* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download BA Philosophy/BA Sociology PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR
Carolyn Sherif wikipedia , lookup
Shelley E. Taylor wikipedia , lookup
Relational aggression wikipedia , lookup
Belongingness wikipedia , lookup
Social loafing wikipedia , lookup
Impression management wikipedia , lookup
Self-categorization theory wikipedia , lookup
Albert Bandura wikipedia , lookup
Social dilemma wikipedia , lookup
Attribution bias wikipedia , lookup
Impression formation wikipedia , lookup
Attitude change wikipedia , lookup
Social tuning wikipedia , lookup
Communication in small groups wikipedia , lookup
Group dynamics wikipedia , lookup
PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR COMPLEMENTARY COURSE For BA Philosophy/BA Sociology IV Semester – CUCBCSS 2014 Admission onwards UNIVERSITY OF CALICU T SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION Calicut unive rsity P.O, Malappuram Kerala, India 673 635. 884 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION UNIVERSITY OF CALICUT SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION STUDY MATERIAL PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR COMPLEMENTARY COURSE For BA Philosophy/BA Sociology IV Semester - CUCBCSS Prepared by: Sri. Eldhose N.J Research Scholar Dept. of Psychology University of Calicut Layout: Computer Section, SDE © Reserved PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 2 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION CONTENT PAGES Module - 2 13-45 Module - 3 46-76 Module - 1 PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR 5-12 Page 3 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION Objectives: To enable the student to 1.Understand and explain behaviour in the social setting 2.Explain the psychological aspects of various social phenomena 3.Understand the psychological aspect of various social issues in the society and Nation PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 4 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION Module 1 INTRODUCTION TO SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY Definition, Nature and Scope Social psychology is the scientific study of how people’s thoughts, feelings, and behaviours are influenced by the actual, imagined, or implied presence of others. By this definition, scientific refers to the empirical method of investigation. The terms thoughts, feelings, and behaviours include all of the psychological variables that are measurable in a human being. The statement that others may be imagined or implied suggests that we are prone to social influence even when no other people are present, such as when watching television, or following internalized cultural norms. Social psychology is abroad and complex field that focuses on how the individual’s behaviour (thoughts, feelings and actions) is influenced by other people. Some of the areas in Social Psychology include: Social influence, Social cognition, Social perception, Attitudes, Attribution, Prejudice, Stereotypes and discrimination, Aggression, Group processes, Altruism, Interpersonal attraction, pro social behaviour among others. Social psychology as a science seeks to understand the nature and causes of individual behaviour and thought in social situations. Social psychologists focus on the factors that help shape social behaviour and thought of individuals. They are interested in the actions, feelings, beliefs, values, memories, ideas about other people, the interpretation of behaviour as observed in social settings. Social psychologists typically explain human behaviour as a result of the interaction of mental states and immediate social situations. In Kurt Lewin's conceptual formula, behaviour can be viewed as a function of the person in the environment, B = f (P, E). In general, social psychologists have a preference for laboratory based, empirical findings. Social psychology theories tend to be specific and focused, rather than global and general. Social psychology is an interdisciplinary domain that bridges the gap between psychology and sociology. During the years immediately following World War II, there was frequent collaboration between psychologists and sociologists. However, the two disciplines have become increasingly specialized and isolated from each other in recent years, with sociologists focusing on "macro variables" (e.g. social structure) to a much greater extent. Nevertheless, sociological approaches to social psychology remain an important counterpart to psychological research in this area. In addition to the split between psychology and sociology, there has been a somewhat less pronounced difference in emphasis between American social psychologists and European social psychologists. As a broad generalization, American researchers traditionally have focused more on the individual, whereas Europeans have paid more attention to group level phenomena. History The discipline of social psychology began in the United States at the dawn of the 20th century. The first published study in this area was an experiment in 1898 by Norman Triplett on the phenomenon of social facilitation. During the 1930s, many Gestalt psychologists, most notably Kurt Lewin, fled to the United States from Nazi Germany. They were instrumental in developing the field as something separate from the behavioural and psychoanalytic schools that were dominant during that time, and social psychology has always maintained the legacy PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 5 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION of their interests in perception and cognition. Attitudes and small group phenomena were the most commonly studied topics in this era. During WWII, social psychologists studied persuasion and propaganda for the U.S. military. After the war, researchers became interested in a variety of social problems, including gender issues and racial prejudice. Most notable, revealing, and contentious of them all were the Stanley Milgram shock experiments on obedience to authority. In the sixties, there was growing interest in new topics, such as cognitive dissonance, bystander intervention, and aggression. By the 1970s, however, social psychology in America had reached a crisis. There was heated debate over the ethics of laboratory experimentation, whether or not attitudes really predicted behaviour, and how much science could be done in a cultural context (see Gergen, 1973). This was also the time when a radical situationist approach challenged the relevance of self and personality in psychology. Social psychology reached maturity in both theory and method during the 1980s and 1990s. Careful ethical standards now regulate research, and greater pluralism and multiculturalism perspectives have emerged. Modern researchers are interested in a many phenomenon, but attribution, social cognition, and the self-concept are perhaps the greatest areas of growth in recent years. Social psychologists have also maintained their applied interests with contributions in health and environmental psychology, as well as the psychology of the legal system. ATTITUDE Definition An attitude cannot be recorded directly. We cannot view someone’s tendency to like something in the way we can see physical attributes, such as eye colour or running speed. Another difficulty is that attitudes can be expressed through many behaviours. For e xample, a person who likes music might listen to it all the time, buy countless CDs, attend numerous music concerts, and buy several magazines about music. How does a researcher go from information about such a variety of behaviours to an estimate of the person’s fundamental attitude towards music? One general approach is to examine one or more specific behaviours that are seen as directly reflecting attitude. For example, a person who has a negative attitude towards a particular immigrant group is likely to seek more physical distance from members of that group, avoid eye contact with them, show unpleasant facial expressions, and so on. Another general approach employs self-report questionnaires, which ask participants to express their attitude towards the particular object. The most common version simply asks respondents to indicate their attitudes towards a named object using semantic-differential scales. So people might be asked to rate their attitude towards immigrants using a scale from −3 (extremely bad) to +3 (extremely good). Typically, though, people rate their attitude using several different scales, each labelled by different adjective pairs (negative/positive, worthless/valuable, unfavourable/ favourable). Responses to the scales are then averaged to form an attitude score for each participant. Of course, self-report measures can be affected by people’s desire to state socially desirable attitudes. So while our respondents above may reveal negative attitudes towards immigrants in their behaviour, their self-reports may appear more positive because they are reluctant to seem prejudiced. Contemporary research therefore frequently uses non-self-report measures in cases like this – i.e. when people’s ability to rate their attitudes accurately is questionable. Despite this weakness, self-report measures have predicted a variety of relevant behaviours in past PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 6 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION research, which suggests that we are at least somewhat accurate in reporting our own attitudes. Other measures elicit attitudes without relying on self-reports and without relying on overt behaviours towards the attitude object. For example, a common approach is to present the names of objects that people might like or dislike on a computer screen. Then the computer presents an adjective (e.g. terrible, pleasant) and respondents are asked to decide whether it means a good thing or bad thing. This question is easy to answer, and most people can answer correctly every time. Nonetheless, responses to adjectives with a positive meaning (e.g. delightful) tend to be faster after people have seen something they like than after seeing something they do not like, whereas responses to adjectives with a negative connotation (e.g. awful) tend to be slower after people have seen something they like than after seeing something they dislike. By contrasting the speed of responses to the positive and negative adjectives, researchers can obtain a measure of attitude that predicts behaviour towards an attitude object (Fazio et al., 1995). THE THREE COMPONENTS OF ATTITUDE An important feature of attitudes is their ability to sum up several types of psychological information. Consider an American who favours US membership in a global pact to reduce air pollution. Her positive attitude towards the pact may summarize relevant cognitions, emotions and behaviours. She may: believe that the pact will be good for the environment (cognition); feel excited when she hears plans for the pact (emotion); and sign a petition supporting the pact (behaviour). The three-component model of attitudes. 1. Effects of beliefs It could be argued that persuasive messages such as advertisements often change attitudes by changing people’s beliefs about the object of the message. For example, antismoking ads attempt to change people’s beliefs about the consequences of smoking, and those beliefs should in turn influence their attitude towards smoking. Consider a simple experiment in which Canadian participants received a booklet describing a study of a new immigrant group to Canada (Maio, Esses& Bell, 1994). The information in the booklet was manipulated to create positive and/or negative beliefs about the group. For example, some participants read that the immigrants scored above average on desirable personality traits (e.g. hardworking, honest), whereas other participants read that the group members scored below average on these traits. After reading the information, participants rated their attitudes towards the group. Not surprisingly, the results indicated that those who received positive information indicated more favourable attitudes towards the immigrant group than those who received negative information. This simple demonstration is important from a practical perspective, because it demonstrates how even second-hand information about others can have a powerful effect on our attitudes towards them. When prejudice has arisen largely from indirect information, interventions encourage direct, positive interactions to change beliefs and reduce the prejudice. 2. Effects of feelings If you look carefully at advertisements, you will find that many give very little information about the objects they are promoting. For example, an advertisement for a Citroen car shows supermodel Claudia Schiffer smiling and undressing on her way to the car, PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 7 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION while upbeat music plays in the background. Rather than focusing on concrete information (e.g. performance, fuel economy), ads like this work by linking the product with positive feelings. 3. Effects of behaviour Initiation rituals have often been prerequisites for acceptance into social groups, such as military squads and college fraternities and sororities. Would-be new members may be asked to perform embarrassing acts, such as streaking nude at a public event or dressing in a strange costume during classes. Why do new recruits not leave a group after enduring such ordeals? One possible explanation is that the behaviour of submission to group rules leads to more positive attitudes towards the group. In other words, the new recruit’s behaviour affects his attitudes. For many decades, the general effect of behaviour on attitudes has captured a great deal of interest. Researchers first began to notice an interesting effect arising from role playing. For example, participants assigned to play the role of a person diagnosed with terminal lung cancer later reported more negative attitudes towards smoking than those who had listened to an audiotape of the roleplay (Janis & Mann, 1965). Similarly, people assigned to debate a particular position on an issue such as legalized abortion subsequently express a more favourable attitude towards the position they have been required to advocate (e.g. Janis & King, 1954). People who merely listen to the participants’ arguments do not show so much attitude change. Something about the role-playing behaviour drives the change. What if the role-playing task explicitly requires counter-attitudinal advocacy – presenting an attitude or opinion that opposes the person’s previous attitude? Suppose university students are asked to write an essay arguing for increased tuition fees – a position that obviously contradicts most students’ feelings on this issue. Amazingly, they still tend to change their attitudes towards the position they have advocated (see Cooper & Fazio, 1984; Harmon-Jones & Mills, 1999). Another interesting finding is that this attitude change is more likely when participants are given only a small incentive to argue the counter attitudinal position than when they are given a large incentive. Several theories help to explain this effect (e.g. Schlenker, 1982; Steele, 1988), but two are particularly prominent. On the one hand, cognitive dissonance theory suggests that a small incentive makes people feel guilt or tension from having acted, behaviourally, against their original attitude without sufficient reason. To reduce their discomfort, they change their attitude (Festinger, 1957). This idea has also been used to explain the effects of initiation rituals. On the other hand, self-perception theory suggests that small incentives cause people to assume that their attitude must actually match the position they have advocated (Bem, 1972), because they can see no external reasons why they performed the behaviour. Current evidence suggests that both theories have some validity. Apparently, cognitive dissonance processes may occur when people perform a behaviour that strongly contradicts their initial attitude (like the tuition fees example), whereas selfperception processes may occur when people perform a behaviour that is not so strongly contradictory (Fazio, Zanna& Cooper, 1977). How Do Attitudes Influence Behaviour? Ever since the beginning of attitude research, investigators have puzzled over the relation between attitudes and behaviour. Why do people sometimes say they like something and then act as if they do not? Are these instances much less frequent than instances where the attitude and behaviour match perfectly? PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 8 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION Measuring the attitude–behaviour link Researchers were intrigued by the results of some early research that revealed very weak relations between attitudes and behaviour. In one study (LaPiere, 1934), a researcher and a young Chinese couple travelled around the Western portion of the US, visiting 250 restaurants, inns and hotels. Despite widespread American prejudice against Chinese people at that time, the researcher and his visitors were refused service at only one of the establishments. Yet, when he later wrote to these establishments requesting permission to visit with ‘a young Chinese gentleman and his wife’, 92 per cent refused permission! These refusals are often interpreted as indicators of negative attitudes towards Chinese people. Viewed this way, they provide some of the earliest evidence that people’s behaviours (in this case, accepting the Chinese couple) can fail to match their attitudes towards the behaviour (i.e. their desire to refuse permission). This raised some doubts about the ability of attitudes to predict behaviours. There were many methodological limitations to LaPiere’s study, however (Campbell, 1963). For example: the attitude and behaviour were measured at different times and locations; n the attitude measure itself was, at best, indirect (LaPiere did not ask the restaurant owners to complete an attitude scale); n the young couple may have looked more pleasant than the proprietors had imagined; n the proprietors may have followed the norm of hospitality to guests once they entered the restaurant; and n the situation in which behaviour was measured may simply have made it too difficult for most pro prietors to refuse the Chinese couple, because of the embarrassing scene that might ensue. Subsequent studies used more stringent procedures (see Wicker, 1969). Using a correlational technique, these studies tested whether people with positive attitudes towards a particular object exhibit more favourable behaviour towards the object than do people with negative attitudes towards the object. Even so, until 1962, researchers still found only weak relations between attitudes and behaviour. The consistent failure to find strong attitude– behaviour correlations led researchers to search for explanations. Fishbein and Ajzen (1975) pointed out that past research often failed to measure a behaviour that directly corresponded to the attitude being measured. For example, suppose we measure the relation between (a) attitudes towards protecting the environment and (b) using a recycling facility in a particular week. Even if someone is a strong environmentalist, there are many reasons why they might fail to recycle in a particular week (lack of a nearby facility, lack of time to sort recyclables, and so on). The problem is that the measured behaviour (recycling in a particular week) is very specific, whereas the attitude object (protecting the environment) is much more general. To better measure ‘general’ behaviour, Fishbein and Ajzen (1975) proposed the multiple act criterion, which involves measuring a large number of behaviours that are relevant to the general attitude being studied. For example, to measure pro-environment behaviour, we could measure numerous pro-environment behaviours, including recycling across several weeks, willingness to sign pro-environment petitions and tendency to pick up litter. This would give us a more precise and reliable measure of behaviour. Weigel and Newman (1976) did just this and found much stronger attitude–behaviour relations by taking an average measure of all of the behaviours, rather than any single behaviour. PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 9 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION Predicting behaviour Behaviour is normally influenced by more than attitudes alone. For instance, as we discussed previously, the behaviour of people towards the Chinese couple in LaPiere’s study would also have been influenced by social norms – the socially prescribed ways of behaving in a situation (Campbell, 1963). Ajzen (1991) developed a model of attitude–behaviour relations that recognized the impact of social norms. According to this theory of planned behaviour (figure 17.7), actual behaviour is influenced by behavioural intentions – intentions to perform or not to perform the behaviour (see also chapter 19). These intentions, in turn, are influenced by: the attitude towards the behaviour – the individual’s evaluations of the positive and negative consequences of performing the behaviour; n the subjective norms regarding the behaviour – the individual’s desire to behave in the same way as people who are important to him think he should behave; and n perceived control over performance of the behaviour – the extent to which the individual believes he can control whether he performs the behaviour. According to the theory, when attitudes and subjective norms support a target behaviour and perceived control over the performance of the behaviour is high, intentions to perform the behaviour should be stronger. People who form strong intentions should be more likely to perform the behaviour. Abundant research has supported these predictions (see Conner & Armitage, 1998), while also making it clear that the theory neglects several additional important predictors of behaviour – such as a sense of moral obligation to perform the target behaviour (Schwartz, 1977) and the pattern of the individual’s past behaviour in similar situations (Ouellette & Wood, 1998). Accessible vs. inaccessible attitudes According to Fazio (1990), attitudes often influence behaviour through a spontaneous process. Effects of attitudes can occur quickly, but only for people whose attitude is accessible (easy to retrieve). When attitudes are accessible, they come to mind instantly when we see the attitude object. The attitude then influences how we behave towards the object. If the attitude is less accessible, it doesn’t come to mind, and so it doesn’t influence our behaviour. For example, suppose you are walking by an ice cream seller. You may spontaneously recall your passion for ice cream, and this attitude may motivate a decision to buy some. But if you don’t spontaneously recall your attitude (because it is inaccessible – perhaps you are distracted by a more pressing thought at the time you walk past the ice cream seller), it will lie dormant and not elicit the decision to buy. Indeed, there’s a great deal of evidence that attitudes do exert a stronger influence on behaviour when they are accessible than when they are diffi cult to retrieve (Fazio, 2000) FORMING AND CHANGING ATTITUDES Incentive for change To understand how attitudes can be changed, it is first important to understand attitude functions – the psychological needs that attitudes fulfil (Maio& Olson, 2000). Early theories proposed a number of important attitude functions (table 17.1). For example, people may have a positive attitude towards objects that help them become popular among people they like, but not objects that make them estranged from those people. This is the social adjustment function, which provides the basis for the entire fashion industry: people tend to like clothing that is popular among people they like. In the earliest model of attitude change, Hovland, Janis and Kelley (1953) suggested that persuasive messages change people’s attitudes when they highlight some incentive for this change. For example, an advertisement PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 10 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION might describe the utilitarian benefits of buying a particular model of car (e.g. good fuel economy) or the social-adjustment benefits (e.g. a sporty look). The incentives must seem important if the message recipients are to change their attitude. Hovland et al.’s theory also suggests that processing of any message must occur in stages if it is to be successful. The intended audience must: 1. pay attention to the message, 2. comprehend the message, and 3. accept the message’s conclusions. McGuire (1969) extended this theory further. According to his model, a message will elicit the desired behaviour only if it succeeds at six stages (figure 17.8). People must: 1. encounter the message (presentation stage); 2. attend to it (attention stage); 3. understand it (comprehension stage); 4. change their attitude (yielding stage); 5. remember their new attitude at a later time (retention stage); and 6. the new attitude must influence their behaviour (behaviour stage). Interestingly, even if the odds of passing each stage are quite good, the chances of completing all the stages can be low. For example, we might optimistically assume that a Nike running shoe ad has an 80 per cent chance of success at each stage. If this were the case, the laws of probability indicate that the odds of successfully completing all of the stages would be only 0.26 (0.8 × 0.8 × 0.8 × 0.8 × 0.8 × 0.8). In other words, the ad would have a 26 per cent chance of getting someone to buy the running shoes. In reality, the odds of completion of each stage (especially yielding and behaviour) may be far lower, creating even lower chances of success (possibly less than 1 per cent). For this reason, modern marketing initiatives take steps to compel completion of each stage, where this is possible. So advertisers will present the message many times, make it attention-grabbing and memorable, and make the message content as powerful as they can. Motivation and ability Two newer models of persuasion, the ‘elaboration likelihood model’ (Petty &Cacioppo, 1986) and the ‘heuristic–systematic model’ (Chaiken, Liberman&Eagly, 1989), predict that the effects of persuasive messages depend on people’s motivation and ability to think carefully about them. If someone is highly motivated and able to process a persuasive message, they should be heavilyinfluenced by the strength of the arguments in the message. But if they are less motivated or able to process the message, then they should be strongly affected by simple cues within the message, such as the presenter’s attractiveness or expertise. Many variables influence motivation and ability. Motivation is high when the message is relevant to personal goals and there is a fear of being wrong. Ability is high when people are not distracted and when they possess high cognitive skills. Although all of these variables have been studied in connection with both models of persuasion, most of this research has focused on the personal relevance of the message. For example, Petty et al. (1983) found that the attractiveness of the spokesperson presenting a message influences PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 11 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION attitudes when the issue is not personally relevant, but has no effect when the issue is personally relevant. In contrast, the strength of the argument within the message in fluences attitudes when the issue is personally relevant, but not when the issue is not personally relevant. These findings support the predictions of the elaboration likelihood model and the heuristic–systematic model. Although many experiments have revealed similar effects, the heuristic–systematic model suggests that high personal relevance should not always lead to the lower use of cues such as the presenter’s attributes. For example, when a personally relevant message contains ambiguous arguments (i.e. it has strengths and weaknesses), people may be more persuaded by a message from an expert source than from an inexpert source (Chaiken&Maheswaran, 1994). According to this model, high personal relevance causes people to use environmental cues when the message arguments themselves provide no clear conclusions. This prediction has received some experimental support (Chaiken&Maheswaran, 1994). PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 12 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION Module 2 Social perception-Nonverbal communication,Attribution,Impression formation and impression management. What is social perception? By social perception, we mean those processes whereby an individual makes sense of and interprets the nature of the other people involved in the conversation, and the nature of the setting in which they find themselves. Ifthat sounds rather a mouthful then we can easily illustrate the importance of social perception with a few examples: In one very famous experiment, Harold Kelley provided a group of university students with a short written description of a visiting lecturer just before he lectured to them for the first time. Unbeknown to the students, two forms of the description were distributed at random. The only difference between the two forms was that the phrase "very warm" was used to describe the lecturer on one version, and the phrase "rather cold" was used on the other. So each student read a description of Mr X like this: Mr X is a graduate student in the Department of Economics and Social Science here at M.I.T. He has had three semesters of teaching experience in psychology at another college. This is his first semester teaching Ec.70. He is 26 years old, a veteran, and married. People who know him consider him to be a rather cold (or, "very warm") person, industrious, critical, practical, and determined. After the class (which included a discussion session lasting about 20 minutes), Kelley asked the students to rate the lecturer. There were marked differences in these ratings depending on which prior description the student had read. "Warm" students saw the lecturer as successful, popular, happy, humorous etc. "Cold" students saw the lecturer as stingy, unsuccessful, unpopular and unhappy. There was also a marked difference in class participation. Fifty-six percent of the "warm" students took part in the discussion; only 32 per cent of the "cold" students did so. This experiment suggests that there is some truth in the statement that we see what we expect to see. The students gained their initial impression from the written description and seemed to stick to it regardless of the evidence available to them. They also behaved in accordance with what they thought was true rather than actual events. This behaviour then reinforced their initial impression. If you participate in a discussion then you're liable to see the leader more positively than if you sit aloof simply because you've received some reactions from him. This is an example of a "self-fulfilling prophecy" - someone is "labelled" in a particular way; this makes other people expect that person to behave in specific ways; these other people then behave to the labelled person on the basis of their expectations; the person reacts and probably lives up to the expectations. An example may make this clearer. Suppose a new pupil arrives at a school after a rumour that he is a "trouble-maker". The other pupils and teachers will expect him to live up to this reputation and may well greet him in a suspicious or hostile way. The newcomer reacts to what he see as a hostile welcome, possibly by retaliating in a hostile way, and the "prophecy" has come true. Of course, labels can also be positive but the process will be the same. There is some evidence that selffulfilling prophecies can have long-term effects. Unfortunately Kelley's experiment only looked at a fairly short event. The class only lasted twenty minutes. What would have PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 13 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION happened to the students' perceptions if the class had lasted longer, or if they had seen the lecturer again on a number of occasions? The spectators' example Another classic experiment within social psychology studied the perceptions of spectators after a particularly rough game of American football between Dartmouth and Princeton. The investigators asked spectators who was responsible for the rough play. If you have ever been involved inteam sports you will probably not be surprised to learn that the supporters' perceptions were consistently different. For example, only 36 per cent of the Dartmouth students thought that their team had started the rough play whereas 86 per cent of the Princeton students thought that the Dartmouth team had. Social psychology traditionally has been defined as the study of the ways in which people affect, and are affected by, others. Communication is one of the primary means by whichpeople affect one another, and, in light of this, one might expect the study of communication to be a core topic of social psychology, but historically that has not been the case. No doubt there are many reasons. Among them is the fact that communication is a complex and multidisciplinary concept, and, across the several disciplines that use the term, there is no consensus on exactly how it should be defined. It is an important theoretical construct in such otherwise dissimilar fields as cell biology, computer science, ethology, linguistics, electrical engineering, sociology, anthropology, genetics, philosophy, semiotics, and literary theory. And although there is a core of meaning common to the way the term is used in these disciplines, the particularities differ enormously. What cell biologists call communication bears little resemblance to what anthropologists study under the same rubric. A concept used in so many different ways runs the risk of becoming an amorphous catch-all term lacking precise meaning, and that already may have happened to communication. As the sociologist Thomas Luckmannhas observed, "Communication has come to mean all things to all men" (Luckmann, 1993, p.68). Elements of nonverbal communication Paralanguage: Besides the actual words, the way a person says something can convey a lot of information. Such elements of paralanguage like inflection, pitch, emphasis placed, pauses and speed can add volumes of meaning to a message. Even elements like laughing and sighing can help in interpreting the actual meaning of a verbal message, as they provide information about the emotional context. Body language: One of the most important elements of body language is facial gestures, most of which are universally recognized. People in different cultures demonstrate similar facial expressions in similar situations, and can recognize the underlying emotion in faces from other cultures. Eye-contact is another important factor that provides information about the emotional state of a person. Posture and gestures as well as movement also convey a lot of information. The way a person stands, holds themselves in different situations and the way they walk can convey information about mood and confidence. Interpersonal space: Proxemics is the study of how people use space to communicate. Typically, people who want to express agreement or intimacy will stand closer, lean in and basically reduce the space between themselves and the receiver of the communication. On the other hand, when people wish to demonstrate disagreement and social distance will also add physical distance to their interaction. Even when PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 14 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION people are is a public space like a bus, and cannot control the physical space between themselves and others, they try to control the social distance by averting their gaze or looking at a neutral object. Personal effects: People use clothing and accessories to not only manage impressions, but also to send a message about who they are. The way a person dresses and the objects they carry with them are often used to convey messages about who they are and how they perceive themselves to others. Nonverbal behavior as nonverbal communication Much of what social psychologists think about nonverbal behaviour derives from a proposal made more than a century ago by Charles Darwin. In The expression of the emotions in man and animals(Darwin,1872), he posed the question: Why do our facial expressions of emotions take the particular formsthey do? Why do we wrinkle our nose when we are disgusted, bare our teethand narrow our eyes when enraged, and stare wide-eyed when we are transfixed by fear? Darwin's answer was that we do these things primarily because they are vestiges of serviceable associated habits - behaviours that earlier in our evolutionary history had specific and direct functions. For a species that attacked by biting, baring the teeth was a necessary prelude to an assault; wrinkling the nose reduced the inhalation of foul odors; and so forth. But if facial expressions reflect formerly functional behaviors, why have they persisted when they no longer serve their original purposes? Why do people bare their teeth when they are angry, despite the fact that biting is not part of their aggressive repertoire? Why do they wrinkle their noses when their disgust is engendered by an odorless picture? According to Darwin's intellectual heirs, the behavioural ethologists (e.g., Hinde, 1972; Tinbergen, 1952), humans do these things because over the course of their evolutionary history such behaviors have acquired communicative value: they provide others with external evidence of an individual's internal state. The utility of such information generated evolutionary pressure to select sign behaviors, thereby schematizing them and, in Tinbergen's phrase, "emancipating them" from their original biological function.1 Noncommunicative functions of nonverbal behaviours So pervasive has been social psychologists' preoccupation with the communicative or expressive aspects of nonverbal behaviours that the terms nonverbal behaviour and nonverbal communication have tended to be used interchangeably. Recently, however, it has been suggested that this communicative focus has led social psychologists to overlook other functions such behaviours serve. For example, Zajonc contends that psychologists have been too quick to accept the idea that facial expression are primarily expressive behaviours. According to his "vascular theory of emotional efference" (Zajonc, 1985; Zajonc, Murphy, &Inglehart, 1989) , the actions of the facial musculature that produce facial expressions of emotions serve to restrict venous flow, thereby impeding or facilitating the cooling of cerebral blood as it enters the brain. The resulting variations in cerebral temperature, Zajonc hypothesizes, promote or inhibit the release of emotion-linked neurotransmitters, which, in turn, affect subjective emotional experience. From this perspective, facial expressions do convey information about the individual's emotional state, but they do so as an indirect consequence of their primary, noncommunicative function. PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 15 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION An analogous argument has been made for the role of gaze direction in social interaction. As people speak, their gaze periodically fluctuates toward andaway from their conversational partner. Some investigators have interpretedgaze directed at a conversational partner as an expression of intimacy or closeness (cf., Argyle & Cook, 1976; Exline, 1972; Exline, Gray, &Schuette, 1985; Russo, 1975). However, Butterworth (1978) argues that gaze direction is affected by two complex tasks speakers must manage concurrently: planning speech, and monitoring the listener for visible indications of comprehension, onfusion, agreement, interest, etc. (Brunner, 1979; Duncan, Brunner, & Fiske, 1979). When the cognitive demands of speech planning are great, Butterwort argues, speakers avert gaze to reduce visual information input, and, when those demands moderate, they redirect their gaze toward the listener, especially at places where feedback would be useful. Studies of the points in the speech stream at which changes in gaze direction occur, and of the effects of restricting changes in gaze direction (Beattie, 1978; Beattie, 1981; Cegala, Alexander, Sokuvitz, 1979), tend to support Butterworth's conjecture. Interpersonal and intrapersonal functions of nonverbal behaviours Of course, nonverbal behaviours can serve multiple functions. Facial expression may play a role in affective experience-by modulating vascular blood flow as Zajonc has proposed or through facial feedback as has been suggested by Tomkins and others (Tomkins & McCarter, 1964)-and at the same time convey information about the expressor's emotional state. Such communicative effects could involve two rather different mechanisms. In the first place, many nonverbal behaviours are to some extent under the individual's control, and can be produced voluntarily. For example, although a smile may be a normal accompaniment of an affectively positive internal state, it can at least to some degree be produced at will. Social norms, called "display rules," dictate that one exhibit at least a moderately pleased expression on certain social occasions. For example, the recent book edited by Feldman andRimé (1991) reviewing research in this area is titled Fundamentals of Nonverbal Behaviour, despite the fact that all of the nonverbal behaviours are discussed in terms of the role they play in communication (see Krauss(1993) . Kraut (1979) found that the attention of others greatly potentiates smiling in situations that can be expected to induce a positive internal state. In the second place, nonverbal behaviours that serve noncommunicative functions can provide information about the noncommunicative functions they serve. For example, ifButterworth is correct about the reason speakers avert gaze, an excessive amount of gaze aversion may lead a listener to infer that the speaker is having difficulty formulating the message. Conversely, the failure to avert gaze atcertain junctures, combined with speech that is overly fluent, may lead anobserver to infer that the utterance is not spontaneous. Viewed in this fashion, we can distinguish between interpersonal and intrapersonal functions that nonverbal behaviours serve. The interpersonal functions involve information such behaviours convey to others, regardless of whether they are employed intentionally (like the facial emblem) or serve as the basis of an inference the listener makes about the speaker (like dysfluency). The intrapersonal functions involve noncommunative purposes the behavioursserve. The premise of this chapter is that the primary function of conversational hand gestures (unplanned, articulate hand movements that accompany spontaneous speech) is not communicative, but rather to aid in the formulation of speech. It is our contention that the information they convey to an addressee is largely derivative from this primary function. PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 16 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION GESTURES AS NONVERBAL BEHAVIORS A typology of gestures All hand gestures are hand movements, but not all hand movements aregestures, and it is useful to draw some distinctions among the types of hanmovements people make. Although gestural typologies abound in theliterature, there is little agreement among researchers about the sorts ofdistinctions that are necessary or useful. Following a suggestion by Kendon(1983), we have found it helpful to think of the different types of handmovements that accompany speech as arranged on a continuum oflexicalization—the extent to whch they are "word-like." Adapters At the low lexicalization end of the continuum are hand movements that tend not to be considered gestures. They consist of manipulations either of the person or of some object (e.g., clothing, pencils, eye glasses)-the kinds ofscratching, fidgeting, rubbing, tapping, and touching that speakers often do with their hands. Such behaviors are most frequently referred to as adapters (Efron, 1941/1972; Ekman & Friesen, 1969b; Ekman & Friesen, 1972) . Other terms that have been used are expressive movements (Reuschert, 1909) , body-focused movements (Freedman & Hoffman, 1967) , self-touching gestures (Kimura, 1976) , manipulative gestures(Edelman &Hampson, 1979) , self-manipulators (Rosenfeld 1966, and contact acts(Bull & Connelly, 1985) . Adapters are not gestures as that term is usually understood. They are not perceived as communicativelintended, nor are they perceived to be meaningfully related to the speech theyaccompany, although they may serve as the basis for dispositional inferences (e.g., that the speaker is nervous, uncomfortable, bored, etc.). It has been suggested that adapters may reveal unconscious thoughts or feelings (Mahl, 1956; Mahl, 1968) , or thoughts and feelings that the speaker is trying consciously to conceal (Ekman & Friesen, 1969a; Ekman & Friesen, 1974) , but little systematic research has been directed to this issue. Symbolic gestures At the opposite end of the lexicalization continuum are gestural signs-hand configurations and movements with specific, conventionalized meanings-that we will call symbolic gestures (Ricci Bitti&Poggi, 1991). Other terms that have been used are emblems (Efron, 1941/1972), autonomous gestures(Kendon, 1983), conventionalized signs(Reuschert, 1909), formal pantomimic gestures (Wiener, Devoe, Rubinow, & Geller, 1972), expressive gestures (Zinober&Martlew, 1985), and semiotic gestures (Barakat, 1973). Familiar symbolic gestures include the "raised fist," "bye-bye," "thumbs-up," and the extended middle finger sometimes called "flipping the bird." In contrast to adapters, symbolic gestures are used intentionally and serve a clear communicative function. Every culture has a set of symbolic gestures familiar to most of its adult members, and very similar gestures may have different meanings in different cultures (Ekman, 1976) Subcultural and occupational groups also may have special symbolic gesture that are not widely known outside the group. Although symbolic gestures often are used in the absence of speech, they occasionally accompany speech, either echoing a spoken word or phrase or substituting for something that was not said. PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 17 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION Conversational gestures The properties of the hand movements that fall at the two extremes of thecontinuum are relatively uncontroversial. However there is considerabledisagreement about movements that occupy the middle part of the lexicalizationcontinuum, movements that are neither as word-like as symbolic gestures nor asdevoid of meaning as adapters. We refer to this heterogeneous set of handmovements as conversational gestures. They also have been called illustrators(Ekman & Friesen, 1969b; Ekman & Friesen, 1972, gesticulations (Kendon, 1980;Kendon, 1983) , and signifying signs (Reuschert, 1909) . Conversational gesturesare hand movements that accompany speech, and seem related to the speechthey accompany. This apparent relatedness is manifest in three ways: First,unlike symbolic gestures, conversational gestures don't occur in the absence ofspeech, and in conversation are made only by the person who is speaking.Second, conversational gestures are temporally coordinated with speech. Andthird, unlike adapters, at least some conversational gestures seem related in formto the semantic content of the speech they accompany. Different types of conversational gestures can be distinguished, and a variety of classification schemes have been proposed (Ekman & Friesen, 1972;Feyereisen&deLannoy, 1991; Hadar, 1989a; McNeill, 1985). We find it useful to distinguish between two major types that differ importantly in form and, we believe, in function. Motor movements One type of conversational gesture consists of simple, repetitive, rhythmicmovements, that bear no obvious relation to the semantic content of the accompanying speech (Feyereisen, Van de Wiele, & Dubois, 1988) . Typically the hand shape remains fixed during the gesture, which may be repeated several times. We will follow Hadar (1989a; Hadar&Yadlin-Gedassy, 1994) in referring to such gestures as motor movements; they also have been called “batons” (Efron, 1941/1972; Ekman & Friesen, 1972 and “beats” (Kendon, 1983; McNeill, 1987). Motor movements are reported to be coordinated with the speech prosody and to fall on stressed syllables (Bull & Connelly, 1985; but see McClave, 1994) ,although the synchrony is far from perfect. Lexical movements The other main category of conversational gesture consists of handmovements that vary considerably in length, are nonrepetitive, complex and changing in form, and, to a naive observer at least, appear related to the semantic content of the speech they accompany. Importance of Nonverbal communication Often, people choose not to provide a lot of information through verbal communication; and on occasion, this may be done with the express purpose of misinforming someone. Being able to read nonverbal communication can help identify the true intent of an individual, as it is extremely difficult to control all the cues simultaneously while continuing to deliver a false message. People don’t need to be trained to read nonverbal cues. Most people will pay attention to and process the information they contain without even realizing it. Sometimes people are aware of what they read from nonverbal communication, particularly in important situations. A recent study found that patients will pay attention to PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 18 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION doctors’ nonverbal cues like expressions, tone of voice and eye contact. Some more perceptive patients also attend to other cues like distance, touch, and posture; and these cues are then used in evaluating the value of the doctor’s message as well as the doctor him/herself. Thus, if the doctor is leaning in and smiling while looking into the patients eyes, the patient is more likely to trust the doctor. ATTRIBUTION Early Theories of Attribution We are all interested in understanding our own behaviour and the behaviours of others as we interact. Knowing the reasons why we act the way we do or say what we say will help us understand behaviour better. The process of attribution is the key to explaining the cause of events or behaviours. In attribution, we try to give reasons, explain or make judgment about the causes of events or behaviours. These reasons are mostly attributed to either external or internal causes. We will look at the various theories that explain the causes of our behaviour and the behaviours of others. Jones and Davis Correspondent Inferences theory (CIT). This theory suggests that the target of any effective attribution is the ability to make inferences that correspond or are in line with the behaviour, that is, the intention of the behaviour and the underlying disposition of the actor. Both the behaviour and the disposition must be seen and recognized as similar. The behaviour should be made to occur by the actor with no external influence or instructions to do so; this will enable one to make a corresponding inference. How can we make Inferences? 1. Analyzing uncommon effects We can infer that intended behaviour agrees with some underlying disposition by analysing uncommon effects. This means looking at what is distinct about the effect of the choice made. Once we have many options and decide on one, then we can co mparethe consequences of the chosen option to the consequences of the other option not chosen. Then what is common about the effect of the choice becomes very important. If there are fewer differences between these comparisons, then we can infer dispositions with confidence. Also the more negative consequences with the chosen option, the more likely we are to attach some importance to the distinctive consequences. 2. We can also look at the actor’s choice. Was this choice or behaviour influenced by situational or internal (free will) factors? 3. We can also make inferences by concentrating on the socialdesirability of the behaviour. Once there is a deviation from what isdesired or accepted, this behaviour catches our attention and hastens our impression formation because of the distinctiveness of the behaviour. We are likely not going to engage in undesirable behaviour that will put us in bad standing with others. 4. We could infer our behaviour based on the desirability of the behaviour being observed. The use of Roles – these are well defined roles that people tend to conform to, if done well their underlying dispositions might not be evident, but if these roles are broken and the PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 19 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION actor deviates from them, it is most likely that the actor’s underlying disposition will be revealed and corresponding inferences about his behaviour will be made. Prior expectations based on past experiences with the actor could also help us to decide if present behaviour is in line with other behaviours of the actors. Having the past and present information, will help us decide if present information will become less important or more important depending on whether it is similar or different from past behaviours. Weiner’s Attribution Theories of Emotion and Motivation. This theory applies the basic principle of attributionto emotions and motivation. The theory posits that our motions and motivation are affected by the attributions we make. This theory puts forth three dimensions of casualty namely; Locus, stabilityand controllability. The locus dimension has it that causes of events or behaviours can be internal or external (person/situation). The stability dimension believes that causes of events or behaviours can be permanent or temporary while the controllability dimension sees causes of eventsor behaviours in terms of their being either controllable or uncontrollable. (Weiner 1986) This theorybelieves causes are multidimensional and a combination of causes could result in emotions like anger, feelings of disappointment, anxiety, or depression which in turn are likely to affect motivation. The above narrations have related the causes of behaviour using the attribution process. Knowing these causes is very important in understanding the Why of behaviour. The general view that the process of attribution involves an actor and an observer and both must come to play during interactions is worth paying attention to. Each theory to some extent, accounts for the various ways we make attributions in the face of information or even limited information. We said earlier that attributions are explanations for events and behaviour. Heider differentiated between two types of causal attribution – personal and situational. Personal attributions refer to factors within the person, such as their personality characteristics, motivation, ability and effort. Situational attributions refer to factors within the environment that are external to the person. For example, if we were discussing why a particular student has failed an important university examination, we would consider personal factors (such as her academic ability and how much effort she invested in preparing for the exam). But we might also look at situational attributions (such as whether she had good tuition, access to library facilities and sufficient time to study). Heider noted that we tend to overestimate internal or personal factors and underestimate situational factors when explaining behaviour. This tendency has become known as the fundamental attribution error, which we’ll return to in the next section. In a similar vein, Jones and Davis (1965) found that we tend to make a correspondent inference about another person when we are looking for the cause of their behaviour. In other words, we tend to infer that the behaviour, and the intention that produced it, correspond to some underlying stable quality. For example, a correspondent inference would be to attribute someone’s aggressive behaviour to an internal and stable trait within the person – in this case, aggressiveness. Jones and Davis argued that this tendency is motivated by our need to view people’s behaviour as intentional and predictable, reflecting their underlying personality traits. PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 20 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION But in reality, making correspondent inferences is not always a straightforward business. The information we need in order to make the inferences can be ambiguous, requiring us to draw on additional cues in the environment, such as the social desirability of the behaviour, how much choice the person had, or role requirements. Like Heider, Kelley (1967) likened ordinary onlookers to naive scientists who weigh up several factors when attributing causality. Kelley’s covariation model of attribution states that, before two events can be accepted as causally linked, they must co-occur. The covariation of events and behaviour was assessed across three important dimensions: 1. Consistency – does the person respond in the same way to the same stimuli over time? 2. Distinctiveness – do they behave in the same way to other different stimuli, or is the behaviour distinctively linked to specific stimuli? 3. Consensus – do observers of the same stimuli respond in a similar way? Kelley argued that we systematically analyse people- and environment-related information, and that different combinations of information lead to different causal attributions. For example, while attributing causality for behaviour like ‘John laughed at the comedian’, we would run through the following considerations: 1. If John always laughs at this comedian, then his behaviour is highly consistent. 2. If John is easily amused by comedians, then his behaviour has low distinctiveness. 3. If practically no one else in the audience laughed at the comedian, then his behaviour has low consensus. A combination of high consistency, low distinctiveness and low consensus would lead to a dispositional (internal) attribution for John’s laughter, such as ‘John has a peculiar tendency to laugh at all comedians; he must be very easily amused.’ In contrast, a combination of high consistency, high distinctiveness and high consensus would lead to an external attribution, such as ‘John likes this comedian, but he doesn’t like many other comedians, and other people like this comedian too; this comedian must be funny’ (McArthur, 1972). THE EFFECTS OF BIAS Both the Jones–Davis and the Kelley models of attribution view the social perceiver as a rational person who uses logical principles of thinking when attributing causality. But empirical research has discovered persistent biases in the attributional processes. According to Fiske and Taylor (1991), bias occurs if the social perceiver systematically distorts (overuses or under-uses) what are thought to be correct and logical procedures. We will now look in more detail at four of the most pervasive biases: the fundamental attribution error, the actor–observer effect, the selfserving bias and the ultimate attribution error. The fundamental attribution error Ross (1977) defined the fundamental attribution error (FAE) as the tendency to underestimate the role of situational or external factors, and to overestimate the role of dispositional or internal factors, in assessing behaviour. The earliest demonstration of the FAE was an experiment by Jones and Harris (1967), in which American college students were presented with another student’s written essay that was either for or against the Castro government in Cuba. Half the participants were told that the essay writer had freely chosen PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 21 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION whether to write a ‘pro’ or ‘anti’ Castro essay (choice condition), and the other half were told that the essay writer was told which position to take (no-choice condition). After reading the essay, participants were asked what the essay writer’s ‘true’ attitude was towards Castro’s Cuba. The participants tended to view the writer’s attitude as consistent with the views expressed in the essay, regardless of the choice/no-choice condition. While they didn’t totally disregard that the no-choice writers had been told what position to take, they viewed this as less important than their attitudinal disposition. In other words, they underestimated the impact of the no-choice condition. In another classic study, Ross, Amabile and Steinmetz (1977) randomly assigned pairs of participants in a quiz game to act as contestant and questioner. Questioners were instructed to set ten difficult general knowledge questions of their own choosing. Despite the relative situational advantage of the questioners, both the contestants and observers of the quiz game rated the questioners as significantly more knowledgeable than the contestants. Heider put forward a largely cognitive explanation for the FAE. He suggested that behaviour has such salient properties that it tends to dominate our perceptions. In other words, what we notice most in (a) behaviour and (b) communication is (c) the person who is central to both. People are dynamic actors – they move, talk and interact, and these features come to dominate our perceptual field. Supporting this cognitive explanation, Fiske and Taylor (1991, p. 67) argued that situational factors such as social context, roles and situational pressures are ‘relatively pallid and dull’ in comparison with the charisma of the dynamic actor. While this is a commonsense and intuitive explanation, we discuss later in this chapter how this bias is only pervasive in Western individualistic cultures. So the FAE turns out to be not so fundamental after all. The actor–observer effect While we tend to attribute other people’s behaviour to dispositional factors, we tend to attribute our own behaviour to situational factors (Jones &Nisbett, 1972). This is called the actor–observer effect (AOE). Consider how easily we explain our own socially undesirable behaviour (such as angry outbursts) to extenuating, stressful circumstances, and yet we are less sympathetic when others behave in this way. Instead, we often conclude that the person is intolerant, impatient, unreasonable, selfish, etc. This bias has been found in both laboratory experiments (Nisbett et al., 1973) and applied clinical settings. For example, psychologists and psychiatrists are more likely to attribute their clients’ problems to internal stable dispositions, whereas the clients are more likely to attribute their own problems to situational factors (Antonio & Innes, 1978). There are several competing explanations for the AOE, but we will outline just two of them here. 1. Perceptual salience As for the FAE, one explanation is perceptual and essentially argues that actors and observers quite literally have ‘different points of view’ (Storms, 1973). As actors, we can’t see ourselves acting. From an actor’s point of view, what is most salient and available are the situational influences on behaviour – the objects, the people, the role requirements and the social setting. But from an observer’s point of view, other people’s behaviour is more dynamic and salient than the situation or context. These different vantage points for actors andobservers appear to lead to different attributional tendencies, i.e. situational attributions for actors and dispositional attributions for observers. Taylor and Fiske (1975) attempted to test the perceptual salience hypothesis by placing observers at three different vantage points around two male confederates who sat PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 22 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION facing each other engaged in conversation. Observers sat either behind confederate A with confederate B in their direct visual field, or behind B, watching A, or to the side, between A and B with both in sight (figure 17.11). After A and B had interacted for five minutes, each observer was asked to rate each confederate on various trait dimensions, and the extent to which their behaviour was caused by dispositional and situational factors. They also rated how much each confederate (a) set the tone of the conversation, (b) determined the kind of information exchanged and (c) caused the other’s behaviour. Consistent with the perceptual salience hypothesis, Taylor and Fiske found that the two observers sitting behind A, watching B, rated B as more causal, while those sitting behind B, watching A, saw A as more causal. The observers sitting in between A and B perceived both confederates as equally influential. In a similar vein, McArthur and Post (1977) manipulated the salience of two people engaged in conversation through the use of lighting. When one participant was made more salient than the other by being illuminated by bright light, observers rated the behaviour of the illuminated person as more dispositionally and less situationally caused. 2. Situational information Another explanation for the AOE focuses on information. Actors have more information about thesituational and contextual influences on their behaviour, including its variability and flexibility across time and place. But observersare unlikely to have such detailed information about the actors unless they know them very well, and have observed their behaviour over time and in many different situations. It therefore seems that observers assume more consistency in other people’s behaviour compared to their own, and so make dispositional attributions for others, while making situational attributions for their own behaviour (Nisbett et al., 1973). The self-serving bias It is well known that people tend to accept credit for success and deny responsibility for failure. More generally, we also tend to attribute our success to internal factors such as ability, but attribute failure to external factors such as bad luck or task difficulty. This is known as the self-serving bias. How often have we heard governments taking credit when there is national economic growth and prosperity, attributing it to their economic policies and prudent financial management? And yet, in times of ec onomic hardship, they are quick to blame external causes, such as the international money markets or worldwide recession. Although the strength of the self-serving bias varies across cultures, it has been found to occur cross-culturally (Fletcher & Ward, 1988; Kashima &Triandis, 1986). The usual explanation is motivational factors: that is, the need for individuals to enhance their selfesteem when they succeed and protect their self-esteem when they fail. Attributing success to internal causes has been referred to as the self-enhancing bias, and attributing failure to external causes as the self-protection bias (Miller & Ross, 1975). But Miller and Ross argue that there is only clear support for the self-enhancing bias, and that people do often accept personal responsibility for failure. They also claim that the self-enhancing bias can be explained by cognitive factors without recourse to motivational explanations. For example, we are more likely to make self-attributions for expected than unexpected outcomes, and most of us expect to succeed rather than fail. Even so, it is difficult to argue against the motivational hypothesis, and the prevailing consensus is that both motivational and cognitive factors have a part in the self-serving bias (Ross & Fletcher, 1985). The motivation for selfenhancement is also linked to achievement attributions. According to Weiner’s (1985; 1986) PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 23 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION attributional theory of motivation and emotion, the attributions people make for success and failure elicit different emotional consequences, and are characterized by three underlying dimensions – locus, stability and control. The locus dimension refers to whether we attribute success and failure internally or externally. Consistent with the selfenhancement bias, we are more likely to feel happier and better about ourselves if we attribute our success internally (to factors such as ability and effort) rather than externally (to good luck or an easy task). In contrast, attributing failure internally is less likely to make us feel good about ourselves than attributing it externally. The stability dimension refers to whether the cause is perceived as something fixed and stable (like personality or ability) or something changing and unstable (such as motivation or effort). n The controllability dimension refers to whether we feel we have any control over the cause. The tendency to attribute negative outcomes and failure to internal, stable and uncontrollable causes is strongly associated with clinical depression and has been referred to as a depressive attributional style (see chapters 14 and 15). The reformulated learned helplessness model of depression (Abramson et al., 1978) views this attributional style as directly causing depression. But others have argued that it is merely a symptom, reflecting the affective state of the depressed individual. Whether it is a cause or symptom, attributional retraining programmes (Försterling, 1985), in which people are taught to make more self-enhancing attributions, are widely accepted as an important therapeutic process for recovery from depression. The ultimate attribution error The self-serving bias also operates at the group level. So we tend to make attributions that protect the group to which we belong. This is perhaps most clearly demonstrated in what Pettigrew (1979) called the ultimate attribution error (UAE). By extending the fundamental attribution error to the group context, Pettigrew demonstrated how the nature of intergroup relations shapes the attributions that group members make for the same behaviour by those who are in-group and out-group members. So prejudicial attitudes and stereotypesof disliked out-groups lead to derogating attributions, whereas the need for positive enhancement and protection of the in-group leads to group-serving attributions. People are therefore more likely to make internal attributions for their group’s positive and socially desirable behaviour, and external attributions for the same positive behaviour displayed by out-groups. In contrast, negative or socially undesirable in-group behaviour is usually explained externally, whereas negative outgroup behaviour is more frequently explained internally. This intergroup bias has been found in a number of contexts (Hewstone, 1990). Taylor and Jaggi (1974) found it among Hindus in southern India, who gave different attributions for exactly the same behaviour performed by Hindu and Muslim actors. Duncan (1976) found that white American college students categorized the same pushing behaviour as ‘violent’ if perpetrated by a black actor but as ‘just playing around’ when perpetrated by a white actor. The most dramatic illustration of the UAE is an investigation by Hunter, Stringer and Watson (1991) of how real instances of violence are explained by Protestants and Catholics in Northern Ireland. Catholic students made predominantly external attributions for their own group’s violence but internal, dispositional attributions for Protestant violence. Similarly, Protestant students attributed their own group’s violence to external causes and Catholic violence to internal causes. There is also substantial evidence of the tendency to PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 24 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION make more favourable attributions for male success and failure. Studies have found that both men and women are more likely to attribute male success to ability and female success to effort and luck, especially in tasks that are perceived to be ‘male’ (Deaux& Major, 1987; Swim &Sanna, 1996). The same bias is found for failure attributions – male failure is explained by lack of effort, whereas female failure is attributed to lack of ability. Bear in mind though that most of these studies were conducted in the seventies and eighties, and relatively few have been published more recently (Swim &Sanna, 1996). Given the social and attitudinal changes associated with women’s roles over this time, and the fact that the effects were relatively small, it is possible that these biases have now diminished in Western societies (Hill &Augoustinos, 1997). IMPRESSION FORMATION Impression formation is a process that explains how we form or develop opinions about other people. How other people look like, how they behave, and the way we see and interpret their behaviour helps us to form opinions about them. How one develops an opinion or an image of another person is very complicated. Sometimes opinion can be formed by observing the behaviour of an individual. This means that most of the times we form opinions about other people with little information about them. (Smith and Queller 2001; 499 - 517) Usually the information about other people comes from our experiences of the people. When we meet people for the first time, we begin to asses them to form our opinion about them. It is this process of assessing them that result in forming impression about them. Thus it is assumed that forming impressions about other people does not happen at once, “immediately or automatically” (fiske 2004). We form impressions about others in three ways 1. Through the process of selection. Here we pay attention to physical appearances or focus on just one aspect of their behaviour. 2. Through the process of organization. In this case, we try to form a complete, acceptable impression of a person. 3. Through the process of inference. We attribute characteristics to people with no direct or immediate evidence, but might be based on stereotypes (Gross 2005; 376) First and Lasting impressions First impressions are usually lasting impressions because they are formed quickly and are very difficult and slow to change. These impressions affect how we perceive or see people’s behaviours and how we react to these behaviours. Our first impressions about people usually guide our future interactions with them, which is very important in developing social relation. (Brehm et al 2005) First impressions are slow because we hold on to existing impressions to preserve a reality that agrees with our expectations. We are likely going to give meaning to new information concerning people based on our expectations of them. When forming first impressions, we are likely going to be influenced by the following; 1. Our assumptions that people we meet are going to have attitudes and values similar to our own (Hoyle 1993) PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 25 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION 2. Our expectations of positive or favourable information from others. This is important because negative behaviours capture our attention because we are not expecting people to act negative towards us. (Taylor, Peplau and Sears, 2003). 3. Negative behaviours carry more weight in shaping first impressions than positive information (Smith and Mackie, 2000) Theories of impressions formation. Scientists have used theories to answer the question of why certain events or processes occur as they do. Social Psychologists have also used theories to understand why impressions are formed. A. Cognitive Theory This theory uses basic cognitive process in explaining impression formation, and states that; 1. When we meet people for the first time, we do not pay equal attention to all the information about them, but focus on what we view as most useful.(DeBrium and Van Lange, 2000; 1188-1205). 2. That we enter various information into memory to be recalled at a later time, this helps us form lasting impressions. 3. Also, that our first impressions of other people depends to some extent on our own characteristics. We see others through the “lens of our own traits, motives and desires” (vinokur and Schull, 2000). Traits are lasting personal qualities or attributes, which influence behaviour across situations. 4. 4. We tend to rely on information about traits, values and principles more than ability or competence. However, the context of meeting is important. For instance, if one is meeting a medical doctor when sick, or an employer meeting an individual for a job interview, might rely more or pay more attention on information about competence and ability of the individual. B. Central and Peripheral Traits Theory. This theory is based on Solomon Ach’s Research of 1946, and has the following views; i. ii. iii. Believe that there is a central and peripheral trait. That the central trait if seen as important, can influence our perceptions of a person and can generate inferences about more traits. That the peripheral traits have very little influence or none at all on other traits, but they help in understanding the central trait. Ach’s example below should help us understand the points above better. Intelligent - Skilful - Industrious – WARM (central trait) – Determined – practical – Cautions Intelligent - Skilful - Industrious – COLD (central trait) – Determined – practical – Cautions If strangers are seen as WARM – this trait can generate additional traits like generous, happy, good – natured, sociable, and popular among others. Other more recent views have shown that the meaning of our central trait can change depending on the context within which it is used. The central trait can also be affected by what we already PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 26 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION know about the individual. Positive or Negative traits can also affect the meaning of the central trait. C. Implicit Personality Theories The implicit Personality theories focus on the beliefs about what traits or characteristics tend to go together. These theories are of the view that:i. ii. iii. When people possess certain traits, they are likely to possess others too. That the culture of the people can shape these beliefs or expectations. There is a general tendency for people to assume that some traits or characteristics go together and can be observed in social situations. iv. Our impressions of others are based mostly on our implicit beliefs more than the actual traits of these people. v. We all have implicit ideas about names, birth order and physical appearance. Sometimes, just by introducing someone, by name, or birth order whether first, last, or only child is enough for us to assume some traits that agree with these. vi. We can assume what people are like even with little or limited information. D. Expectancy theory. This view suggests that the impression we form about others, and the way we behave based on this impressions is mostly influenced by our expectations. Our expectation on how an individual will be like can influence our behaviour towards that person. When our behaviour towards an individual causes him/her to meet our expectation, the result is a self-fulfilling prophesy. Self-fulfilling prophecy means behaving in a way that encourages an expected outcome. It is believed that we usually do things that cause others to conform to our impressions (Madon, Guyll, Spoth, Cross, and Hilbert 2003; 1188 – 1205). When as parents we expect our children to behave in certain ways, they may sense this expectation and act in ways that may likely confirm to our expectation of them. If they sense these expectations as positive or one that encourages them or make them to excel, they are likely to put in more effort. But if it is sensed as negative or one that expects them not to excel, they may not put in any effort at all. It has been found that children of mothers who expected their children to abuse alcohol were more likely to abuse alcohol later in life than the children of mothers who did not convey such expectations. (Madon et al 2001). E. Primacy and Recency Effect. This theory relies on the order in which we learn things. Our first impression is affected by what we learned first about a person, which is viewed as the primacy effect while what we learn later is referred to as the recency effect. For recency effect the following assertions have been made:i. ii. iii. When later information does not agree with earlier information, we tend to place more value on the first information as describing the real person and disregard the later information. People usually pay more attention to the information that came in first when they are trying to form an impression about someone. Once they have formed an impression, other information becomes irrelevant and they do not pay attention to them. First information affects the meaning of the later information because this information is made to agree with the first one. If our first impression about a person is a positive PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 27 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION one, any later information even if negative will be made to agree with the first positive information. Generally, it seems that primacy effectis more powerful than the recency effects because impressions are slow to change. Any information about a person is shaped by what we already know or believe about them - our first impression. However, there are certain conditions that are likely to influence this:-1 i. ii. iii. It is believed that negative impressions carry more weight because this may reflect socially undesirable behaviours or traits that may be harmful or disastrous. Thus a negative first impression may be more resistant to change than a positive one. (Jones and Davis, 1965) The primacy effect seems to be stronger especially in relation to strangers while therecency effect may be stronger for friends or people we already know very well. Information of the past concerning people we know well may change our perception of them. Since primacy effect might remain strong because of decrease attention of the later information, that people can be encouraged to pay attention to both primacy and recency information, before making any judgment. (Ludin, 1957). The information above has shown the importance of impression formation in developing relations. First impressions form the basis for future interactions with others. While these impressions are important, they however, can be inaccurate as explained by the theories. Care must be taken so that we do not rely too much on first impressions to explain peoples’ behaviours. This will ensure that relationships are not destroyed before they mature. IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT How others see us, is very important to us. That is why most of the time we behave in a way that they will approve of. In our attempt to impress others, we are likely to manipulate how they see us. Remember that while others are forming impressions about us, we are often consciously or unconsciously also trying to present a good image of ourselves to them. The process we go through in order to present this self image is referred to as Impression Management. When we are regarded favourably by others it is seen as a prerequisite for many positive life outcomes like respect, friendship, job success and romantic relationships (Learny 2004).We will look at the major components of Impression Management and how self-monitoring and self-disclosure are used in Impression Management. Impression Management also known as self-presentation has been an area of interest that enhances social interaction. Impression Management is the process of presenting a public image of the self to others (Turner, 1991). It is believed that we benefit from Impression Management because it increases our personal wellbeing through motivating us in three ways; 1. By increasing the reward of social relationship that allows us to belong 2. The Enhancement of our self-esteem. This is an effort to increase our appeal to others. Self-enhancement requires the use of some strategies that might include: a. Style of dressing to boost our physical appearance PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 28 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION b. Personal grooming c. Use of positive terms to describe self in favourable manner d. Sometimes going the extra mile to enhance self-appeals. 3. Establishment of desired identities (self-understanding). For Impression Management to Succeed, we need to “take the role of others” that is to be able to psychologically step into someone else’s shoes, see from their viewpoint and adjust our behaviour accordingly (Fiske & Taylor 1991;). If we can imagine how others see us or are likely to see us, thenwe can make adjustments to meet these imagined views. We are always trying to correct our behaviour to be in line with these “other” views. MAJOR COMPONENTS OF IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT We usually take into account other people’s viewpoint by adjusting our behaviours. Just how we do this has been explained by Fiske & Taylor (1991) and Fiske (2004) who suggested the following components in Impression Management. 1. Behaviour Matching- Here we act in ways that match the behaviour of our target person 2. Conform to Situational Norms - Every situation has its expected appropriate behaviour – we try to adopt behaviour that identifies with the situation. 3. High Self monitors - Those who present self in a positive way are likely to make a favourable impression. 4. Ingratiation - Ways we appreciate or flatter others can result in favourable responses from them. This has been shown to backfire if not interpreted well by the target person. 5. Consistency - Once our beliefs and behaviours are perceived to be consistent, we are likely to impress others favourably. 6. Verbal and Non – verbal behaviours - What we say and what we do should agree or match. Sometimes the non-verbal, mostly body language will give way or ‘leak’ revealing our true feelings. If what we say does not match what our body is trying to convey, then the non-verbal is taken seriously as telling the true story (Argyle et al 1972; Mehrabian 1972: 325-402). 7. Self-promotion - This is trying to present self in a way that will be seen by others as competent. This could result in a negative view of self by others if interpreted as being conceited or a fraud. 8. Intimidation- This means conveying the impression that one is dangerous sending a message of “do not come near or you get hurt”, “do not go against my wish or you suffer”. Most of the times, this may result in loss of credibility if interpreted by others as empty threats. 9. Exemplification - A case of presenting self as worthy, moral and saintly; might not go down well with others who may interpret this as ‘holier than thou’ among others. 10. Supplication - When one wants to be seen as helpless, could also backfire and one might be seen as lazy or manipulative. SOCIAL COGNITION-SCHEMAS It would be very difficult to function if we went ab out our everyday lives without prior knowledge or expectations about the people, roles, norms and events in our community. PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 29 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION Social cognition research suggests that our behaviour and interactions in the social world are facilitated by cognitive representations in our minds called schemas – mental or cognitive structuresthat contain general expectations and knowledge of the world. A schema contains both abstract knowledge and specific examples about a particular social object. It ‘provides hypotheses about incoming stimuli, which includes plans for interpreting and gathering schema-related information’ (Taylor & Crocker, 1981, p. 91). Schemas therefore give us some sense of prediction and control of the social world. They guide what we attend to, what we perceive, what we remember and what we infer. All schemas appear to serve similar functions – they all influence the encoding (taking in and interpretation) of new information, memory for old information and inferences about missing information. Not only are schemas functional, but they are also essential to our wellbeing. A dominant theme in social cognition research is that we are cognitive misers, economizing as much as we can on the effort we need to expend when processing information. Many judgements, evaluations and inferences we make in the hustle and bustle of everyday life are said to be ‘top of the head’ phenomena (Taylor & Fiske, 1978), made with little thought and considered deliberation. So schemas are a kind of mental short-hand used to simplify reality and facilitate processing. Schema research has been applied to four main areas: person schemas, self schemas, role schemas and event schemas (Fiske & Taylor, 1991). Person schemas Person schemas – often referred to as person prototypes – are configurat ions of personality traits that we use to categorize people and to make inferences about their behaviour. (The prototype is the ‘central tendency’, or average, of the category members.) In most Western cultures we tend to categorize individuals in terms of their dominant personality traits. We may infer from our observations and interactions with A that he is shy, or that B is opinionated. Most people would agree that Robin Williams is a prototypical extrovert and Woody Allen is a prototypical neurotic. Trait or person schemas enable us to answer the question: ‘what kind of person is he or she?’ (Cantor &Mischel, 1979). In so doing, they help us to anticipate the nature of our social interactions with individuals, giving us a sense of control and predictability. Self-schemas Just as we represent and store information about others, we do the same about ourselves, developing complex and varied schemas that define our self-concept based on past experiences. Self schemas are cognitive representations about ourselves that organize and process all related information (Markus, 1977). They develop from self-descriptions and traits that are salient and important to our self-concept. Indeed, they can be described as components of self-concept that are central to our identity and self-definition. For example, people who value independence highly are said to be self-schematic along this dimension. People for whom dependence–independence is not centrally important are said to be aschematic on this dimension. Different self schemas become activated depending on the changing situations and contexts in which we find ourselves (Markus &Kunda, 1986; Markus &Wurf, 1987). For example, your self schema as fun-loving and frivolous when you are with your friends may be quite different from your self schema as serious and dutiful when you are with your family. You will have schemas for your real self and also for your ‘ideal’ and ‘ought’ selves (Higgins, 1987) PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 30 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION Role schemas The norms and expected behaviours of specific roles in society are structured into role schemas. They will include both achieved roles – including occupational and professional roles, such as doctor or teacher – and ascribed roles, over which we have little control – such as age, gender and race. The roles and expectations associated with these categories are commonly referred to as stereotypes – mental representations of social groups and their members that are widely shared (Hamilton & Sherman, 1994; Macrae, Stangor&Hewstone, 1996; Stangor& Lange, 1994). Prolific empirical research on stereotypes views the process of categorizing individuals into their respective social groups as highly functional in that it simplifies the inherent complexity of social information. Social categories such as male/female, black/white, old/young are viewed as highly salient and prior to any other kind of person categorization. Fiske (1998) refers to age, gender and race as the‘top three’ because they are the most central and visually accessible categories. So when we meet someone for the first time, we attend to obvious and salient physical cues in guiding our interactions with them. With increased familiarity, the notion is that stereotypes based on physical cues become less important, and we may subsequently employ trait-based or person schemas. Event schemas Commonly referred to as cognitive scripts (see chapter 12), event schemas describe behavioural and event sequences in everyday activities (Schank& Abelson, 1977). They provide the basis for anticipating the future, setting goals and making plans. We know, for example, that the appropriate behavioural sequence for eating at a restaurant is to enter, wait to be seated, order a drink, look at the menu, order the meal, eat, pay the bill and leave. The key idea here is that our commonsense understanding of what constitutes appropriate behaviour in specific situations is stored in long -term memory, and it is activated unconsciously whenever we need it. Categorization and Stereotyping Before we can apply a schema to a social object, we have to categorize (or label) it as something – a book, a tree, an animal, or whatever. In other words, we identify objects, people and events as members of a category, similar to others in that category anddifferent from members of other categories. Mostly we employ categories automatically and with little conscious effort. Categories help to impose order on the stimulus world, and are fundamental to perception, thought, language and action (Lakoff, 1987; see chapter 12). Research on categorization stems from the pioneering work of cognitive scientist Eleanor Rosch and her colleagues (Rosch, 1975; 1978). Models for social categorization The categorization of social objects, people and events is assumed to be a more complex process than categorization of inanimate objects because social objects are variable, dynamic and interactive. Nevertheless, members of a social category share common features. Some instances contained in the category are considered to be more typical than others – the most typical, or prototypical, representing the category as a whole. The more features an instance shares with other category members, the more quickly and confidently it is identified as a member. For example, you may quickly decide that Sue is a prototypical politician because she is publicity seeking, charming, cunning and ambitious, whereas Paul, who is shy, indecisive, and avoids publicity would be considered atypical of the category ‘politician’. In contrast to the prototype model, an exemplar-based model suggests that categories are PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 31 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION represented by specific and concrete instances (exemplars) of the category (Smith & Zarate 1992). For example, arriving at an abstracted average of two very different politicians, such as Bill Clinton and Margaret Thatcher, may be too cognitively demanding. These extreme instances may be better represented as concrete exemplars within an overall general category of ‘politician’. People may rely on a combination of prototype and exemplarbased models, depending on the social objects in question and the conditions under which the information is processed (Brewer, 1988; Fiske &Neuberg, 1990; Hamilton & Sherman, 1994). Hierarchical structure of categories Categories are hierarchically structured, with more abstract and general categories of information at the top of a pyramid structure and more specific categories at the bottom. Information can be processed at different levels of abstraction, moving from a concrete specific instance to a more general level of inference. Like natural object categories, social stereotypes can be differentiated into lower-order sub-categories, or sub-types (Fiske, 1998). For example, a super-ordinate category (such as ‘woman’) may comprise a number of subtypes (such as career woman, housewife and feminist). Listing the prototypical features of these category sub-types is considerably easier, as they contain more detailed information than broader and more abstract super-ordinate categories (Andersen &Klatzky, 1987). Brewer, Dull and Lui (1981) found this to be the case with young people’s representations of the elderly. The ‘elderly’ category was differentiated further into three elderly sub-types – the senior citizen, the elderly statesman and the grandmotherly type. In turn, each of these subtypes was associated with distinctive characteristics and traits. How Do Schemas Work? What do schemas do in information-processing terms? How do they function as organizing structures that influence the encoding, storing and recall of complex social information? Schemas are theory-driven Because schemas are based on our prior expectations and social knowledge, they have been described as ‘theory-driven’ structures that lend organization to experience. We use these background theories to make sense of new situations and encounters, which suggests that schematic processing is driven by backgroundtheories and suppositions rather than actual enviromental data (Fiske & Taylor, 1991). Schemas facilitate memory Schemas help us process information quickly and economically and facilitate memory recall. This means we are more likely to remember details that are consistent with our schema than those that are inconsistent (Hastie & Park, 1986; Stangor& McMillan, 1992). For example, Cohen (1981) presented participants with a video of a woman having dinner with her husband. Those who were told that she was a librarian were more likely to remember that she wore glasses, whereas those who were told she was a waitress were more likely to remember her drinking beer. It seems that these occupational categories were used as organizing frameworks to attend to and/or encode and/or subsequently recall information that was consistent with stereotypic expectations of librarians and waitresses (see chapter 11 for some suggestions of ways in which we may try to tease apart which of these three memory components were affected in this study). PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 32 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION Schemas are energy-saving devices Simplifying information and reducing the cognitive effort that goes into a task preserves cognitive resources for more important tasks. Schemas, such as stereotypes, therefore function as energy saving devices (Macrae, Milne &Bodenhausen, 1994). In ambiguous situations, schemas help us to ‘fill in’ missing information with ‘best guesses’ and ‘default options’ based on our expectations and previous experience. They can also provide short cuts by utilizing heuristics such as representativeness (Kahneman&Tversky, 1972, 1973a). With limited information, we can use the representativeness heuristic to determine the degree to which a stimulus is representative of a more general category. Is John, who is shy and mild-mannered, more likely to be an accountant or a business executive? See chapter 12 for a discussion of situations in which these heuristics may be useful or misleading. Schemas are evaluative and affective Schemas also serve to evaluate social stimuli as good or bad, normal or abnormal, positive or negative, and some contain a strong affective component, so that when they are activated the associated emotion is cued. For example, the prototypic used-car salesman may automatically evoke suspicion, or a prototypic politician may trigger cynicism and distrust (Fiske, 1982; Fiske &Pavelchak, 1986). This is probably an important feature of some people’s race stereotypes, eliciting strong negative emotions and evaluations. Schemas are unifi ed, stable structures that resist change Once developed and strengthened through use, schemas become integrated structures. Even when only one of its components is accessed, strong associative links between the components activate the schema as a unitary whole (Fiske & Dyer, 1985). Well-developed schemas that are activated frequently resist change and persist, even in the face of disconfirming evidence. So a male chauvinist with a highly accessible and frequently activated stereotype that women are less capable than men is rarely convinced otherwise, even when presented with evidence to the contrary. Consistent with the ultimate attribution error described above, instances that disconfirm the stereotype are treated as ‘exceptions to the rule’. This notion is consistent with the subtyping model of stereotype change, which predicts that disconfirming instances of the stereotype are relegated to ‘exceptional’ sub categories or subtypes that accommodate exceptions while leaving the overall stereotype largely intact (Weber & Crocker, 1983). For example, Hewstone, Hopkins and Routh (1992) found that, despite a one-year school liaison programme that facilitated positive interactions between a police officer and secondary school students, this experience did not change the students’ overall negative representations of the police. Instead, these particular officers were judged by the school students to be atypical of the police in general. There is considerable empirical support for the subtyping model (Hewstone, 1994; Johnston &Hewstone, 1992). Othermodels have received less empirical support. These include the book-keeping model, which proposes that there is constant fine-tuning of a schema with each new piece of information (Rumelhart& Norman, 1978), and the conversion model, which proposes that there is dramatic and sudden change in the schema in response to salient contradictions (Rothbart, 1981. PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 33 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION SOCIAL PROCESSING The continuum model of processing We have seen how our preconceptions and prejudices can lead to biases and distortions. But we don’t always behave like cognitive misers. By contrast, in certain situations we engage in a careful and piecemeal analysis of the ‘data’. Fiske and Neuberg (1990) proposed that the processing of social information is a kind of continuum, as we move from schema or category-based processing to more piecemeal data-based processing (figure 17.17). These authors propose that we use category-based processing when the data are unambiguous and relatively unimportant to us and piecemeal processing when the data are ambiguous, relatively important, and the need for accuracy is high. For example, the time and effort we spend forming impressions of others depends on their relative importance to us and on our motivations for getting to know them. Everyday superficial encounters are usually based on people’s salient social group memberships, such as gender, race, age and occupation. These social categories access for us an associated range of expectations that are usually stereotypical. If we are motivated to move beyond this category-based processing, we take a more piecemeal and data-driven approach. Fiske and Neuberg’s (1990) continuum model of processing has led to a significant revision of the cognitive miser model that characterized the approach to social cognition in the 1980s. More recent research has demonstrated that perceivers are more like motivated tacticians (Fiske, 1992; 1998), using processing strategies that are consistent with their motivations, goals and situational requirements. Automatic vs. Controlled Processing While processing can take place anywhere along the continuum just described, most person impressions seem to be first and foremost category-based (this kind of schematic processing apparently being the ‘default option’). This is why so much recent attention has focused on the primacy and importance of stereotypes in perception. In-depth processing requires controlled attention, intention and effort, whereas it appears that category-based perception can occur automatically and beyond conscious awareness (Bargh, 1994; Wegner &Bargh, 1998). This distinction between automatic and controlled processing was applied by Devine (1989) to the activation of stereotypes. Devine argues that most people, through socialization, acquire knowledge of social stereotypes early in childhood and that, through repeated exposure, stereotypes of salient social groups become well-learned knowledge structures that are automatically activated without deliberate thinking. This model suggests that this unintentional activation of the stereotype is equally strong for high and low prejudiced people. For example Devine found that the activation of a negative stereotype associated with African Americans (‘hostile’) occurred for both high and low prejudiced participants when stereotypic primes were presented subliminally (beyond conscious awareness). So when people do not have the opportunity to consciously monitor and appraise information, the ability to suppress the stereotype becomes difficult, even for unprejudiced people. This, of course, suggests that stereotyping may be inevitable, and in some situations difficult to control. Given that stereotyping is usually linked to prejudice and discrimination, it paints a rather bleak picture for intergroup relations. But Devine argues that, while stereotypes can be automatically activated, what distinguishes low prejudiced from high prejudiced people is the conscious development of personal beliefs that challenge the stereotype. These egalitarian beliefs are deployed during conscious processing, and are able to override the automatically PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 34 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION activated stereotype. In contrast, people high in prejudice have personal beliefs that are congruent with negative stereotypes, so during conscious processing they need not control or inhibit the automatically activated stereotype. While several studies now support Devine’s claim that stereotypes of salient social groups are widely known and shared, there is less support for the claim that stereotypes are automatically activated equally for everyone, regardless of their prejudice levels (Augoustinos, Ahrens & Innes, 1994; Lepore& Brown, 1997; Locke, MacLeod & Walker, 1994). For example, Locke et al. (1994) found that the predominantly negative stereotype of Australian Aboriginal people was only activated in people high in prejudice. Similarly, Lepore and Brown (1997) found that only highly prejudiced respondents activated the negative stereotype of AfricanCaribbean people in Britain. So, according to these studies, it seems that stereotypes are not activated to the same extent for all people, and are therefore not necessarily inevitable. Rather, people’s attitudes and values – in this case, low levels of prejudice – inhibit and constrain the activation of stereotypes, not only consciously, but also unconsciously. THE POWER OF STEREOTYPES In our discussion of attribution theory, we argued that attributions are not only internal cognitive phenomena but also social and cultural explanations shaped by widely shared representations within a society, community or group. The same can be said for schemas, categories and stereotypes. While these have been largely discussed as cognitive constructs, it is important to recognize that they are also essentially cultural and social in nature, i.e. cultural knowledge that is determined by dominant and consensual representations learned by members of a society. Because they are acquired early in life, widely shared and pervasive, stereotypes of groups are more than just ‘pictures in our heads’. They are socially and discursively reproduced in the course of everyday communication (Augoustinos& Walker, 1998). They are also ideological in nature, because they are often used to rationalize and justify why some groups are more powerful and more dominant than others (Jost&Banaji, 1994). So social stereotypes can be used as political weapons to justify existing group inequalities, gender stereotypes have been used to justify gender inequalities, and race stereotypes have been used to justify racism and prejudice. Other approaches in social psychology, such as social representations theory (Augoustinos& Walker, 1995), social identity theory (Tajfel& Turner, 1979) and self-categorization theory (Oakes, Haslam& Turner, 1994), regard social categories and stereotypes very differently from the predominantly cognitive and information-processing account we have outlined above. Rather than energy-saving devices that facilitate cognition by simplifying reality, stereotypes (and the social categories on which they are based) are viewed within these contrasting frameworks as rich in symbolic meaning, and as being used to make sense of the power and status relations between different social groups (Oakes et al., 1994: Leyens, Yzerbyt&Schadron, 1994). Heuristics People save time and effort in making judgments by using heuristics (Tversky&Kahneman, 1974). Heuristics are timesaving mental shortcuts that reduce complex judgments to simple rules of thumb. They are quick and easy, but can result in biased information processing (Ajzen, 1996), which is one of the ways of identifying that they have PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 35 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION been used instead of more time-consuming, but more accurate, strategies. Below we outline two of the most commonly used types of heuristics: representativeness and availability. The Representativeness Heuristic The representativeness heuristic is the tendency to allocate a set of attributes to someone if they match the prototype of a given category (Kahneman&Tversky, 1973). It is a quick and-easy way of putting people in to categories. For instance, if you arrive at a hospital in need of help, you’ll look for the person wearing a white coat and stethoscope, because these specific attributes indicate that the person is (representative of) a doctor. Similarly, when you enter your lecture class you might very quickly identify your professor as being the one with the dubious fashion sense and slightly unkempt looking hair. Later on we will talk at greater length about the use of representativeness information in the context of social categorization, but for now it is important to note one important drawback of using this mental shortcut. While assessing representativeness to a category prototype may often be a good way of making inferences about someone, like any heuristic it is prone to error. In particular, there is the base rate fallacy, which is the tendency to ignore statistical information (base rates) in favor of representativeness information. For example, even if you told someone that gender is uncorrelated with managerial and administrative roles in some corporation (i.e. that there are an equal number of men and women at different levels of power), they would probably still be more likely to attribute more of the managerial (high power) roles to men than women, because such roles are more representative of men than women. The Availability Heuristic The availability heuristic is the tendency to judge the frequency or probability of an event in terms of how easy it is to think of examples of that event (Tversky&Kahneman, 1973). It is related to the concept of accessibility, which is the extent to which a concept is readily brought to mind. The difference is that availability can refer to one’s subjective experience of accessibility – the awareness that something is accessible – whereas accessibility is typically regarded as an objective measure of how quickly something can be brought to mind, without explicit awareness being a necessary component. The availability heuristic can be illustrated with varied examples from everyday life. For instance, you might feel more trepidation about taking a flight if you have just heard about a horrific plane crash. In this example, your assessment of how likely it is that the plane journey will be a safe one will be influenced by the availability of information to the contrary. A neat experiment illustrates this heuristic. Schwarz and colleagues (1991) asked participants to recall 12 or six examples of when they had been either assertive or unassertive. After having completed this task participants were then asked to rate their own assertiveness. Counter to what one might logically expect, participants who recalled six examples of their own assertive behaviour subsequently rated themselves as more assertive than people who had recalled 12 examples of their own assertive behaviour. The same effect occurred for people who recalled examples of unassertive behaviour: those who recalled six examples of unassertive behaviour rated themselves less assertive (more unassertive) than those who recalled 12 examples of unassertive behaviour. These findings are really quite different from what one might expect. Surely someone who can recall more examples of assertive behaviour should regard themselves as more assertive than someone who can only recall a few examples. Similarly, someone who can PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 36 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION recall more examples of when they have been unassertive should logically then rate themselves as more unassertive. In contrast, the more examples of assertive or unassertive behaviour people were asked to generate, the less assertive or unassertive respectively they perceived themselves to be. The explanation for this effect lies with the availability heuristic. The key is in thinking about how easy or difficult the task might be to people. On average people don’t normally spend much time listing the number of times they are assertive or unassertive in their lives. As such, being asked to list assertive or unassertive behaviours might be something they are not used to and, one imagines, is a task that gets more difficult after the first two or three examples that come to mind. We can assume that being asked to recall 12 examples of assertive or unassertive behaviour would be more difficult that being asked to recall six examples of assertive or unassertive behaviour. While doing this task, it is therefore reasonable that people recognized that they were finding it difficult to come up with examples of assertive or unassertive behaviour after the first few that came to mind, and that this realization should be much greater when participants had to labour on and come up with 12 examples. As they tried to think of 12 examples of behaviour, participants will have eventually become aware that such examples were not coming easily to mind. In other words, there were no more examples available to them. They therefore concluded that they must not be particularly assertive or unassertive (depending on what type of behaviour they had been instructed to generate). In sum, it seems that people attend to the difficulty of retrieving instances of certain behaviors and not just the content. The False Consensus Effect The availability heuristic is an important explanatory mechanism that we will see again several times in the course of this book. It is also responsible for a highly robust bias called the false consensus effect (Gross & Miller, 1997). This is the tendency to exaggerate how common one’s own opinions are in the general population. Ross, Greene, and House (1977) illustrated this effect by asking participants whether they would walk around campus for 30 minutes wearing a sandwich board advertising a cafeteria. Whether they agreed or not, the experimenter then asked them how many other students asked would make the same choice as they did. Ross et al. found that whatever choice the participant made, they estimated that the majority of other people would agree with them and make the same choice. Clearly, this consensus estimate is not objectively possible. If, for example, 70 per cent of people support one political party, then 30 per cent must not – you cannot have 50 per cent of people not supporting this party. There must therefore be a false consensus, whereby people believe that everybody usually agrees with them. The availability heuristic provides the explanation for the false consensus effect. Our own self-beliefs are easily recalled from memory, making them most available when we are asked to judge whether others agree with us. This makes it likely that our judgments of others’ attitudes and opinions will, at least to some extent, be influenced by our own. The Anchoring Heuristic It is often the case that a distinction is made between the availability heuristic and another called the anchoring heuristic. Anchoring is the tendency to be biased towards the starting value (or anchor) in making quantitative judgments (Wyer, 1976). There have been a number of illustrations of this effect. Plous (1989) carried out a survey during the Cold War in which he asked the same question in two slightly different ways. For half of the participants he asked whether they thought there was a greater than 1 per cent chance of a nuclear war occurring soon, and for the other half he asked whether they thought there was a PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 37 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION less than a 90 per cent chance of a nuclear war occurring soon. Both questions asked for a quantitative estimate of probability, so one imagines how the question asked should not have an impact on the judgments made. In fact, there is quite a considerable effect of the anchor provided in the question. Participants who received the 1 per cent question anchor estimated a 10 per cent chance of a nuclear war occurring, while those who received the 90 per cent anchor estimated a 25 per cent chance of a nuclear war occurring. A similar effect was observed by Greenberg et al. (1986), who found in a mock jury study that participants asked to consider first a harsh verdict were subsequently harsher in their final decision than participants asked first to consider a lenient verdict. In sum, it appears that our judgments on a range of issues are significantly influenced by the point at which we start our deliberations. While the anchoring heuristic has often been considered to be distinct from the availability heuristic, in essence it comes down to the same psychological mechanism. The starting point or anchor exerts an impact on judgment because it is the most available source of information relevant to the issue at hand. Either way, this bias has some clearly important implications for a range of social contexts from the way in which lawyers structure questions in the courtroom (to elicit particular answers), to the way that opinion pollsters gauge attitudes. The Motivated Tactician In this section we have seen how heuristics are sometimes used in social judgment over and above more rational, logical, but time-consuming ways of thinking. In other words, people can sometime be cognitive misers rather than naïve scientists, preferring ease and speed over accuracy. As we noted above in our discussion of attribution theory, participants can and do use the complex systems outlined by models proposed by Jones and Davis (1965) and Kelley (1967), but this only appears to be the case under certain conditions. Other times people seem to revert to making quick and easy judgments using mental shortcuts like availability or representativeness, or relying on simple cues like perceptual salience (which can also be considered a type of availability). These heuristic shortcuts are much less accurate than using more rational, logical modes of thought, but they do approximate a response that is often within acceptable parameters. So what determines whether people will adopt one of these strategies over the other? When are people naïve scientists and when are they cognitive misers? According to Kruglanski (1996) people are flexible social thinkers who choose between multiple cognitive strategies (i.e. speed/ease vs. accuracy/logic) based on their current goals, motives, and needs. Kruglanski argued that people are neither exclusively cognitive misers nor naïve scientists, but in fact motivated tacticians. Put another way, people are strategic in their allocation of cognitive resources and as such can decide to be a cognitive miser or a naïve scientist depending on a number of factors. Macrae, Hewstone, and Griffiths (1993) PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 38 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION Figure.1The motivated-tactician: conditions of heuristic versus systematic processing outline a number of factors that determine whether people will adopt logical, rationale, and time-consuming processing strategies in social inference, or whether they will go for a quick and easy, but quite possibly adequate, solution (see Figure 1.) First, people will be more likely to be a cognitive miser when they are short oftime, than when they have plenty of it. This makes sense. Heuristics are quick and easy, they save time, therefore, when we have to make a quick decision. So although it is less accurate, heuristics may be the best option open to use in order to make a judgment that at least approximates an adequate response. Second is cognitive load. Heuristics do not require much thought – they can be made off the cuff, simply made from a “gut instinct” or intuition (or, what we would nowcall, availability). In contrast, the naïve scientist approach requires a lot of thought, analysis, and contemplation. If we are busy with lots on our mind, we’re unlikely to devote much time to social perception, and are much more likely to use heuristics because, again, they approximate a right answer without having to give the issue at hand much thought. Third is importance. Heuristics are useful for providing estimates, but they cannot match more logical, rational, and detailed analyses. If a decision we have to make is important to us (e.g. whether to go for that new job) then we are much less likely to use a heuristic and much more likely to be a naïve scientist. Fourth, and final, is information level. As we noted in our discussion of attribution theory, people can and do make use of complex attribution rules in forming impressions, combining consensus, consistency, and distinctiveness information in elaborate ways, but only when they have all the necessary PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 39 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION pieces of information. If we don’t have all the facts then sometimes it is simply impossible for us to be naïve scientists; we may simply not have enough information to be able to rationally and logically make a detailed analysis of the issue at hand. In such situations the only recourse is to use a heuristic shortcut to approximate the correct response. Priming Priming is an implicit memory effect in which exposure to one stimulus (i.e., perceptual pattern) influences the response to another stimulus. The seminal experiments of Meyer and Schvaneveldt in the early 1970sled to the flowering of research on priming of many sorts. Their original work showed that people were faster in deciding that a string of letters is a word when the word followed an associatively or semantically related word. For example, Nurseis recognized more quickly following Doctor than following Bread. Various experiments supported the theory that activation spreading among related ideas was the best explanation for the facilitation observed in the lexical decision task. The priming paradigm provides excellent control over the effects of individual stimuli on cognitive processing and associated behavior because the same target stimuli can be presented with different primes. Thus differences in performance as a function of differences in priming stimuli must be attributed to the effect of the prime on the processing of the target stimulus. Priming can occur following perceptual, semantic, or conceptual stimulus repetition. For example, if a person reads a list of words including the word table, and is later asked to complete a word starting with tab, the probability that he or she will answer table is greater than if they are not primed. Another example is if people see an incomplete sketch they are unable to identify and they are shown more of the sketch until they recognize the picture, later they will identify the sketch at an earlier stage than was possible for them the first time. The effects of priming can be very salient and long lasting, even more so than simple recognition memory. Unconscious priming effects can affect word choice on a word-stem completion test long after the words have been consciously forgotten. Priming works best when the two stimuli are in the same modality. For example visual priming works best with visual cues and verbal priming works best with verbal cues. But priming also occurs between modalities, or between semantically related words such as "doctor" and "nurse". TYPES OF PRIMING Positive and Negative Priming The terms positive and negative priming refer to when priming affects the speed of processing. A positive prime speeds up processing, while a negative prime lowers the speed to slower than un-primed levels. Positive priming is caused by simply experiencing the stimulus, while negative priming is caused by experiencing the stimulus, and then ignoring it. Positive priming effects happen even if the prime is not consciously seen. The effects of positive and negative priming are visible in event-related potential (ERP) readings. Positive priming is thought to be caused by spreading activation. This means that the first stimulus activates parts of a particular representation or association in memory just PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 40 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION before carrying out an action or task. The representation is already partially activated when the second stimulus is encountered, so less additional activation is needed for one to become consciously aware of it. Negative priming is more difficult to explain. Many models have been hypothesized, but currently the most widely accepted are the distractor inhibition and episodic retrieval models. In the distractor inhibition model, the activation of ignored stimuli is inhibited by the brain. The episodic retrieval model hypothesizes that ignored items are flagged 'do-notrespond' by the brain. Later, when the brain acts to retrieve this information, the tag causes a conflict. The time taken to resolve this conflict causes negative priming. Although both models are still valid, recent scientific research has led scientists to lean away from the distractor inhibitor model. Perceptual and Conceptual Priming The difference between perceptual and conceptual primes is whether items with a similar form or items with a similar meaning are primed, respectively. Perceptual priming is based on the form of the stimulus and is enhanced by the match between the early and later stimuli. Perceptual priming is sensitive to the modality and exact format of the stimulus. An example of perceptual priming is the identification of an incomplete word in a word-stem completion test. The presentation of the visual prime does not have to be perfectly consistent with later testing presentations in order to work. Studies have shown that, for example, the absolute size of the stimuli can vary and still provide significant evidence of priming. Conceptual priming is based on the meaning of a stimulus and is enhanced by semantic tasks. For example, table, will show priming effects on chair, because table and chair belong to the same category. Repetition Repetition priming, also called direct priming, is a form of positive priming. When a stimulus is experienced, it is also primed. This means that later experiences of the stimulus will be processed more quickly by the brain. This effect has been found on words in the lexical decision task. Semantic In semantic priming, the prime and the target are from the same semantic category and share features. For example, the word dog is a semantic prime for wolf, because the two are both similar animals. Semantic priming is theorized to work because of spreading activation within associative networks. When a person thinks of one item in a category, similar items are stimulated by the brain. Even if they are not words, morphemes can prime for complete words that include them. An example of this would be that the morpheme 'psych' can prime for the word 'psychology'. PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 41 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION Associative priming In associative priming, the target is a word that has a high probability of appearing with the prime, and is "associated" with it but not necessarily related in semantic features. Dog is an associative prime for cat, since the words are closely associated and frequently appear together (in phrases like "raining cats and dogs"). A similar effect is known as context priming. Context priming works by using a context to speed up processing for stimuli that are likely to occur in that context. A useful application of this effect is reading written text. The grammar and vocabulary of the sentence provide contextual clues for words that will occur later in the sentence. These later words are processed more quickly than if they had been read alone, and the effect is greater for more difficult or uncommon words. Response priming In the psychology of visual perception and motor control, the term response priming denotes a special form of visuomotor priming effect. The distinctive feature of response priming is that prime and target are presented in quick succession (typically, less than 100 milliseconds apart) and are coupled to identical or alternative motor responses. When a speeded motor response is performed to classify the target stimulus, a prime immediately preceding the target can thus induce response conflicts when assigned to a different response as the target. These response conflicts have observable effects on motor behavior, leading to priming effects, e.g., in response times and error rates. A special property of response priming is its independence from visual awareness of the prime: For example, response priming effects can increase under conditions where visual awareness of the prime is decreasing Masked priming The masked priming paradigm has been widely used in the last two decades in order to investigate both orthographic and phonological activations during visual word recognition. The term "masked" refers to the fact that the prime word or pseudo word is masked by symbols such as ###### that can be presented in a forward manner (before the prime) or a backward manner (after the prime). These masks enable to diminish the visibility of the prime. The prime is usually presented less than 80 ms (but typically between 40-60 ms) in this paradigm. In all, the short SOA (Stimuli Onset Asynchrony, i.e. the time delay between the onset of the mask and the prime) associated with the masking make the masked priming paradigm a good tool to investigate automatic and irrepressive activations during visual word recognition. Forster has argued that masked priming is a purer form of priming, as any conscious appreciation of the relationship between the prime and the target is effectively eliminated, and thus removes the subject's ability to use the prime strategically to make decisions. Results from numerous experiments show that certain forms of priming occur that are very difficult to occur with visible primes. One such example is form-priming, where the prime is similar to, but not identical to the target (e.g., nature-mature). Kindness priming Kindness priming is a specific form of priming that occurs when a subject experiences an act of kindness and subsequently experiences a lower threshold of activation when subsequently encountering positive stimuli. A unique feature of kindness priming is that it PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 42 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION causes a temporary increased resistance to negative stimuli in addition to the increased activation of positive associative networks. Potential sources of error in social cognition / conceptualizing errors in social cognition It is an important aspect of human nature that we always consciously aspire to think rationally or logically so that our decisions or judgements about people or events are not wrong. However, it is easier said than done. Social psychologist have found out that the motivation to reason in an error-free manner is often overpowered by the adaptive processes where we tend to reduce the cognitive effort required to understand the social world. Keeping this in mind, this lecture is devoted to the several sources of errors in social cognition: - Cognitive-experiential self-theory - Paying attention to inconsistent information - Planning fallacy - Automatic vigilance - Potential costs of thinking too much - Counterfactual thinking - Magical thinking i. Cognitive-experiential self-theory Cognitive-experiential self theory posits that errors in our judgement results from our intuition based on past experiences precedes the rational thinking while analyzing a problem or a situation. - For example, a basketball player prefers a pair of old shoes (that may be dangerous in terms ofinjuries owing to inadequate body-balance) to a pair of new and proper basketball shoes as it occurs to him that the old shoes had proved to be ‘lucky’ for him in the past. ii. Paying attention to inconsistent information Information inconsistent with a person, his/her role or any event may be highly effective inattracting our attention but it may distract the observer or the listener from the consistent and relevant information. Social psychologists have provided evidence that inconsistent information is better remembered that the consistent information about gender roles (Bardach& Park, 1996). According to their study, the qualities that are usually not associated with a gender (‘nuturant’ for males and ‘competitive’ for females) were better remembered by participants than those that are usually associated with a gender (‘adventurous’ for male and ‘emotional’ for females). These findings do indicate that the inconsistent information may overshadow the important consistent information. iii. Planning fallacy When it comes to deciding about the time we will take to complete a task, we often overshoot the time period that we had assigned to ourselves. This is known as planning fallacy. The reason for this is that while initially taking the decision about the time required, rather than focusing on the time we had taken to accomplish a task in the past, we generally focus on events or actions to occur in future. This tendency disallows us to do a realistic estimate of time needed. Furthermore, as social psychologists have founded out, at the time PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 43 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION of initial decision-making, even if one is reminded of the excessive time incurred in the past, the delay is usually attributed to some external factors rather than one’s own capabilities to the finish the work in time. iv. Automatic vigilance Social psychologists have indicated that rather than focusing on the common aspects of a situation, we tend to focus more on the negative aspects of the situation. Thus, an understanding of the common aspects of a social phenomenon may not take place as adequate attention was not given to these. In their study, Hansen and Hansen (1988) noted that their participants were easily able to locate an angry face in the crowd of normal or happy people but, locating a normal face in the crowd of angry people was not as easy. v. Potential costs of thinking too much There are times when we overdo careful thinking and this may result in confusion, frustration and wrong judgement too. Wilson and Schooler (1991), in the study, asked half of the participants to rate the several strawberry jams and the other half of them to analyze the reasons for the ratings they themselves gave to each jam. The researchers also took the opinion ofexperts (whoprofessionally compared various products) about the correctness of judgement made of the two group of participants. They found out that, according to the experts, the judgement of the second half of the participants (consisting of participants who analyzed their own rating) were not as accurate as that of the first half (consisting of participants who simply rated the jams). vi. Counterfactual thinking We regret our actions more when we find out that ours was a rather unusual behaviour. For example, a student who usually goes to school on his bicycle, went to the school on-foot on the day he became the victim of an accident. Another student went to the school on-foot as he usually does. It is common to expect that the first rather the second student will be more regretful of his actions. The reason for the more regret in the case of the first student is the alternatives that were not chosen by him. Both the students might have required our sympathies equally but perhaps more sympathies (due to more regret) would be given to the first student. vii. Magical thinking Magical thinking is the kind of thinking that involves irrational assumptions often associated with (Zusne& Jones, 1989) : a. law of similarity, or b. law of contagion Law of similarity states that we assume that people similar to each other in appearance may behaving similar fundamental characteristics. - For example, some children might not like to eat a biscuit in the shape of a house lizard. Law of contagion is the belief that when two people or objects come in contact with each other,they pass on their properties to one another and such an impact last long after the contact is over. - For example, on might not like to use wear the coat used by an HIV patient even after it is dry-cleaned. PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 44 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION viii. ix. x. Statistical Regression Statistical tendency for extremes to be followed byless extreme performances that are closer to average e.g., Sports Illustrated jinx or the sophomore slump Illusion of Control A false belief that one can control chance situationse.g., Throwing dice softly for low numbers Conjunction Fallacy Conjunction Fallacy is the tendency to see an event as more likely as it becomes more specific because it is joined with elements that seem similar to events that are likely. Example: Linda is 31 years old, single, outspoken, and very bright. She majored in philosophy. As a student, she was deeply concerned with issues of discrimination and social justice, and also participated in anti-nuclear demonstrations Which is more probable? xi. 1. Linda is a bank teller 2. Linda is a bank teller and is active in the feminist movement 85% of those asked chose option 2 The probability of two events occurring together (in “conjunction") will always be less than or equal to the probability of either one occurring alone Illusory Correlation Perception of stronger relationship than actually exists e.g., believing that professional athletes who are Black are dangerous Particularly likely when two rare events occur together Basis of superstitious behavior (e.g., basketball player believing their new socks are lucky because they played their best game). PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 45 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION Module 3 Defining a group Two or more people constitute a group if. 1. they have some common purpose or goal... 2. there exists a relatively stable structure -- a hierarchy (perhaps a leader), an established set of roles, or a standardized pattern of interaction... 3. this collection of people see themselves as being part of that group Why do groups form? There are a number of general tendencies within us such as: The similarity-attraction effect: we like people who are similar to us in some way Exposure: we like people whom we have been exposed to repeatedly Reciprocity: we like people who like us Basking in reflected glory: we seek to associate with successful, prestigious groups Furthermore, we also tend to avoid individuals who possess objectionable characteristics. Further more there are number of reasons why people join groups which are as follows; Affiliation Humans are by nature gregarious. Groups provide a natural way for people to gather in order to satisfy their social needs. Goal achievement Problems and tasks that require the utilization of knowledge tend to give groups an advantage over individuals. There is more information in a group than in any one of its members, and groups tend to provide a greater number of approaches to solving any particular problem Power Individuals gain powerin their relationship with their employers by forming unions. Status Membership in a particular service clubs or a political body may be seen to confer status on members. So as to gain that status people join in such groups Self-esteem As suggested by Maslow, people have a basic desire for self-esteem. Group membership maynurture self-esteem. If one belongs to a successful group, the self-esteem of all members may be boosted. PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 46 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION Security Sometimes individuals need protectionfrom other groups or more powerful individuals -- "there is safety in numbers". These individuals may seek security in group membership. Neighbours may form a "Block Watch" group to ensure the security and protection of their neighbourhood. The important characteristics of groups are as follows: Social interaction. The members of a group affect each other and there is a definite pattern of interaction among them. Stability. Groups also must possess a stable structure. Although groups can change, which often they do, there must be some stable relationship that keeps the group members together and functioning as a unit. Common interests or goals.Members of a group must share some common interests or goals that bind the group together. Recognition as being a group. It is not just being together would ensure the formation of a proper group. The members of the group must also perceive themselves as a group. They must recognize each other as a member of their group and can distinguish them from nonmembers. Types of Groups There can be different types of groups that mightexist, which have been depicted in Figure 3 below. The most common way of distinguishing between groups is to categorizing the groups into formal or informal groups. Formal groupsare deliberately created by the organization in order to help the organizational members achieve some of the important the organizational goals. The informal groups, in contrast, develop rather spontaneously among an organization’s members without any direction from the organizational authorities. There are various types of formal groupsthat are found in an organization. These are: Command groupwhich is determined bythe organizational chart depicting the approved formal connections between individuals in an organization. Examples of command group are Director and the faculty members in a business school, school principal and teachers, production manager and supervisors, etc. Task groups, comprising someindividuals with special interest or expertise, are created bythe organizational authorities to work together in order to complete a specific task. Task groups are often not restricted to the organizational hierarchy and can be cross functional in nature. Examples of task group might be people working on a particular project. Standing committee is a permanent committee in an organization to deal with some specific types of problems that may arise more or less on a regular basis. Examples of standing committees include the standing committee in a university to discuss various academic and administrative issues. Task force / ad hoc committee, in contrast, isa temporary committee formed byorganizational members from across various functional areas for a special purpose. Meetings can also comeunder this category. PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 47 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION Various types of informalgroups are: Interest groups are formed when a group of employees band together to seek some common objectives, like protesting some organizational policy or joining the union to achieve a higher amount of bonus. Friendship groups develop among the organizational members when they share some common interest like participating in some sports activities or staging the office drama, etc. Reference groups are the groups, with which individuals identify and compare themselves. These could be within the organization when a middle level executive compares himself with the higher level executive and longs for the perks and benefits enjoyed by the latter. The reference group might exist outside the organization as well when an individual compares himself with his batch mates working in other organizations or an ideal group of people he likes to become. Figure 2. Type of group Formation of Groups Two models of group development have been offered bythe researchers in the field of social sciences to explain how groups are formed. These are: a) Five-Stage Model and b) Punctuated Equilibrium Model. According to the Five-Stage Model of group development, groups go through five distinct stages during the process of its development. These are as follows: Forming is the initial stage of group development when the group members first comein contact with others and get acquainted witheach other. This stage is characterized predominantly bya feeling of uncertaintyamong the group members as they now try to establish ground rules and pattern of relationship among themselves. Storming is the next stage that is characterized bya high degree of conflict among the members. Members often show hostility towards each other and resist the leader’scontrol. If these conflicts are not adequately resolved, the group may even be disbanded. But, usually the group eventually comes in terms with each other and accepts the leadership role at the end of this stage. PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 48 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION Norming is the third stage of the group development process during which the group members become closer to each other and the group starts functioning as a cohesive unit. The group members now identify themselves with the group and share responsibility for achieving the desired level of performance of the group. Norming stage is complete when the group members can set a common target and agree on the way of achieving this. Performing is the fourth stage when the group is finally ready to start working. As the group is now fully formed after resolving their internal conflicts of acceptance and sharing responsibility, they can now devote energy to achieve its objectives. Adjourning is the final stage when the group, after achieving the objectives for which it was created, starts to gradually dissolve itself. Many interpreters of the five-stage model have assumed that a group becomes more effective as it progresses through the first four stages. While this assumption may be generally true, what makes a group effective is more complex than this model acknowledges. Under some conditions, high levels of conflict are conducive to high group performance. So we might expect to find situations in which groups in Stage II outperform those in Stages III or IV. Similarly, groups do not always proceed clearly from one stage to the next. Sometimes, in fact, several stages go on simultaneously, as when groups are storming and performing at the same time. Groups even occasionally regress to previous stages. Therefore, even the strongest proponents of this model do not assume that all groups follow its fivestage process precisely or that Stage IV is always the most preferable. Another problem with the five-stage model, in terms of understanding work- related behaviour, is that it ignores organizational context.4 For instance, a study of a cockpit crew in an airliner found that, within 10 minutes, three strangers as- signed to fly together for the first time had become a high-performing group. What allowed for this speedy group development was the strong organizational context surrounding th~ tasks of the cockpit crew. This context provided the rules, task definitions, information, and resources needed for the group to per- form. They didn't need to develop plans, assign roles, determine and allocate re- sources, resolve conflicts, and set norms the way the five-stage model predicts. An Alternative Model: For Temporary Groups with Deadlines Temporary groups with deadlines don't seem to follow the previous model. Studies indicate that they have their own unique sequencing of actions(or inaction): (1) Their first meeting sets the group's direction; (2) this first phase of group activity is one of inertia; (3) a transition takes place at the end of this first phase, which occurs exactly when the group has used up half its allotted time; (4) a transition initiates major changes; (5) a second phase of inertia follows the transition; and (6) the group's last meeting is characterized by markedly accelerated activity . This pattern is called the punctuated equilibrium model and is shown below. The first meeting sets the group's direction. A framework of behavioural pat- terns and assumptions through which the group will approach its project emerges in this first meeting. These lasting patterns can appear as early as the first few seconds of the group's life. Once set, the group's direction becomes "written in stone" and is unlikely to be reexamined throughout the first half of the group's life. This is a period of inertia that is, the group tends to stand still or become locked into a fixed course of action. Even if it gains new insights that challenge initial patterns and assumptions, the group is incapable of acting on these new insights in PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 49 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION Phase 1. One of the more interesting discoveries made in these studies was that each group experienced its transition at the same point in its calendar-precisely halfway between its first meeting and its official deadline-despite the fact that some groups spent as little as an hour on their project while others spent six months. It was as if the groups universally experienced a midlife crisis at this point. The midpoint appears to work like an alarm clock, heightening members ‘awareness that their time is limited and that they need to "get moving." This transition ends Phase 1 and is characterized by a concentrated burst of changes in which old patterns are dropped and new perspectives are adopted. The transition sets a revised direction for Phase 2. Phase 2 is a new equilibrium or period of inertia. In this phase, the group executes plans created during the transition period. The group's last meeting is characterized bya final burst of activity to finish its work. In summary, the punctuated-equilibrium model characterizes groups as exhibiting long periods of inertia interspersed with brief revolutionary changes triggered primarily by their members' awareness of time and deadlines. Keep in mind, however, that this model doesn't apply to all groups. It'sessentiallylimited to temporarytask groups that are working under a time-constrained completion deadline. Group Structurerefers to the pattern of interrelationship that exists among the group members, and makes the group functioning orderly and predictable. Four important aspects of group’s structure are: Role or the typical part played by an individual group member in accordance with the expectations of other members from him. Role expectations refer to the behaviours that are expected from the person playing the role. The person holding the role is known as the role incumbent. Role ambiguity takes place when the person holding the role feels confused and does not know what is being expected from him. The role incumbent is said to suffer from the problem role identity when he faces difficulty in accepting the assigned role. Norms or the rules and mutual expectations that develop within the group. This refers to the generally agreed upon rules that guide the group members’ behaviour. Norms have profound effect on members’ behaviours it ensures conformity among them. Norms can be of two types: prescriptive when it dictates behaviours that should be performed and proscriptive when it dictates specific behaviours that should be avoided by the group members. Status or the relative prestige or social position given to groups or individuals by others. People often join the core group or a renowned club because of the prestige associated with these groups. Group cohesiveness referring to the strength of group members’ desires to remain a part of the group. This also refers to the degree of attraction of the group members for each other and the ‘we-feeling’ among the members. The degree of cohesiveness has been found to depend on external threats, the difficulty in getting included in the group, the amount of time spent by the group members with each other and the success of the group. PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 50 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION Individual’s Performance in Groups Groups are formed with individuals, but the output of the groups is not just the sumtotal of individual’s contribution towards the group. The term group synergy refers to the fact the action of two or more group members result in an effect that is different from the individual summation of their contributions. This occurs because of the social facilitation effect which refers to the tendency for performance of an individual group member to improve in response to the presence of other members. However, the group performance is not always guaranteed to improve as often group members are found to exert less individual effort. This is known as social loafing when members are found to enjoy a ‘free ride’ which tends to increase with group’s size. Group dynamics A group can be defined as two or more individuals that are connected to each another by social relationships. Groups tend to interact, influence each other, and share a common identity. They have a number of emergent qualities that distinguish them from aggregates: Norms - implicit rules and expectations for group members to follow, e.g. saying thank you, shaking hands. Roles - implicit rules and expectations for specific members within the group, e.g. the oldest sibling, who mayhave additional responsibilities in the family. Relations - patterns of liking within the group, and also differences in prestige or status, e.g. leaders, popularpeople. Temporary groups and aggregates share few or none of these features, and do not qualify as true social groups. People waiting in line to get on a bus, for example, do not constitute a group. Groups are important not only because they offer social support, resources, and a feeling of belonging, but because they supplement an individual's selfconcept. To a large extent, humans define themselves by the group memberships which form their social identity. The shared social identity of individuals within a group influences intergroup behaviour, the way in which groups behave towards and perceive each other. These perceptions and behaviours in turn define the social identity of individuals within the interacting groups. The tendency to define oneself by membershipof a group leads to intergroup discrimination, which involves favourable perceptions and behaviours directed towards the in-group, but negative perceptions and behaviours directed towards the outgroup. Intergroup discrimination leads to prejudice and stereotyping, while the processes of social facilitation and group polarization encourage extreme behaviours towards the outgroup. Groups often moderate and improve decision making, and are frequently relied upon for these benefits, such as committees and juries. A number of group biases, however, can interfere with effective decision making. For example, group polarization, formerly known as the "risky shift," occurs when people polarize their views in a moreextreme direction after group discussion. More problematic is the phenomenon of groupthink. This is a collective thinking defect that is characterized by a premature consensus or an incorrect assumption of consensus, caused bymembers of a group failing to promote views which are not consistent with the views of other members. Groupthinkoccurs in a variety of situations, including isolation of a group and the presence of a highly directive leader. Janis offered the 1961 Bay of Pigs Invasion as a historical case of groupthink. PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 51 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION Groups also affect performance and productivity. Social facilitation, for example, is a tendency to work harder and faster in the presence of others. Social facilitation increases the likelihood of the dominant response, which tends to improve performance on simple tasks and reduce it on complex tasks. In contrast, social loafing is the tendency of individuals to slack when working in a group. Social loafing is common when the task is considered unimportant and individual contributions are not easy to see. Social psychologists study group-related (collective) phenomena such as the behaviour of crowds. An important concept in this area is deindividuation, a reduced state of self-awareness that can be caused by feelings of anonymity. Deindividuation is associated with uninhibited and sometimes dangerous behaviour. It is common in crowds and mobs, but it can also be caused by a disguise, a uniform, alcohol, dark environments, or online anonymity. Group How do groups affect our behaviour? To find out, social psychologists study the variousinfluences that operate in the simplest of groups-one person in the presence of another-and those that operate in more complex groups, such as families, teams, and committees. Social Facilitation Having noticed that cyclists’ racing times were faster when they competed against each other than when they competed with a clock, Norman Triplett (1898) hypothesized that the presence of others boosts performance. To test his hypothesis, Triplett had adolescents wind a fishing reel as rapidly as possible. He discovered that they wound the reel faster in the presence of someone doing the same thing. This phenomenon of stronger performance in others’ presence is called social facilitation. For example, after a light turns green, drivers take about 15 percent less time to travel the first 100 yards when another car is beside them at the intersection than when they are alone (Towler, 1986). But on tougher tasks (learning nonsense syllables or solving complex multiplication problems), people perform less well when observers or others working on the same task are present. Further studies revealed why the presence of others sometimes helps and sometimes hinders performance (Guerin, 1986; Zajonc, 1965). When others observe us, we become aroused. This arousal strengthens the most likely response—the correct one on an easy task, an incorrect one on a difficult task. Thus, when we are being observed, we perform welllearned tasks more quickly and accurately, and unmastered tasks less quickly and accurately. James Michaels and his associates (1982) found that expert pool players who made 71 percent of their shots when alone made 80 percent when four people came to watch them. Poor shooters, who made 36 percent of their shots when alone, made only 25 percent when watched. The energizing effect of an enthusiastic audience probably contributes to the home advantage enjoyed by various sports teams. Studies of more than 80,000 college and professional athletic events in Canada, the United States, and England reveal that home teams win about 6 in 10 games (somewhat fewer for baseball and football, somewhat more for basketball and soccer-see (table 1). Table 1 Home advantage in major team sports PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 52 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION What you do well, you are likely to do even better in front of an audience, especially a friendly audience; what you normally find difficult may seem all but impossible when you are being watched. Social facilitation also helps explain a funny effect of crowding: Comedy routines that are mildly amusing to people in an uncrowded room seem funnier in a densely packed room (Aiello et al., 1983; Freedman &Perlick, 1979). As comedians and actors know, a “good house” is a full one. The arousal triggered by crowding amplifies other reactions, too. If sitting close to one another, participants in experiments like a friendly person even more, an unfriendly person even less (Schiffenbauer&Schiavo, 1976; Storms & Thomas, 1977). The practical lesson: If choosing a room for a class or setting up chairs for a gathering, have barely enough seating. Social Loafing Social facilitation experiments test the effect of others’ presence onperformance on an individual task, such as shooting pool. But what happens to performance when people perform the task as a group? In a team tug-of-war, for example, do you suppose your effort would be more than, less than, or the same as theeffort you would exert in a one-on-one tugof-war? To find out, Alan Ingham and hisfellow researchers (1974) asked blindfolded University of Massachusetts students to“pull as hard as you can” on a rope. When Ingham fooled the students into believingthree others were also pulling behind them, they exerted only 82 percent as much effort as when they knew they were pulling alone. BibbLatané (1981; Jackson & Williams, 1988) describe this diminished effort associal loafing. In 78 experiments conducted in the United States, India, Thailand,Japan, China, and Taiwan, social loafing occurred on various tasks, though it was especially common among men in individualistic cultures (Karau& Williams, 1993). In one of Latané’s experiments, blindfolded people seated in a group clapped or shouted as loud as they could while listening through headphones to the sound of loud clapping or shouting. When told they were doing it with the others, the participants produced about one third less noise than when they thought their individual efforts were identifiable. PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 53 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION Why this social loafing? First, people acting as part of a group feel less accountable and therefore worry less about what others think. Second, they may view their contribution as dispensable (Harkins & Szymanski, 1989; Kerr &Bruun, 1983). As manyleaders of organizations know-and as you have perhaps observed on student group assignments-if group members share equally in the benefits regardless of how much they contribute, some may slack off. Unless highly motivated and identified with their group, they may free-ride on the other group members’ efforts. Deindividuation So, the presence of others can arouse people (as in the social facilitation experiments) or can diminish their feelings of responsibility (as in the social loafing experiments). But sometimes the presence of others both arouses people and diminishes their sense of responsibility. The result can be uninhibited behaviourranging from a food fight in the dining hall or screaming at a basketball referee to vandalism or rioting. Abandoning normal restraints to the power of the group is termed deindividuation.To be deindividuated is to be less self-conscious and less restrained when in a group situation. Deindividuation often occurs when group participation makes people feel aroused and anonymous. In one experiment, New York University women dressed in depersonalizing Ku Klux Klan–style hoods delivered twice as much electric shock to a victim as did identifiable women (Zimbardo, 1970). (As in all such experiments, the “victim” did not actually receive the shocks.) Similarly, tribal warriors who depersonalize themselves with face paints or masks are more likely than those with exposedfaces to kill, torture, or mutilate captured enemies (Watson, 1973). Whether in a mob, at a rock concert, at a ballgame, or at worship, to lose self-consciousness (to become deindividuated) is to become more responsive to the group experience. EFFECTS OF GROUP INTERACTION Group Polarization and Groupthink We have examined the conditions under which being in the presenceof others can Motivate people to exert themselves or tempt them to free-ride on the efforts ofothers. Make easy tasks easier and difficult tasks harder. Enhance humour or fuel mob violence. Research shows that interacting with others can similarly have both bad and goodeffects. Group Polarization Educational researchers have noted that, over time, initial differences between groups of college students tend to grow. If the first-year students at College X tend to be more intellectually oriented than those at College Y, that difference will probably be amplified by the time they are seniors. And if the political conservatism of students who join fraternities and sororities is greater than that of students who do not, the gap in the political attitudes of the two groups will probably widen as they progress through college (Wilson et al., 1975). Similarly, notes Eleanor Maccoby (2002) from her decades of observing gender development, girls talk more intimately than boys do and play and fantasize less aggressively-and these gender differences widen over time as they interact mostly with their own gender. PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 54 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION FIGURE 2. Group polarization. If a group is likeminded, discussion strengthens its prevailing opinions. Talking over racial issues increased prejudice in a high-prejudice group of high school students and decreased it in a low prejudice group (Myers & Bishop, 1970) This enhancement of a group’s prevailing tendencies-called group polarizationoccurs when people within a group discuss an idea that most of them either favour or oppose. Group polarization can have beneficial results, as when it amplifies a soughtafter spiritual awareness or reinforces the resolve of those in a self-help group, or strengthens feelings of tolerance in a low-prejudice group. But it can also have dire consequences. George Bishop and I discovered that when high-prejudice students discussed racial issues, they became more prejudiced (Figure 2). (Low-prejudice students became even more accepting.) The experiment’s ideological separation and polarization finds a seeming parallel in the growing polarization of American politics. The percentage of landslide counties-voting 60 percent or more for one presidential candidate-- increased from 26 percent in 1976 to 48 percent in 2004 (Bishop, 2004). More and more, people are living near and learning from others who think as they do. One experiment brought together small groups of citizens in liberal Boulder, Colorado, and other groups down in conservative Colorado Springs, to discuss global climate change, affirmative action, and same-sex unions. Although the discussionsincreased agreement within groups, those in Boulder generally moved further left andthose in Colorado Springs moved further right (Schkadeet al., 2006). Thus ideological separation +deliberation =polarization between groups. The polarizing effect of interaction among the like-minded PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 55 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION applies also to suicide terrorists. After analysing terrorist organizations around the world, psychologists Clark McCauley and Mary Segal (1987; McCauley, 2002) noted that the terrorist mentality does not erupt suddenly. Rather, it usually arises among people who get together because of a grievance and then become more and more extreme as they interact in isolation from any moderating influences. Increasingly, group members (who may be isolated with other “brothers” and “sisters” in camps) categorize the world as “us” against “them” (Moghaddam, 2005; Qirko, 2004). Suicide terrorism is virtually never done on a personal whim, reports researcher Ariel Merari (2002). The like-minded echo chamber will continue to polarize people, speculates the 2006 U.S. National Intelligence estimate: “We assess that the operational threat from self-radicalized cells will grow.” The Internet provides a medium for group polarization. Its tens of thousands of virtual groups enable bereaved parents, peacemakers, and teachers to find solace and support from kindred spirits. But the Internet also enables people who share interests in government conspiracy, extra-terrestrialvisitors, White supremacy, or citizen militias to find one another and to find support for their shared suspicions (McKenna &Bargh, 1998). Group Think Does group interaction ever distort important decisions? Social psychologist Irving Janis began to think so as he read historian Arthur M. Schlesinger, Jr.’s account of how President John F. Kennedy and his advisers blundered into an ill-fated plan to invade Cuba with 1400 CIA-trained Cuban exiles. When the invaders were easily captured and soon linked to the U.S. government, Kennedy wondered in hindsight, “How could we have been so stupid? To find out, Janis (1982) studied the decision-making procedures that led to the fiasco. He discovered that the soaring morale of the recently elected president and his advisers fostered undue confidence in the plan. To preserve the good group feeling, any dissenting views were suppressed or self-censored, especially after President Kennedy voiced his enthusiasm for the scheme. Since no one spoke strongly against the idea, everyone assumed consensus support. To describe this harmonious but unrealistic group thinking, Janis coined the term groupthink. Janis and others then examined other historical fiascos—the failure to anticipate the 1941 Japanese attack on Pearl Harbor, the escalation of the Vietnam War, theU.S. Watergate cover-up, the Chernobyl nuclear reactor accident (Reason, 1987), andthe U.S. space shuttle Challengerexplosion (Esser&Lindoerfer, 1989). They discovered that in these cases, too, groupthink was fed by overconfidence, conformity, self-justification, and group polarization. Groupthink surfaced again, reported the bipartisan U.S. Senate Intelligence Committee (2004), when “personnel involved in the Iraq WMD issue demonstrated several aspects of groupthink: examining few alternatives, selective gathering ofinformation, pressure to conform within the group or withhold criticism, and collective rationalization.” This groupthink led analysts to “interpret ambiguous evidence as conclusively indicative of a WMD program as well as ignore or minimize evidence that Iraq did not have [WMD] programs.” Despite such fiascos and tragedies, two heads are better than one in solving some types of problems. Knowing this, Janis also studied instances in which U.S. presidents and their advisers collectively made good decisions, such as when the Truman administration PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 56 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION formulated the Marshall Plan, which offered assistance to Europe after World War II, and when the Kennedy administration worked to keep the Soviets from installing missiles in Cuba. In such instances-and in the business world, too, Janis believed-groupthink is prevented when a leader welcomes various opinions, invites experts’ critiques of developing plans, and assigns people to identify possible problems. Just as the suppression of dissent bends a group toward bad decisions, so open debate often shapes good ones. This is especially so with diverse groups, whose varied perspectives enable creative or superior outcomes (Nemeth &Ormiston, 2007;). None of us is as smart as all of us. SOCIAL INFLUENCE-CONFORMITY Individual behaviour is influenced by the group he/she belongs to. Have you ever wondered why you prefer to go along with the group? In conformity, your response to social activity is indirect. The group does not need to ask that you join them, but because everyone seems to agree and act in a certain way, you are likely to join them. (Lahley, 1998; 530). Conformity is yielding to group pressure. This may take different forms and sometimes could be as a result of some motives other than group pressure. Conformity is a change in belief or behaviour in response to real or imagined group pressure. The presence of others whether actual or implied results in conformity. We tend to do in private what we think people should do or the right thing to do. In conformity, there is the outward expression of the norm and the private acceptance. The two must agree in order for one to feel comfortable. When one outwardly conforms to what she/he does not privately accept, then one is likely to experience dissonance or disagreement. Sometimes people decide to agree with the group just to reduce dissonance. Factors Affecting Conformity People tend to conform because factors like Group size, Group Unity (cohesiveness), fearOf ridicule, Task difficulty, privacy, group norms among influence our individual response and behaviour in social situations. i. Group size and Unanimity (Majority) Based on experiments, it was found that the size of the group, and the level of agreement among members affect conformity. When one person tries to influence another, the level of conformity is low, where two try to influence one person, the level of conformity rises, with three people the level rises higher and beyond 5 people conformity levels drops off or even decreases. Once the majority is unanimous, the pressure to conform is high, but where one person disagrees within the group, conformity decreases. Recent findings have shown that conformity does increase with group size of up to 8 members or more. ii. Minority Influence Though not common, it does happen that the minority in a group can influence the behaviour or the beliefs of the majority. This minority influence is usually indirect, and occurs slowly, and involves only a moderate change in the majority view; it is possible for an individual to resist group pressure. (Peplau& Sears 2002; David & Turner 2000). PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 57 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION Minority Influence – Minority position must be consistent with current trends of events, and must avoid appearing rigid and dogmatic providing room for some degrees of flexibility, This will help them not to appear hell bent on their ideas and will encourage acceptance by the majority. Minorities do hold strong views and are more concerned over being right. As a result, they do overestimate the number of people who share their views which is usually less than they perceive, though in a way this helps them to remain resolute against majority position, which does pay off in most cases. The minority views might encourage the majority to access why heminorities are adamant in their views or positions and this might result in change, no matter how small in the majority. iii. Fear of Ridicule. Usually wrong answers or inappropriate behaviour is ridiculed by others, such that when the group is wrong, the fear of being ridiculed suppresses the minority view resulting in conformity to the group. iv. Ambiguity of the situation – Task Difficulty. When faced with difficult tasks, people are likely going to yield to majority answer, even if this answer is not correct, especially if the majority feels confident that they are right. In a situation where people become uncertain, they tend to rely more on other people’s opinions thus increasing conformity to group norms. v. Privacy in Responses. In the face of group pressure, it is easier for the individual not to conform if asked to respond privately than when asked to respond publicly. Thus there seems to be less conformity in anonymous res ponses or where responses are given in private or in writing. Anonymous responses decrease conformity though it does not remove it or make it disappear. vi. Group Norms Group norms are very powerful in influencing behaviour. People are mostly influenced by three powerful motives like the desire to be liked or accepted by others, the desire to be right and have an accurate understanding of the world knowing what is right and what is wrong, and finally the desire to receive rewards and avoid punishment. Varieties of Conformity. A. Normative social influence. Norms, as defined by a people affect their behaviour. Normative social influence involves altering our behaviour to meet the expectations of others. It is also seen as tactics of getting people to like us. Reasons for normative social influence have been given as follow: The need for approval or acceptance by the group. The norms guarding the group distaste the behaviour of its individual members. PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 58 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION Norms are societal standard or what society defines as acceptable and expects members to conform. Culture that emphasizes the welfare of the individual over that of the group is likely going to have less conformity in its members than cultures that emphasizes the welfare of the group above that of the individual. Some cultures in Nigeria might increase conformity on its members compared to some western cultures. Think of your own culture; is it individualistic or collectivistic in nature? B. Informational Social Influence. This type of influence is based on our tendency to depend on others as sources of information about the social world. Our behaviour is mostly influenced by the information we receive from others. The certainty about what is right or wrong reduces our confidence and motivates us towards conformity, while certainty reduces our reliance on our ability to make decision and reduces our likelihood to conform. We are likely to be influenced for the following reasons? The belief that others have some information’s that we do not have, so we conform or agree to go along. If our need for direction and information is met, we are likely to conform. Group Process in Conformity. Risky Shift – this is the phenomena where the group is likely going to advice the individual to take risks more than the average individual advice of its members. There are factors that might predict how risky or cautious these group advices will be, which include: Group polarization – it has been found that discussions between group members with similar attitudes in order to reach difficult decisions, strengthens the individual inclinations of the members. (Cooper et al 2004). The group can take extreme decisions than the mean of the individual member’s position which could be towards a riskier or more cautious direction. Group polarization could be as a result of: Exchange of information – usually relevant information from members might result in supportive arguments beyond what the individual had thought of – example of informational social influence. Definition of the identity of the group compared to other groups normative social influence. Social categorization process which occurs in three steps: 1. Seeing self as a member of a group (the in group) 2. Identify these in group characteristics as different from the out group. 3. Stereo- typing self as a member of the group (Cooper at al 2004) Group think – A mode of thinking in which the desire to reach unanimous agreement override the motivation to adopt proper, rational decision making procedures (Janis 1971, 1982).Usually there is a separation of decent from group harmony, excessive cohesion, close knit group and a direction leader to enhance group link. PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 59 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION The information given above has shown that for the individual to survive in society there is the need to positively conform to some societal norms. The society and the groups must deliberately choose to set standards than are progressive for her members. Norms must be re-evaluated from time to time in order to maintain positive conformity especially for the younger population or members. COMPLIANCE Compliance involves direct request from one person to another or from a group to another. In compliance, people yield to this request which is different from conformity where there is no request yet one feels the need to conform. A request places less demandon the individual and allows one the liberty to comply or not. Behaviour comes as a result of compliance to a request, which in most cases is seen as coming from a peer or a friend. Principles of Compliance There are many techniques for gaining compliance usually through requesting, selling orconvincing. These techniques are all based on the following principles. Friendship/Liking There is a higher likelihood of our responding to request from our friends or people we like more than non acquaintance or people we do not like Commitment /Consistency Once we have taken a position or are committed to certain action, it is easier for us to comply with request that agrees with our position. Scarcity We appreciate and value what is scarce or not readily available and are likely going to comply with requests that focus on scarcity than those that make no reference to scarcity. This is seen as a once in life time opportunity. Reciprocity The idea of giving back to those who have given to us is easier than given to those we have no obligation to. We are more likely to comply to request from those groups of persons who have given to us before, agreeing with the notion that “one good turn deserves another” or “treat others the way the treat us”. It is assumedthat unsolicited gifts force the receiver to reciprocate in line with implied or stated requests. (Tourangeu 2004) Social Validation Request for actions that agree with the norm and accepted by all are likely to be complied with than those that deviate from what is acceptable. What we do and think about must agree with what others are doing or thinking. Knowing that others have done the same or complied spur likelihood to also complied. Authority PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 60 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION It is easier to comply with requests from someone that has a higher authority or appears to have authority over us or more than we do. Techniques in compliance Based on the principles above, the following techniques are used in compliance. i. Ingratiation Techniques This is getting others to like us so that they will be more willing to agree to our request based on the principle of friendship or liking. Impression management techniques are also used here to achieve ingratiation or increase compliance. Some ingratiation techniques include: ii. Flattery – praising others. This could backfire if taken as insincere by the receiver. Improving Appearance – physical attractiveness have been shown to succeed as compliance agent Positive nonverbal cues -Smiles, handshakes, head nods, sitting next to all send signals that are to some positive in nature. Small favours for target persons – Increases likelihood to comply. Incidental similarity – similarities in names, birthdays and towns are likely to increase the tendency to comply to the persons requests. Cooperation with others – showing them that you are on their side might make them comply. Foot – in – the – door – (FITD) Technique. In this technique, requests begin with small ones and when granted, move on to make larger ones, usually the desired request. In this case, the chance of compliance is increased after the initial small request for compliance was successful. Most free samples or free trials in commercials for products capitalize on this technique. The catch word is that once you accept the free sample, it becomes easier to request that you buy the product. This technique induces increased compliance. Because it relies on the principle of consistency, where refusing the larger request will not be consistent with our first behaviour of complying with the small request, it has been found also that when people comply with small request this leads to their complying to larger ones for two reasons: iii. People find it easier to comply to requests that cost little in terms of money and input. People feel committed to the cause or issues involved when they comply to smaller requests. (Burger and Guadagne 2003) Door – in – the – face (DIF) Technique. Door-in-the-face is another way of obtaining compliance that is almost the opposite of – FITD technique. Here one begins with asking for a big favour or making an almost impossible request that is likely going to be turned down. Once request is denied; the person making the request agrees that it was excessive or asking for too much, and compromises by making a smaller request – usually this smaller request is what the individual really wanted initially. Idea is that when this first request is compared with the first one the individual is likely going to comply with the second partly because there was a promise – middle ground so to speak, and also that seemed more reasonable than the fist (perceptual contrast). PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 61 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION Note that if the second request was presented without the first one, it is more likely that the request will not result in compliance. The approach relies on the reciprocity norm or principle. The first technique slams the door in the face of the person requesting. Finally, DIF compliance is emotional because when first large request is turned down, we feel “Bad” negative emotions or guilty or “bad” negative emotions, so we look for ways to reduce these negative emotions in order to feel comfortable. Thus the second request provides the opportunity to make “amends” thus we are more likely to make use of the chance to make up and relieve our guilt or negative emotions (miller 2002). iv. That -is- not -all techniques. In this form of gaining compliance an initial request is followed by an extra incentive, before the target person is able to make any response. The extra incentive in this technique is an effective means of increasing the chance of others saying “yes” or complying to various requests. v. Playing- hard - to – get. Techniques This technique suggests that a person or object is scarce and hard to obtain. We tend to place more value on what is rare, scarce or not easily available. Thus we are ready to go the extra mile or put in more effort to obtain the items or outcome. This technique is used in the area of romance, job hunting and even marketing. Playing hard to get increases the desirability of the individual item or request to the point where the receiver’s choice of compliance is higher than will be if request or item is available or not scarce. vi. The fast approaching – Deadline Technique. Still using the scarcity principle and the fact that we place more value on what is scarce is the deadline technique. Here a time frame is attached to a behaviour or item beyond which it is assumed that it will not be available. This technique increases compliance when the target person is told that he/she has limited time to take advantage of some offer or to obtain some item or agree to some requests. Usually this is a sales strategy that works well for people in business with the sole intension of busting sales rather than the claimed notion that stock will run out. This message of deadline still has indirect condition that implies a rise in price of deadline is missed which cases are not the case. In fact, most at times the price goes down after enough sales. vii. Low – Ball (lb) Approach This technique tries to obtain (one oral/ verbal) commitment to do something, after this commitment has been made; the cost of fulfilling the commitment is increased. This process of gaining compliance has a deal or an offer to make it less attractive to the receiver after he/she has accepted he deal or offer. Success of this technique is dependent on the importance the individual places in the initial commitment, because s/he feels obligated to keep the promise even when conditions that led to the commitment no longer exist or doing so might cost more than it was planned for. (Burger & Cornelius 2003) The ideas above have shown that for compliance to succeed, the individuals concerned must make the interaction less stressful and allow each other appreciate the quality of the product. Where the need to comply is seen as a must, gives the individual a feeling of PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 62 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION insecurity. Society must evaluate what she wants for the group that will be accepted by her individual members with minimal stress. OBEDIENCE Obedience leads to behaviour change as result of response to a demand usually from those in authority or authority figures (Blass 2004). To what extend can we be affected by demands from others, and can these demands influence us to hurt others? We will try to make sense of why people obey, identify the factors that result in obedience, the destructive aspects of obedience, and how we can resist destructive obedience. Reasons for obedience The Milgram’s experiments on obedience where twenty – six of his forty participants obeyed the experimental instruction all the way and supposedly administered up to 450 volt maximum shock (electric) level to the learner was an amazing account of how far people can go, even if reluctantly, to obey those in authority or those seem to have authority despite their own misgivings about the effect of the obedience. The high rate of obedience noticed by Milgram was attributed to the following factors: 1. The presence of the perceived authority figure. This ensured obedience in two ways Diffusion of responsibility. The belief that the authority figure is ultimately in charge relieved the person from following orders from taking personal responsibility for his/her actions. Serve as Agents of Force. They tend to intimidate us into following orders. Fear of the consequences of disobedience may lead us to obey orders. 2. The timing of the request made. If people are not given time to think through what is being demanded of them, they are most likely to obey more than those who have time to think over the demand. It has been found that once people do not have time to think through a demand, they become more vulnerable to persuasive attempts. 3. Graduation of demand. This refers to demand from the less stressful to the more stressful one or from a small demand to more demanding one. One is obeying increasing demands. In our example, if the demand for 450 volts was made at the onset, not many people would have obeyed. But starting with 15 volts and adding to that gradually narrows the gap between the less to the highest volts making obedience more rational than it would have been. Increasing the shock level gradually is a good example of the foot – in– the–door technique in compliance. Once one has committed to administering the lesser shock, then chances of obeying further instructions to administer higher shocks becomes much easier. 4. Psychological Distance. Our obedience will depend on how we feel between our actions and the result of those actions. It also means the degree to which we can dissociate ourselves from the consequences. 5. Socialization. PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 63 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION We learn from significant others like our parents, Teachers, older siblings as we grow up. It is assumed that obedience is also learned and may become a difficult habit to resist. (Brown 1986) 6. Social situations Social roles place certain individuals at advantage in relationships. The Parent Child, Teacher – Student, Doctor – Patient, Employer – Employee, roles where the parent, Teacher, Doctor, Employer have power more than the child, Student, Patient and Employee in this relationship or settings respectively. How society empowers these groups when interacting can increase or reduce the power these authority figures have. If these roles are changed, will the use of power also change? Most likely no. Factors that affect obedience In addition to the reasons people obey according to Mailgram’s experiment, the following factors were also listed as affecting obedience or influencing people to obey. Status and prestige of authority figure. The following forms of social power influences people to obey for the following reasons: Experts Social Power The authority figure is able to command obedience because it is believed that this person is knowledgeable and is a responsible expert. Legitimate Social Power This person can influence others to obey because it is assumed that he/she has the right or legal authority to tell them what to do. (Blass & Schmitt, 2001) Behaviour of others. If other people in a similar situation disobey orders or demands, chances are that others would do likewise. If demands are made to a group and some members do not obey these demands, the level of obedience for the group will drop or decrease. Personality Characteristics. Not everyone is obedient to Authority in the same way. But those with Authoritarianism personalities are prone to follow authority figures without questioning. They also have the tendency to react violently against people identify by authority figures as not for the values of their in – group (Blass 1999) an example is the evident found suggesting that German soldiers who high on authoritanism obeyed orders to all Jews during world war 22 compared to other German men similar in age and background. (Steiner &Fahreberg 2000) Resisting Destructive Obedience Obeying orders or demands from authority figures have been shown to be very destructive where the recipient is destructive. Where the recipient is expected to “obey before complaining” sometimes they do not have time to reflect on this demands or PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 64 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION orders but follow through immediately. Some strategies have been suggested to help people resist the effects of destructive obedience. Personal Responsibility Reminder that those exposed to taking commands from authority figures are also responsible for any harm inflicted or produced. This means that there is a shift from those in authority assuming responsibility for those obeying authority figures. Destructive Commands should be seen as inappropriate Beyond certain points, total submissions to destructive commands are inappropriate. Here, models acting roles of rejecting commands should be made available and individuals exposed to them. Question Authority Figures. When motives, reasons and relational behind certain commands are questioned by those receiving such commands, this reveals a lot that could make these authority figures rethink and re-evaluate their actions. Know that authority figures have the power to command obedience but that this power is not irresistible. Though most of those authority figures have the machinery to enforce obedience, and resisting may be very dangerous, it is however not impossible. Most challenges to authority figures cost a lot but people have tried and won and have also changed the course of history and improve the quality of life for their fellow human beings. People like Mahatma Gandi, Martin Luther king Jr of USA and Nelson Mandela of South Africa among others are examples of people who have dared to challenged authorities in their times and changed the course of history for their people, and for humanity. Know the power authority figures have to command blind obedience. Knowing that Authority figures can command such blind obedience from subordinates can help people to prepare ahead of time on how to react during such occasions. Individuals can resist blind commands and help others do likewise if armed with the knowledge above. This study has revealed that authority figure is major factor in obedience and compliance which society also accepts and does little to discourage it so that the individual remain in a subordinated relationship However, he/she must be empowered to use strategies that will help especially in handling destructive obedience, while not underestimating the authority figures. PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOUR – HELPING BEHAVIOUR Prosocial behaviour focuses on behaviours that are intended to be of benefit to others, which might include helping, comforting, cooperating, sharing, showing concern, defending, donating, and reassuring. What benefits others could change with time and place, and will depend also on our definition. This action might not provide any direct benefit to the person helping, and might require the individual to make some sacrifice. Any act intended to benefit another person is helping behaviour. PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 65 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION Bystander Intervention. We learn early in life to help others. These might vary from culture to culture, where some cultures might support rewards for helping others while some might encourage the behaviour without the expectation of any reward. The presence of others when help is required may affect the individual willingness to help in emergency situation. Latene’ and Darley (1970) are of the view that the chances for people to engage in the Prosocial act of helping others requires series of decision by them as they witness emergence , which must include the following: 1. Notice that something is wrong. 2. Define it as a situation that requires help. 3. Decide on whether to take personal responsibility. 4. Implement the decision to intervene. A. Notice that something is wrong or unusual. Emergencies usually occur when we least expect them. This means that we are mostly not prepared to respond immediately partly because we are not sure of what to do, or we have not fully appreciated the extent of the emergency. Because there are so many activities going on around us, we may not pay attention to all of them, this might include emergencies. Our mood affects how we notice events in our surrounding. People in good moods pay attention to others which makes them more likely to help. (Dovidio and penner 2004). People in deep thought or not in good moods may not be able to concentrate or pay attention to events or others and may just be unable to notice when anything unusual happens and might not be able to help. People who are too busy may be preoccupied with what they are doing, that they may not be able to notice any unusual things happening around them. Milgram (1970) found that urban dwellers may restrict attention to personally relevant events, and may not notice strangers and their needs as way of coping with stimulus overload in their environment. This view was supported by the works of Heade and Yousif (1992) &Yousif and korte (1995) who found urban dwellers to be less likely to help compared to rural dwellers. We can infer from their study of different countries that, urban centres are made up of more strangers than the rural settlements. People in rural areas are few and know themselves better. B. Define what is happening as a situation that requires help. Even when we are able to notice that something is not right or is unusual, we may not be able to help if our definition of the event is not associated with an emergency. Interpreting the event as an emergency requires that we have all the information we need to evaluate the situation. In most cases we are left with little information, or distorted information that leaves us confused about what is happening and this reduces our willingness to help. Our ability to interpret emergencies correctly helps us to respond quickly and decisively to them. The presence of others, have been found to affect the individual’s willingness to help, due to what is referred to as pluralistic ignorance. This is the tendency of the individual surrounded by stranger to hesitate and not help in emergency, but rely on these bystanders for information (which in most cases is not accurate)and uses this to justify his/her failure to offer help. But if these individual is surrounded by friends, may communicate more and the inhibiting effect will be less.(Latene’ and Darley 1968, Rutkowski,Gruder and PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 66 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION Romer,1983) Evidence has shown that when people are alone they may be able to define events as emergency faster, and even respond or decide to help immediately. Two friends may also respond f aster while two strangers may not respond at all or do so slowly. (Latane’and Rodin 1969). C. Deciding whether to take personal responsibility. Accepting personal responsibility by any individual will be less likely in the presence of many bystanders. The phenomena is also known as bystander effect which means diffusion of responsibility- which is the denial of personal responsibility believing that someone else might do what is necessary or right. Individuals when alone as bystander take responsibility, and act because the options are few or none at all. D. Deciding on the type of help to give. The bystander’s competence to help in a given situation can go a long way in deciding to help, whether alone or in the presence of other bystanders. When we know that some bystanders are competent to help more than ourselves, the more likely for diffusion of responsibility to increase. But if the individual bystander believes he/she is more equipped to help, the chances of helping will increase, and will likely do so immediately regardless of other bystanders. Sometimes, it is not apathy that inhibits bystanders from helping, it may be the issues of competence as discussed where he/she, may sincerely want to help but is not competent to do so. E. Implementing the decision to intervene. This is the point at which the bystander decides to finally engage in a helping act. Once all the hurdles from the four steps have been crossed, this remaining step might be hindered by our fear of the potential consequences of our behaviour if we fail. Here one must weigh the positive against the negative effects of helping ,and depending on the outcome, could result in our helping or not helping at this final stage.(Fritzche,Finkelstein and penner.2000). Factors in Helping Behaviour. In addition personality factors helping behaviours. to the bystander effect, some situational, emotional and among others will be looked at as they either enhance or inhibit our 1. Situational Factors. Helping those we like Those we like are mostly family members or friends, and most of what we have discussed so far centred on helping strangers. The following reasons have been given for why we tend to help generally. i. ii. iii. iv. Age and Race – Similarity of a stranger to you in terms of age and race may increase your likelihood to offer help. Physically attractive victims receive more help compared to unattractive ones. Women in distress are more likely to be helped by men. This could be due to gender difference, sexual attraction or because women are more willing to ask for help more than men. Holding similar values encourages helping behaviour. PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 67 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION The evolutionary theory is of the view that we are likely to help family members because this behaviour will contribute to the survival of our prehistoric ancestors (Buss 2003). Family ties, evidence show that people are likely to donate organs to family members than to strangers. While some views may see this behaviour in terms of greater attachment or a stronger sense of social obligation to relatives than to strangers or others, the evolutionary view will see the case of one donating to a family member to save life, as helping to ensure the survival of the genes shared with the family member who receives the organ. Helping those who mimic us. Mimicry is the automatic tendency to imitate the behaviour of those we come in contact with. This tendency could also be unconscious. Mimicry has been seen to enhance liking and results in increased helping behaviour by those who have been mimicked. This helping behaviour is not only offered to those who mimicked them but extended to others too. (Van Baaren et al 2004). Helping those who are not responsible for their problem. We may find that it is easier for people to help accident victims and people we evaluate as victims of brutal attacks, because our attribution to their problems will be that they were not responsible for these problems. To the accident victims we may attribute their problem to rough driving, bad car or bad road, thus not their fault. Generally people are less likely to help those they believe to have caused their problems or are responsible for their problems (Higgins and Shaw. 1999; Weiner, 1980). Exposure to Prosocial models The bystander who offers to help provides a model for other bystanders, and is likely to increase helping behaviour in these bystanders. Other findings are that modelled helping behaviour on television, unlike the modelling of negative behaviour such as aggression on television has increase the helping behaviour of the viewers. 2. Emotional factors Emotional states have been associated with helping behaviour just like any other form of behaviour. Positive or negative emotions have their effects on the helping behaviour of the individual. Positive emotions People are likely to help when in a good mood rather than a bad one. Pleasant fragrance makes people feel better and improve helping behaviour. Lemon or floral odour have been found to increase the willingness to help. Other findings are that people in good moods may not engaged in helping behaviour especially if it means doing something difficult and unpleasant (Rosenhan, Salovey and Hargis 1981). Negative emotions An individual in a negative mood is less likely to help others. Because unhappy people are pre- occupied with their own problems, they are less likely to engage in any helping behaviour. But in cases where helping is likely to improve one’s mood and make him PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 68 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION feel better, helping behaviour is more likely when one is in a bad mood compared to a neutral mood. This negative emotion must not be too intense, the emergency not complicated, and if helping will be interesting and satisfying not dull and unrewarding (Amato1986; Cialdini, kenrickand bowman 1982 cunningham et al 1990). 3. Personality and Prosocial Behaviour Altruistic Personality Multiple aspect of the personality is necessary for prosocial behaviour, and altruistic personality is high on five dimensions found mostly in people who engage in Prosocial behaviour du ring emergencies. The personality characteristic or disposition includes empathy, belief in a just world, and acceptance of social responsibility, having an internal locus of control and not being egocentric. These five dispositions are as follow: i. Empathy Empathy is rare among people high in aggressiveness but people who engage in helping behaviour are higher in empathy than those who do not. Empathic people are usually described as responsible, socialized, conforming, tolerant, self-controlled, and highly motivated - make good impressions. ii. Belief in a just world People high in helping behaviour believe the world to be fair and behaviour is rewarded if good, and punished if bad. They also believe that helping others is the right thing to do and not expect anything in return; but that helping also results in benefits for the helper for his/her good work. iii. Social Responsibility Those who help also believe that each person is responsible for doing his/her best in helping people in need. iv. Internal Locus of Control We have a choice of which way to behave, either to maximize good outcomes or minimize bad outcomes. People who do not help have external locus of control and see outcomes in terms of pure luck, fate or people in high places. v. Low egocentrism Altruistic people tend not to be self-absorbed and competitive. Basic motivation in helping behaviour The reasons people could be motivated to engage in prosocial behaviour are many and most theories focus on the people’sdesire for rewards and the avoidance of punishment. Is it then rewarding to help, and does punishment follow the lack of engagement in helping behaviour? The following views were put forth to explain why people are motivated to help. Empathy – Altruism hypothesis This view proposes that some prosocial behaviour are motivated mainly by the desire to help someone in need and by the fact that it feels good to help. Altruistic PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 69 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION – unselfish helping people are willing to help even when the cost is high because have empathy for the individual in need. Increased information or additional information increases empathy which influences helping behaviour. Another view is that the desire to help could also come from our need to relieve ourselves from the additional information received which is seen as a selfish reason. When many people need help, it is not possible to feel empathy for all of them. In some cases empathy reduces with large numbers of people. The individual usually will decide on helping one person from the group known as selective altruism. Most Organizations involved in charity work use photograph of one person or child to arouse empathy so that help can be directed towards this individual. Generally altruistic behaviour results in positive emotions. Helping make us feel good. Negative – State Relief Model Here altruistic behaviour could result in the individual feeling bad after perceiving a person in need, and will want to help just to relief this bad feeling. Improving negative mood becomes the reason for wanting to help someone in need. One might not need to feel empathy before helping. Empathic Joy Hypothesis The view here suggests that the individual is likely to help because the reward of accomplishing something is expected. The individual will feel joy and have a sense of satisfaction for making some positive impact on the lives of people. The motivation to help is really the positive emotion anticipated at the end by the helper. This requires feedback from the victim who has been helped. The combination of empathy and expected feedback increased helping behaviour more than either empathy or expected feedback alone. Arousal – Cost – Reward (ACR) model This model was introduced and revised by Piliavan et al (1969, 1981) to cover emergency and non-emergency helping behaviours. This mode identified two distinct concepts; Arousal as the basic motivational construct which is an emotional response to the need of others. The motivation is to relieve unpleasant experiences that come from the distress aroused by the victim’s need for help. The cost-reward involves the cognitive processes used to assess the cost of helping or not helping. Cost for helping might include time lost, effort, risk or danger to self, embarrassment, interference with ongoing activity, mental stress. The cost for not helping might be guilt feelings, blame from others, self-blame from knowing that another is suffering. Rewards here for helping might include fame, gratitude from victim and relatives, self satisfaction, avoiding guilt, money . This cost-reward may vary from person to person, and even from situation to situation for the same person. The information here have explained the reasons and motivation behind bystander’s prosocial behaviours. We will always come across people in need, and or decision to help or not to help will have to be made on a daily basis. Society must make prosocial behaviour rewarding and less tasking for people, so that relief for those in dire need will be available. Activities aimed at motivating and increasing PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 70 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION empathy should be the focus of society, and prosocial behaviour encouraged by all. AGGRESSION-PERSPECTIVES, CAUSES PREVENTION AND CONTROL Aggression is seen always as involving physical or symbolic behaviour with the intention of harming someone. The intention or reasons for being Aggressive are many, but usually focus on either of the following two goals. One of the intentions for being Aggressive could be to satisfy some needs known as Instrumental aggression, while the second goal or reason could be from a desire to hurt someone known as Hostile aggression – usually directed at the object of Aggression which could be an individual or a group. Aggression could also be natural or pathological. The natural aggression sometimes known as Positive Aggression is mostly directed at self-defence or other form of social injustice, while the Pathological Aggression comes from within as a result of frustration which is mostly hostile in nature, and comes with the intention to harm someone all the time. Social Causes of Aggression Most of the time the actions of others or what they say result in arousing aggressive feelings from us. Similarly, some happenings or events that do not give us the freedom to act the way we want might lead to Aggression. Some of the major social causes are as follow: 1. Frustration The view that aggression is always a consequence of frustration and that frustration always leads to some form of aggression has been aired by Neal Miller, John Dollard et al (1939) in their popular frustration – aggression hypothesis. Frustration leading to some form of aggression does not always find release at the source of frustration. Sometimes the aggression is redirected, transferred or displayed to a lower target or another target at an opportune time. 2. Direct Provocation Physical or verbal provocation is a strong cause of human aggression. Provocation is actions by others that tend to trigger attention from the recipient, often because these actions are perceived as stemming from malicious intent. Once people are at the receiving end of Aggression, the tendency is to return as much Aggression as was received or more, especially if we are sure that the other party meant to harm us in the first place. There are three types of provocation; i. Condescension – involves the Expression of arrogance or disdain by others (Harris 1993) ii. Harsh and unjustified criticism; if criticism is directed at attacking the person not the behaviour, can provoke aggression (Baron, 1993) iii. Derogatory statements about families. Most people might tolerate attack on their persons, but might not stand insults or attacks directed at members of their families. 3. Heightened Arousal. Heightened arousal in the form of emotions could result in the expression of aggression in response to provocation, frustration or other factors. PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 71 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION According to the Excitatory Transfer Theory, physiological arousal tends to die slowly, and a portion may persist, continue and be carried from one situation to another. Usually the presence of this stored or repressed emotional arousals termed residual arousal, may not be noticed by the individual or might be noticed, but is likely going to be attributed to present source of irritation (Zillman, 1983, 1988,1994, Tayler et al, 1991). 4. Exposure to media violence. Media violence is the depictions of violent actions in the mass media. Exposure to, or witnessing aggression, results in the expression of aggression and violent behaviour by viewers. The exposure to violence by the media seems to strengthened beliefs, expectations and other cognitive processes related to aggression. The effect of media violence is real, lasting and important, and has implications on the society in terms of her safety and wellbeing of victims of aggressive actions. High levels of a ggression, was formed among people who viewed violent films or programmes. (Bandura, Ross & Ross 1963; Busshman&Huesman 2001) Other findings reveal that the more violent films or television programs people watched as children, the higher the rate or level of their Aggression as Teenagers or Adults, and also the more they are likely to be arrested for violent crimes. These findings were replicated (repeated) in other countries like Australia, Findland, Israel, Poland and South Africa with similar results. This means that violence as viewed through the media results in Aggression, and this cuts across cultures. Recent works reveal aggression was not only as a result of violent films, but also could come from news programs, violent lyrics in popular music, and violent video games among others. (Anderson, Carnegey& Eubanks 2003; Anderson et al 2004) 5. Pornography and Aggression Pornography is erotic material viewed in any of the media. The association between the viewing of pornographic films or erotic materials and several forms of anti-social behaviour that includes sexual relation to violent crimes has been established. Most child molesters and rapists confirm that these crimes were committed immediately after viewing erotic materials (Silbert& Pines 1984; Marshal 1989). Men high in promiscuity and hostility who view pornography are associated with sexual Aggression more than men low in promiscuity and hostility who viewed pornographic materials. (Malamouth et al 2000). Aggressive pornography is associated with violence against women. Most men who are likely to abuse and exploit women may also be those who view a lot of pornography. 6. Sexual Jealousy Real or imagined infidelity occurs across societies. These cultures of honour view such behaviours by women as threatening male honour and do lead to drastic responses. Cultures where daughters are found not be virgins result in violence to protect the family honour. In cultures of honour, jealousy becomes a very powerful cause for aggression than on other cultures (Blass et al 1992; Vandello& Cohen 2003; Puente & Cohen 220, Packer 2004). PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 72 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION 7. Cultural factors in Aggression Cultural beliefs, norms and expectations in a given culture suggest that aggression is appropriate or even required under certain circumstances. Some cultures emphasize what is called “Cultures of Honour” where there are strong norms, suggesting that aggression is an appropriate response to insults to one’s honour. Sexual jealousy is another avenue where the norm of one’s honour comes to play. Personal Causes of Aggression Some personal characteristics make certain people more vulnerable than others in the expression of aggression. While some may remain calm in the presence of provocation and frustration, others easily react aggressively to the slightest provocation or frustrations. Some of the traits or personal characteristics likely to play key roles in explaining aggression are as follow: 1. The Type A behaviour pattern. People exhibiting the type A behaviour pattern usually have high levels of competitiveness, time urgency and hostility. When you meet people who are extremely competitive, always in a hurry and especially irritable and aggressive, then you are interacting with people with the type A behaviour pattern (Glass 1977; Strube 1989). The type A behaviour pattern persons are the opposite of the type B behaviour pattern group. The type B group are usually not competitive, not always “fighting the clock”, and do not easily lose their tempers. The type A group are usually aggressive compared to the type B groups in most situations. (Baron, Russel& Arms, 1985; Carver & Glass 1978). The type A behaviour pattern individuals engage in hostile aggression with the intention of inflicting harm or injury on their victims, and are more likely to be engaged in child abuse, spouse abuse while the type B individuals are more likely to engage in instrumental aggression and their goal usually is not to cause harm but achieve other goals like praise or gain control. 2. Hostile Attributional Bias This is perceiving evil intend in others. Hostile attributional bias refers to the tendency to perceive hostile intentions or motives in the actions of others when these actions are ambiguous. How we evaluate and interpret the cause for other people’s behaviours determine our reaction. If their behaviours are perceived as hostile and intentional or provocative, then it is likely that these will result in Aggression. Actions are usually dependent on our attributions concerning the exhibited behaviour. People high in hostile attributional bias mostly do not give people the benefit of the doubt, and they are likely to assume that any provocative behaviour by others are intentional and react aggressively. 3. Narcissism and Aggression Narcissism refers to excessive self-love, which means holding an over inflated or exaggerated view of one’s own qualities or achievements. Persons high in Narcissism do react to slights of others or from feedbacks that attack their self-image or ego. The opinion the narcissist have for themselves are unrealistically high, and any attempt at building the self-esteem of young people to the point where they develop this unrealistic high opinion of themselves increase their potential for violence. PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 73 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION 4. Sensation seeking and Aggression Sensation seeking and impulsivity are likely to go together for one who likes taking risks and looking for excitement. Such people might be higher than others in exhibiting aggression for the following reasons. i. People high in sensation seeking and impulsiveness experience anger and hostile feelings more than others. May have low threshold for anger and their emotions are easily aroused. Might view Aggressive exchange with others as exciting and dangerous, and when bored, might seek new experiences that may lead them to entertain hostile thoughts. (Zuckerman 1994). ii. iii. In addition (2003) suggested sensation seeking. to the points above, Joireman, Anderson and Strathman the following tendencies related to aggression for people high in i. ii. iii. Attraction to Aggression – Eliciting situations. Are more likely to experience anger and hostility. Likely to focus on the immediate rather than the delayed consequences of their behaviour iv. Tend to show both physical and verbal aggression at a higher level compared to others. 5. Gender and Aggression Like all other issues, are there any gender differences in aggression? To some extent yes, there is research support that males are more aggressive than females, they do engage in higher incidence of many aggressive behaviours than females. (Harris 1994) Males are likely to perform aggressive actions and serve as target for such behaviour, which usually continues across life span, though it may vary in size and across situations as follows There is a gender difference in the absence of provocation than in its presence, with the males more likely to be aggressive against others even when not provoked in any way, but in the presence of provocation gender differences disappear. Once provoked, we assume that men and women respond in similar ways. Size and direction of gender differences vary with types of aggression. The males for instance, engage more in direct aggression, like physical assault, pushing, shoving, shouting, and insults. While females engage more in in-direct aggression where their actions are concealed from the victims and might come in form of gossiping, spreading rumours, telling others not to associate with intended victims and making up stories. Environmental/Situational Cause of Aggression. Factors relating to the environment or situations within certain contexts do result in aggression are as follow: 1. Climate and Aggression PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 74 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION The relationship between climate and aggression has been studied extensively. Findings are that heat increases aggression but beyond some level the reverse may be the case, with aggression decreasing as temperature rises. High temperature makes people very uncomfortable and tired, or fatigued and not likely to engage in aggressive behaviour for the following additional reasons. High temperature reduces aggression for both provoked and unprovoked persons, because for one who is hot, focus will be on reducing this discomfort rather engaging in fights with others. Hotter years were associated with higher rates of violent crimes. Heat has been linked to aggression in these ways: o People get hot and become irritable and may lash out at others o But exposure to high temperature for long makes people become uncomfortable and focus shifts on making self-comfortable. 2. Air Pollution and Aggression Chemical changes in the air are likely to result in aggression if inhaled in large quantities. Ethyl Mercaplan A mild unpleasant smelling pollutant common in the urban areas, has been associated with aggression, where people have been found to be more aggressive when breathing air that contains this chemical. (Rothan et al, 1979). Ozone and level in the air increases the frequency of aggressive family disturbances. i. Non smokers have been found to be more Aggressive breathing smoke – filled - air compare to clean air (Zillman, Baron &Tambori 1981). ii. Lead. Association between long term exposures to toxins like lead resulting in aggression has established (Needleman 1996) 3. Noise An unwanted and uncontrollable sound has been associated with the display of aggression especially when the noise is unpredictable and irregular (Bell et al, 2000; Grein&Mc, 1984). 4. Living Arrangements. Buildings with few tenants or residents are less likely to provoke aggressive behaviours from tenants compared to tenants of crowded apartment or buildings. This is because crowding tends to result in physiological arousal which might make people tense, uncomfortable and likely to report negative feelings. This tension or arousal can make people like each other less and become more aggressive. Behaviour problems among juvenile delinquents have been shown to have direct bearing on crowding for these young ones related to their living condition. (Ray et al 1982; Bell et al 2000) 5. Alcohol and Aggression. Alcohol consumption especially in large quantities was found to make users behave more aggressively and respond to provocations more strongly than non-users. PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 75 SCHOOL OF DISTANCE EDUCATION This effect of alcohol on users has been attributed to reduced cognitive functioning and social perception. Alcohol has been found to impair or distort higher order cognitive functions like the evaluation of stimuli and memory. Alcohol also has the effect of reducing the user’s ability to process positive information about someone he/she does not liked in the first instance or one that is viewed in negative terms. (Bartholow et al 2003). Alcohol also results in disinhibition by the user which allows one to take unreasonable risks, which might result in aggressive behaviour at the slightest provocation. The causes of aggression are as many as the different types of aggression that are even known. Understanding aggression and their causes should be the focus of society and those in the helm of affairs in any nation. While aggression might be positive and desirable in some instances, the negativeand undesirable aggression that seems to rear its head in most interactions should be tackled and society made to emphasize positive aggression. Reference 1. Baron, R, A, Byrne D., &Bronscombe N.R (2006). Social Psychology (11th Edition). New Jersey: Prentic – hall, Inc. 2. Baron, R.A., and Byrne, D. (2002). Social Psychology, 10th Ed. New Delhi: Pearson Education. 3. Baron, R.A., and Byrne, D. (1997). Social Psychology, 7th Ed. New Delhi: Pearson Education. 4. Myers, D.G. (1990). Social Psychology, 3rd Ed. New York: McGraw Hill Inc 5. Fernald, L.D., Fernald, P.S (2007). Introduction to social psychology (5th Edition). Delhi: A. T.B.S. Publishers and Distributors (Regd). 6. Kelley H.H (1973). The processes of causal attribution. American Psychologist, 28, PSYCHOLOGY OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR Page 76