* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Movement-Related Neuronal Activity Selectively - Research
Cognitive neuroscience of music wikipedia , lookup
Metastability in the brain wikipedia , lookup
Haemodynamic response wikipedia , lookup
Neural oscillation wikipedia , lookup
Embodied language processing wikipedia , lookup
Eyeblink conditioning wikipedia , lookup
Subventricular zone wikipedia , lookup
Electrophysiology wikipedia , lookup
Development of the nervous system wikipedia , lookup
Feature detection (nervous system) wikipedia , lookup
Optogenetics wikipedia , lookup
Neuropsychopharmacology wikipedia , lookup
JOURNALOF NEUROPHYSIOLOGY Vol. 64, No. 1, July 1990. Printed in U.S.A. Movement-Related Neuronal Activity Selectively Coding Either Direction or Muscle Pattern in Three Motor Areas of the Monkey MICHAEL D. CRUTCHER Department of Neurology, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, AND GARRETT E. ALEXANDER SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS I. Movement-related neuronal activity in the supplementary motor area (SMA), primary motor cortex (MC), and putamen was studied in monkeys performing a visuomotor tracking task designed to determine 1) the extent to which neuronal activity in each of these areas represented the direction of visually guided arm movements versus the pattern of muscle activity required to achieve those movements and 2) the relative timing of different types of movement-related activity in these three motor areas. 2. A total of 455 movement-related neurons in the three motor areas were tested with a behavioral paradigm, which dissociated the direction of visually guided elbow movements from the accompanying pattern of muscular activity by the application of opposing and assisting torque loads. The movement-related activity described in this report was collected in the same animals performing the same behavioral paradigm used to study preparatory activity described in the preceding paper. Of the total sample, 87 neurons were located within the arm region of the SMA, 150 within the arm region of the MC, and 2 18 within the arm region of the putamen. 3. Movement-related cells were classified as “directional” if they showed an increase in discharge rate predominantly or exclusively during movements in one direction and did not have significant static or dynamic load effects. A cell was classified as “muscle-like” if its directional movement-related activity was associated with static and/or dynamic load effects whose pattern was similar to that of flexors or extensors of the forearm. Both directional and muscle-like cells were found in all three motor areas. The largest proportion of directional cells was located in the putamen (52%), with significantly smaller proportions in the SMA (38%) and MC (4 1%). Conversely, a smaller proportion of muscle-like cells was seen in the putamen (24%) than in the SMA (41%) or MC (36%). 4. The time of onset of movement-related discharge relative to the onset of movement (“lead time”) was computed for each cell. On average, SMA neurons discharged significantly earlier (SMA lead times 47 t 8 ms, mean t SE) than those in MC (23 k 6 ms), which in turn were earlier than those in putamen (-33 ? 6 ms). However, the degree of overlap of the distributions of lead times for the three areas was extensive. 5. The directional neurons appeared to code for movement direction per se, independent of the pattern of muscle activations required. Thus, in all three areas, there was evidence of neural processing related to “high-level” aspects of motor control that are logically antecedent to the final specification of muscle activations. The evidence that movement-related neurons in the SMA tend to discharge earlier than their counterparts in MC and these in turn earlier than those in putamen suggeststhat there is some degree of sequential processing from the SMA to the MC and thence to the putamen. On the other hand, the existence of both directional neurons and neurons with muscle-like activity patterns in each of these areas and the significant overlap in the timing of movement-related activity of these cells strongly suggest 0022-3077/90 $1 SO Copyright 0 Maryland 21205 that multiple levels of motor processing proceed in parallel within all three motor structures. INTRODUCTION Many investigations of single-cell activity in central motor structures have described movement-related discharge that was correlated with the direction of limb movement. In most of these studies, however, no attempt was made to dissociate the direction of limb movement from the accompanying pattern of muscle activity. Consequently, the pattern of muscle activity covaried with the direction of limb movement, because of the inherently directional nature of muscle activations. A few early studies did dissociate these variables, however, by using opposing and assisting loads (Conrad et al. 1977; Evarts 1967, 1968, 1969). Each of these studies stressed the relation of neuronal activity to force or muscle pattern. In fact, there have been many studies of central motor structures that, although not addressing the issue of direction versus muscle pattern directly, have described relations of single-cell activity to muscular force (Cheney and Fetz 1980; Evarts et al. 1983; Fromm 1983b; Kalaska and Hyde 1985; Liles 1985; Schmidt et al. 1975; Smith 1979; Smith et al. 1975). As a result, a widespread impression has emerged that certain motor structures, particularly the primary motor cortex, may be essentially concerned with controlling either force or the pattern of activity of different muscle groups. Recently, studies of single-cell activity in two components of the basal ganglia-thalamocortical “motor circuit,” the putamen (Crutcher and DeLong 1984a) and globus pallidus (Mitchell et al. 1987), of primates have been carried out with the use of motor tasks that dissociated the direction of limb movement from the pattern of muscle activity. In both areas the activity of substantial proportions of movement-related neurons was found to depend on the direction of limb movement independent of the associated pattern of muscle activity. In the present study monkeys were trained to perform similar tasks in which visually guided elbow movements were made with opposing and assisting loads that dissociated the direction of elbow movement from the pattern of muscular activity required to make the movement. Task-related neuronal activity was recorded from the supplementary motor area (SMA), primary motor cortex (MC), and putamen. As discussed in the preceding paper (Alexander and Crutcher 1990), all three areas are important components of the basal ganglia-thalamocortical motor circuit (Alexander et al. 1986). 1990 The American Physiological Society 151 M. D. CRUTCHER 152 AND G. E. ALEXANDER The present study was designed to determine whether representations of movement direction and/or muscle pattern were distributed evenly across these three motor areas or whether there was evidence for functional specialization within the different regions. Because this experiment involved recording in all three areas by the use of the same paradigm and animals, it permitted a more direct comparison of movement-related activity in SMA, MC, and putamen than could be accomplished by comparing data obtained in different laboratories with different experimental paradigms. This made it possible to address the additional issue of whether there were significant differences in the timing of movement-related activity among these three areas, as had been suggested by earlier comparisons of physiological data from different laboratories (Anderson et al. 1979; Crutcher and DeLong 1984a; Georgopoulos et al. 1982, 1989; Murphy et al. 1982; Tanji and Kurata 1982; Thach 1978). Some of these results have been presented in preliminary form (Crutcher and Alexander 1987, 1988). ASSISTED (FL) VELOCITY FLEXION NO LOAD OPPOSED FL) VELOCITY I METHODS The behavioral paradigms, recording techniques, and data collection procedures were described fully in the first paper of the series (Alexander and Crutcher 1990). Additional details regarding the methods of data analysis are described below. Analysis of variance The principal data analysis was done with the use of a 3-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with repeated measures (because of the repeated presentation of each trial type). The three factors were epoch within the trial, direction of movement, and load. (In some casesloads were not applied, in which casea 2-way ANOVA was used.) Four epochs within each trial were analyzed: the preinstruction hold period prior to the first lateral target presentation, the first movement period, the postinstruction hold period prior to the presentation of both side targets, and the second movement period. The movement periods were defined as the time from 100 ms prior to the onset of movement to the end of movement. However, if the change in activity of a movement-related cell began early in the reaction time and was relatively brief, the reaction time rather than the movement period was used as the epoch for measuring movement-related activity. The dependent variable was the average discharge rate during each of these epochs for each trial. Two directions of movement (extension and 1. Sensorimotorfields of cells with movement-reluted activity TABLE SMA’ Elbow Shoulder Distal Active arm Negative Total tested Not tested Grand total It I Il11 100 MS/DIV 25 (30) 17 (20) 6 (7) 28 (33) 8 (10) 84 (100) 67 151 MC Putamen 69 (5 1) 14 (10) 16 (12) 29 (22) 7 (5) 90 (57) 9 (6) 4 (3 38 (24) 16 (10) 135 (100) 45 157 (100) 83 180 240 Numbers in parentheses are percentages of cells tested by examination of the animal outside the task. SMA, supplementary motor area; MC, primary motor cortex. *Includes cells with combined preparatory and movement-related activity. I I I I I jj+ TARGET I I I I I I AL MOVEMENT Task-related EMG activity recorded from the biceps muscle. Average EMG activity is shown for 10 trials of each class, and single-trial velocity records are shown for 1 class. Trials are aligned on the onset of movement. The activity pattern shows the typical static load effect (increased tonic activity with a constant flexor load) during the hold period that preceded visually triggered elbow movements. There was also a dynamic load effect during the movement interval. Like other prime flexors (or extensors) of the elbow, this muscle showed increased activity with opposing loads and reduced activity with assisting loads. An upward deflection of the velocity trace represents extension. FL, flexor load; EL, extensor load. FIG. 1. flexion) and three levels of load (0.1 Nm opposing extension, 0.1 Nm opposing flexion, and no load) were used. Several other significance tests were carried out in conjunction with the main ANOVA. Three orthogonal comparisons between epoch means were performed to clarify the source of significant epoch effects (Keppel 1973). These included comparisons between the hold and the movement epochs, between the preinstruction hold and postinstruction hold periods, and between the first and second movement periods. In addition, the simple main effects for direction and load were calculated if the main effect (for direction or load) or the main effect X epoch interactions were significant (Keppel 1973). This analysis permitted us to identify the source of significant main effects and interactions. For example, if the main effect for direction was significant in the main 3-way ANOVA, it might be the result of significant relations to direction in one or both of the two movement periods, or the postinstruction period, or all three. By calculating the simple main effects for direction for each of the four epochs, we were able to determine which epochs of the task exhibited directional activity. We also calculated the simple main effects for load to determine whether there were significant static load effects in the preinstruction hold period, dynamic load effects during the flexion or extension movement periods, or load effects during the preparatory (postinstruction) period prior to extension or flexion movements. For all of the above tests, a significance level of P < 0.00 1 was used. This rather conservative significance level was chosen for the following reason. We carried out a seriesof preliminary analyses on data from 20 cells, using several different significance levels: 0.05, 0.01, and 0.00 1. A 3-way ANOVA on 60 trials with MOVEMENT-RELATED ACTIVITY four epochs per trial is extremely sensitive. We found that using 0.05 or 0.0 1 yielded “significant” results on responsesthat were so subtle that they were difficult (and, in some cases,impossible) to detect by eye. The significance level of 0.00 1 was therefore chosen so that only relatively clear responses would be found significant, and this level was then used for all cells. Analysis of cross cIassijications On the basisof the above analyses, each cell from each area was classifiedaccording to whether it showed movement-related activity, preparatory activity, or both and whether the movement-related activity was “directional” or “muscle-like.” Each of the resulting contingency tables of the frequencies of cells of different categories in the SMA, MC, and putamen was then analyzed by the use of three X* tests of homogeneity: one comparing each pair of structures. If any of the X* tests were significant, the contingency table was broken up into multiple 2 X 2 tables, and log odds ratios were calculated (Reynolds 1977). Latencies of task-related activity The latencies of task-related changes in neural activity were determined for each cell on a trial-by-trial basis, with the use of the following techniques. Two different algorithms were used to detect increases and decreases in cell activity. These same algorithms were also used to detect the onsets and offsets of preparatory activity reported in the preceding paper (Alexander and Crutcher 1990). IN THREE MOTOR AREAS 153 For excitations, each spike in the trial was assigned the value of one and then decayed exponentially with a time constant of 50 ms. All of the decaying exponentials were then summed to produce a continuous spike function for that trial where bursts in activity would be represented by a scalloped increase in the function. Next, the mean and the standard deviation of the value of the spike function at 1-ms intervals were calculated for 1 s prior to the target presentation. High (P < 0.00 1) and low (P < 0.1) thresholds for the spike function were calculated, and the period from 50 ms after the stimulus to the end of the movement was then scanned for a significant increase in activity. If the function stayed above the high threshold for at least 10 ms and above the low threshold for at least 75 ms, the onset of the response was taken as that point at which the function first exceeded the value of the low threshold. For inhibitions, the same basic procedure was used except that the spike train was converted into an interspike interval function, such that decreasesin cell activity were represented by an increase in the spike function, and the mean and standard deviation of the interspike intervals in the prestimulus period were used to determine the high and low thresholds. For cells with movement-related activity, the “lead time” was calculated on a trial-by-trial basis as the amount of time by which the onset of neural activity preceded the onset of limb movement. The median value for all trials in the preferred direction was used as the time of the onset of the response for each cell. The procedure for determining the time of the first change in electromyographic (EMG) activity for the 39 muscles studied was MUSCLE- LIKE CELL DIRECTIONAL CELL EXTENSION EXTENSION .I’ “I my,mya NO LOAD I 0’8 : , ‘ 0 888 I l Ia#m al '(1: ;I+:' at Ila~~I~ll I t I I ' "'0' ' " I :.a I at II a 0' " "I'("':' II I @ ‘ , Ybb , ID ',I ,l,n,,,; I 0, ‘#a '#,'",a# ;a#~‘b# I , , I’D 0 I I ,‘, I,, , : b’ : ‘I’ I I‘,, b,#: (8,; I Il@ ,b,&L# : "'Al ,, . J I I Y’.7” : ’ NO LOAD ‘ I.",. , . ,I. ‘I ,a&,; ) ‘. I a I , l t a,1 I a I ,, ‘$9 IRS : m, Y 0, 888 'I 88 rDw'ma I I @mm I I ,,,,‘, , , a“’ 0 . I et ma, ,I , . aloaw‘o# DYO ’ , I ,,, , * ,**, 0’ . ) I 1. 8 I 188011 NOLOAD aao , , ‘ ”,I ,‘ ‘i ‘ *.I,‘# ’ ,, ‘ , , I’ : , . m P :* m#9m T ’ ‘.I I,*,,,, ;:; .%?b:: I 0‘ Lbe ,: ‘0 “1 , ’ : ‘b *a ‘ , ;miy$-’ ,,,( 8~80 -1, II ‘? 0 OPPOSED I mm, YD,,, l 00 ,I, ,,‘.’ ‘ , lol, I 7 I”,‘ 1,‘ . I I,‘. , II a I 8'88 I I m,‘ , , :’ 00 I l ' I+# ID i ID@ II I @'.I I :I; I' ,':I, D ASSISTED I I a‘r s ., ‘1 I. , , I , ,a ‘I ,I I,,,.988,,a,an,I‘ m~mma~A, I ,* a, m 00 I.9 I n I ' lelomBl1* I lI a~~,~;u*#Dan 60 II , ;.I .‘ , I 0 l 0 aw’ ‘a ‘ , ‘D’DJ r’l,+“fl ,I alal, 'I' " : ,' I ‘: ’. I , I:,‘, 8 ‘I I ;‘a ; “b: , :, +,,” , .’ . I I I I .I':,, l "b' ,I’ :,, l’.‘,l:“:ll+:b“‘ 1 I ,, , , I’ :I+,', OPPOSED ImaD, 4 I,(I, ,bp”,ba ;‘, I,I,’I,*1+/’,$ffg+w+,,,’ , ‘ii ?‘&‘%‘#~# ‘ .rf: , , 0 I ‘I ,“#“,, IO, I ‘l0’8l’ ia“, , :7 ‘,@I ‘8 1, ,,,a; I 'a',' I18,1## 1.8, ‘ ,. 8 . I‘D ‘ ‘ :‘ I 8 , ‘ I ‘b‘ , ‘ ,#‘I“ I@ .‘, ‘ ’ : ‘ ':I*" , , I,‘ . :'I I', ‘ ,'I I " ,, I“" I', , 0 : ' 8 I' ‘ , ‘. lb a '0 ,' ,, a‘ a lb 8, ‘ ,.I‘ f ‘ FLEXION m 9: o,,,#‘, I, I I I‘"",: , ,: ,, ma I 9,” ,,*#I bab’b9bbab ‘ , : . D'DB I , , ba#lD , , ; Ip ,; I-; I , , , ' ,r! . f . I, 9; ; I ' I ' ,I” 1 I, ,l,:;@‘#’ ‘ I‘. ‘ lN a"., I a : ‘ ‘ H‘ ,I I NO LOAD I ,““‘## ,,‘ , I “I ’ ,, ’ ,‘I . I’ ’ . ;I , ,;,; ’ $,,&I;, 'I' ‘ , FLEXION ‘ 1: ‘“by@, ‘ ,I , I, , c Ibd‘a"b I 1 ‘,,;: I ‘I 1 y; I I1 I ! OPPOSED :‘b I ’ #b’b“ ,‘I I, , “@ (’ @bl#l ‘, ‘~‘“t 8D ma#.‘,#a, I’: OPPOSED ” “’ ’ ‘ ’” Ia I‘” 8 I@ I“, , ; ‘ ,;,a,:,(i“i,‘,, , I‘ @ l”l”“l”“L” 1ooMS/DIV TARGET MOVEMENT P A TARGET A MOVEMENT FIG. 2. Raster displays of 2 cells with movement-related activity recorded within the motor cortex. Each small tick indicates the occurrence of a single action potential, and each row represents the neuronal activity recorded during 1 trial. Large ticks indicate the times of occurrence of the target shifts that triggered the elbow movements. Trials are aligned on the onset of movement and sorted by reaction time. The rasters are sorted according to class, using the same conventions as in Fig. 1. The directional cell on the Ze@showed increased discharge in relation to extension movements that was independent of the loading conditions. The cell also showed a reciprocal reduction in activity during flexion movements. In contrast, the muscle-like cell whose activity is presented on the right showed increased discharge during extension movements that was characterized by a dynamic load effect (the movement-related activity was increased with loads that opposed extension and decreased with loads that assisted extension). The cell also showed a comparable static load effect during the hold period prior to the onset of movement. The overall pattern of activity in this cell was the mirror image of the activity of the biceps muscle shown in Fig. 1. 154 M. D. CRUTCHER AND G. E. ALEXANDER MUSCLE-LIKE CELL DIRECTIONAL CELL NO LOAD EXTENSION 0(III a f)‘;$,$‘:#@“ypr:w,‘,5 “+‘##‘+, ,E,’ I1 +;a+fa , : y$l,q III a’# :aeaa,’ t I 0,aa,,:, Iaa, aa,B nI I , I,y’bI ‘##/8’a(If’ ‘8‘I , ’ 1 I I ,$g’i’,i~~ ao* I’, a‘i&d, I “bpaa,‘~’,I I I b 8’80 8” ###, a,, . I lrn I I I I : ’ ‘,’ +y,‘,’ ’ I D &$ 1’ a , I’aI. a::. 1’1,I : I’, I I 8. .. ' 8' ' ,' '. 'I ' ' ,'I , a l~8 a#',' I 8, I a ma 0 9,' ,: b b'ala#### 8mDaa a* urn. man 'a," ? ASSISTED OPPOSED I I I I ,',,0',','; by,' ~a#',' I ,I. l"10 I, , ,'0',',(1,', *a 8 a I, a, ,I , "I , I'D a NO LOAD I’ a (' I,,,' 8 A' I, ,l (I @I: , a 0##~, ; 1; ;;;a# I@ a I 8' Ir a nbo 'J'a I,, : la Irn EXTENSION 888, ASSISTED . ,,’ .’ ‘I ’ ,‘I e ‘a I I I I , a I I ,, a;,”I I I I ’ I I 1 I , I”, ,’ 1 B , , 1 a’,’ {, B t ‘,.‘[ t I I OPPOSED I II * I I I I I FLEXION ‘an”‘I ’ : NO LOAD I , ##I apy'a,;; ,: I,, . I,";; . b', f 8'8'#, ,#,I ' I I I ",'A ,’ A a' #'GV I II a I, I I, “I’& a, ‘a 0 8oninmn a ,a , I, II I II ;#~,a#aaaDl I,, I 8 ,I,,, 8 :,+., I I , I ‘8 '8' ";'#, I 88’ l’,‘, 0 I 08 t a I I I : NO LOAD ' ', l FLEXION ,,,I #t,":~/##~,' 8,DII ASSISTED ,, I II 'I ,‘,‘a I I ,’ i’ II ' 0 a a a I ASSISTED , OPPOSED ,, ,‘, a ?I’ b ;I ‘1 I ('I:: 8, . II II ’al‘aaa’,,‘# ‘, ’ ’ ;~~a),‘8~a, , ~‘I,, I , a I , OPPOSED I8 :. a TARGET I 0 TARGET MOVEMENT MOVEMENT FIG. 3. Activity of 2 movement-related cells in the SMA. Left: activity of a directional cell that showed a selective increase in discharge in relation to flexion movements that was independent of the loading conditions. The cell also showed a reciprocal reduction in activity in relation to extension movements. The muscle-like cell on the right showed dynamic and static loading effects similar to those of the biceps muscle shown in Fig. 1. Conventions are the same as in Fig. 2. DIRECTIONAL CELL MUSCLE - LIKE CELL EXTENSION NOLOAD , ‘, :,, ) I I a ASSISTED , ,,I I I I 1 I I I I ‘a I I I I I I ,I” I , . , . I I ’ , an , 8: ASSISTED , : I., ” I I .., ’ , I I , 74 '1 I 8 NO LOAD , ' I , ' ASSISTED OPPOSED la 8 , , .’ ‘I a . . ’ II I TARGET ' ’ ,,f#('." ' EXTENSION ; 8 I I I I I a I I I I I II OPPOSED I"',, t ' , ’ . II FLEXION $#‘k#S~ I a?‘. . II I I 1 i I ’ 1 II I II , NO LOAD NO LOAD ’ , OPPOSED i , *I FLEXION 1 a’;&.’ ‘A., a;& 1 I aam’ ,;:##g I I B RI: #a ’* , 11,,## D I , 0 ASSISTED I ha:: I rp;,,’ ’ a. I ,‘,’ lb1‘:,‘!I,’ ’D MOVEMENT TARGET MOVEMENT FIG. 4. Activity of 2 movement-related putamen neurons. The directional cell whose activity is shown on the left discharged selectively in relation to flexion movements, irrespective of the loading conditions. The muscle-like cell on the right showed a pattern of dynamic and static loading effects that was similar to the EMG activity of the biceps muscle illustrated in Fig. 1. The static load effect for this cell is more subtle than that of the muscle-like cells shown in Figs. 2 and 3, but is still significant at the 0.00 1 level. Conventions are the same as in Fig. 2. MOVEMENT-RELATED ACTIVITY the same as for neuronal excitations except for three differences. First, the EMG activity was not smoothed with the exponential decay procedure. Second, because of this the onset found by the algorithm occasionally had to be manually corrected. Third, only unloaded trials were used. RESULTS Locations of recorded cells EMG activity One of the goals of this study was to determine whether there were cells in each of these motor structures whose activity was related to the direction of limb movement independent of the required muscle activations. To do this, we applied constant loads (0.1 Nm) to the monkey’s arm via the torqueable manipulandum (Alexander and Crutcher 1990). These loads either opposed or assisted the 2. Categories of movement-related activity SMA MC Putamen Directional Muscle-like Other 33 (38) 36 (41) 18 (21) 61 (41) 55 (36) 34 (23) 114 (52) 52 (24) 52 (24) Total 87 (100) 150 (100) 218 (100) 1 1 SMA vs. MC Directional Ratio Confidence interval P value TABLE 3. AREAS 155 Load effects in muscle-like ceI1.s SMA MC Putamen Dynamic and static Dynamic only Static only 23 (64) 4 (11) 9 (25) 24 (43) 13 (24) 18 (33) 23 (44) 21 (41) 8 (15) Total 36 (100) 55 (100) 52 (100) NS 1 Cells with movement-related activity were recorded throughout the respective “arm” areas of the SMA, MC, and putamen. The locations of all cells with movement-related activity for each of the three structures were shown in the first paper of this series (Alexander and Crutcher 1990). The location of each cell within an area of arm representation was confirmed by the somatotopic features of I) local neuronal responses to a sensorimotor examination and/or 2) the movements evoked by microstimulation (Alexander and Crutcher 1990). The sensorimotor fields of cells with movement-related activity are shown in Table 1. Cells were classified on the basis of their responses to a sensorimotor examination of the animal outside the task (see Alexander and Crutcher 1990). The majority of cells were related to elbow movements. Significantly fewer cells were related to movements of the distal arm or shoulder. Cells were classified as active arm only if their activity was related to active arm movements outside the task and that activity could not be attributed confidently to a specific joint. Cells were classified as negative if their activity was not modulated during the sensorimotor examination. TABLE IN THREE MOTOR 1 [ NS P < SMA vs. Putamen vs. muscle-like 1 1 P < 0.05 0.01 MC vs. Putamen odds ratios 0.8 1 2.42 0.5 1 1.48, 0.45 NS 4.19, 1.36 0.003 0.83, 0.31 0.007 Numbers in parentheses are percentages. NS, not significant. Other abbreviations, see Table 1. 1 1 Both vs. Dynamic SMA Ratio Confidence interval P value 1 NS 1 P < 0.05 Both vs. Static vs. putamen odds 1 Dynamic vs. Static ratios 5.25 0.89 0.17 17.70, 1.56 0.008 2.71, .29 NS 0.71, 0.04 0.015 Numbers in parentheses are percentages. Abbreviations, and 2. see Tables 1 movement. An example of EMG activity in the task for one of the prime movers is shown in Fig. 1. This biceps muscle was active during unloaded flexion movements and was more active when the static load opposed the flexion movement (flexor load) but was almost inactive during assisted flexion movements (extensor load). We refer to this modulation of movement-related activity with loads as a dynamic load efict. This muscle also had a pronounced static load e@ct; that is, it was tonically active when the constant load opposed movements in the preferred direction. The biceps was inactive during extension movements. This was the characteristic pattern of activity for most muscles that were active in the task. There was no basis for suspecting that any significant portion of the directional neuronal responses observed in this study (see below) could be accounted for by “muscleassociated” neurons that happened to code for activations of directional muscles. Of the 39 muscles examined in this study (Alexander and Crutcher 1990), only one, the cervical rhomboid, showed task-related activity that was directional in nature (that is, directional activity that was independent of loading conditions). Neuronal activity related to movement direction versus muscle pattern Cells with a pattern of activity like that of muscles were found in all three motor areas. An example of such a cell from the MC is shown on the right of Fig. 2. This cell’s activity was related to extension movements, and it showed both dynamic and static load effects. We categorized cells of this type as muscle-like cells. However, we also recorded cells whose movement-related activity was directional but which did not have either static or dynamic load effects. An example of such a cell from the MC is shown on the left of Fig. 2. This cell was equally related to extension movements whether the constant load assisted or opposed the movement. These cells were designated as directional cells. Examples of directional and muscle-like cells from the SMA and putamen are shown in Figs. 3 and 4, respectively. M. D. CRUTCHER 156 TORQUE RESPONSE AND G. E. ALEXANDER MOVEMENT RESPONSE PRE-INSTRUCTION INTERVAL EXTENSION FLEXOR LOAD ASSISTED EXTENSOR LOAD OPPOSED 0 0 0 FLEXION I “I I 0 0 , 0 , 0 0 0 0 I 0 0 EXTENSOR LOAD 0 I 0°1 ASSISTED O - FLEXOR LOAD WPOSED I-100 MS/ON / A / FP f&&j, TORQUE A TARGET MOVEMENT FIG. 5. Active directional activity in motor cortex. Right haZ$ activity of the cell during the active elbow movements to capture the side targets. This neuron discharged selectively in relation to extension movements irrespective of the loading conditions and was therefore classified as directional. Left haI& activity of the cell during the application of the torque load. The cell was judged to be of the active variety because there was no evidence of short-latency (~60 ms) proprioceptive input to this cell as the load was applied. Thus there was no increase in activity (short-latency or otherwise) when the elbow was passively extended during the application of the flexor load. (The weak response after the application of extensor loads was because of the active extension required for the animal to reposition the cursor when the forelimb was passively displaced in the direction of flexion.) An upward deflection of the velocity trace represents extension. TORQUE RESPONSE PRE-INSTRUCTION INTERVAL MOVEMENT RESPONSE EXTENSION I , I , I II,, ,, , I’ FLEXOR LOAD ,oo,:,, I l ,, , ,o 1,101 ’ , ‘Y I I ‘ I , , ,'I Ill,, I :o ,I,,,.d I 0 I ; ’ O ,‘:I 1 I o:, I@ 'I ' , , .oo.o,o 00 IO 00,. ,,a 0 I, 00 ,o,, ,I, mom l l OD , , , :I',, , 0 ,a 7 ;;f,oeo I ; I 00000 l m IO I 0’. l om I I , I l100l0, , 0 * IO 00’ IO I I ,...‘@’ I I , ,DlO,8,, II0 I DO 0 ,lOO,rn 8, ‘: O”’ 0 ’ 0,,,I0#~l~lI 001, I I , , I I ,I, *000,001 om aa 8 0 * ,I I 0 I, ‘0.0000 Ioo.#o,o : I ,‘I, ,‘I’, , ,o I, I I Il~.~. I ,o','e EXTENSOR LOAD eeoeo 0 0 *,0.:.*:, y;*;m , 88 0 0:’ 0 'I@ 0 , : , II ,I, 0 I ,I I. , :o 0 : '(' II , IO' ' "I ' '0 ' "I ,I I I I I IO, 0 I , ,: 0:’ I ,* IO, I 4 I' , "I I I : 0 ,, I I:.: 0, 1 ,,I I ,a0 , ,,I I) 0 0 ‘I ‘de : ‘,’ ’ , I, I I ‘pl, ;; I J I I ; ‘e@y I, I,, ,’ I I I I lb’ I ,’ I” t ,**mm I ‘11l1 0 I ,+ 0 ASSISTED ~,';,l)~,;#e",(;i;;yl I I IO ,‘I ***a ,I I ,I , , , , OPPOSED FLEXION EXTENSOR LOAD ASSISTED FLEXOR LOAD OPPOSED lOOMS/DlV / / A TOWE TARGET MOVEMENT FIG. 6. Passive directional activity in motor cortex. This cell showed a directionally selective increase in discharge with active flexion movements irrespective of the loading conditions, but also showed evidence of passive proprioceptive inputs of similar directionality. Thus part or all of the response apparently related to active movement could potentially be explained by movement-induced proprioceptive inputs. Conventions are the same as in Fig. 5. MOVEMENT-RELATED TABLE 4. ACTIVITY IN THREE MOTOR AREAS 157 Categories of directional cells posed flexion. Some cells showed the greatest (or the least) activity in the unloaded condition. A few cells were bidirecSMA MC Putamen tional; they were movement related but they were equally active for both directions of movement. (The total number Active 31 (94) 48 (79) 97 (85) of cells shown in Table 2 is less than the number of cells Passive 2 (6) 13 (21) 17 (15) with movement-related activity in Table 1 because a small Total 33 (100) 61 (100) 114 (100) number of cells were not tested with loads and, thus, could not be categorized as directional or muscle-like.) [ [ NS 1 NS 1 x2 analyses were performed to determine whether the NS 1 1 proportions of directional, muscle-like, and other cells Numbers in parentheses are percentages. Abbreviations, see Tables 1 were different in the three areas (Table 2). In this respect, and 2. the putamen was significantly different than both the MC (P < 0.05) and the SMA (P < O.Ol), but the two cortical The proportions of cells with different categories of moveareas did not differ significantly. The log odds ratio analysis ment-related activity are shown in Table 2. There were provided further information. For example, the “odds” of more than twice as many directional as muscle-like cells in a cell in the MC being directional rather than muscle-like the putamen, whereas for the two cortical areas these two were 6 1 to 55 or 1.11 to 1. For the putamen, the odds were types of cells occurred with about equal frequency. Move114 to 52 or 2.19 to 1. The ratio of these odds was 0.5 1. If ment-related activity was categorized as directional if there the relative frequency of directional and muscle-like cells was a significant (P < 0.001) direction effect for one or both was the same for these areas, then the ratio would be 1. of the movement epochs and there were no significant Because the ratio was significantly different than 1 (P < static or dynamic load effects (see METHODS). Muscle-like 0.007), we can conclude that there were relatively more cells had significant direction effects (because muscles are directional than muscle-like cells in the putamen cominherently directional), but they also had significant (P < pared to the MC. This was also true in comparing the 0.001) static load effects, dynamic load effects, or both, that putamen to the SMA (P < 0.003). The two cortical areas were “appropriate” for their preferred direction of movedid not differ significantly. Because there were almost ment. For example, the MC muscle-like cell shown in Fig. identical proportions in the “other” category for the three 2 was related to extension movements and had increased areas, the odds ratio analysis was not done for this category. movement-related activity when the load opposed extenCells were categorized as muscle-like if they had a signifision and decreased activity when the load assisted exten- cant dynamic load effect, static load effect, or both. The sion. Cells were classified as “other” if they did not fit frequencies of different types of muscle-like cells are shown either of the above patterns of activity. This could happen in Table 3. Overall, only one-half of the cells categorized as in one of several different ways. The most common type of muscle-like had both static and dynamic load effects, cell in this category had a static or dynamic load effect in whereas approximately one-fourth had dynamic load efthe “wrong” direction. For example, the cell was related to fects only, and one-fourth had static load effects only. The flexion movements but showed greater activity when the only significant x2 was that comparing the putamen and load assisted flexion and less activity when the load op- SMA. The log odds ratios indicate that this was because I) DlRECT/ONAL MOTOR CORTEX CELLS WITH OR MUSCLE - LIKE ACTIVITY / No. Cells: V-2) (34) L4 xi / Cl 1 mm A A Directlonrl A A MUSCk -Uke FIG. 7. Locations of MC cells with directional or muscle-like activity in 3 hemispheres. The solid triangles represent the locations of the active directional cells. The open triangles show the locations of muscle-like cells with dynamic load effects. The central sulcus is indicated, and rostral is to the left (regardless of whether data are from the right or left hemisphere). Locations of the recording areas relative to other surface landmarks are shown in Fig. 5 of the preceding paper (Alexander and Crutcher 1990). The fourth MC hemisphere is not shown in the present figure because most cells in that hemisphere were not tested with loads. Letters denote the movements evoked at threshold where microstimulation was effective: E, elbow; S, shoulder; W, wrist; F, finger. M. D. CRUTCHER 158 AND G. E. ALEXANDER the SMA had a preponderance of cells with both static and dynamic load effects and a relative paucity of cells with only dynamic load effects, and 2) the putamen had relatively more cells with only dynamic load effects and fewer with only static load effects. These differences were due primarily to the small proportion of cells with static load effects in the putamen compared to the SMA. 280 CR z c, 140 0 - i40 -iSO 251 “Active” versus “passive” directional activity An attempt was made to determine whether any of the directional movement-related activity could have been because of directional sensory feedback during the movement. If there was a short-latency response (~60 ms) to the load applications, which passively moved the arm in the preferred direction, the cell was considered to be passively driven and was therefore categorized as a “passive” directional cell. If not, the cell was labeled as an “active” directional cell. The torque application was considered to be an effective probe for proprioceptive input because in each monkey the resulting peak velocity induced by torque application was at least twice that of the active movement associated with side target capture (see Fig. 5). The activity patterns of active and passive directional TABLE 5. Muscle lead times Spinodeltoid Biceps Flexor carpi radialis Brachialis Acromiodeltoid Splenius capitus Supraspinatus Brachioradialis Triceps, lateral Cervical paraspinous Infraspinatus Pronator teres Palmaris longus Triceps, long Pectoralis major Flexor digitorum profundus Atlantoscapularis anterior Extensor carpi ulnaris Teres major Extensor carpi radialis Flexor digitorum superficialis Sternocleidomastoid Flexor carpi ulnaris Trapezius Thoracic paraspinous Lumbar paraspinous Extensor digitorum communis Cervical rhomboids Latissimus dorsi Semitendinosus Cleidooccipitalis Panniculus carnosus Serratus anterior Pectoralis minor Temporalis Quadriceps femoris Biceps femoris Gastrocnemius Tibialis anterior 82 80 79 77 73 71 65 62 56 56 48 48 48 47 41 37 35 33 27 17 6 4 2 2 -13 -16 -42 -47 -48 -50 -63 -65 -69 NR NR NR NR NR NR Values represent the median of single trial lead times. NR, no response. 35 30 25 20 15 10 5 0 -280 Lead time (ms) FIG. 8. Onset latency histograms for all cells with movement-related activity. The onsets of activity in SMA neurons were significantly earlier than those in MC, which were in turn significantly earlier than those in putamen. cells from the MC are shown in Figs. 5 and 6, respectively. For the active directional cell (Fig. 5) there was an increase in activity before and during the active extension movements to capture the side target (right half of the figure). There was also a weak increase in activity, which began - 150-200 ms after the application of the extensor load (a load opposing extension). This was the time of the active extension movement required to bring the arm back to the center after the passive flexion produced by the torque application. The passive directional cell shown in Fig. 6 had a directional increase in activity during active elbow flexion. Namely, it showed increased discharge in relation to the active flexion movements to capture the side target (right side of the figure) and to the active flexion to recover from the application of the flexor load (150-200 ms latency). However, this cell also had a brisk short-latency response to the application of the extensor load (which passively flexed the elbow). Therefore all or part of the directional activity seen during active flexion movements may have been because of directional proprioceptive inputs. The numbers and percentages of both types of directional cells are shown in Table 4. Most cells were of the active variety for all three motor areas. Differences between the SMA, MC, and putamen in terms of the relative proportions of the two types of directional cells were not significant. Locations of cells with directional The relative like patterns whether there these types of the SMA and or muscle-like activity locations of cells with directional or muscleof activity were examined to determine was a differential distribution of cells with activity in any of the three motor areas. For putamen, there was no significant difference MOVEMENT-RELATED TABLE 6. ACTIVITY IN THREE MOTOR AREAS 159 Lead times of all movement-related cells Median, ms Mean, ms n SMA MC Putamen 58 47 t, 8 114 31 23 k 6 156 -13 -33 k 6 229 [ P< 0.009 [ P<O.OOl ] P < Values are means t SE. Abbreviations, Passive Directional -iSO ] 0.001 see Table 1. -280 in the anteroposterior distributions of directional and muscle-like cells (t tests, P > 0.05). The locations of these two cell types within the MC are shown in Fig. 7. For the MC, the perpendicular distance from the central sulcus was measured. Although there was a high degree of overlap in the distributions of the two cell types, the active directional cells tended to be located more rostrally than the musclelike cells (t test, P < 0.01). Muscle-like 0 I I 140 Lead Responseonsetlatencies One of the principal goals of this study was to compare the timing of movement-related neuronal activity in these three motor areas. For reference, the times of activation (lead times) of task-related muscles are shown in Table 5. The earliest muscle lead time was 82 ms. Although the range of lead times was surprisingly large, most task-related muscles (73%) became active before the onset of movement, and the later muscles were generally trunk muscles with weak task-related activity. The response onset latency histograms for all cells with movement-related activity in these areas are shown in Fig. 8. Although there is a great deal of overlap in the distributions, it is clear that the SMA generally leads the MC, which in turn leads the putamen. T tests comparing the I b time I -ii80 (ms) FIG. 10. Onsets of movement-related activity in these different classes of directional and muscle-like cells were not significantly different in motor cortex. means for pairs of areas indicated that these differences were significant (Table 6). These latency differences between motor areas were also evident when the percentages of cells with onsets in activity before the earliest change in EMG activity were compared. These percentages were 34% for the SMA, 24% for MC, and 5% for the putamen. In addition to comparing the lead times between these areas for all cells with movement-related activity, we also compared the lead times between these areas for cells with different categories of movement-related activity. Response latency histograms of passive and active directional Passive Directional Cells 260 140 6 -ire -i80 v) z! 280 140 0 - 140 -280 6 Active Directional Active Directional Cells 11 -280 2 zho IlO b -iSO Muscle-like Muscle-like -1'40 Lead time (ms) FIG. 9. Onset latency histograms for passive and active directional cells and muscle-like cells in the SMA. The onsets of activity in active directional cells were significantly earlier than the onsets in muscle-like cells. Lead time (ms) FIG. 1 1. Onsets of movement-related activity in these different classes of directional and muscle-like cells were not significantly different in putamen. M. D. CRUTCHER 160 AND G. E. ALEXANDER cells and of muscle-like cells with dynamic load effects in the SMA, MC, and putamen are shown in Figs. 9-l 1, respectively. The statistics are shown in Table 7. Just as for all cells with movement-related activity, within categories of cells the SMA was significantly earlier than the MC, which was significantly earlier than the putamen (P values are shown in Table 7). The only exception was that the difference in lead times between the dynamic muscle-like cells in the SMA and MC was not significant. In addition to comparing the relative times of activation between different structures, we also compared the lead times of different categories of cells within structures. For example, if the directional movement-related activity of some cells was because of sensory feedback during the movement, then one might expect these cells to have later lead times than directional cells without sensory driving. The response latency histograms for passive and active directional cells in SMA, MC, and putamen are shown in Figs. 9- 11, and the means and medians are shown in Table 7. The responses of most passive directional neurons occurred after the onset of movement. The earliest lead time for these cells was 63 ms. However, the latency difference between the active and passive directional cells was not statistically significant for either the MC or putamen (P values not shown). The t test for the SMA was not done because of the paucity of passive directional cells in our sample. There is a related question concerning the relative timing of response onset latencies for different categories of neurons. If active directional activity represents a command signal for the direction of movement, which is logically antecedent to a command related to muscle activation patterns (see Fig. 1 of Alexander and Crutcher 1990), then one might expect active directional cells to have earTABLE SMA MC Putamen 53 53 ms 1 -16 ms -14-+ 16ms 13 -53 ms -69& 18ms 17 [ P < 0.05 ] Active directional cells Median, ms Mean, ms n 83 76 +_ 8 ms 23 [ P<O.OOl 25 15 _+ 15 ms 21 1 C ] -13 ms -40 t 10 ms 93 [ P<O.OOl P < 0.001 [ Dynamic muscle-like cells Median, ms Mean, ms n 23 ms 17+ 11 ms 42 NS 50 ms 35 + 10 ms 34 1 P < 0.05 ] 1 -21+ DISCUSSION Neuronal activity related to the direction of limb movement (directional cells), independent of the associated pattern of muscle activity, was found in all three motor areas examined in this study. In addition, however, neuronal activity that appeared to be related to the pattern of muscle activity (muscle-like cells) was also found in all three structures. The fact that large proportions of both types of cells were found in all three motor areas suggests that neural representations of both of these levels of motor processing may be distributed throughout the basal ganglia-thalamocortical motor circuit. This finding does not imply that the SMA, MC, and putamen are functionally equivalent. Clear differences between these motor areas have been described (Kurata and Tanji 1985; Okano and Tanji 1987; Tanji and Kurata 1982; Tanji et al. 1980). However, the fact that processes as fundamental as the specification of movement direction and muscle activation patterns appear to be so widely distributed argues against a strictly hierarchical organization of the motor system and suggests that the degree of functional localization within the motor system (i.e., the control of a given motor function by a single motor area) may be less than is commonly assumed. Activity coding movement direction 7. Lead times of directional and muscle-like cells Passive direct ional cells Median, ms Mean, ms n lier lead times than muscle-like cells. This expectation was only partially borne out by our data. The response latency histograms for the muscle-like cells in SMA, MC, and putamen are also shown in Figs. 9- 11, respectively, and the means and medians are in Table 7. For the SMA, the lead times of active directional cells were significantly earlier than those for the muscle-like cells (t test, P < 0.001). In contrast, for the MC and putamen the difference was not statistically significant for either area (P values not shown). -5 ms 9ms 43 [ P < 0.001 ] I Values are means + SE. Abbreviations, see Tables 1 and 2. *Includes muscle-like cells with dynamic load effects. Many previous studies have described neuronal activity that was related to the direction of limb movement in the SMA (Tanji and Kurata 1982), MC (Evarts and Fromm 1978; Evarts and Tanji 1976; Fetz et al. 1980; Georgopoulos et al. 1982, 1985, 1989; Kalaska et al. 1983; Kubota and Funahashi 1982; Lamarre et al. 1983; Murphy et al. 1982; Schmidt et al. 1975; Schwartz et al. 1988), and putamen (DeLong 1973; Kimura 1986; Liles 1979, 1985; Liles and Updyke 1985). However, because muscles are inherently directional, in all of these studies the pattern of muscular activity covaried with the direction of limb movement, making it impossible to determine whether directionally selective neuronal responses were related to movement direction per se or to force or muscle pattern. However, a few studies have dissociated these variables by the use of opposing and assisting loads. The earliest studies of this type were carried out in the MC and strongly emphasized the relation of neuronal activity to muscular force (Conrad et al. 1977; Evarts 1967, 1968, 1969). Nevertheless, each of these early reports mentioned that neurons had been found whose activity was directional and unaffected by the loading conditions. The activity of a small proportion of pyramidal tract neurons and most corticorubra1 neurons in the MC has been reported to be unrelated MOVEMENT-RELATED ACTIVITY to different static loads (Fromm 1983a). More recently, a preponderance of directional over muscle-like cells with movement-related activity has been described in the putamen (Crutcher and DeLong 1984a) and in the globus pallidus (Mitchell et al. 1987). A recent study using a two-dimensional tracking task in the MC has also described cells with directional activity with little or no dependence on the inertial load (Kalaska et al. 1989). The results of these recent studies and the present study suggest that directional movement-related activity, independent of muscle activity patterns, is much more abundant and widespread than earlier studies had suggested. In the present study we found approximately equal proportions of directional and muscle-like cells in both SMA and MC, whereas the proportion of directional cells in the putamen was more than twice that of muscle-like cells. These results could be an indication that the putamen preferentially receives inputs from directional neurons in the cortex. However, the fact that 52% of cells in the putamen were classified as directional compared with 38-4 1% for the cortex does not necessarily mean that there is a fundamental difference between the putamen and cortical motor areas. It is possible that some cells in the putamen had load effects that were too weak to be detected with the relatively conservative probability level that was used in the present study, because movement-related activity in the putamen was generally less robust than that in either the SMA or MC. A recent study of single-unit activity in the putamen (Liles 1985) found that for 59% of cells the movement-related activity increased with increasing magnitudes of opposing loads, and that 4 1% of cells were unaffected by the loads (up to 0.2 Nm). These proportions are comparable to those for the SMA and MC in the present study. This suggests that, although it is possible that the basal ganglia are less involved in the specification of the dynamics of movement than the SMA and MC, this conclusion must remain tentative pending further study. There are several possible roles that directional cells might play in the control of movement. The activity of these cells could represent an intermediate step in the transformation that the motor system performs to translate the spatial location of the target or goal of the movement into the muscle commands necessary to accurately reach that goal. The directional cells might therefore be thought of as representing either the trajectory or the kinematics of the movement. Some of the directional movement-related activity may represent the neural correlate of a command signal for the direction of limb movement and might be involved in movement initiation. This possibility would be most plausible for cells whose activity begins well before the onset of movement. Directional cells with relatively late onsets of discharge could be receiving a corollary discharge of such a command signal. This could be used either for on-line error correction during the movement or for comparison of the intended movement with the final outcome. The activity of directional cells that began to discharge after the onset of muscle activity might also have been driven by proprioceptive feedback associated with the active movement. To determine whether the directional movement-related responses that were load independent could be accounted IN THREE MOTOR AREAS 161 for by proprioceptive inputs activated during movement execution, we analyzed the responses of these cells to the application of torque loads. If there was a short-latency response (~60 ms) to the load applications, the cell was judged to be passively driven and was therefore categorized as a passive directional cell. Otherwise, the cell was labeled as an active directional cell. The overwhelming majority of directional cells in all three motor areas (79-94%) were judged to be active directional cells. It is possible that we underestimated the number of directional cells with passive driving because the load application may not have been effective in activating all receptors that were driven during the active limb movements. However, we believe that the torque application was a reasonably effective probe for proprioceptive input, because the resulting peak velocity induced by torque application was approximately twice that of the active movement associated with side target capture. Thus, if a cell had been receiving directional somatosensory input during the active movement, it should also have been activated by the torque application, especially given the evidence that somatosensory information is generally suppressed during active movements (Coquery 1978; Coulter 1974; Starr and Cohen 1985). The lack of evidence of proprioceptive inputs to the active directional cells should not, however, be construed as evidence that these motor areas do not receive proprioceptive input. A significant percentage of neurons in the MC (Fetz et al. 1980; Lemon and Porter 1976; Rosen and Asanuma 1972; Wong et al. 1978) and putamen (Alexander and DeLong 1985; Crutcher and DeLong 1984b; Liles 1985) and a modest number in the SMA (Brinkman and Porter 1979; Hummelsheim et al. 1988; Wise and Tanji 198 1) respond to somatosensory stimulation. In the present study less than one-half of our total sample of movement-related cells (SMA, 36%; MC, 32%; putamen, 44%) were judged to be active directional cells without apparent proprioceptive driving. Moreover, we selected cells for study only if they were related to active movement. This may well have excluded some cells with somatosensory driving that were not active during voluntary movements. Muscle-like activity Neurons with patterns of activity similar to those of muscles were found in each of the three motor areas examined in this study. This is consistent with the large number of studies that have described neural relations to muscular force (Cheney and Fetz 1980; Conrad et al. 1977; Crutcher and DeLong 1984a; Evarts 1967, 1968, 1969; Evarts et al. 1983; Fromm 1983b; Fromm and Evarts 198 1; Hepp-Reymond et al. 1978; Kalaska et al. 1989; Kalaska and Hyde 1985; Liles 1985; Mitchell et al. 1987; Schmidt et al. 1975; Smith 1979; Smith et al. 1975; Thach 1978). Such musclelike activity may represent a neural correlate either ofjoint torque or of the specification of muscle activation patterns. No attempt was made to dissociate these two variables in this experiment. However, like the directional cells, some of the muscle-like cells became active quite late in the reaction time or even after the onset of movement. Their activity was therefore unlikely to represent a command signal for the generation of a calculated joint torque or the acti- 162 M. D. CRUTCHER AND vation of a specific muscle. These cells might have been receiving a corollary discharge of a command signal to muscles. Alternatively, muscle-like cells with late onsets might be receiving muscle-like feedback from the periphery. Relative timing ofmovement-related activity This is the first direct comparison of single-cell activity in SMA, MC, and putamen. By examining all three structures in the same study, we hoped to be able to make more valid comparisons among them. We found that the average lead time in the SMA was before that in the MC, which was before that in the putamen. These timing differences were consistent with previous work. In studies that directly compared the SMA and MC, it was reported that the movement-related activity in the SMA began earlier than that in MC (Okano and Tanji 1987; Tanji and Kurata 1982). Also, previous studies that examined only one motor area have strongly suggested that movement-related activity in the striatum (Crutcher and DeLong 1984a; Liles 1985) and globus pallidus (Anderson and Horak 1985) began later than that in the MC (Georgopoulos et al. 1982, 1989; Murphy et al. 1982; Thach 1978). The onsets of movement-related activity observed in the present study were generally later than those reported in the previous studies of these motor areas. For example, in the present study the median lead time for the MC was 3 1 ms, whereas two previous studies of the MC found median lead times of 54 ms (Thach 1978) and 110 ms (Georgopoulos et al. 1982). Similarly, in the present study the median lead time for the putamen was - 13 ms, whereas a previous study of the putamen found a median lead time of 15 ms (Crutcher and DeLong 1984a). These latency differences may have been because of differences in the techniques used to determine the response onset latency for each cell. We determined the onset latency for each trial in the cell’s preferred direction and used the median of single-trial lead times as the latency for that cell. This approach will obtain a later onset latency than any technique based on determining the time of onset of the earliest trials, such as the commonly used method of examining the histogram of all trials. Which of the two techniques is preferable is unclear. The fact that neuronal activity was studied in three interconnected motor structures provided a unique opportunity to address the issue of serial versus parallel processing within the basal ganglia-thalamocortical motor circuit. Superficially, the significant differences in average lead time among the three structures suggest that there may be some degree of sequential processing from the SMA to MC and thence to the putamen. This is consistent with the commonly held view that the SMA is at or near the “highest” level of the motor system (Eccles 1982; Kurata and Tanji 1985; Mann et al. 1988; Roland et al. 1980; Tanji and Kurata 1985). However, the timing data obtained in this study are difficult to reconcile with the concept of strictly serial processing within the motor system. Although the mean lead times for the SMA, MC, and putamen were significantly different, the degree of overlap of the distributions was striking. This, together with the fact that the durations of the movement-related activity were often G. E. ALEXANDER quite long, indicate that many directional and muscle-like cells in all three areas were active simultaneously. Overall, then, the results presented here strongly suggest that multiple levels of motor processing proceed in paralle/ within each of the three motor areas examined in this study. Recently, Hyland et al. (1990) reached similar conclusions after comparing movement-related neuronal activity in SMA and MC. The software employed in this study was developed by F. H. Baker and R. G. Cutler. M. R. DeLong provided helpful comments on the manuscript. Technical assistance was provided by L. H. Rowland. B. A. Zuckerman assisted in the preparation of the manuscript. This work was supported by National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Grant NS-17678. Address for reprint requests: G. E. Alexander, Dept. of Neurology, Meyer 5- 185, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, 600 N. Wolfe St., Baltimore, MD 2 1205. Received 24 April 1989; accepted in final form 15 February 1990. REFERENCES ALEXANDER, G. E. AND CRUTCHER, M. D. Preparation for movement: neural representations of intended direction in three motor areas of the monkey. J. Nezuwphysid. 64, 133- 150, 1990. ALEXANDER, G. E. AND DELONG, M. R. Microstimulation of the primate neostriatum. II. Somatotopic organization of striatal microexcitable zones and their relation to neuronal response properties. J. Neurophysid. 53: 1417-1430, 1985. ALEXANDER,G. E., DELONG, M.R., ANDSTRICK, P.L.Parallelorganization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annu. Rev. Newmci. 9: 357-38 1, 1986. ANDERSON, M. E. AND HORAK, F. B. Influence of the globus pallidus on arm movements in monkeys. III. Timing of movement-related information. J. Newoph~~siol. 54: 433-448, 1985. ANDERSON, R. J., ALDRIDGE, J. W., AND MURPHY, J. T. Function of caudate neurons during limb movements in awake primates. Bruin Res. 173: 489-501, 1979. BRINKMAN, C. AND PORTER, R. Supplementary motor area in the rnonkey: activity of neurons during performance of a learned motor task. J. New~physid 42: 68 l-709, 1979. CHENEY, P. D. AND FETZ, E. E. Functional classes of primate corticomotoneuronal cells and their relation to active force. J. Nezrr~~physinl. 44: 773-791, 1980. CONRAD, B., WIESENDANGER, M., MATSUNAMI,K.,ANDBROOKS, V.B. Precentral unit activity related to control of arm movements. Ex~. Brain Res. 29: 85-95, 1977. COQUERY, J. M. Role of active movement in control of afferent input from skin in cat and man. In: Active Touch, edited by G. Gordon. New York: Pergamon, 1978, p. 16 1-169. COULTER, J. D. Sensory transmission through lemniscal pathway during voluntary movement in the cat. J. Nezuwphysiol. 37: 83 l-845, 1974. CRUTCHER, M. D. AND ALEXANDER, G. E. Comparison of movementrelated neuronal activity in primate motor cortex and putamen. Socl. Newosci. Abstr. 13: 244, 1987. CRUTCHER, M. D. AND ALEXANDER, G. E. Supplementary motor area (SMA): coding of both preparatory and movement-related neuraI activity in spatial rather than joint coordinates. Sm. Newosci. Abstr. 14: 342, 1988. CRUTCHER, M. D. AND DELONG, M. R. Single cell studies of the primate putamen. II. Relations to direction of movement and pattern of muscular activity. Exp. Brain Res. 53: 244-258, 1984a. CRUTCHER, M. D. AND DELONG, M. R. Single cell studies of the primate putamen. I. Functional organization. Ex~. Brain Res. 53: 233-243, 1984b. DELONG, M. R. Putamen: activity of single units during slow and rapid arm movements. Science Wash. DC 179: 1240- 1242, 1973. ECCLES,J. C. The initiation of voluntary movements by the supplementary motor area. Arch. Ysychiatr. Nervenkr. 23 1: 423-44 1, 1982. EVARTS, E. V. Representation of movements and muscles by pyramidal tract neurons of the precentral motor cortex. In: Nez1r~)p}zC?sio/oSic~a/ MOVEMENT-RELATED ACTIVITY Basis of Normal and Abnormal Motor Activities, edited by M. D. Yahr and D. P. Purpura. New York: Raven, 1967, p. 2 15-253. EVARTS, E. V. Relation of pyramidal tract activity to force exerted during voluntary movement. J. Neurophysiol. 3 1: 14-27, 1968. EVARTS, E. V. Activity of pyramidal tract neurons during postural fixation. J. Neurophysiol. 32: 375-385, 1969. EVARTS, E. V. AND FROMM, C. The pyramidal tract neuron as summing point in a closed-loop control system in the monkey. In: Cerebral Motor Control in Man: Long Loop Mechanisms, edited by J. E. Desmedt. Basel: Karger, 1978, p. 56-69. EVARTS, E. V., FROMM, C., KROLLER, J., AND JENNINGS, V. A. Motor cortex control of finely graded forces. J. Neurophysiol. 49: 1199- 12 15, 1983. EVARTS, E. V. AND TANJI, J. Reflex and intended responses in motor cortex pyramidal tract in monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 39: 1069- 1080, 1976. FETZ, E. E., FINOCCHIO, D. V., BAKER, M. A., AND Soso, M. J. Sensory and motor responses of precentral cortex cells during comparable passive and active joint movements. J. Neurophysiol. 43: 1070- 1089, 1980. FROMM, C. Contrasting properties of pyramidal tract neurons located in the precentral or postcentral areas and of corticorubral neurons in the behaving monkey. In: Motor Control Mechanisms in Health and Disease, edited by J. E. Desmedt. New York: Raven, 1983a, p. 329-345. FROMM, C. Changes of steady state activity in motor cortex consistent with the length-tension relation of muscle. Pfluegers Arch. 398: 318-323, 1983b. FROMM, C. AND EVARTS, E. V. Relation of size and activity of motor cortex pyramidal tract neurons during skilled movements in the monkey. J. Neurosci. 1: 453-460, 198 1. GEORGOPOULOS, A. P., CRUTCHER, M. D., AND SCHWARTZ, A. B. Cognitive spatial motor processes. III. Motor cortical prediction of movement direction during an instructed delay period. Exp. Brain Res. 75: 183-194,1989. GEORGOPOULOS, A. P., KALASKA, J. F., AND CAMINITI, R. Relations between two-dimensional arm movements and single-cell discharge in motor cortex and area 5: movement direction versus movement end point. Exp. Brain Res. Suppl. 10: 175-l 83, 1985. GEORGOPOULOS, A. P., KALASKA, J. F., CAMINITI, R., AND MASSEY, J. T. On the relations between the direction of two-dimensional arm movements and cell discharge in primate motor cortex. J. Neurosci. 2: 1527-1537, 1982. HEPP-REYMOND, M. C., WYSS, U. R., AND ANNER, R. Neuronal coding of static force in the primate motor cortex. J. Physiol. Paris 74: 287-291, 1978. HUMMELSHEIM, H., BIANCHETTI, M., WIESENDANGER, M., AND WIESENDANGER, R. Sensory inputs to the agranular motor fields: a comparison between precentral, supplementary-motor and premotor areas in the monkey. Exp. Brain Res. 69: 289-298, 1988. HYLAND, B., CHEN, D.-F., MAIER, V., PALMERI, A., AND WIESENDANGER, M. What is the role of the supplementary motor area in movement initiation? Prog. Brain Res. In press. KALASKA, J. F., CAMINITI, R., AND GEORGOPOULOS, A. P. Cortical mechanisms related to the direction of two-dimensional arm movements: relations in parietal area 5 and comparison with motor cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 5 1: 247-260, 1983. KALASKA, J. F., COHEN, D. A. D., HYDE, M. L., AND PRUD’HOMME, M. A comparison of movement direction-related versus load direction-related activity in primate motor cortex, using a two-dimensional reaching task. J. Neurosci. 9: 2080-2 102, 1989. KALASKA, J. F. AND HYDE, M. L. Area 4 and 5: differences between the load direction-dependent variability of cells during active postural fixation. Ex~. Brain Res. 59: 197-202, 1985. KEPPEL, G. Design and Ana/ysis: A Researcher’s Handbook. Engelwood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1973. KIMURA, M. The role of primate putamen neurons in the association of sensory stimuli with movement. Neurosci. Res. 3: 436-443, 1986. KUBOTA, K. AND FUNAHASHI, S. Direction-specific activities of dorsolatera1 prefrontal and motor cortex pyramidal tract neurons during visual tracking. J. Neurophysiol. 47: 362-376, 1982. KURATA, K. AND TANJI, J. Contrasting neuronal activity in supplementary and precentral motor cortex of monkeys. II. Responses to movement triggering vs. nontriggering sensory signals. J. Neurophysiol. 53: 142-152, 1985. IN THREE MOTOR AREAS 163 LAMARRE, Y., BUSBY, L., AND SPIDALIERI, G. Fast ballistic arm movements triggered by visual, auditory and somesthetic stimuli in the monkey. I. Activity of precentral cortical neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 50: 1343-1358, 1983. LEMON, R. N. AND PORTER, R. Afferent input to movement-related precentral neurons in conscious monkeys. Proc. R. Sot. Land. B Biol. Sci. 194: 313-339, 1976. LILES, S. L. Topographic organization of neurons related to arm movement in the putamen. In: Advances in Neurology, edited by T. N. Chase, N. S. Wexler, and A. Barbeau. New York: Raven, 1979, vol. 23, p. 155-162. LILES, S. L. Activity of neurons in putamen during active and passive movements of wrist. J. Neurophysiol. 53: 2 17-236, 1985. LILES, S. L. AND UPDYKE, B. Projection of the digit and wrist area of precentral gyrus to the putamen: relation between topography and physiological properties of neurons in the putamen. Brain Res. 339: 245-255,1985. MANN, S. E., THAU, R., AND SCHILLER, P. H. Conditional task-related responses in monkey dorsomedial frontal ortex. Ex~. Brain Res. 69: 460-468,1988. MITCHELL, S. J., RICHARDSON, R. T., BAKER, F. H., AND DELONG, M. R. The primate globus pallidus: neuronal activity related to direction of movement. Exp. Brain Res. 68: 491-505, 1987. MURPHY, J. T., KWAN, H. C., MACKAY, W. A., AND WONG, Y. C. Activity of primate precentral neurons during voluntary movements triggered by visual signals. Brain Res. 236: 429-449, 1982. OKANO, K. AND TANJI, .I. Neuronal activities in the primate motor fields of the agranular frontal cortex preceding visually triggered and selfpaced movement. Exp. Brain Res. 66: 155- 166, 1987. REYNOLDS, H. T. The Analysis of Cross-C’lassiJication. New York: Free, 1977. ROLAND, P. E., LARSEN, B., LASSEN, N. A., AND SKINHOJ, E. Supplementary motor area and other cortical areas in organization of voluntary movements in man. J. Neurophysiol. 43: 118-l 36, 1980. ROSEN, I. AND ASANUMA, H. Peripheral afferent inputs to the forelimb area of the monkey motor cortex: input-output relations. Exp. Brain Res. 14: 257-273, 1972. SCHMIDT, E. M., JOST, R. G., AND DAVIS, K. K. Reexamination of the force relationship of cortical cell discharge patterns with conditioned wrist movements. Brain Res. 83: 213-223, 1975. SCHWARTZ, A. B., KETTNER, R. E., AND GEORGOPOULOS, A. P. Primate motor cortex and free arm movements to visual targets in three-dimensional space. I. Relations between single cell discharge and direction of movement. J. Neurosci. 8: 29 13-2927, 1988. SMITH, A. M. The activity of supplementary motor area neurons during a maintained precision grip. Brain Res. 172: 3 15-327, 1979. SMITH, A. M., HEPP-REYMOND, M. C., AND WYSS, U. R. Relation of activity in precentral cortical neurons to force and rate of force change during isometric contractions of finger muscles. Exp. Brain Res. 23: 315-332, 1975. STARR, A. AND COHEN, L. G. “Gating” of somatosensory evoked potentials begins before the onset of voluntary movement in man. Brain Res. 348:183-186,1985. TANJI, J. AND KURATA, K. Comparison of movement-related activity in two cortical motor areas of primates. J. Neurophysiol. 48: 633-653, 1982. TANJI, J. AND KURATA, K. Contrasting neuronal activity in supplementary and precentral motor cortex of monkeys. I. Responses to instructions determining motor responses to forthcoming modalities. J. Neurophysiol. 53: 129-141, 1985. TANJI, J., TANIGUCHI, K., AND SAGA, T. Supplementary motor area: neuronal response to motor instructions. J. Neurophysiol. 43: 60-68, 1980. THACH, W. T. Correlation of neural discharge with pattern and force of muscular activity, joint position, and direction of intended next movement in motor cortex and cerebellum. J. Neurophysiol. 4 1: 654-676, 1978. WISE, S. P. AND TANJI, J. Supplementary and precentral motor cortex: contrast in responsiveness to peripheral input in the hindlimb area of the unanesthetized monkey. J. Camp. Neural. 195: 433-45 1, 198 1. WONG, Y. C., KWAN, H. C., MACKAY, W. A., AND MURPHY, J. T. Spatial organization of precentral cortex in awake primates. I. Somatosensory inputs. J. Neurophysiol. 4 1: 1107- 1119, 1978.