Download Selectivity and sparseness in the responses of striate complex cells

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Nervous system network models wikipedia , lookup

Psychoneuroimmunology wikipedia , lookup

Incomplete Nature wikipedia , lookup

Mathematical model wikipedia , lookup

Psychophysics wikipedia , lookup

Channelrhodopsin wikipedia , lookup

Holonomic brain theory wikipedia , lookup

Response priming wikipedia , lookup

Metastability in the brain wikipedia , lookup

Evoked potential wikipedia , lookup

Biological neuron model wikipedia , lookup

Mixture model wikipedia , lookup

Perception of infrasound wikipedia , lookup

Neural coding wikipedia , lookup

Stimulus (physiology) wikipedia , lookup

Efficient coding hypothesis wikipedia , lookup

Feature detection (nervous system) wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Vision Research 45 (2005) 57–73
www.elsevier.com/locate/visres
Selectivity and sparseness in the responses of striate complex cells
Sidney R. Lehky
a
a,*
, Terrence J. Sejnowski
b,c
, Robert Desimone
d
Cognitive Brain Mapping Laboratory, RIKEN Brain Science Institute, Hirosawa 2-1, Wako-shi, Saitama 351-0198, Japan
b
Howard Hughes Medical Institute, The Salk Institute, 10010 North Torrey Pines Road, La Jolla, CA 92037, USA
c
Department of Biology, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA
d
Laboratory of Neuropsychology, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD 20892, USA
Received 19 February 2004; received in revised form 23 June 2004
Abstract
Probability distributions of macaque complex cell responses to a large set of images were determined. Measures of selectivity
were based on the overall shape of the response probability distribution, as quantified by either kurtosis or entropy. We call this
non-parametric selectivity, in contrast to parametric selectivity, which measures tuning curve bandwidths. To examine how receptive
field properties affected non-parametric selectivity, two models of complex cells were created. One was a standard Gabor energy
model, and the other a slight variant constructed from a Gabor function and its Hilbert transform. Functionally, these models differed primarily in the size of their DC responses. The Hilbert model produced higher selectivities than the Gabor model, with the
two models bracketing the data from above and below. Thus we see that tiny changes in the receptive field profiles can lead to major
changes in selectivity. While selectivity looks at the response distribution of a single neuron across a set of stimuli, sparseness looks
at the response distribution of a population of neurons to a single stimulus. In the model, we found that on average the sparseness of
a population was equal to the selectivity of cells comprising that population, a property we call ergodicity. We raise the possibility
that high sparseness is the result of distortions in the shape of response distributions caused by non-linear, information-losing transforms, unrelated to information theoretic issues of efficient coding.
2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Vision; Macaque monkey; Modeling; Information theory; Sparse coding
1. Introduction
How the visual system encodes images includes questions of why individual visual units have particular
receptive field organizations, and how populations of
units act together. One approach to systematically characterizing receptive fields is to quantify their stimulus
selectivities. Here we are concerned with determining
selectivities of striate complex cells to ‘‘complicated’’
stimuli, including natural images. This will involve con*
Corresponding author. Address: Computational Neuroscience
Laboratory, The Salk Institute, 10010 North Torrey Pines Road, La
Jolla, CA 92037, USA.
E-mail address: [email protected] (S.R. Lehky).
0042-6989/$ - see front matter 2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.visres.2004.07.021
sideration primarily of non-linear models of complex
cells, although some single-unit data will be analyzed
as well. Complex cells comprise a large percentage of
the units in V1, with estimates ranging from around
40–90% of the total (De Valois, Albrecht, & Thorell,
1982; Hubel & Wiesel, 1968; Schiller, Finlay, & Volman,
1976).
Related to selectivity is the idea of sparseness, which
attempts to explain receptive fields in terms of information theoretic notions of efficient coding (Atick, 1992;
Barlow, Kaushal, & Mitchison, 1989; Field, 1994; Field,
1999; see Simoncelli & Olshausen (2001) and Simoncelli
(2003), for reviews). Under sparse coding, a small subset
within a neural population will respond strongly to a
stimulus, while most will respond poorly. Sparse codes
58
S.R. Lehky et al. / Vision Research 45 (2005) 57–73
can be efficient from an information theoretic standpoint when they arise from sets of receptive fields that
are matched to statistical regularities in the environment. Efficiency in this case means reduced redundancy
(decorrelation) among responses of units within a population while seeking to preserve image information as far
a possible. This decorrelation is produced by linear
operations on the stimulus input, in the form of convolution with a set of receptive fields. (Some recent sparseness models go beyond decorrelation, and attempt to
pick out higher order visual structures by including an
additional non-linear layer; for example see Karklin &
Lewicki (2003)).
In this study, we are concerned with what we call
‘‘non-parametric’’ selectivity. Non-parametric selectivity
is determined by the shape of the probability density
function (pdf) of response magnitudes to a large set of
stimuli (Fig. 1B). The shape is quantified by measures
such as kurtosis or entropy, which will be explained
more fully in Section 2. These measures seek to pick
out pdfs that are ‘‘peakier’’ than a Gaussian, and with
heavier tails. Such distributions indicate responses that
are close to spontaneous levels for most stimuli but
occasionally are much larger, with intermediate responses being less common than under a Gaussian distribution. The Gaussian distribution serves as a
reference distribution indicative of low selectivity. In
contrast to this non-parametric selectivity, the more
commonly used parametric selectivity does not depend
on response probability distributions, but rather measures the bandwidth of response tuning curves to some
parameter, such as orientation or spatial frequency
(Fig. 1A).
The use of non-parametric selectivity is appropriate
when dealing with ‘‘complicated’’ stimuli, such as natural images, that are not ordered by some metric. One
might want to use such stimuli to characterize the sys-
(A)
tem under more ecological conditions, or perhaps because receptive fields are so complicated that we do
not understand along what parameters they parse their
input.
Selectivity is defined here in terms of the probability
distribution of responses of a single unit to a population
of stimuli. Sparseness, on the other hand, is in some
sense the converse of selectivity. Sparseness is determined by the distribution of responses of a population
of units to a single stimulus. In both the cases, although
the probability distributions are measuring different
things, the measurement of the distribution shapes can
be calculated using the same methods.
To compare our terminology with that used in some
previous studies, our ‘‘non-parametric selectivity’’ is
equivalent to the ‘‘lifetime sparseness’’ used by Willmore
and Tolhurst (2001). What Vinje and Gallant (2000,
2002) call the sparseness of complex cells is, under
our terminology, not sparseness but non-parametric
selectivity.
Although selectivity and sparseness measure different
things, they are nonetheless related quantities. In particular, the average sparseness of a population over multiple stimulus inputs must equal the average selectivity of
the neurons within the population (Földiak, 2002), provided responses of units are uncorrelated. Therefore
populations that exhibit sparse coding will be composed
of neurons showing high selectivity. We label systems
exhibiting this equivalence between selectivity and
sparseness as ‘‘ergodic’’. (This is a term taken from statistical mechanics, where the average of a single system
across time is compared with the average of an ensemble
of systems at one time.) The relationship between
sparseness and selectivity will be further discussed in
the section on ergodicity.
Algorithms that generate sparse codes have been
applied to natural images (Bell & Sejnowski, 1997;
(B)
non-parametric
Response
Probability density
parametric
Parameter value
Response
Fig. 1. Two approaches to defining selectivity. (A) Parametric selectivity: Selectivity is defined in terms of the bandwidth of the tuning curve for a
stimulus parameter (orientation, spatial frequency, etc.). (B) Non-parametric selectivity: selectivity is defined by a statistic. such as kurtosis or
entropy, describing the shape of the probability distribution of response magnitudes. This measure is appropriate when a stimulus set, such as natural
images, is not ordered by some parameter.
S.R. Lehky et al. / Vision Research 45 (2005) 57–73
Olshausen & Field, 1996, 1997; van Hateren & van der
Schaaf, 1998). The resulting receptive fields resemble
Gabor functions, similar to striate simple cells (Jones
& Palmer, 1987), although not incorporating the rectifying non-linearity of actual simple cells. These results
have been used to support the hypothesis that V1
implements statistically efficient coding through
sparseness.
Moving to V1 complex cells, Vinje and Gallant (2000,
2002) have also interpreted observation of sparseness in
their data as supporting the efficient coding hypothesis.
While both our data and modeling confirm that complex
cell responses are indeed sparse, we shall question
whether a high sparseness index is indicative of efficient
coding in this case. Although efficient coding can be a
useful basis for understanding receptive field organization in the early, linear stages of visual processing, other
conceptual frameworks may be necessary for dealing
with non-linear, information-losing transforms such as
occur in complex cells and beyond in the visual
pathways. After documenting some response properties
of complex cells and modeling them, we shall present
a more general discussion of the limitations of
information theoretic modeling in the context of such
a system.
2. Materials and methods
59
Fig. 2. Examples of synthetic stimulus images, and resulting responses.
Responses are taken from experimental data for a single complex cell.
By presenting a large set of images and tabulating the probability
distribution of the resulting responses, the non-parametric selectivity
of a unit can be measured.
2.1. Data acquisition
Twenty-four V1 units were recorded from an anesthetized macaque monkey (M. fasicularis). Receptive fields
were within 5 of fixation. All were known to be complex cells from the equality of responses to both white
and black bars against a gray background. Stimuli consisted of 157 synthetic patterns of two types, 78 random
textures and 79 shaded paraboloid figures, displayed
within a circular aperture of 1.5 (examples shown in
Fig. 2). Patterns were presented in random order, and
interspersed with more traditional stimuli such as bars
and gratings, although responses to those simpler stimuli are not considered here. Presentation of each pattern
was repeated 30 times. Stimulus duration was 200 ms,
with a 250 ms blank period between stimuli, a fast presentation pace in order to accumulate data for many patterns. Details of the physiological techniques have been
described previously (Lehky, Sejnowski, & Desimone,
1992), and the data presented here represent a subset
of data from that publication.
2.2. Data analysis
The peristimulus time histogram (PSTH) for each image was determined from the spike train running with a
50 ms lag relative to stimulus presentation, to take V1
latency into account. Spikes rates were estimated using
a two-stage adaptive kernel technique (Silverman,
1986). In the first stage, each spike was convolved with
a Gaussian having rinit = 20 ms and the resulting curves
summed to give a preliminary PSTH. In the second
stage, going back to the raw spike train, each spike
was convolved with a Gaussian whose r was inversely
related to the local spike p
rate
ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiestimated in the preliminary PSTH, with r ¼ rinit m=r, where m is the geometric mean of the spike rate over the stimulus duration,
and r is the spike rate at a given instant. Data from all
repetitions of each stimulus were pooled prior to forming the kernel estimate.
For each unit, 157 PSTHs were determined, one for
each stimulus image. Each PSTH was then reduced to a
single summary number. We use mean response as the
summary statistic in the presentation below, but using
peak responses instead would not have made much difference. From these 157 numbers a probability distribution of response magnitudes was compiled. As the
observed probability distributions for the 24 units were
positively skewed, for descriptive purposes they were
fit with gamma distributions:
f ðr j a; bÞ ¼
1
r
ra1 e b
ba CðaÞ
ð1Þ
60
S.R. Lehky et al. / Vision Research 45 (2005) 57–73
2.3. Measuring selectivity
Non-parametric selectivity measures should depend
solely on the response distribution shape, and not its
scale (variance) or location (mean). A number of measures are available, although all have drawbacks of various sorts and there is room for the development of new
methods for dealing with this issue.
2.3.1. Kurtosis
The most common selectivity index is the reduced
kurtosis of the response probability distribution. This
is the normalized fourth moment of the distribution, reduced by subtracting three:
4
hðri rÞ i
SK ¼
3
r4
ð2Þ
where ri is the units response to the ith image, r is the
mean response over all images, r is the standard deviation of the responses, and h Æ i is the mean value operator. Subtracting three normalizes the measure so that a
Gaussian has a kurtosis of zero. Larger values of SK
correspond to greater selectivity. A practical problem
with using kurtosis is that it involves raising data to
the fourth power, which makes estimates of this measure
highly sensitive to noise in the data.
2.3.2. Activity fraction
Another selectivity statistic in the literature is:
!2 ,
n
n
X
X
A¼
ri =n
ðr2i Þ=n
i¼1
ð3Þ
i¼1
(Rolls & Tovee, 1995), where n is the number of stimulus
images. It measures the fraction of units active on average over a set of inputs, generalized for continuous-valued units rather than binary ones. Small values of A
indicate high selectivity. The activity fraction measure
was slightly modified by Vinje and Gallant (2000):
SA ¼
1A
1 1=n
2.3.3. Entropy
Introduced here is a measure of selectivity based on
the entropy of the response probability distribution.
This has the advantage of connecting more directly to
information theoretic definitions of sparseness than does
kurtosis. In this measure, selectivity is quantified as the
decrease in entropy relative to a Gaussian distribution,
which has maximum entropy for a fixed variance:
S E ¼ H G H ðrÞ
1
H G ¼ log2 ð2per2 Þ ¼ 2:074 bits
2
The activity fraction index takes the two terms in the
variance formula and divides rather than subtracts
them, leading to a measure that is still proportional to
variance.
ð7Þ
with variance normalized to one. Entropy of the probability distribution of the data is:
Z
H ðrÞ ¼ pðrÞlog2 ðpðrÞÞ dr
ð8Þ
where p(r) is the response probability density function
(pdf). For practical calculations Eq. (8) is discretized to:
H ðrÞ ¼ i¼1
ð6Þ
where HG is the entropy of a Gaussian, and H(r) is the
entropy of a units response distribution taken from the
data. Equating high selectivity with low entropy captures the characteristic of a highly selective unit having
little response to most stimuli and a large response to
a few. The value of SE has a minimum of zero, and increases with no upper bound as selectivity increases (because the entropy of the data H(r) can have negative
values, as it involves a continuous-valued variable, response magnitude). Since the variance of a distribution
affects its entropy, all calculations are done after rescaling the data to unit variance.
The entropy of the Gaussian reference distribution is
given by Rieke, Warland, van Steveninck, and Bialek
(1997):
ð4Þ
thereby inverting and rescaling the index.
A problem with activity fraction is that, in addition
to being a measure of probability distribution shape, it
is also sensitive to the distributions mean and variance.
The close relation between activity fraction and variance
can be seen by comparing Eq. (3) with a common formula for variance:
!2
n
n
X
X
2
ðri Þ=n ðri =nÞ
ð5Þ
i¼1
To properly use this measure, the data should be
standardized for mean and variance beforehand. We will
not be using activity fraction, in preference for the entropy measure described next.
M
X
pðrj Þlog2 ðpðrj ÞÞDr
ð9Þ
j¼1
where responses from n stimulus images have been
placed in M bins.
The value of H(r) will depend on the bin size Dr,
which in turn depends on the number of bins into which
the response range has been divided. The number of bins
therefore needs to be standardized,pand
ffiffiffi this is done by
defining the number to be M ¼ n, where n is the
number of images in the stimulus set. Expressing the
selectivity index SE (Eq. (6)) in terms of Eqs. (7) and
(9), we get:
M
X
pðrj Þlog2 ðpðrj ÞÞDr
ð10Þ
S E ¼ 2:074 þ
j¼1
S.R. Lehky et al. / Vision Research 45 (2005) 57–73
If selectivity of a neuron is defined in terms of its response entropy, then selectivity can be related to the mutual information between stimulus and response:
61
metric selectivity, where the parameter of interest by
its nature defines the stimulus set.
2.4. Modeling
Iðr; sÞ ¼ H ðrÞ H ðrjsÞ
¼ S E þ ðH G H ðrjsÞÞ
ð11Þ
H(rjs) is the conditional entropy of the response given
the stimulus, and is essentially entropy due to noise in
the system. Given fixed noise entropy H(rjs) (as well as
HG, fixed by definition), increasing selectivity decreases
mutual information. In other words, there is a conflict
between maximizing selectivity of a unit and maximizing
information transfer.
A drawback to the entropy measure is that each cell
needs to be tested with a large number of stimulus
images in order for entropy to be accurately estimated.
This is demonstrated in Fig. 3, which shows the entropy
of different-sized samples of a Gaussian distributed random variable. Entropy asymptotically approaches the
theoretical value from below as stimulus set size increases. In general, it is not possible to accurately determine the shape of a probability distribution from a small
data sample, particularly the tails, and that will affect
calculations of entropy.
A second potential problem with SE is that there are
situations where, unlike in Fig. 3, the entropy does not
converge as the size of the stimulus set increases. However, that will not be an issue for any probability distributions encountered in this study.
A general point about non-parametric selectivity is
that its value depends not only on the receptive field
organization of a cell, but also on the particular stimulus
set used. Non-parametric selectivity is always relative to
the stimulus set, not absolute. This is not true of para-
In addition to data from striate complex cells, we
looked at selectivities of model units. This allowed us
to expand the stimulus set beyond what was used experimentally, and allows examination of the effects of varying receptive field properties in a well-defined manner.
Two models of complex cells will be presented, with
rather subtle differences in their receptive fields that lead
to large differences in their selectivities. The first is based
on Gabor functions. For this model, the complex cell responses are defined as the quadrature pair summation of
two subunits with Gabor receptive fields (Emerson,
Korenberg, & Citron, 1992; Szulborski & Palmer,
1990), at sine and cosine phase respectively:
qffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi
C ¼ ðGsin SÞ2 þ ðGcos SÞ2
ð12Þ
where C is the complex cell response, and G * S is the result of convolving a Gabor subunit with the stimulus
image. This class of energy model for complex cells is
widely used (Adelson & Bergen, 1985; Heeger, 1992; Ohzawa, DeAngelis, & Freeman, 1990; Pollen & Ronner,
1983; Spitzer & Hochstein, 1988). The Gabor subunits
resemble striate simple cells, except that they are not
half-wave rectified. Each Gabor subunit can be viewed,
therefore, as representing the pooled output of a pair of
simple cells with opposite contrast polarities.
The Gabor functions are sinusoidal plane waves with
spatial frequency f, orientation h, and phase /, under a
Gaussian envelope:
h
y 0 2 i
0 2
12 ðrxx Þ þ ry
G/ ¼ e
sinð2pfx0 þ /Þ
ð13Þ
where x 0 and y 0 are within the rotated coordinate
system:
x0 ¼ x cosðhÞ þ y sinðhÞ
Entropy
2
y 0 ¼ x sinðhÞ þ y cosðhÞ
1
0
0
100
200
300
400
Stimulus set size
500
Fig. 3. Entropy of a Gaussian distribution as a function of the size of
the stimulus set. Entropy increases with the size of the stimulus set up
to an asymptote of 2.07, which is the theoretical value for a Gaussian
with unit variance. This demonstrates that under the entropy measure
of selectivity (Eq. (10)), a probability distribution will have an
artifactually low entropy if stimulus sample size is small. A stimulus
set of at least several dozen images is required to get a reasonable
estimate of this parameter.
ð14Þ
Twenty-four model complex cells were created, having
four spatial frequency tuning curves and six orientations
per spatial frequency. Peak spatial frequencies were
f = [0.031, 0.062, 0.125, and 0.250] cycles/pixel, which
given 128 · 128 input images translates to [4, 8, 16, 32]
cycles/picture. Orientations were [0, 30, 60, 90, 120,
150]. With respect to the Gaussian envelope, rx defines
the spatial frequency bandwidth, and was set to 0.65/f.
The ratio ry/rx defines the orientation tuning bandwidth, and was set to 1.7. These appear to be physiologically realistic values (De Valois et al., 1982; Kulikowski
& Vidyasagar, 1986), producing a spatial frequency
bandwidth of 1.6 octaves and orientation bandwidth
of 33, full width at half-maximum.
S.R. Lehky et al. / Vision Research 45 (2005) 57–73
where H( Æ ) represents the Hilbert transform. The Hilbert transform in essence takes the Fourier transform
of a waveform, shifts the phase by 90, and then does
the inverse Fourier transform, producing a waveform
that is identical to the original other than the phase shift.
We used the ‘‘hilbert’’ command in the signal processing
toolbox of Matlab (www.mathworks.com), and mathematical details of implementing the transform are given
in their documentation. Morrone and Burr (1988) have
previously used the Hilbert transform for modeling
receptive fields in the context of biological vision.
The Hilbert transform of a sine Gabor is almost identical to a cosine Gabor (Fig. 4), except that it integrates
to zero. The difference in the receptive field profiles is so
small it would be almost impossible to detect through
mapping experiments. The difference does show up
clearly, however, in their spatial frequency amplitude
spectra (Fig. 5) at low frequencies.
So, to summarize, we had two sets of 24 model complex cells. One was based on the Gabor model and the
other on the Hilbert model, and the only difference
was a tiny change in the receptive field profile for one
of their two subunits.
The model units were tested with the same set of 157
images that had been presented to the actual complex
cells (example images shown in Fig. 2). During testing,
each image was centered on the model receptive field,
in the same manner as was done during the actual neurophysiological experiments. Statistics were collected for
response probability distributions, and once it had been
verified that the model units had similar properties to
those seen in the data, they were presented with an expanded stimulus set of 500 natural images (Fig. 6). Each
(A)
Response
1.0
cosine gabor
0
-1.0
Spatial position
(B)
1.0
Response
Sine and cosine Gabor functions are only approximate quadrature pairs. This reflects the fact that while
the sine Gabor integrates to zero, the cosine Gabor does
not, so that the two functions have different mean values
or different zero spatial frequency amplitudes. A quadrature pair should be identical other than a 90 phase
shift, and the Gabor pair does not fulfill that condition.
In observational terms, this means that complex cells
constructed from Gabor pairs are only approximately
phase invariant, showing a ripple in their responses as
a grating is drifted across their receptive fields.
In addition to the above Gabor model units, a second
set of 24 model complex cells was constructed that were
perfectly phase invariant. We call this the Hilbert model,
because synthesizing these units required the use of the
Hilbert transform. In this model, the two subunits were,
first, a sine Gabor the same as before, and second, instead of a cosine Gabor, the Hilbert transform of a sine
Gabor. Given this pair of subunits, they were combined
in the same manner as previously:
qffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi
2
2
C ¼ ðGsin SÞ þ ðH ðGsin Þ SÞ
ð15Þ
hilbert pair
0
-1.0
Spatial position
(C)
8
x 10
–3
gabor-hilbert
6
Response diff
62
4
2
0
Spatial position
Fig. 4. Subunit receptive field profiles for Gabor and Hilbert models
of complex cells. Although differing only slightly, they lead to very
different statistics in their responses to natural images. (A) Gabor
model: the two subunits had receptive fields consisting of a sine Gabor
function, and a cosine Gabor (pictured here). (B) Hilbert model: the
two subunits had receptive fields consisting of a sine Gabor function,
the same as before, and the Hilbert transform of a sine Gabor (pictured
here). This Hilbert transform receptive field resembles a cosine Gabor,
except that it integrates to zero while the Gabor does not. (C)
Difference between the cosine Gabor and the Hilbert transform
receptive fields (note scale).
of the natural images was sampled multiple times by
each model unit, as the receptive field was shifted about
to different patches within the image. The total number
of sampled image patches ranged from 500 at the lowest
spatial frequency up to 24,000 at the highest frequency.
S.R. Lehky et al. / Vision Research 45 (2005) 57–73
gabor
hilbert
Amplitude
0.8
0.6
0.4
0.2
0
0
1.0
2.0
Spatial Frequency
3.0
Fig. 5. Spatial frequency spectra of a cosine Gabor and the Hilbert
transform of a sine Gabor. Differences in response statistics to natural
images for the two models arise because of these different spatial
frequency spectra. Spatial profiles of the two receptive fields are
pictured in Fig. 4. Spatial frequency is on a relative scale, with curve
peak set to 1.0.
tions (Eq. (1)), also shown in Fig. 7, although no theoretical significance is placed on that description. These
three examples illustrate distributions with selectivities
near the maximum, minimum and median over all recorded units, with both the kurtosis SK (Eq. (2)) and entropy SE (Eq. (10)) selectivity indices indicated in each
case.
The distributions of the selectivity indices for the 24
neuron in the data are shown in Fig. 8. Also given are
their median values, SK = 0.84 and SE = 0.23, indicating
that selectivities of units are typically substantially
SE =0.74
0.2
Probability density
1.0
63
SK =3.54
0.1
0
SE =0.22
Probability density
0.2
Responses of an example striate complex cell to several images are shown in Fig. 2. Clearly the response
magnitudes differ substantially from image to image,
and it is straightforward to examine the statistical distribution of these responses over a set of inputs. In addition to differences in response magnitude there are
differences in response temporal waveforms, which will
not be considered here.
Response probability distributions for three example
neurons from the data are shown in Fig. 7. They are
positively skewed and are well fit by gamma distribu-
S E=0.09
0.2
Probability density
3. Results
0.1
0
Fig. 6. Examples from among the 500 natural images presented to
model complex cells.
High frequency units had smaller receptive field diameters, allowing a greater number of image samples.
SK =0.76
SK =0.07
0.1
0
0
10
20
30
Spikes/sec
40
50
Fig. 7. Histograms of response probability distributions for three
macaque complex cells. Cells were presented with a set of 157 synthetic
stimulus images. Also shown is the best-fit gamma function in each
case, and spontaneous activity (vertical dashed line). Both the kurtosis
(SK) and entropy (SE) measures of selectivity are given for each unit.
These examples show response distributions for units with high and
low selectivities, plus a distribution close to the median.
64
S.R. Lehky et al. / Vision Research 45 (2005) 57–73
greater than for the Gaussian reference distribution
(which would have SK = SE = 0).
There was a negative correlation between the median
activity of a neuron and selectivity (Fig. 9). The least active neurons were the most selective. However, the level
of activity in itself does not determine selectivity. For
example, a Gaussian distribution with a high mean or
a low mean both indicate exactly the same selectivity.
There must be a change in the shape of the response distribution that correlates with activity, and in this case
the distributions change shape by becoming more skewed as mean activity drops. Highly skewed distributions
register as more selective under our measures. Increased
skewness as the mean decreases is a general property of
random variables whose variances are large relative to
their means, and which are also constrained to have only
non-negative values (as is the case with firing rates). The
relationship shown in Fig. 9 is an indication that high
selectivity measures can arise from distortions in the
shape of response probability distributions due to nonlinearities in the system, rather than the sort of linear
transforms discussed by Field (1994), as will be discussed further below.
3.1. Selectivity in model complex cells
Fig. 8. Distributions of kurtosis and entropy measures of selectivity
for 24 macaque complex units presented with synthetic images.
Selectivity (S K )
6.0
r=–0.46
4.0
2.0
0.0
Selectivity (S E )
0.8
r=–0.57
0.6
0.4
0.2
0.0
0
20
40
60
80
Median resp. (spikes/sec)
Fig. 9. Negative correlation in the data between selectivity and the
median stimulus response of the 24 complex cells. This is shown for
both the kurtosis (SK) and entropy (SE) measures of selectivity.
The model complex cells had positively skewed response probability distributions, and negative correlations between response magnitude and selectivity, both
features of the data. Fig. 10 shows the distributions,
for synthetic images, of the kurtosis and entropy selectivity indices under the Gabor model (Eq. (13)). and
the Hilbert model (Eq. (15)). The median selectivities
for the two models and for the data, again for synthetic
images, are summarized in Table 1. The table shows that
the selectivity of the data is bracketed by the two models, being greater than the Gabor model and less than
the Hilbert model, but closer to the Gabor model. Being
intermediate to the two models in this manner suggests
that actual complex cells have subunits that do not integrate to zero, with the discrepancy from zero being
somewhat smaller than for Gabor model units. An
implication of this is that actual complex cells should
not exhibit perfect phase invariance but show a ripple
response to drifting gratings. This is in fact an observed
feature of complex cells (reviewed by Spitzer & Hochstein, 1988). Higher selectivity when receptive fields integrate to zero, as shown in Table 1, has previously been
noted by Baddeley (1996a) in modeling of linear units
resembling retinal ganglion cells.
Having verified that the modeling provides a reasonable match to the data for synthetic stimulus images, we
can now look at model responses to natural images.
These are also shown in Table 1. Selectivities of model
complex cells are higher for natural images than
synthetic ones (with the exception of one condition).
S.R. Lehky et al. / Vision Research 45 (2005) 57–73
(B)
(A)
10
12
Gabor
Hilbert
median=0.70
8
6
4
–2
0
2
4
Selectivity (S K )
4
0
6
–2
0
2
4
6
Selectivity (S K )
10
12
median=0.15
Gabor
10
Hilbert
median=0.37
Frequency
8
8
6
4
6
4
2
2
0
6
2
2
0
median=1.93
8
Frequency
Frequency
10
Frequency
65
0
0. 1
0. 2
0. 3
Selectivity (S E )
0
0
0. 3
0.6
Selectivity (SE )
0. 9
Fig. 10. Distributions of kurtosis and entropy measures of selectivity for Gabor and Hilbert model complex cells presented with synthetic images.
These are analogous to selectivity measures for the data shown in Fig. 8. (A) Gabor model. (B) Hilbert model.
Table 1
Selectivity measures for complex cell experimental data and the two
models of complex cells
Kurtosis
Gabor model
Data
Hilbert model
Entropy
Synthetic
Natural
Synthetic
Natural
0.7
0.8
1.9
2.2
{2.8}
7.1
0.15
0.23
0.37
0.08
{0.24}
0.53
Results for both synthetic and natural images are included. Values in
brackets are estimated by linear interpolation between the Gabor and
Hilbert models, weighed in accord with the results for synthetic images.
Selectivities of biological complex cells to natural images
were estimated by performing a linear interpolation between the Gabor and Hilbert models, weighed by the
synthetic image results. The estimated selectivities are
given within brackets in Table 1. The complex cell kurtosis estimated here for natural images, 2.8, is not far
from to the value of 4.1 reported by Vinje and Gallant
(2000, 2002) for macaque complex cells.
3.2. Spatial frequency dependence of selectivity
Selectivities are higher for the Hilbert model than the
Gabor model, regardless of which measure is used
(Table 1), and the question arises why that is. To examine this issue, we start by plotting the response probability distributions separately for model complex cells
tuned to different spatial frequencies. At each spatial frequency, responses for units tuned to all orientations
were pooled, as we did not note interesting orientation
specific effects. This is done for the Gabor model in
Fig. 11A, and the Hilbert model in Fig. 11B, in both
the cases using natural images as inputs.
Examination of Fig. 11 shows that there is a large difference between the models in their response probability
distributions for different spatial frequencies. Response
distributions of the Gabor model are almost independent of spatial frequency tuning, while those of the Hilbert model show distributions whose skewness (and
selectivities) increase sharply for units tuned to higher
spatial frequencies.
A possible explanation for this difference arises when
one examines the spatial frequency amplitude spectra of
the stimuli (Fig. 12), and the spectra of the Gabor and
Hilbert model subunits (Fig. 5). The image amplitude
spectra (both natural and synthetic) exhibit a 1/f frequency dependence, as has been widely reported (for
example, Baddeley, 1996a; Field, 1987; Ruderman &
Bialek, 1994). The stimulus amplitudes at low spatial
frequencies are enormous compared to those at high frequencies. Although it is the phase spectrum and the
alignment of phases to produce localized forms that lead
to the important structures in natural images, the amplitude spectrum can be used as an index indicating stimulus intensity at different spatial scales, when averaging
over a large set of image samples.
Continuing development of the argument here, the
Gabor model includes subunits with significant sensitivity to low spatial frequencies, including zero frequency,
66
S.R. Lehky et al. / Vision Research 45 (2005) 57–73
(B)
Probability density
(A)
50
Gabor
0.031 cycles/pixel
S E = 0.39
Hilbert
0.031 cycles/pixel
S E = 0.10
40
S K = 4.05
S K = 1.93
30
20
10
Probability density
50
0.062 cycles/pixel
S E = 0.09
Gabor
40
0.062 cycles/pixel
Hilbert
S E = 0.48
S K = 2.99
30
S K = 6.55
20
10
Probability density
50
0.125 cycles/pixel
S E = 0.07
Gabor
40
0.125 cycles/pixel
Hilbert
SE = 0.58
S K = 7.99
S K = 2.77
30
20
10
Probability density
50
0.250 cycles/pixel
Gabor
40
30
0.250 cycles/pixel
Hilbert
SE = 0.06
SE = 0.69
S K = 2.29
S K = 10.1
20
10
0
0.1
0.2
Response
0.3
0
0.1
0.2
0.3
Response
Fig. 11. Response distributions of model complex cells with different spatial frequency tunings, when presented with natural images. Peak
frequencies of the spatial tuning curves are indicated, as are the selectivity measures. These results show that the selectivities of Gabor units are
independent of spatial frequency tuning, while selectivities of Hilbert units increase for high frequency units. Response is on a relative scale with the
optimal stimulus producing a value of 1.0. (A) Gabor model. (B) Hilbert model.
and this low frequency sensitivity is largely independent
of the position of the peak. Given a 1/f input stimulus,
the response distributions of such units will be dominated by the strong, non-specific signal at the left tail
of their frequency tuning, rather than the weak signal
at their peak. We therefore see Gabor model responses
in Fig. 11A that are independent of the tuning curve
peak, and with relatively low selectivity.
The Hilbert model, on the other hand, incorporates
subunits that have reduced sensitivities to low spatial
frequencies and are completely insensitive to zero fre-
quency (Fig. 5). Without sensitivity to the strong signal
at frequencies near zero, Hilbert response distributions
are more dependent on the peak location of their spatial
frequency tuning curves than Gabor distributions
are. As spatial frequency tuning increases, localized
structures at those spatial scales become increasingly
rare, and responses of Hilbert units show increases
selectivity.
As actual complex units have properties intermediate
between the Gabor and Hilbert models (Table 1), we
predict that their selectivities will show moderately spa-
S.R. Lehky et al. / Vision Research 45 (2005) 57–73
0
(A)
0.0
natural images
synthetic images
Log probability
Log Amplitude
67
-2.0
-4.0
-6.0
-4
-8
-12
0
0.1
0.2
0.3
0.4
0.5
Spatial Freq. (cycles/pixel)
0
0.1
0.2
0.3
Response
(B)
Fig. 13. Tail of the response distribution of a high spatial frequency
Hilbert unit. Linearity of the semi-log plot indicates that the tail
follows an exponential decay. Dashed line shows linear best fit.
Amplitude
0.06
0.04
0.02
0
10
20 30 40 50
1/ f (pixels/cycle)
60
Fig. 12. Spatial frequency spectra of the stimulus images. (A)
Amplitude spectra of synthetic and natural stimulus images, averaged
over all images in each set. The four vertical lines at the bottom
indicate the spatial tuning peaks of the four sets of model units whose
response distributions are graphed in Fig. 11. (B) Amplitude spectra
plotted as a function of inverse frequency, showing that the stimulus
images had approximately 1/f spectra.
tial frequency dependence, in between that which appears in Fig. 11A and B.
3.3. Response distribution tails
Probability distributions associated with high selectivity or sparseness are often described as being ‘‘heavy-tailed’’, so we shall examine tail properties here.
Events falling on the tails are by definition rare, so it
is difficult to characterize them with the small samples
typically available from the neurophysiological data.
However with a model, such as we developed for complex cells, this limitation is bypassed. Heavy-tailed distributions have been used to model a variety of
systems in economics, communications engineering,
and physics (Adler, Feldman, & Taqqu, 1998), following
the seminal work of Mandelbrot (1963), for situations in
which there is an occasional large extremal event mixed
in with the usual small events.
The tail of a distribution is defined as the complement
of the cumulative distribution function F r ðrÞ ¼ 1 F ðrÞ
(Bryson, 1983). A ‘‘heavy-tail’’ is one that decreases
more slowly than some reference distribution. An exponentially decaying tail is commonly used as the dividing
line between light-tailed and heavy-tailed distributions.
This would classify the Gaussian distribution (whose tail
is the square of an exponential) as light-tailed, and distributions with power law tails (the Cauchy distribution
for example) as heavy-tailed because they decay much
more slowly.
We examined the tail of the response distribution
showing the highest selectivity, that coming from a high
spatial frequency Hilbert model complex cell (Fig. 11B,
bottom panel). A semi-log plot of its right tail yields a
straight line (Fig. 13), indicating that it does not follow
a power law but rather is exponentially decaying. This is
consistent with previous reports of exponential tails for
striate simple cells and inferotemporal units (Baddeley
et al., 1997; Treves, Panzeri, Rolls, Booth, & Wakeman,
1999). The response distribution of the model complex
cell is therefore not heavy-tailed, despite being leptokurtotic. Although high kurtosis arises when a distribution
is heavy-tailed, it can also arise if the distribution is thintailed and skewed. Skewness and not heavy-tailedness
appears to be the source of the high measures of selectivity seen in complex cells, judging from these modeling
results.
3.4. Ergodicity: the relationship between selectivity
and sparseness
Another issue is the relationship between the distribution of responses of single units across time when presented with a set of images (selectivity) and the
distribution of responses within a neural population
measured simultaneously (sparseness). If neural responses are presented on a matrix in which each column
represents a different neuron and each row represents a
different stimulus image, the question is how do response distributions compare if they are measured along
columns (selectivity) or along rows (sparseness). Why be
concerned with this issue? As a technical matter, its far
easier to isolate units one at a time while presenting each
S.R. Lehky et al. / Vision Research 45 (2005) 57–73
(A)
50
Gabor selectivity
Probability density
one with many stimuli, rather than record an entire population simultaneously, even though the population
sparseness may be the quantity of theoretical interest.
Therefore it is useful to understand the relationship between selectivity and sparseness. For convenience in the
following discussion, we shall use the terms ‘‘selectivity’’
and ‘‘sparseness’’ to refer to the response distributions
themselves, and not just summary statistics on those distributions such as entropy or kurtosis.
If the selectivities for individual units are the same as
the population sparseness, we call the neural system ergodic, by analogy with the concept from statistical
mechanics. To refine this a bit more, if the selectivity
of each individual unit is the same as average population
sparseness, then the system is strongly ergodic. Obviously under strong ergodicity all units have the same
selectivity. It should be noted that units can respond
to quite different sets of stimuli and yet still have identical selectivities (response probability distributions), as
we are defining the term. If individual units have different selectivities, but the average selectivity is the same as
the average population sparseness, then the system is
weakly ergodic. Weak ergodicity necessarily occurs if responses of units are uncorrelated.
When examining ergodicity in simulations here, the
population size of model units was expanded from the
previous 24 to 126 by increasing the number of different
spatial frequency and orientation tunings included in the
population. Fig. 14 shows response probability distributions for Gabor model complex cells plotted both ways,
by individual units (selectivity) and by population
(sparseness). Fig. 15 shows selectivity and sparseness
for Hilbert model units.
The top panel of Fig. 14 shows response distributions
for individual units (selectivities). The distributions are
displayed with histograms having 10 bars. Each color
represents the responses of a different unit. By following
a color across the histogram, one can see the response
distribution of a particular unit when presented with
the entire stimulus set. Low spatial frequency units are
at the blue end of the spectrum, and high frequency
units are at the red end. The black outline around each
bar indicates the average distribution over all units.
The bottom panel shows response distributions
across the population (sparseness) for individual stimuli.
The population always remains the same, and each color
represents the response distribution of that population
to a different stimulus image. The black outline bars
show the average distribution of responses for the population over the stimulus set (average sparseness). There
is high variability for the different colors within each histogram bar, as the response distribution of the population jumps about with each individual image.
The black outline bars in the two panels are practically identical, which indicates that the average selectivity of individual units is the same as the average
40
30
20
10
0
0
0.05
0.10
0.15
Response
0.20
(B)
50
Gabor sparseness
Probability density
68
40
30
20
10
0
0
0.05
0.10
0.15
Response
0.20
Fig. 14. Comparison of selectivity and sparseness in Gabor model
units. Responses are from a population of 126 model units with
various orientation and spatial frequency tunings, when presented with
natural images. (A) Histogram with 10 bars showing response
distribution of individual units presented with a series of images
(selectivity). Each color indicates a different unit, and the black outline
bars show the average distribution over all units. (B) Histogram
showing distribution of responses within an entire population measured simultaneously (sparseness). Each color corresponds to a different
stimulus image presented to the population. The black outline bars
show the average distribution of the population over the entire set of
stimulus images. Response is on a relative scale with the optimal
stimulus producing a value of 1.0. (The crowding of colored bars
causes them to bleed into white spaces between them, making the solid
colored areas appear taller than they actually are.)
sparseness of the population. That satisfies the condition
for the system to be weakly ergodic. Looking more closely at the response distributions for the individual
Gabor complex cells in Fig. 14A, they all appear to be
very similar regardless of spatial frequency tuning. That
is indicated by the flatness within each histogram bar of
all the different colors. Therefore, the selectivity of each
individual unit matches the average sparseness of the
population. Thus, Gabor model complex cells have response distributions that are strongly ergodic.
Moving from Gabor units to Hilbert units, Fig. 15 is
analogous to Fig. 14. The near identity of the black outline histogram bars in the top and bottom panels of Fig.
15 show that Hilbert units satisfy weak ergodicity, as did
Gabor units. In other words, average selectivity is the
same as average sparseness. However, unlike Gabor
S.R. Lehky et al. / Vision Research 45 (2005) 57–73
(A)
50
Probability density
Hilbert selectivity
40
30
20
10
0
0
0.05
0.10
0.15
Response
0.20
(B)
69
sparseness (response distributions across all units in a
population measured simultaneously), which we call
ergodicity, was shown in simulations of model cells
(Fig. 14 and 15). This model prediction of ergodicity remains to be confirmed by experimental studies.
If one accepts the existence of ergodic equivalence,
then the selectivity measures of individual units in Table
1 can also be considered as average sparseness measures
of the population as a whole. That is, populations of
striate complex cells would be expected to show a moderate degree of elevated sparseness relative to a Gaussian reference distribution. However, we shall argue
below that such sparseness would not necessarily indicate efficient coding.
50
Probability density
Hilbert sparseness
4.1. Comparison with previous models
40
30
20
10
0
0
0.05
0.10
0.15
Response
0.20
Fig. 15. Comparison of selectivity and sparseness in Hilbert model
units. Histograms show responses for (A) individual units across time
(selectivity) and (B) across the population simultaneously (sparseness).
This is analogous to Fig. 14.
units, Hilbert units each have different response distributions as a function of spatial frequency, indicated by the
fact that histogram bars in Fig. 15A are not flat for different colors. Therefore, Hilbert units are not strongly
ergodic.
Actual complex cells are expected to have properties
intermediate between the Gabor and Hilbert models
(again referring to Table 1). Therefore, we predict they
will be weakly ergodic but not strongly ergodic. This
means that the average selectivity of complex cells will
be equal to average sparseness across the population,
but that the selectivity of each individual complex cell
will not match average sparseness.
4. Discussion
Non-parametric selectivity measures were calculated
from both striate complex cell data and model complex
cells. These indicated average selectivities of individual
units that were moderately greater than that of a Gaussian reference distribution (Table 1).
The numerical equivalence between selectivity (response distributions of individual units across time when
presented with a sequence of stimulus images) and
Our simulations indicating the equivalence of selectivity and sparseness are in complete disagreement with
the simulations of Willmore and Tolhurst (2001), who
found no relation between the two. It is not clear why
this difference exists, particularly as there are conditions
under which selectivity and sparseness are mathematically required to be identical, as Földiak (2002) has
pointed out. One difference between our methods is that
they measured selectivity/sparseness indices for each
individual response distribution and then averaged the
indices, whereas in Figs. 14 and 15 we are averaging
the probability distributions on which sparseness measurements are based rather than averaging the sparseness
measurements themselves.
Three factors were found to influence non-parametric
selectivity in our models of complex cells. The first is
whether the linear subunits integrate exactly to zero or
not (or equivalently, whether they have a DC response
or not). Complex cells with subunits that integrate to
zero have higher selectivity. The second is location of
the peaks of spatial frequency tuning curves of the complex cells. Units tuned to higher spatial frequencies are
more selective, again provided receptive fields integrate
to zero. The third is average response of the complex
cells over the entire stimulus set. Units with low average
responses are more skewed and produce higher selectivity measures.
Baddeley (1996a) has previously reported, from simulations, that linear, circularly-symmetric units with
inhibitory surround, similar to retinal ganglion cells,
produce higher selectivity if the receptive fields integrate
to zero. We can confirm that this property still holds
true for oriented, non-linear, phase-independent complex cells. However, we have offered a different explanation for the origin of this effect. Baddeley (1996a)
ascribed it to non-stationarity in the image statistics,
whereas our explanation focuses on the 1/f nature of
the image spatial frequency spectrum (in conjunction
with alignments in its phase spectrum). Baddeley (1996a)
70
S.R. Lehky et al. / Vision Research 45 (2005) 57–73
also reported that for his circular linear units, higher
spatial frequency tuning led to higher selectivity, which
we also see in complex cells. Again, however, the explanations for these effects differ, with Baddeley ascribing it
to image non-stationarity while we offer an explanation
in terms of the 1/f image spectrum.
Although the entropy or kurtosis values we observed
in model complex cells indicates a moderate degree of
sparseness, the modeling indicates that sparseness measures are very sensitive to slight changes in subunit receptive field profiles, with higher sparseness for receptive
fields that integrate to zero. We know from data in the literature (Spitzer & Hochstein, 1988) that receptive fields
do not integrate to zero, based on observations of a residual ripple in complex cell responses to drifting gratings
which indicate imperfect phase invariance. This suggests
that evolutionary pressure to create very high sparseness
in these units is weak, or constrained by other objectives.
Furthermore, the exponential tails seen in the response distributions of model complex cells (Fig. 13)
do not lead to high sparseness values compared to other
possible shapes, such as a power law tail. Exponential
tails have also been noted in V1 simple cells and inferotemporal cells (Baddeley et al., 1997; Treves et al., 1999),
indicating that this property is commonplace and perhaps ubiquitous in visual cells. On the other hand,
power law tails, leading to much higher sparseness, have
never been reported. Rather than being associated with
high sparseness and information efficiency, exponential
tails can be associated with energy (metabolic) efficiency (Baddeley, 1996b; Baddeley et al., 1997), as they
maximize output entropy for a fixed firing rate. See
Laughlin and Sejnowski (2003) for a more general discussion of metabolic efficiency as a constraint in brain
organization.
The fact that minor changes in the receptive field
organization underlying complex cells can lead to large
changes in sparseness/selectivity raises the possibility
that those parameters may be under dynamic control.
For example, it is possible that attention can cause slight
adjustments in the receptive field structure leading to a
change in selectivity, although such an effect has not
been reported.
4.2. Comparisons with previous experimental data
Vinje and Gallant (2000, 2002) have previously reported data on sparseness in macaque striate complex
cells. On our terminology, they were actually measuring
non-parametric selectivity rather than sparseness. However, by the ergodicity principle that our modeling indicates, selectivity is equivalent to sparseness. Our data
leads to similar estimates of selectivity in complex cells
to those of Vinje and Gallant.
Vinje and Gallant (2000, 2002) further reported that
selectivity in macaque striate complex cells increases
when the stimulus diameter is expanded to include the
non-classical receptive field surround. Based on the present study we can identify two possible mechanisms that
may underlie this effect. The first relates to the negative
correlation between selectivity and the average activity
level (Fig. 9). The observed increase in selectivity for
broad stimuli may simply be secondary to non-specific
inhibition from the non-classical surround, which would
reduce the average activity level of the unit. We know
from Gallant, Connor, and Van Essen (1998) that the
non-classical surround does have an inhibitory effect
on activity in these units. The second possibility is that
the surround affects the receptive field profiles of the
complex cell subunits so that they more closely integrate
to zero. As we have seen, changing the receptive fields in
this manner greatly increases selectivity.
The first mechanism, lateral inhibition, is so non-specific and ubiquitous that increased sparseness resulting
from it could easily be an epiphenomenal side effect
rather than a deliberate means of increasing coding efficiency. On the other hand, if the second mechanism, fine
tuning of receptive field profiles, were shown to occur,
that could more convincingly be interpreted as a purpose-built mechanism for increasing efficiency.
A fundamental issue in which we would disagree with
Vinje and Gallant is the idea that high sparseness measures calculated from data are necessarily an indicator of
statistically efficient coding in the system. Baddeley
(1996a) and Treves et al. (1999) have already established
that high sparseness measures can arise for reasons
unrelated to coding efficiency, and this is a point we
would like to expand on.
On one explanatory level, excess sparseness in complex cells arises because they have response distributions
that are skewed (but not heavy-tailed). The skewness in
turn arises from the squaring non-linearity by which
subunit responses of complex cells are combined (Eq.
(12)). (To give a simple example, if a set of normally distributed random numbers are squared, the resulting
probability density function will be skewed and exhibit
large excess kurtosis.) Thus, we see high sparseness
measures arising from non-linear distortions in the neural response distributions, rather than the linear transforms that underlie information theoretic explanations
of the origins of sparseness (Field, 1994). It is not selfevident that sparseness measures arising from such
non-linear probability distortions need to be interpreted
in terms of efficient coding. Rather, the non-linearities
seen in complex cells could be involved in the implementation of image processing algorithms, and unrelated to
issues in statistical efficiency.
4.3. Implications for the efficient coding hypothesis
It may be objected that perhaps the non-linear distortions are a means of implementing the high sparseness
S.R. Lehky et al. / Vision Research 45 (2005) 57–73
required for efficient coding. However, a feature of the
squaring non-linearity underlying complex cells is that
it is an information-losing transform. Information theory deals with the reliability and efficiency of information transmission in the presence of noise, and the
significance of any deterministic information-losing
transforms within the system is orthogonal to the concerns of the theory, other than as arbitrary external constraints that an information theoretic analysis must deal
with. Information theory will not predict nor explain the
nature of information-losing transforms within the visual system, for example why complex cells implement
a particular non-linearity and not some other. Yet it
seems likely that understanding the functional role of
information-losing transforms will prove central to
understanding vision, as the organism creates representations that highlight aspects of the environment that
are of ecological significance.
Simoncelli and Olshausen (2001) appear to recognize
this problem, presenting a weakened form of the efficient
coding hypothesis in which information is not preserved.
Efficient coding is in that case only relative to whatever
information is spared at each stage. ‘‘The hypothesis
states only that information must be represented efficiently; it does not say what information should be represented. . .’’, as they say. Obviously, identifying what
information is being represented must come prior to
determining if that information is efficiently represented.
This reinforces the point we are making, that it is
premature to interpret high sparseness measures calculated from complex cell data as indicative of efficient
coding, as Vinje and Gallant do, without understanding the requirements of the visual algorithms being
implemented.
If one accepts that high sparseness measures can occur for reasons unrelated to information efficient coding,
it still remains possible to retain some suggested benefits
of sparse coding without invoking information-theoretic
optimality arguments for its origin. For example, the
ability of sparse codes to increase the storage capacity
of associative memory under some models (Baum, Moody, & Wilzek, 1988; Palm, 1980; Treves & Rolls, 1991)
remains whether the sparseness arises from information-efficient transforms or from information-losing system non-linearities. Sparseness also has value in
reducing energy costs regardless of the other benefits.
In more general terms, the criticism here of visual
models that center on efficient coding and redundancy
reduction is that they attempt to explain properties of
receptive fields purely in terms of the statistical properties of the input stimulus, without considering the goals
of the organism for which the visual apparatus was constructed. To understand why V1 has particular receptive
fields, it may be necessary to look not only at the structure of the stimulus, but also at the structure of the higher visual areas into which V1 feeds, analyze what these
71
higher areas are trying to accomplish, and determine
what kind of inputs best serve those ends (Lehky & Sejnowski, 1999). Including considerations of these higher
areas may produce visual representations that not only
reflect image statistics but also incorporate the requirements for visuomotor coordination and other behaviors
the organism needs in order to survive.
The idea that a theory of sensory processing can be
developed purely by examining the internal structure
of the stimuli without any reference to the organism as
an integrated sensorimotor system has been criticized
in particular by those espousing an ‘‘embodied’’ viewpoint (for example, see Churchland, Ramachandran, &
Sejnowski, 1994; Clark, 1997; Lakoff, 1987; MerleauPonty, 1945; Varela, Thompson, & Rosch, 1991; Winograd & Flores, 1987). Perhaps a broader way of saying
the same thing is that sparse coding is motivated by issues in information theory, and as with all information
theoretic models it is fundamentally concerned with
the syntax of the signal rather than semantics or ‘‘meaning’’. Ultimately it may not be possible to have a satisfactory theory of the brain without confronting the
constellation of issues relating to meaning (of which
the problem of categorization is a part). The basic point
here is that by focusing on issues arising from information theory (such as sparse coding), one is led to ask fundamentally the wrong kinds of questions concerning
visual processing at higher levels.
It is important to emphasize that what is being challenged here is the use of information theoretic optimality
principles as a core explanation of why sensory systems
are structured the way they are, and not the application
of information theory as a tool for data analysis per se.
Information theoretic analyses of data (for example,
Optican & Richmond, 1987; Rolls, 2003) can lead to
interesting insights on sensory processing without making the claim that the system is optimized along information theoretic principles.
In view of the above criticisms, explanations of receptive field structure in terms of information theoretic
measures of efficient coding are most convincing when
confined to peripheral parts of sensory pathways, such
as the retina and lateral geniculate nucleus, which
involve linear signal transforms and reduced contamination by cognitive (non-stimulus) feedback. The presence
of information-losing non-linearities at higher levels,
such as in the complex cells studied here, indicate other
factors in addition to information theory that should be
taken into account in order to understand receptive
fields.
Acknowledgments
The experimental phase of this project was supported
by the McDonnell-Pew Foundation while SRL was at
72
S.R. Lehky et al. / Vision Research 45 (2005) 57–73
NIMH. SRL thanks Dr. Keiji Tanaka for his support
during the modeling phase of the project at RIKEN.
References
Adelson, E. H., & Bergen, J. R. (1985). Spatiotemporal energy models
for the perception of motion. Journal of the Optical Society of
America, A, 2, 284–299.
Adler, R. J., Feldman, R., & Taqqu, M. S. (Eds.). (1998). A practical
guide to heavy tails: statistical techniques and applications. Boston:
Birkhaüser.
Atick, J. J. (1992). Could information theory provide an ecological
theory of sensory processing? Nature, 3, 213–251.
Baddeley, R. (1996a). Searching for filters with ‘‘interesting’’ output
distributions: an uninteresting direction to explore? Network, 7,
409–421.
Baddeley, R. (1996b). An efficient code in V1? Nature, 381, 560–561.
Baddeley, R., Abbot, L. F., Booth, M. C. A., Sengpiel, F., Freeman,
T., Wakeman, E. A., & Rolls, E. T. (1997). Responses of neurons in
primary and inferior cortices to natural scenes. Proceedings of the
Royal Society of London, B, 264, 1775–1783.
Barlow, H. B., Kaushal, T. P., & Mitchison, G. J. (1989). Finding
minimum entropy codes. Neural Computation, 1, 412–423.
Baum, E. B., Moody, J., & Wilzek, F. (1988). Internal representations
for associative memory. Biological Cybernetics, 59, 217–228.
Bell, A. J., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1997). The ‘‘independent components’’
of natural scenes are edge filters. Vision Research, 37, 3327–3338.
Bryson, M. C. (1983). Heavy-tailed distributions. In S. Kotz, C. B.
Read, & N. L. Johnson (Eds.). Encyclopedia of statistical sciences
(vol. 3, pp. 598–601). New York: John Wiley.
Churchland, P. S., Ramachandran, V. S., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1994). A
critique of pure vision. In C. Koch & J. Davis (Eds.), Large-scale
neuronal theories of the brain (pp. 23–60). Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
Clark, A. (1997). Being there: putting brain, body and world together
again. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
De Valois, R. L., Albrecht, D. G., & Thorell, L. G. (1982). Spatial
frequency selectivity of cells in macaque visual cortex. Vision
Research, 22, 545–559.
Emerson, R. C., Korenberg, M. J., & Citron, M. C. (1992).
Identification of complex-cell intensive nonlinearities in a cascade
model of cat visual cortex. Biological Cybernetics, 66, 291–300.
Field, D. J. (1987). Relations between the statistics of natural images
and the response properties of cortical cells. Journal of the Optical
Society of America, A, 4, 2379–2394.
Field, D. J. (1994). What is the goal of sensory coding? Neural
Computation, 6, 559–601.
Field, D. J. (1999). Wavelets, vision, and the statistics of natural
scenes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London
A, 357, 2527–2542.
Földiak, P. (2002). Sparse coding in the primate cortex. In M. A. Arbib
(Ed.), The handbook of brain theory and neural networks (2nd ed.,
pp. 895–898). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gallant, J. L., Connor, C. E., & Van Essen, D. C. (1998). Neural
activity in areas V1, V2, and V4 during free viewing of natural
stimuli compared to controlled viewing. NeuroReport, 9,
2153–2158.
Heeger, D. J. (1992). Half-squaring in responses of cat striate cells.
Visual Neuroscience, 9, 427–443.
Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1968). Receptive fields and functional
architecture of the monkey striate cortex. Journal of Physiology
(London), 195, 215–243.
Jones, J. P., & Palmer, L. A. (1987). An evaluation of the twodimensional Gabor filter model of simple receptive fields in cat
striate cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 58, 1233–1258.
Karklin, Y., & Lewicki, M. S. (2003). Learning higher order structure
in natural images. Network, 14, 483–499.
Kulikowski, J. J., & Vidyasagar, T. R. (1986). Space and spatial
frequency: analysis and representation in the macaque striate
cortex. Experimental Brain Research, 64, 5–18.
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things: what categories
reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Laughlin, S. B., & Sejnowski, T. J. (2003). Communication in neuronal
networks. Science, 301, 1870–1874.
Lehky, S. R., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1999). Seeing white: qualia in the
context of decoding population codes. Neural Computation, 11,
1261–1280.
Lehky, S. R., Sejnowski, T. J., & Desimone, R. (1992). Predicting
responses of nonlinear neurons in monkey striate cortex to complex
patterns. Journal of Neuroscience, 12, 3568–3581.
Mandelbrot, B. B. (1963). The variation of certain speculative prices.
Journal of Business, 36, 394–419.
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1945). Phenomenologie de la perception (translated
1962). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Morrone, M. C., & Burr, D. C. (1988). Feature energy in human
vision: a phase dependent energy model. Proceedings of the Royal
Society of London, B, 235, 221–245.
Ohzawa, I., DeAngelis, G. C., & Freeman, R. D. (1990). Stereoscopic
depth discrimination in the visual cortex: neurons ideally suited as
disparity detectors. Science, 249, 1037–1041.
Olshausen, B. A., & Field, D. J. (1996). Emergence of simple-cell
receptive field properties by learning a sparse code for natural
images. Nature, 381, 607–609.
Olshausen, B. A., & Field, D. J. (1997). Sparse coding with an
overcomplete basis set: a strategy employed by V1? Vision
Research, 37, 3311–3325.
Optican, L. M., & Richmond, B. J. (1987). Temporal encoding of twodimensional patterns by single units in primate inferior temporal
cortex. III. Information theoretic analysis. Journal of Neurophysiology, 57, 162–178.
Palm, G. (1980). On associative memory. Biological Cybernetics, 36,
19–31.
Pollen, D. A., & Ronner, S. (1983). Visual cortical neurons as localized
spatial frequency filters. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and
Cybernetics, 13, 907–916.
Rieke, F., Warland, D., van Steveninck, R. D. R., & Bialek, W. (1997).
Spikes: exploring the neural code. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Rolls, E. T., & Tovee, M. J. (1995). Sparseness of the neuronal
representation of stimuli in the primate temporal cortex. Journal of
Neurophysiology, 73, 713–726.
Rolls, E. T. (2003). An information theoretic approach to the
contribution of the firing rates and the correlations between firing
rates. Journal of Neurophysiology, 89, 2810–2822.
Ruderman, D. L., & Bialek, W. (1994). Statistics of natural images:
scaling in the woods. Physical Review Letters, 73, 814–817.
Schiller, P. H., Finlay, B. L., & Volman, S. F. (1976). Quantitative
studies of single-cell properties in monkey striate cortex. I.
Spatiotemporal organization of receptive fields. Journal of Neurophysiology, 39, 1288–1319.
Silverman, B. W. (1986). Density estimation for statistics and data
analysis. Monographs on statistics and applied probability 26.
London: Chapman & Hall.
Simoncelli, E. P. (2003). Vision and statistics of the visual environment. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13, 144–149.
Simoncelli, E. P., & Olshausen, B. A. (2001). Natural image statistics
and neural representation. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24,
1193–1216.
Spitzer, H., & Hochstein, S. (1988). Complex-cell receptive field
models. Progress in Neurobiology, 31, 285–309.
Szulborski, R. G., & Palmer, L. A. (1990). The two-dimensional spatial
structure of nonlinear subunits in the receptive fields of complex
cells. Vision Research, 30, 249–254.
S.R. Lehky et al. / Vision Research 45 (2005) 57–73
Treves, A., Panzeri, S., Rolls, E. T., Booth, M., & Wakeman, E. A.
(1999). Firing rate distributions and efficiency of information
transmission of inferior temporal neurons to natural stimuli.
Neural Computation, 11, 601–632.
Treves, A., & Rolls, E. T. (1991). What determines the capacity of
autoassociative memories in the brain? Network, 2, 371–397.
van Hateren, J. H., & van der Schaaf, A. (1998). Independent
component filters of natural images compared with simple cells in
primary visual cortex. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London,
B, 265, 359–366.
Varela, F., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
73
Vinje, W. E., & Gallant, J. L. (2000). Sparse coding and decorrelation
in primary visual cortex during natural vision. Science, 287,
1273–1276.
Vinje, W. E., & Gallant, J. L. (2002). Natural stimulation of
the
nonclassical
receptive
field
increases
information
transmission efficiency in V1. Journal of Neuroscience, 22,
2904–2915.
Willmore, B., & Tolhurst, D. J. (2001). Characterizing the sparseness
of neural codes. Network, 12, 255–270.
Winograd, T., & Flores, F. (1987). Understanding computers and
cognition: a new foundation for design. Reading, MA: Addison
Wesley.