* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download 354 A
Parent management training wikipedia , lookup
Discrete trial training wikipedia , lookup
Psychological trauma wikipedia , lookup
Personality disorder wikipedia , lookup
Factitious disorder imposed on another wikipedia , lookup
Conversion disorder wikipedia , lookup
Schizoaffective disorder wikipedia , lookup
Depersonalization disorder wikipedia , lookup
Antisocial personality disorder wikipedia , lookup
Memory disorder wikipedia , lookup
Fragile X syndrome wikipedia , lookup
Claustrophobia wikipedia , lookup
Impulsivity wikipedia , lookup
Anorexia nervosa wikipedia , lookup
Panic disorder wikipedia , lookup
Selective mutism wikipedia , lookup
Munchausen by Internet wikipedia , lookup
Conduct disorder wikipedia , lookup
Diagnosis of Asperger syndrome wikipedia , lookup
Mental disorder wikipedia , lookup
Dissociative identity disorder wikipedia , lookup
Asperger syndrome wikipedia , lookup
Social anxiety disorder wikipedia , lookup
Autism spectrum wikipedia , lookup
Test anxiety wikipedia , lookup
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders wikipedia , lookup
Spectrum disorder wikipedia , lookup
Depression in childhood and adolescence wikipedia , lookup
Eating disorders and memory wikipedia , lookup
Anxiety disorder wikipedia , lookup
Eating disorder wikipedia , lookup
History of mental disorders wikipedia , lookup
Causes of mental disorders wikipedia , lookup
Death anxiety (psychology) wikipedia , lookup
Separation anxiety disorder wikipedia , lookup
Externalizing disorders wikipedia , lookup
Fall, 2014 Psychology 354 Psychopathology I: Child and Adolescent Psychopathology Instructor: Rex Forehand, Ph.D. Tuesday 8:30 – 11:15 Overview This course will provide an introduction to the field of psychopathology during childhood and adolescence. Topics will include systems of classification and taxonomy in psychopathology, research methods in developmental psychopathology, epidemiology, etiology, course, risk and protective factors, and specific problems and disorders of childhood and adolescence. Throughout the course a developmental perspective will be emphasized by examining psychological problems and disorders within the context of biological, psychological, and social developmental processes. The importance of empirical research in studying psychopathology will also be emphasized. You should leave this course with (a) a solid foundation in research in child and adolescent psychopathology, and (b) the ability to apply research in this field to understanding individual cases and to your own research. Books and Readings: The text for the course will be: 1. Mash, E.J., & Barkley, R.A. (2014). Child Psychopathology (3rd Edition). New York: Guilford. 2. A duplicated book of course notes and outside readings. 3. DSM-5 Class Structure Most (but not all) classes will be structured as follows: 1. Description of Disorder 2. Causal Factors 3. Model Development Course Evaluation and Grades Course grades will be based on the following: 1. Class participation (students are expected to be on time, attend all classes, to complete all assigned readings before the class, and to participate in class). I need to know before class if you have an illness or emergency that prevents you from attending. 2. A weekly report on the percent variance accounted for by each of four categories of correlates/risk/marker/causal risk factors (See Table 1 in Kazdin et al. distributed article) and page 8 for each of 10 disorders (ADHD, ODD, Conduct Disorder, Adolescent 1 Alcohol Abuse, Depression, Enuresis, Anxiety, Anorexia, Intellectual Disability, Autism). The report (see last 4 pages of syllabus for 2 examples) should be a singlespaced (with double spacing between paragraphs) typed paper which includes: (a) a summary of the percent variance accounted for by each of the 4 categories; and (b) a justification of why you assigned the percent variance to each category. Your justification should be based on the DSM-5, the Mash and Barkley assigned reading, any outside readings assigned, and any additional articles you may choose to read on the disorder. You will turn these in each week and selected class members will present each week. Please bring a copy of your summary to class for me, the “coordinator” of the week, and yourself. You will email your percent variance to the “coordinator” by Monday noon before class. The “coordinator” will put the percent variance for each of you on the template sheet, calculate means, pass out to everyone during class, and summarize. See page 6 for a list of coordinators by week. 3. Class presentations: Each student will make two additional kinds of presentations. a. Summary of a research article I distribute (see page 6 for list of presenters and articles). Your presentation should be 7 to 10 minutes, and include the study title, journal, authors, purpose, hypotheses, brief methodology, results, and conclusions. IMPORTANT: Point out why this study is important for the particular disorder we are examining. You can either do a PowerPoint presentation (have a copy of the slides for each class member) or summarize the study verbally with a summary page of the study for each class member. Individuals presenting were randomly determined. b. A 20- to 30-minute PowerPoint presentation (see page 7 for time and date for each student presentation) in one of the last two classes on an assigned chapter. Your presentation should consist of: definition and description of disorder or topic area, prevalence, causes, and developmental models on disorder(s). Distribute your slides to all class members. Your presentation should be a “formal” one (e.g., Power Point). 4. Second-year students (those who have had assessment) also will make a 2-minute presentation of the primary assessment instruments used for each disorder. You should be able to do this based on your assessment classes and discussing with graduate students and faculty members who are experts in the area. Prepare a ½-page handout (12 copies) to hand out in class. This is to introduce first-year students to the primary assessment instruments for children. See page 6 for date of your presentation. 5. Exams: There will be two exams involving the application of course material. Each exam will consist of one or, more likely, two vignettes and you will answer them in 4 typed pages (and double spaced) in a take home exam. 6. Course Grade: Number 1 above: Number 2 above: Number 3 above: A B 15% 20% (part of Number 1: Class Participation) 15% 2 Number 5 above: 50% (25% per exam) 7. NOTE: There is one “extra class” at the end in case we don’t get through all the material or in case I have to “call off” a class. If the latter occurs, we will move everything back one week except Rob Althoff, who has to come on 10/28. 3 Schedule* Mash & Barkley Book Readings (Chapters) 8/26 Overview, Introduction 9/2 Developmental Psychopathology: Overview of Causes, Classification 1 9/9 Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (Neurodevelopmental Disorder) 2 9/16 Oppositional Defiant Disorder/ 3 Conduct Disorder (Disruptive Impulse-Control, & Conduct Disorders) 9/23 Conduct Disorder Continued and Development & Psychopathology in Adolescence: (Substance Related) 4 (Substance-Related & Addictive Disorders) (Take Home Exam) Read DSM-5 Outside Reading (Assignments are in Outside Readings Book each week) ADHD ODD, Impulse Control, CD SubstanceRelated & Addictive 9/26 (Friday) Grand Rounds: Martha Wadsworth (10:30-11:45) 9/30 Anxiety Disorders Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder (Turn in Exam Before Class) 10/7 Tic Disorders & Hair-Pulling (Justin Parent – 8:30 – 9:10) Depressive Disorders 8 Anxiety 9 (Just OCD part of chapter) Obsessive-Compulsive Remainder Chapter 9 5 Depressive Disorders 10/14 Feeding & Eating Disorders (Anorexia Nervosa & Bulimia Nervosa) 16 Feeding & Eating 10/21 Intellectual Disability (Neurodevelopmental Disorder) 12 Intellectual Disabilities 10/28 Pediatric Bipolar & Disruptive Mood Dysregulation (Dr. Rob Althoff-8:30 – 9:30); Enuresis, Encopresis (Elimination Disorders) 6 Bipolar, Enuresis, Encopresis 11/4 Autism Spectrum Disorders (Neurodevelopmental Disorder) 11 11/11 Presentations (1 - 3 – see page 7) 19 (5 pages on Encopresis & Enuresis) Autism Spectrum 4 11/18 Presentations (4 - 7 – see page 7) 11/25 Thanksgiving 12/2 Make-up class (Take Home Exam: Due Monday, 12/8, by noon) ___________________ *Note: There is a required colloquium scheduled during the semester (9/26) and may be more scheduled. 5 1. Research Article Presentations* Date Topic Presenter Research Article 8/26 2. Overview of Assessment Instruments 3, Coordinators* Coordinator % Variance Sarah 9/9 Developmental Trajectories Kelsey (Miller) 9/16 ADHD Hannah (Bauermeister) Hannah Sarah 9/23 ODD/CD Virginia 1 (Lahey) Kelsey Emily (2 articles) 2 (Cunningham) Elias 1 (Martel) Elias Joanna Ivori 2 (Maumorstein) 9/30 Substance Use 10/7 Anxiety Geoff (Feng) Emily Kelsey 10/14 Depression Jonah Jonah Hannah Kerry 1 (Koffler) (Please include 3 models on page 459) 2 (Gallerani) Geoff Virginia 10/21 Eating Disorder Joanna (Measelle) 10/28 Enuresis None None 11/4 Intellectual Emily (Einfeld) Kelsey Kerry Sarah (Landa) Hannah Jonah Disability 11/11 Autism *NOTE: Research article presentations, overview of assessment instruments, and coordinators for % variance are independent (i.e., unrelated) activities that were put on the same page to save space. 6 Presentations on November 11 (#s 1-4) and December 18 (#s 5-7)* Name Topic Time Allocated 1. A. Jonah Chapter 7 (Suicidal & Non-Suicidal Self-Injurious) 30 minutes 2. A. Virginia B. Emily Chapter 10 (Childhood PTSD) 20 minutes 20 minutes 3. A. Kelsey (Sleep) B. Kerry Chapter 14 (Infancy & Toddlerhood: ½ on Sleep) 20 minutes 20 minutes 4. A. Sarah Chapter 17 (Early Onset Schizophrenia) 30 minutes 5. A. Ivori B. Hannah Chapter 13 (Learning Disabilities) 20 minutes 20 minutes 6. A. Joanna B. Geoff Chapter 18 (Personality Disorders) 20 minutes 20 minutes 7. A. Elias Chapter 19 (Health-Related & Somatic: Selected Parts) 30 minutes *When there are 2 presenters, you decide where to divide the chapter. 7 Categories of Correlates/Risk/Marker/Causal Risk Factors and Examples of Each 1. Temperament/Genetics: Biological and genetic factors – includes child characteristics such as gender and race. 2. Family Characteristics (a) Parenting (1) warmth/positive dimension (2) consistent discipline (3) structure (rules, supervision, monitoring) (4) psychological control (guilt induction, verbal manipulation) (b) (c) (d) (e) SES # siblings 2 parents vs 1 parent (divorce) family stress 1. conflict (between parents, between siblings, between parent & child) 2. parent depressive symptoms 3. parent antisocial personality 4. parent physical illness 3. Extrafamilial (peers, school, immediate neighborhood) (a) peers (b) school 1. general environment 2. teacher variables (c) neighborhood 1. risks 2. resources 3. population density (rural/urban) 4. Culture/Society (a) differences by “cultural” groups (b) differences 1. across regions of country 2. across countries 8 Percent Variance Accounted for By (1) Disorders Temperament/ Genetics/Child (2) Family Characteristics (3) Peers/School/Community (4) Culture/Society Total ADHD 100 ODD 100 CD 100 Alcohol Abuse 100 Depression 100 Enuresis 100 Anxiety 100 Anorexia 100 Intellectual Disability 100 Autism Spectrum 100 NOTE: No Percent Variance for Disruptive Mood Dysregulation, Bipolar, Obsessive-Compulsive, Encopresis, or Bulimia. 9 Child Psychopathology Percent Variance: Eating Disorders Temperament/ Genetics/Child 50% Family Characteristics 20% Peers/School/ Community 10% Cultural/Society 20% Research has pointed to a definite biological and genetic vulnerability in developing eating disorders. Neuroendocrine studies have shown low levels of norepinephrine in patients with anorexia nervosa (AN) and bulimia nervosa (BN), although it is not clear if this is a cause or result of the disorders (Mash & Barkley, 2003). In BN, on the other hand, individuals may suffer from a hyposerotonergic functioning causing an increase in meal size and the binging behaviors seen (Mash & Barkley, 2003). The genetic component of eating disorders has been studies through family studies and an increased risk for eating disorders has been found in relatives of individuals with AN and BN (Mash & Barkley, 2003). One study’s findings indicate that more than 50% of the risk for developing AN can be attributed to genetics (Tyre, 2005). Temperament is an impacting factor as well with perfectionism being the main trait associated with both AN and BN. Individuals with AN also have a tendency to conform, have an obsessive nature to them and an overcontrol of their emotionality. I think that child characteristics play a larger role in eating disorders than any of the other disorders that we have looked at thus far. Females have a much greater likelihood of developing eating disorders than males, with clinical samples of eating disorder patients showing 90 – 95% female (Mash & Barkley, 2003). Ethnicity is a risk factor as eating disorders occur mainly in white females, although recent research has shown that rates in black, Hispanic and Asian populations are rising (Tyre, 2005). Sexual orientation is another child characteristic that plays a role. Homosexuality is a risk factor for development in males, while in females this may serve as a protective factor (Mash & Barkley, 2003). In females, an early menarche is a specific risk factor for BN (Mash & Barkley, 2003). With these findings on the biological and genetic impact, the influence of temperament and the contribution of child characteristics, I have placed 50% of the variance in this category. Additionally, certain family environments put individuals at a greater risk for the development of eating disorders. In general, modeling of family members in terms of weight preoccupation and dieting, valuing thinness and having control over weight are all risk factors for the development of eating disorders (Mash & Barkley, 2003). One study looking specifically at BN found that families who tend to comment on the patients’ weight, shape or eating habits put these individuals at risk for the disorder, as well as tension or lack of attendance at family meals (Mash & Barkley, 2003). Research continues to be done on family interactions, and although most are inconclusive, there does seem to be a greater enmeshment, or overinvolvement, in families of patients with eating disorders, indicating that this type of home environment may put individuals at risk (Mash & Barkley, 2003). Fami8ly influences do appear to play a role in the development of eating disorders, although not to the degree that had been thought in the past due to the newer research on the biological vulnerability to the disorders. Therefore, I allotted 20% variance to this category. Research done on eating disorders has shown that culture has an influence on their development. As cultures become more “Westernized,” rates increase indicating the impact of the socially constructed Western ideal of thinness on the disorders (Mash & Barkley, 2003). The idea of beauty and interpersonal nature being feminine traits contributes to this socialization and is a function of our society today (Mash & Barkley, 2003). I consider this cultural influence to have a considerable impact on the development of AN and BN and have therefore placed 20% of the variance into the category of cultural 10 and society factors. Intertwined with this is the role of peers, school and community. Although there does not seem to be specific research on the impact of these factors, they go hand-in-hand with the overall social construction of the disorder. Environments where the cultural value of thinness and beauty are clearly demonstrated (in peers, school and social environments) put the individuals in them at greater risk for internalizing these values and for developing the disorders. Despite the lack of research on this category, my beliefs about these influences have caused me to place 10% of the variance in peer, school and community factors. 11 Percent Variance: Generalized Anxiety Disorder Temperament/Genetics/Child: 50% Family Characteristics: 30% Peers/School/Community: 10% Cultural/Society: 10% Anxiety is one of the most commonly diagnosed disorders in childhood and adolescence, and research has demonstrated its complex etiological profile (Mash & Barkley, 2003). As is the case with so many psychological disorders, the factors contributing to the etiology of anxiety in youth often interact with one another to form bidirectional and multidirectional causal pathways. Generalized Anxiety Disorder (GAD) is among the commonly seen types of anxiety disorders; one epidemiological study found a GAD prevalence of 5.9% in a sample of 15 year-old adolescents and another reported rates as high as 10.8% (Mash & Barkley, 2003). The factors associated with the etiology of GAD are discussed below. Given the evidence for the considerable contributions of genetics, temperament, and other child-specific factors to the etiology of GAD, I have allocated 50% of the variance to the Temperament/Genetics/Child category. Mash & Barkley (2003) discuss the evidence related to genetics and anxiety in general, namely that although heritability of specific anxiety disorders is unlikely, genes are thought to contribute to a general risk factor for anxiety proneness. Specifically, Mash & Barkley (2003) indicate that genetics may present a general risk factor for anxiety disorders, but environmental experiences influence how this vulnerability manifests (similar to the diathesis-stress model of depression). Evidence for the general heritability of anxiety as it relates to GAD is presented in the case study of Matthew (Anxiety and Somatoform Disorders). In this example, both of Matthew’s parents displayed some symptoms of anxiety; while neither parent had GAD specifically, the presence of genes related to the vulnerability for anxiety in parents may contribute to the development of GAD in children (psychosocial factors will be discussed below). Furthermore, research has shown that GAD is diagnosed more commonly in females than in males, and that this finding is stable across cultures (Anxiety and Somatoform Disorders; Mash & Barkley, 2003). While social factors related to this finding are likely, it is possible that this reflects a sex-linked characteristic of the genes related to a vulnerability for anxiety. There is also considerable evidence for the contributions of temperament to the etiology of anxiety disorders. While Mash & Barkley (2003) present this evidence in the context of anxiety disorders in general, given the pervasive nature of GAD it is likely that these factors are relevant to this disorder. Behavioral Inhibition has been proposed as a likely underlying temperamental characteristic of children who present with anxiety disorders (Mash & Barkley, 2003). The Behavioral Inhibition profile consists of the child’s sociability, physiology, and shy or fearful behaviors, and Mash & Barkley (2003) cite longitudinal evidence that this temperamental style is associated with those behaviors consistent with anxiety in children. Again, although these findings are not specific to GAD, the excessive and uncontrollable nature of worries in GAD may be related to factors that have widespread influence (i.e., temperament). Furthermore, Mash & Barkley (2003) discuss the biobehavioral system thought to be associated with the Behavioral Inhibition temperament, namely the Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS). This is a complex system of nervous system components that is associated with (among other things) increased caution and processing of threat-relevant information (Mash & Barkley, 2003). Thus, this system may contribute to the symptoms of GAD. In addition to the contributions of genetics and temperament, child-specific factors such as cognitive style may play a role in the etiology of GAD (Mash & Barkley, 2003). Finally, cognitive processes have been found to contribute to the etiology of GAD in children. Children with GAD tend to overestimate the likelihood of negative consequences and worry in the absence of objective cause for concern (Mash & Barkley, 2003). These types of thought patterns and information processing biases represent cognitive distortions that may vary among children, and constitute the psychological vulnerability patterns discussed in Mash & Barkley (2003). I have allocated 30% of the variance associated with the etiology of GAD to the Family Characteristics category. The findings related to the heritability of general genetic risk factors for anxiety must take into account the psychosocial aspects of the family unit, as environmental influences may be at work (Mash & Barkley, 2003). As discussed above, unique environmental experiences work to determine the expression of this genetic vulnerability, and in children and adolescences these experiences are likely to stem from the family unit. As 12 referenced above, the case of study of Matthew in Anxiety and Somatoform Disorders demonstrates how parental symptoms of anxiety may be related to GAD in children. While this may be due to genetics, it may also be due to parenting behaviors or other aspects of the family environment that are common to both parents and children. Furthermore, Mash & Barkley (2003) discuss the important role of attachment in the etiology of anxiety, and point to findings indicating that insufficient affection from caregivers played a role in the development of GAD in children. Parenting behaviors also contribute to the etiology of GAD, as parents can condition processes in their children—such as cognitive schemas—that increase anxious cognitions (Mash & Barkley, 2003). For example, as discussed in Anxiety and Somatoform Disorders, when parents model or exhibit anxious responses, or reinforce any avoidant or anxious behaviors in their children, they may set the foundation for the development of more persistent or severe symptoms of anxiety. Relating more generally to anxiety, Mash & Barkley (2003) note that child anxiety disorders are significantly influenced by parents’ anxiety. Furthermore, they discuss how children’s experience with anxiety may be related to past experiences with control (Mash & Barkley, 2003). For children and adolescents, situations in which they have felt out of control may have occurred within the family unit (e.g., experiences with domestic violence). Regarding extrafamilial contributions to the etiology of GAD, I have allocated 10% of the variance to the Peers/School/Community category. The finding that GAD has been found to be unstable over time indicates that there are environmental factors at work (Mash & Barkley, 2003). This is related to the general finding presented above that unique environmental influences impact how genetic and psychological vulnerabilities come to manifest in the presentation of anxiety disorders; it is possible that these environmental influences stem from a child’s peer group, school, or community (Mash & Barkley, 2003). In addition, events relating to a child’s experience of control may stem in part from the peer group, school, or community (Mash & Barkley, 2003). There may also be influences of peer relationships on GAD; these factors might not cause GAD per se but may work to exacerbate a child’s symptoms. For example, a child with GAD who requires frequent reassurance and experiences persistent worries regarding peer interactions may actually be rejected by his or her peers, thus resulting in a bidirectional relationship between child-specific factors and the peer group (Mash & Barkley, 2003). Given the gender differences found in the diagnosis of anxiety disorders, it is possible that peer groups are influencing the expression of anxiety symptoms in children (Mash & Barkley, 2003). Finally, Mash & Barkley (2003) cite evidence that children with OAD (now represented by GAD) come from middle or upper income families, indicating some effect of SES on GAD in children. In my opinion, the cultural influences on GAD stem largely from those relating to anxiety in general. I have allocated 10% of the variance associated with the etiology of GAD to the Cultural/Society category, largely due to the discussion presented by Mash & Barkley (2003) regarding the acceptability of fear expression across cultures. These differences may influence the extent to which fears are expressed—and, to some extent, the ways in which fears are experienced or perceived to be experienced among individuals. Furthermore, the finding that GAD is diagnosed more commonly in females than males across cultures speaks to the widespread influence of societally-determined gender norms among a variety of cultures (Anxiety and Somatoform Disorders). Finally, as discussed in Anxiety and Somatoform Disorders, the presentation of GAD varies by age (i.e., in terms of what children worry about). Culture may therefore influence the content and severity of worries at a given developmental stage, which would influence how children experience their own anxiety in addition to the diagnosis and progression of GAD symptoms over time. 13