Download Possible Links among Mirror Neurons and Genes

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Aging brain wikipedia , lookup

Neuroeconomics wikipedia , lookup

Self-awareness wikipedia , lookup

Nervous system network models wikipedia , lookup

Synaptic gating wikipedia , lookup

Metastability in the brain wikipedia , lookup

Optogenetics wikipedia , lookup

Environmental enrichment wikipedia , lookup

Neuroanatomy wikipedia , lookup

Biology and consumer behaviour wikipedia , lookup

Clinical neurochemistry wikipedia , lookup

Vilayanur S. Ramachandran wikipedia , lookup

Activity-dependent plasticity wikipedia , lookup

Mirror neuron wikipedia , lookup

Neuropsychopharmacology wikipedia , lookup

Neurogenomics wikipedia , lookup

Autism therapies wikipedia , lookup

Autism spectrum wikipedia , lookup

Heritability of autism wikipedia , lookup

Discrete trial training wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
都留文科大学大学院紀要 第20集(2016年 3 月)
THE TSURU UNIVERSITY GRADUATE SCHOOL REVIEW,
No.20(March, 2016)
Possible Links among Mirror Neurons and
Genes Related to Autism
Mai MOCHIZUKI
Abstract
Autism includes many neurodevelopmental disorders and deficits in communication. Although
researchers have considered various origins, the onset mechanism is still not clear. The aim of
this article is to provide some clues for interaction of autism with mirror neuronal and genetic
factors. First, the impact of neural brain cells considered to influence autism will be discussed
with reference to mirror neurons. Then, the discussion will move to genes related to autism.
Consequently, it is argued that a single factor cannot explain the mechanism of autism, yet the
relations among various factors are responsible for autism.
Keywords: Autism, Mirror Neurons, CAPS2, SHANK3, CTNND2, FOXP2
0. Introduction
Autism is well known for its relation to deficits in communication and impaired social
interaction. The mechanism of autism has been investigated. However, it is not fully clear. It
seems obvious that many factors are involved in autism; for example, mirror neurons or genes
and so on. This article shows the relation between autism and these factors. Section 1 revisits
autism, its definition, and characteristics. In section 2, we explore the relation between autism
and mirror neurons. Section 3 discusses the related genetic factors to autism. CAPS2, SHANK3,
and CTNND2 will mainly be introduced. Section 4 considers the implications of recent studies
and makes some proposals. Finally, in section 5, we summarize the ideas in this article and
suggest a future perspective.
1. Autism and its Symptoms
Autism is a well-recognized impairment for communication. However, the causes and
symptoms are not fully recognized. Autism is recently called, “Autistic Spectrum Disorder
(ASD).” It is a range of conditions classified as neurodevelopmental disorders in American
61
都留文科大学大学院紀要 第20集(2016年 3 月)
Psychiatric Association ed. (2000) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 5th
edition (DSM-5). The main symptoms are a delay in speech development, echolalia, difficulty
in imitating, non-verbal communication disorder, and difficulty in joint attention.
Autism has some characteristic symptoms. The characteristic behaviors of ASD may be
apparent in infancy (18 to 24 months), but they usually become clearer during early childhood
(24 months to 6 years) (Autism Society of America). The American Autism Association
provides main symptoms as follows:
1) Communication Impairments
・Delays in development of spoken language
・Idiosyncratic repetitive language
・Lack of pragmatic aspect of language
・Inability to initiate or maintain language
2) Social Interaction
・Lack of appropriateness in verbal and non behavior
・Lack of ability to develop peer relationships
・Lack of apparent social and emotion reciprocity
3) Behavior Patterns
・Restricted, repetitive and stereotyped patterns of behavior
・Difficulty in motor control
・Peculiar attachment to inanimate objects
・Distressed by a change in routine
(American Autism Association 2015)
Alternatively, autism has a lot of pathognomonic characteristics. Although the typical
characteristics of autism are mentioned above, the Autism Society of America shows more signs
to look for in children as follows:
・Lack of or delay in spoken language
・Repetitive use of language and/or motor mannerisms (e.g., hand-flapping, twirling objects)
・Little or no eye contact
・Lack of interest in peer relationships
・Lack of spontaneous or make-believe play
・Persistent fixation on parts of objects
(Autism Society of America 2015)
It follows that a child who shows these symptoms is suspected to have autism.
62
Possible Links among Mirror Neurons and Genes Related to Autism
2. Mirror Neurons
Mirror neurons consist of nerve cells producing an action potential in the brain such as primates.
They react like “mirrors,” and are therefore named “Mirror neurons.” This system is called, the
“Mirror Neuron System (MNS)” which is thought to control sympathy. The mirror neuron was
discovered by a group of Italian researchers led by Giacomo Rizzolatti in 1996. He recorded
neuronal activity related to specific movement when a macaque monkey was going to eat food.
He showed that the neuron of approximately 10% of the inferior frontal cortex and inferior
parietal cortex of the monkey showed a similar reaction to both the specific hand movement
and the overall movement. Conclusive evidence of the existence of the human mirror neuron
is not provided because the experiment (installing an electrode in the inferior frontal cortex of
the macaque monkey) is non-replicable in human subjects. However, it is clear that inferior
frontal gyrus and the superior parietal lobe of the Homo Sapiens showed activity in both when
they observed an action that a subject acted time and others by a brain imaging study by fMRI
(functional magnetic resonance imaging). Given the data of the newborn baby of the Homo
Sapiens using the eye-tracking device, the mirror neuron system develops by 12 months after
birth, and it assists a newborn baby in understanding the action of others (Terje et al. 2006).
Recently, it is pointed out that autism results from the mirror neuron defect. Although
scientists have conducted some research, this relation is not yet clear. We will consider other
related factors in the next section.
2.1 Empathy
One of the characteristics of autism is a lack of empathy and emotional engagement with others
(APA, 2000; C.L. Gillberg, 1992). Empathy is the ability to share someone else’s feelings.
Human beings usually possess sympathy instinctively. However, Preston and de Waal (2002)
define empathy as a phenomenon in which the perception of an object’s state activates the
subject’s corresponding representation, which in turn activates somatic and autonomic
responses. Lack of empathy is an early sign of autism (Charman et al. 1997, Sigman, Kasari,
Kwon, and Yirmiya 1992), and we have shown empathic behavior deficits as early as 20 months
of age in children with autism.
2.2 Imitation
Lack of ability to imitate is also a characteristic of autism. Imitation plays a central role in the
development of understanding other people, as both imitation and the attribution of mental states
involve translation from another person’s perspective into one’s own (Hadijikhani 2007). This
ability indicates the presence in the newborn of an active intermodal matching. Because they do
not see their face, only the way newborns can match expressions is thought proprioception (A.N.
Meltzoff and Moore, 1997). Until recently, it was thought that neonatal imitation was unique
to apes and humans, but recent data has shown the capacity to match facial and hand gesture is
also present in rhesus macaques (Ferrari et al. 2006).
63
都留文科大学大学院紀要 第20集(2016年 3 月)
2.3 Facial Expression
Facial expression is present in mammals mainly. The facial expression of emotion has a
biological basis (Darwin 1965). Poverty of facial expression is also one of the characteristics
of autism. Furthermore, most autistic children do not meet eyes with another person. This is
possibly connected to joint attention. Joint attention is the shared focus of two individuals on
an object. Joint attention is hard to develop in children with autism. It has understanding and
expression. First, a baby understands the thing that others see, and the stage appears that the
joint ownership of eyes is watching together. Next, they understand the thing that others prick
with a finger. Then, they ask for joint attention themselves; for example, a baby shows others an
object to attract their attention. This connects with the theory of mind that we argue in the next
section.
2.4 Theory of Mind
Theory of Mind (ToM) is the ability to attribute mental states including beliefs, intents,
desires, pretending, and knowledge. ToM refers to the notion that many autistic individuals do
not understand that other people have their own plans, thoughts, and points of view (Autism
Research Institute). Furthermore, it appears that they have difficulty understanding other
people’s beliefs, attitudes, and emotions. The origin of this is a philosophical debate. ToM was
originally based on Premack and Woodruff' (1978). Primates such as chimpanzees pay attention
to what other animals of similar species feel or think and act accordingly. They discovered this
function worked and called it “Theory of Mind.”
The ToM development begins early in life. At five months of age, typical children can
recognize different facial expressions, but understanding the meaning occurs a few months later
(Indiana Resource Center for Autism). Once young children can reliably interpret the facial
expressions of others, and begin to use this nonverbal information to guide their behavior. Most
investigation of ToM development has focused on 3 to 4-year-old children. It is evident that
there is a rapid development in this area between the ages of 3 to 4. Three-year-olds typically
fail to recognize their own and other’s false-beliefs (holding beliefs that conflict with reality).
Wimmer and Perner (1983) developed a means of measuring false-belief understanding, and
although many subsequent studies have modified the task, the classic false-belief task is as
follows:
A story character, Maxi puts chocolate into a cupboard x. In his absence his mother
displaces the chocolate from x into cupboard y. Subjects have to indicate the box
where Maxi will look for the chocolate when he returns. Only when they are able to
represent Maxi’s wrong belief (‘Chocolateis in x’) apart from what they themselves
know to be the case (‘Chocolate is in y’) will they be able to point correctly to box x.
(Wimmer and Perner: 1983: 106)
64
Possible Links among Mirror Neurons and Genes Related to Autism
Correct responses to false-belief tasks increase with age: children at 30 months answer falsebelief questions correctly only about 20% of the time. Children at 44 months are correct about
50% of the time, and at four years of age children perform better than chance, answering most
correctly (Wellman et al., 2001). Various studies have shown that the majority (around 80%) of
children with autism, even those with average intelligence, fail measures of false-belief (BaronCohen, 1993). In other words, children with autism across the age span will often answer that
Maxi will immediately search for the candy in the drawer. As typical children mature, they
increasingly would understand Maxi’s perspective and would suggest that Maxi would search
for candy in the cabinet.
All autistic children do not necessarily fail in the false-belief task. Therefore, the Theory
of Mind hypothesis is still disputed.
3. Autism and Other Factors
In the previous sections, I have argued for the relation between autism and mirror neuron. In
recent years, new possibilities have been pointed out. The recent autistic study develops into
genetics. The rate of autistic boys is double the rate of girls (Rutter et al. 1990). Moreover,
monozygotic twins have a higher ratio of autism than dizygotic twins (Folstein & Rutter 1997).
Some genes that may affect autism are as follows:
3.1 CAPS2
We expect that the study of CAPS2 will lead to an elucidation of the onset mechanism of
autism.
Sadakata et al. hypothesized that CAPS2 is a candidate autism susceptibility gene
(Sadakata et al. 2007 and Sadakata et al. 2009). Therefore, they conducted tests on mice
and obtained a relevant result (see Sadakata et al. 2013). They suggest that adequate levels
of CAPS2 protein should be critical for normal brain development and behavior and that
allelic changes due to copy number variation (CNV) may contribute to autistic symptoms in
combination with deficits in other autism-associated genes.
3.2 SHANK3
It is likely that the mutation of Shank3 is related to autism. Lana et al. conducted tests and
obtained a relevant result (Duffney et al. 2015). The Shank3 gene, located on chromosome
22q13.3 in humans, was first implicated in ASD from genetic analysis of the 22q13.3
microdeletion syndrome (Duffney et al. 2015). They discovered that Shank3-deficient
mice exhibit autism-like social deficits and repetitive behaviors, as well as the significantly
diminished NMDA receptor (NMDAR) synaptic function and synaptic distribution in the
prefrontal cortex. Interestingly, they found that juvenile male Shank3-deficient mice exhibited
ASD-like behavioral deficits, including social interaction deficiency and repetitive grooming
65
都留文科大学大学院紀要 第20集(2016年 3 月)
(Duffney et al. 2015). This study may lead to an explanation for why that there are more autistic
boys than girls.
There is another result of research on Shank3. Researchers at MIT and Duke succeeded
in recreating autism, in mice (MIT News 2011). The researchers focused on Shank3, which
mutated in mice used for this study. They show that mice with Shank3 deletions exhibit selfinjurious repetitive grooming and deficits in social interaction (Peca et al. 2011). Their findings
demonstrate a critical role for Shank3 in the normal development of neuronal connectivity and
establish causality between a disruption in the Shank3 gene and the genesis of autistic-like
behaviors in mice.
3.3 CTNND2
Recently, a new factor was discovered. As I have mentioned earlier, autism emerges in more
men than women. However, this gene was discovered in a female autistic. Turner et al. (2015)
investigated the genes of 13 women who suffered from severe autism. As a result of the
investigation, in the genes of 13 people, much mutation was observed in heredity sequence
named CTNND2 in comparison with persons who do not have autism. It is a major discovery to
find a gene peculiar to a women.
4. Discussion
A relation between autism and certain factors has been argued. Due to the development of the
brain imaging study, neuroscience has been paid much attention. Moreover, the recent autism
study is mainly based on genetics. The relations between autism and mirror neurons have been
pointed out, but recently the genetic factor is also investigated. Thus, it is clear that autism is
not attributed to only one factor. If there are such interactions, they may explain the impairment
severity in sex differences with regard to symptoms. The sex differences are relevant to Shank3
(Duffney et al. 2015). It was a result that there were many ratios of the autism of male mice.
Autism is four times more prevalent in male than female (Kogan 2007). On the other hand, a
gene peculiar to women is discovered (Turner 2015). We discovered many mutations in heredity
sequence called CTNND2. In the explanation on mice, the gene showed an influence on the
hippocampal neurons that were important to the long-term memory of the brain. This genetic
study needs further research. Note also that monozygotic twins have a higher ratio of autism
than dizygotic twins (Folstein & Rutter 1997). Thus, it is obvious that a hereditary factor is
related to autism.
Also, FOXP2 may well be related to autism. This gene was first found to be involved in
a speech and language disorder. The disorder was first discovered in a British family known
as KE family. Various disruptions in the FOXP2 gene have been reported, and the symptoms
caused by disruptions mentioned above tend to include a broad spectrum of deficits, including
speech and language problems, verbal dyspraxia, low-performance IQ, developmental delay,
66
Possible Links among Mirror Neurons and Genes Related to Autism
and brain abnormalities (Nudel et al. 2013). Moreover, as a thorough study on genes repressed
by FOXP2, CNTNAP2 has been implicated in disorders such as specific language impairment
and autism, among others, it shed some light on the mechanism through which FOXP2
may influence speech and language (Nudel et al. 2013). Practically, autistics have a speech
impediment. FOXP2 may control other genes, and mutations may happen in genes such as
CAPS2 by an accident of FOXP2. Because it is clear that FOXP2 is related to speech, further
study of FOXP2 and autistic genes is expected in future research.
Considering the circumstances mentioned above, it is clear that there is a relation between
autism and genetic factor is obvious. Autism has several symptoms (see section 1.1.), and there
is an individual difference. The optional factors such as genes or mirror neurons may cause
these factors. It is the mutation of FOXP2 that may trigger some mutations of several genes
causing autism. However, further research is necessary to conform this speculation.
On the other hand, there is quite a unique case --- Christopher. He is a language savant.
Savants are people who are mentally and often physically impaired but who have one dazzling
talent; mathematical, artistic or musical ability (Smith and Tsimpli 1995). Normally savants
have language impairment and low-performance IQ. However, Cristopher can read, write,
translate and communicate in fifteen or twenty different languages. In this case, there may be no
mutation of FOXP2. It may be the case that only mutation of other genes is involved. Further
study of this case is necessary for the future.
5. Conclusion
What we have so far observed is that the relations among various factors are responsible for
autism. The recent cutting-edge imaging research allows us to study autism in many aspects. As
it stands, the mechanism of autism is still mysterious and difficult to elucidate. However, new
factors such as CAPS2, SHANK3, and CTNND2 among other genes would lead to elucidation of
the mechanism of autism. There will be more discoveries about the mechanism of autism in the
future. Especially, an interdisciplinary study of autism is indispensable for the elucidation of the
mechanism. Consequently, we may also discover new genes that may be peculiar to language (the
faculty of language).
Acknowledgements
I am grateful to the anonymous reviewer and Takashi Imai for many constructive suggestions
and comment on earlier drafts of this article. I am also grateful to Mari Tanada and especially
Hywel Evans for useful suggestions for editing.
67
都留文科大学大学院紀要 第20集(2016年 3 月)
References
American Psychiatric Association (APA). (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders (5th edition). DSM-IV-TR. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1993). From attention–goal psychology to belief–desire psychology:the
development of a theory of mind and its dysfunction. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. TagerFlusberg, and D. Cohen. (Eds.), Understanding other minds: perspectives from autism.
(pp.59-82). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Berwin, B., Floor, E. and Martin, T.F. (1998). CAPS (mammalian UNC-31) protein localizes to
membranes involved in dense-core vesicle exocytosis. Neuron 21, 137–145.
Bibel, M. and Barde, Y.A. (2000). Neurotrophins: key regulators of cell fate and cell shape in
the vertebrate nervous system. Genes & Development 14, 2919-2937.
Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., and Drew, A. (1997). Infants
with autism: an investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention, and imitation.
Developmental Psychology 33(5), 781-789.
Cisternas, F.A., Vincent, J.B., Scherer, S.W. and Ray, P.N. (2003). Cloning and characterization
of human CADPS and CADPS2, new members of the Ca2+- dependent activator for
secretion protein family. Genomics 81, 279-291.
Darwin, C. (1965). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals (Vol. (original work
published in 1872)). Chicago, USA: University of Chicago.
Dean, C., Liu, H., Dunning, F.M., Chang, P.Y., Jackson, M.B. and Chapman, E.R. (2009).
Synaptotagmin-IV modulates synaptic function and long-term potentiation by regulating
BDNF release. Nature Neuroscience 12, 767-776.
Duffney, J. Lara., Zhong., Ping, Wei, Jing., Matas, Emmanuel., Qin, Luye., Ma, Kaijie., Dietz, M.
David., Kajiwara,Yuji., Buxbaum, D. Joseph. and Yan, Zhen. (2015). Autism-like Deficits
in Shank3-Deficient Mice Are Rescued by Targeting Actin Regulators. Cell Reports 11,
1400-1413.
Ferrari, P. F, Visalberghi, E., Paukner, A., Foggai, L., Ruggiero, A., and Suomi, S. J.(2006).
Neonatal Imitation in Rhesus Macaques. PLOS Biology 4 (9). doi: 10.1371/journal.
pbio.0040302
68
Possible Links among Mirror Neurons and Genes Related to Autism
Folstein, S. & M. Rutter. (1977). Infantile autism: a genetic study of 21 twin pairs. Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry 18, 297-321.
Giacomo Rizzolatti et al. (1996) Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions.
Cognitive Brain Research 3, 131-141.
Gillberg, C. (1992). The Emanuel Miller Memorial Lecture 1991. Atism and autistic-like
conditions: subclasses among disorders of empathy. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry 33(5), 813-842.
Hadijikhani, Nouchine. (2007). Mirror neuron system and autism. In Carlisle, C., Paul. (Ed.),
Progress in Autism Reserch (pp. 151-166). New York: Nova Science Publishers, Inc.
Hayashi, M.K., Tang, C., Verpelli, C., Narayanan, R., Stearns, M.H., Xu, R.M., Li, H., Sala,
C. and Hayashi, Y. (2009). The postsynaptic density proteins Homer and Shank form a
polymeric network structure. Cell 137, 159-171.
IMGSAC (2001) Further characterization of the autism susceptibility locus AUTS1 on
chromosome 7q. Hum. Mol. Genetics. 10, 973-982.
Jorde, L. B. et al. (1991). Complex segregation analysis of autism. Am. J. Hum. Genet 49, 932938.
Kogan, M. D. et al. (2009) Prevalence of parent-reprted diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder
among children in the US, 2007. Pediatrics 124, 1359-1403.
Lessmann, V., Gottmann, K. and Malcangio, M. (2003) Neurotrophin secretion: current facts
and future prospects. Progress in Neurobiology 69, 341-374.
Lu, B. (2003). BDNF and activity-dependent synaptic modulation. Learning and M e m o r y 1 0 ,
86-98.
Matsuda, N. et al. (2009) Differential activity-dependent secretion of brain derived neurotrophic
factor from axon and dendrite. Neuroscience 29, 14185-14198.
Meltzoff, A. N. and Moore, M. K. (1997). Explaining facial imitation: A theoretical model.
Early Development and Parenting 6, 179-192.
Mowla, S.J. et al. (1999) Differential sorting of nerve growth factor and brain derived
neurotrophic factor in hippocampal neurons. Neuroscience 19, 2069-2080.
69
都留文科大学大学院紀要 第20集(2016年 3 月)
Naisbitt, S., Kim, E. Tu, J. C., Xiao, B., Sala, C., Valtschanoff, J., Weinberg, R.J., Worley, P.F.
and Sheng, M. (1999). Shank, a novel family of postsynaptic density proteins that binds to
the NMDA receptor/PSD-95/GKAP complex and cortactin. Neuron 23, 569-582.
Nudel. Ron et al. (2013). FOXP2. Cognitive Science 5, 547-560.
Peçã, Joao et al. (2011). Shank3 mutant mice display autistic-like behaviors and striatal
dysfunction. Nature 472, 437-442.
Premack, D. and Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? The
Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1(4), 515–526. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00076512
Preston, S. D. and de Waal, F. B. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behavioral
and Brain Sciences 25(1), 109-122.
Rutter, M., H. Macodonald, A. LeCouteur, R. Harrington, P. Bolton, A. Bailey. (1990). Genetic
factors in child psychiatric disorders: II. Empirical findings. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry 31, 39-83.
Sadakata, T. et al. (2007). Autistic-like phenotypes in Cadps2-knockout mice and aberrant
CADPS2 splicing in autistic patients. Journal of Clinical Investigation 117, 931-943.
Sadakata, T. and Furuichi, T. (2009). Developmentally regulated Ca2+- dependent activator
protein for secretion 2 (CAPS2) is involved in BDNF secretion and is associated with
autism susceptibility. Cerebellum 8, 312-322.
Sadakata, T., Itakura, M., Kozaki, S., Sekine, Y., Takahashi, M. and Furuichi, T. (2006)
Differential distributions of the Ca2+-dependent activator protein for secretion family
proteins (CAPS2 and CAPS1) in the mouse brain. Journal of Comparative Neurology 495,
735-753.
Sadakata, T., Mizoguchi, A., Sato, Y., Katoh-Semba, R., Fukuda, M., Mikoshiba, K. and
Furuichi, T. (2004) The secretory granule-associated protein CAPS2 regulates neurotrophin
release and cell survival. Neuroscience 24, 43-52.
Sadakata, Tetsushi., Shinoda, Yo., Oka, Megumi., YSekine, Yukiko and Furuichi Teiichi. (2013).
Autistic-like behavioral phenotypes in a mouse model with copy number variation of the
CAPS2/CADPS2 gene. FEBS Letters 587, 54-59.
Sheng, M. and Kim, E. (2000). The Shank family of scaffold proteins. Journal of Cell Science
70
Possible Links among Mirror Neurons and Genes Related to Autism
113, 1851-1856.
Shinoda, Y. et al. (2010). Calcium-dependent activator protein for secretion 2 (CAPS2) promotes
BDNF secretion and is critical for the development of GABAergic interneuron network.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of United States of America 108, 373-8.
Sigman, M. D., Kasari, C., Kwon, J. H., and Yirmiya, N. (1992). Responses to the negative
emotions of others by autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Child Development
63(4), 796-807.
Smith, Neil, and Tsimpli, lanthi-Maria. (1995). The Mind of a Savant. Oxford and Cambridge:
Blackwell.
Speidel, D. et al. (2003) A family of Ca2+-dependent activator proteins for secretion:
comparative analysis of structure, expression, localization, and function. Journal of
Biological Chemistry 278, 52802–52809.
Terje Falck-Ytter1, Gustaf Gredebäck1 & Claes von Hofsten. (2006). Infants predict other
people’s action goals. Nature Neuroscience 9, 878 -879. doi: 10.1038/nn1729
Trafton, A. (2001). Re-creating autism, in mice, Mice with a particular gene mutation avoid
interacting with other mice and show compulsive, repetitive behavior. MIT News.
http://news.mit.edu/2011/autistic-mouse-0321.
Turner, N. Tychele et al. (2015). Loss of δ-catenin function in severe autism. Nature, 1-6.
doi:10.1038/ nature14186
Wellman, M. Henry, Cross David, and Watson, Julanne. (2001). Meta-Analysis of Theory-ofMind Development: The Truth about False Belief. Child Development 72, 655-684.
Wimmer, H. & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function
of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition 13, 103-128.
Internet Resources
Autism Society
http://www.autism-society.org/what-is/symptoms/
American Psychiatric Association
71
都留文科大学大学院紀要 第20集(2016年 3 月)
https://www.appi.org/Course/Book/Subscription/JournalSubscription/id-3457/DSM5®_Classification?utm_source=Internal-Link&utm_medium=Banner-Row&utm_
campaign=DSM-5-Classification
American Autism Association
https://www.myautism.org
Autism Research Institute
http://www.autism.com
Received:October 30, 2015
Revision received:November 18, 2015
Accepted:December 01, 2015
72