Associate Recall; Digit Symbol Incidental Memory, California Verbal Learning Test, Kimura Recurring Figures, Trail Making Test, Verbal Fluency, Crovitz Cueing Procedure, Autobiographical Memory Interview, Famous Events and Famous Names Test, Grooved Pegs D/ND, Seashore Rhythm Test, Halstaed Category Test Other assessment A patient showed a persistent loss of retrograde memories extending back to her childhood following probable herpes simplex encephalitis. This profound retrograde amnesia in contrast with her anterograde memory was produced by damage of bilateral temporal lobes and anterior inferotemporal regions in the absence of frontal lobe pathology Key words: retrograde amnesia; encephalitis; temporal lobe; magnetic resonance imaging Scan, EEG and related measures EEG, CT, MRI Standardized assessment Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Modified Card Sorting Test, Trail Making Test, Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test, Autobiographical Memory Interview, Famous Faces Test, Public Events Test, TV Programs Test, Go/No Go Task, Stroop Test, Token Test Other assessment Psychiatric examination Personal photographs test (collection of private photographs with the help of the patient’s mother and husband), assessment of perceptual-motor skills Lesion location Lesion location d d EEG: focal theta activity in the left frontal temporal region CT, MRI, SPECT; PET: normal d Lesion type None Language English d d MRI: bilateral lesions of temporal regions. Left hemispherical lesions: temporal pole [Brodmann (BM) 38], part of anterior parahippocampal gyrus (BM 28), anterior parts of the inferior and middle temporal gyri (BM 20, 21), parts of amygdala and hippocampus and left insular cortex (extreme capsule, claustrum, external capsule, putamen, part of frontoparietal operculum, anterior superior temporal gyrus (BM 22). Right hemispherical lesions: less extensive damage of temporal pole (BM 38), part of anterior parahippocampal gyrus EEG: dysrhythmia with spikes in the left frontal and anterior temporal regions CT: normal Lesion type Herpes simplex encephalitis Language English 282 Previous cases: Isolated retrograde amnesia Isolated prolonged retrograde amnesia Y. Yoneda, A. Yamadori, E. Mori and H. Yamashita Abstract A patient (male; 21 years old) with isolated retrograde amnesia following encephalitis is reported. His memory deficit consisted of a loss of personal and public events 1 year prior to the incident while sparing anterograde memory performance. Functional neuroimaging (single-photon emission CT) revealed a left temporal lobe pathology. The authors assumed that the registration and storage of new information and the recall of events prior to a brain damage might involve different neural structures. It is hypothesized that recalling ability is mostly dependent on left temporal neocortical regions while intact regions of the medial temporal lobe memory system, such as hippocampal formation, and thalamic regions represent the anatomical basis of learning ability for new information. Journal European Neurology 1992; 32: 340–2 Neurocase Reference Number: P828 Primary diagnosis of interest Isolated (focal) retrograde amnesia Author’s designation of case Not stated Key theoretical issue d A dissociation between unimpaired anterograde memory performance and dense retrograde amnesia for 1 year is attributed to left temporal lobe dysfunction Key words: amnesia; retrograde; temporal lobe; single-photon emission computed tomography Scan, EEG and related measures EEG, CSF examination, CT, SPECT, MRI Standardized assessment Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Mini-Mental Status Test, Raven’s Progressive Matrices, Digit Span, Token Test, Miyake’s Retention Test, Auditory Verbal Learning Test, Benton Visual Retention Test, Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test Other assessment Autobiographical memory testing with personal information collected from family and colleagues, semantic remote memory testing with collected famous events, amytal interview Lesion location d d CT, MRI: normal SPECT: high tracer uptake in bilateral temporoparietal regions, predominantly on the left side; a second SPECT scan (2 months later) revealed low tracer uptake in the left posterior temporal lobe Lesion type Encephalitis Language English