* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download The pattern of ocular dominance columns in macaque visual cortex
Emotional lateralization wikipedia , lookup
Time perception wikipedia , lookup
Neuroplasticity wikipedia , lookup
Aging brain wikipedia , lookup
Human brain wikipedia , lookup
Neuroeconomics wikipedia , lookup
Neuroesthetics wikipedia , lookup
Microneurography wikipedia , lookup
Neural correlates of consciousness wikipedia , lookup
C1 and P1 (neuroscience) wikipedia , lookup
Cortical cooling wikipedia , lookup
Eyeblink conditioning wikipedia , lookup
Inferior temporal gyrus wikipedia , lookup
Hierarchical temporal memory wikipedia , lookup
The Pattern of Ocular Dominance Columns in Macaque Visual Cortex Revealed by a Reduced Silver Stain SIMON LEVAY, DAVID H. HUBEL AND TORSTEN N . WIESEL Department of Nezc?obiology, Hn,-vu?d Medicrrl School, 25 Shattitck Strecrt, Boston, Mnsscichicsetts 021 1 5 ABSTRACT A pattern of alternating dark and pale bands was observed in the striate cortex of the macaque monkey. The bands, which ran parallel to the surface, were seen in tangential sections stained with a reduced silver method for normal fibers and were most clear in layer 4C a, immediately deep to the line of Gennari. The dark bands were about 300 F wide and showed blind endings and bifurcations. The light bands were about 50 p wide and did not branch or terminate within area 17. Because the dark bands were similar in width to the bands of terminal degeneration which have been shown to result from single-layer lesions of the lateral geniculate body, i t seemed possible that they corresponded to ocular dominance columns. To test this idea, the boundaries of ocular dominance columns were marked in a physiological experiment: tangential electrode penetrations were made in an anesthetized monkey and, a s the electrode was advanced horizontally in the fourth layer, the eye preference of single units and of the background activity was monitored. Small electrolytic lesions were placed at the points where a change in eye preference occurred. The brain was subsequently fixed, sectioned tangentially and stained with the silver method. All the lesions- a total of 12 -fell directly on the pale bands. Moreover, the electrode had not passed over any pale bands without a lesion being placed. It was concluded that the dark bands do correspond to single ocular dominance columns and the pale bands to the boundaries between columns. The banding appearance i s due to a greater density of tangential fibers within columns than at the borders of columns. These tangential fibers are in part the preterminal arborizations of geniculocortical axons, since some of them have been shown to degenerate after geniculate lesions. The ocular dominance columns were mapped for most of the striate cortex, using serial tangential sections stained with the silver method. The overall pattern was similar in several monkeys, though the details of the branching arrangements varied from animal to animal. The columns met the 17-18 border at right angles. On the outer surface of the hemisphere the columns converged from the 17-18 border, turned medially with repeated fusions of columns, and streamed over the lip of the calcarine fissure. In the roof of the fissure they met a second system of columns oriented parasagittally. In terms of the visual field, the columns ran roughly horizontally for the central 10" of the field, and circumferentially beyond that. The columns were not mapped in the stem of the fissure, the area corresponding to the far periphery of the field. The constancy of column width across the cortex probably allows a functional matching between ocular-dominance and orientation columns. The monkey's striate cortex is subdivided into two independent sets of columns, one for eye preference and one for preferred stimulus orientation, and these columnar systems are superimposed on a n orderly projection of the visual field (Hubel and Wiesel, '68, '74a). The ocular dominance J. COMP. NEUR.,159: 559-576. columns are generated by a spatial segregation of the afferents from the ipsilaterally and contralaterally innervated layers of the lateral geniculate nucleus. By placing lesions in single layers of the lateral geniculate nucleus Hubel and Wiesel ('72) were able to show that the afferent terminals in 559 560 SIMON L E V A Y DAVID H . H U B E L A N D TORSTEN N. WIESEL layer 4 were arranged in parallel and alternating left and right eye bands. This method of demonstrating the columns was limited by the necessity of making the geniculate lesions small enough to be entirely restricted to a single layer, so that the area of cortex over which the columns could be reconstructed was also rather small. The study therefore left two intriguing questions unanswered: did the columns form a pattern that was constant from animal to animal, and, if so, what relationship did this pattern have to the visual field representation? During the course of a n investigation intended to reveal the anatomical basis of orientation columns, we recently noticed a pattern of parallel bands in the fourth layer of the cortex. The bands were seen in tangential sections stained with a reduced silver method for normal fibers. In this paper we describe this banding pattern and show that it corresponds to the ocular dominance system, as defined physiologically. By making reconstructions from serial sections, we show that the ocular dominance columnar system does form a constant overall pattern, with a definite relationship to the visual field map. METHODS Six macaque monkeys (five Rhesus monkeys, Macaca mulatta, and one crab-eating macaque, M. fascicularis) were used in the present study. Two of the animals were normal young adults; the other four had a surgically-induced strabismus. All four strabismic monkeys were shown by behavioral testing to have normal visual acuity in both eyes, but lacked binocular fusion (work to be published separately). At the termination of physiological experiments, most of which were not relevant to the present study, the animals were perfused with 10% formol saline, and blocks of striate cortex, or the whole occipital lobe, were cut in serial frozen sections oriented tangentially to the pial surface. To demonstrate the banding pattern in the fourth layer, sections were stained with a slight modification of Liesegang's reduced silver method (Jones, 'SO), itself a variation of one of Cajal's silver methods. The sections were washed, left overnight in 50% alcohol containing sixteen drops of concentrated ammonium hydroxide per 100 ml, washed again, and silvered in a 0.75% silver ni- trate solution at room temperature for six hours. Without washing, the sections were reduced in the following mixture: 50r; w/v acacia powder in water: 0.5C; hydroquinone: l o 1 , silver nitrate: 80 ml 40 nil 3 ml The reduction was allowed to proceed, in subdued light, until the white matter was completely black (20-30 min). The sections were then rinsed and mounted on albumenized slides. This method gives an intense staining of large and medium sized axons, and leaves all but the largest cell bodies unstained. In the experiments designed to test the relationship between the banding pattern and the ocular-dominance system, tangential electrode penetrations were made in the striate cortex of anesthetized, paralyzed monkeys, using recording techniques previously described (Hubel and Wiesel, '74a). We used electrodes with uninsulated tips of 25-35 p , which allowed a continuous monitoring of unit activity as well as the unresolved background activity characteristic of the fourth layer. When the electrode tip was within the fourth layer, the electrode was advanced in 20-50 p steps, and the eye preference of the units and of the background activity was noted. At the points where eye preference changes, i.e., at the borders of ocular dominance columns, small electrolytic lesions were placed (1 pA for 1 sec, electrode negative, producing lesions 30-50 p in diameter). A t the conclusion of the experiment the animal was perfused and a block of cortex containing the electrode tracks was sectioned tangentially and stained with the reduced silver method. RESULTS Architecture of the fourth layer in uertical sections The pattern of bands to be described in this paper is visible only in tangential sections of the cortex. To clarify the structures involved in forming this pattern, however, we shall first describe the appearance of the fourth layer in vertical sections stained with the Nissl and reduced silver methods (fig. 1). The nomenclature of the layers follows that used by Lund ('73) and differs slightly from that used by Hubel and Wiesel ('72) in that layer 4C is enlarged at the expense of 4B. Layer 4C is subdivided into two parts. The upper part, layer 4C C I , lies TOPOGRAPHY OF OCULAR D O M I N A N C E C O L U M N S 56 1 3 5 562 SIMON LEVAY, DAVID H . HUBEL AND TORSTEN N. WIESEL immediately under the stria of Gennari. It is moderately rich both in cells and in tangential fibers and receives the afferents from the magnocellular layers of the geniculate (Hubel and Wiesel, '72). Layer 4C p is more densely cellular and contains fewer tangential fibers; it receives the bulk of the afferents from the parvocellular layers of the geniculate. Layer 4B contains a dense tangential fiber plexus, the stria of Gennari, and a few large cells; it receives no direct input from the geniculate. Layer 4A lies immediately above the stria and receives a second, less dense tier of terminals from the parvocellular layers of the geniculate. Besides the tangential fibers, there are also fascicles of vertically-oriented fibers passing through the fourth layer. These fiber bundles (fig. l b ) consist mainly of the descending axons of pyramidal cells in the supragranular layers, with a smaller contribution from the ascending collaterals of more deeply-lying cells and geniculate axons ascending to layer 4A. Tangential sections - the banding in layer 4C Because the surface of the cortex is convex, sections cut tangential to the pial surface show the cortical layers as concentric rings, with the deepest layers in the center and the pial surface at the outside. When such sections were stained with the reduced silver method a pattern of bands could be seen in the fourth layer (fig. 2). This pattern was seen most readily when the slides were viewed at low magnification - for example, by projecting them in a photographic enlarger. The pattern consisted of a system of dark-staining bands, each about 300 p wide, alternating with pale bands that were much thinner, about 50 p wide. The borders between light and dark bands were not at all sharp, so that the apparent width of the light bands varied somewhat with different staining intensities and with different conditions of viewing the sections. The bands were most clearly visible in layer 4C a, just below the line of Gennari, but could usually also be made out in 4C p, and sometimes also in 4A and 5. Identification o f t he dark bands w i t h single ocular dominance columns The fact that the dark bands were the same width as the bands of degeneration resulting from laminar lesions of the geniculate (Hubel and Wiesel, '72) suggested to us that we might be looking at the pattern of ocular dominance columns. This possibility was reinforced by observation of the pattern of branching of the bands. The thin pale bands were never seen to branch, nor did they terminate except at the boundary of area 17. The wide dark bands, on the other hand, often branched or ended blindly. Fusion frequently occurred between every second dark band, but not between immediately adjacent dark bands. This manner of branching stood out clearly when the entire banding pattern was reconstructed (see below), and it indicated to us that there were two sets of dark bands forming two separate, non-communicating systems. In terms of ocular dominance columns, these two systems would correspond to columns for the left and right eye respectively, while the thinner pale bands would represent the boundaries between adjacent columns. We therefore set out to test this idea directly by marking the column boundaries with lesions in a physiological experiment and subsequently comparing the positions of the lesions with the banding pattern in the fixed and stained brain. More or less tangential penetrations were made through the cortex in order to cross as many columns as possible while the electrode remained within the fourth layer. The layer was recognized during the experiment by the prominence of units lacking stimulus orientation specificity, the small size of the receptive fields, the lack of scatter in receptive field position (Hubel et al., '74), the strict monocularity of single units, and the characteristic rich monocularly-driven background activity of many small unresolved units. Four penetrations were made, all in the right hemisphere. The first three were made in a mediolateral direction, a short distance behind the 17-18 border. In each of the first two penetrations the electrode was Fig. 2 Low-power micrograph of a tangential section of striate cortex stained by the reduced silver method. The dark-staining ring is the line of Gennari (layer 4B). In layer 4C, the area within the ring, there are alternating dark (fiber-dense) and light (fiber-poor) bands. In the area marked with a n asterisk, the section grazes layer 5; here the bands are not visible. Bar = 1 mm. TOPOGRAPHY OF OCULAR D O M I N A N C E C O L U M N S Figure 2 563 564 SIMON LEVAY, D A V I D H . H U B E L A N D T O R S T E N N . WIESEL angled rather too steeply, so that only one change in eye preference was encountered while the electrode remained within the fourth layer. Both these transition points were marked with lesions. In the third penetration the angle was better, and within layer 4 five successive changes in eye preference were marked. For the fourth penetration the electrode was turned through 90" and directed posteriorly, near the medial edge of the hemisphere. In this penetration three changes in eye preference occurred at normal intervals, but then there was an unusually long sequence, almost 2 mm in length, during which all cells were driven strongly by the left eye, but with faint background activity from the right eye. A reversal from left to right eye followed, and finally there was a fifth reversal, from right to left, before the electrode left the fourth layer. When the part of the cortex containing the electrode tracks was subsequently examined in serial tangential sections, all twelve lesions were found to lie directly on the thin pale bands. A section from this series is illustrated in figure 3. It contains two of the lesions, those made during the first two penetrations. The lesions were easily identified at high magnification. With dark-field illumination the electrode tracks were also visible. Because the sections were cut tangentially, the reconstruction of the banding pattern for the whole block was a relatively easy task. The sections were projected one at a time in a photographic enlarger, and wherever layer 4 appeared in the section the thin pale bands and the lesions were traced onto a master diagram (fig. 4a). The reconstruction shows, first, that all the lesions were placed on the narrow pale bands (represented a s solid lines in this and subsequent reconstructions). It also shows that in each of the two penetrations in which several consecutive lesions were made no pale bands were passed over without a lesion being placed. The experiment therefore demonstrated that the broad dark bands do correspond to single ocular-dominance columns and that the narrow pale bands are the boundaries between columns. From the physiology, each of the columns, between lesions could be assigned either to the right or to the left eye. The consistency of the assignments across the reconstruc- tion is shown in figure 4b, in which the columns belonging to the left (contralateral) eye have been shaded. It can also be seen that the two sets of columns, corresponding to the right and left eyes, are topologically similar structures: both are elaborately branched, and both have some isolated, blind-ending segments. The areas of the two columnar systems in the reconstruction were measured, and found to be almost equal: the contralateral columns occupied 2 % more area than the ipsilateral. The relationship between the bands and ocular dominance columns has been confirmed in several other animals which were examined either to clarify the organization of the visual field representation in the fourth layer (Hubel et al., '74) or to investigate the effects of visual deprivation on the columns. Structural basis of t h e banding pattern Since the silver method used here stained only nerve fibers, the appearance of pale and dark bands presumably reflected a difference in fiber density. Figure 5 shows, at higher magnification than in the previous illustrations, a single pale band flanked by parts of the two adjacent dark bands. At this magnification the pale band is not at all striking as a discrete entity. One can see, however, that the tangentially oriented fibers are sparser within the pale band than in the dark bands. There is also a suggestion that the vertical bundles of fibers, seen a s dense round clumps in the figure, are somewhat thinner within the pale band. We re-examined the Fink-Heimer preparations of the cortex used in the laminarlesion study (Hubel and Wiesel, '72) to see whether geniculate axons could be contributing to the banding pattern. With lesions of either the magnocellular or the parvocellular layers of the geniculate, one finds many degenerating preterminal axons oriented tangentially in layer 4C (fig. 6). Although some of these degenerating fibers crossed columnar borders (usually running clear across the adjacent column as if to arborize in the next column belonging to the same eye), a much greater number were confined to single columns. We therefore believe that the geniculate axons themselves may make a substantial contribution to the banding pattern visible in reduced silver preparations. But there may also be TOPOGRAPHY OF OCULAR DOMINANCE COLUMNS Fig. 3 Tangential section from the brain in which the boundaries of ocular dominance colu m n s were marked with lesions. Note that in comparison with figure 3 this section i s deeper into the cortex, so t h a t layer 4C, where t h e b a n d s are visible, is a n a n n u l a r area j u s t within the line of Gennari. Most of the center of the field is occupied by layer 5. I n t h e upper part of the field the b a n d s a r e running vertically in the micrograph. The two pale b a n d s indicated with arrows are each marked with a lesion: the lesions are t h e small pale spots about one-half inch from the arrowheads. These two lesions are those made on the first and second penetration of the experiment (compare fig. 4a). T h e m a n y sharply-bordered, dead-white holes are blood vessels; the line a t bottom right is a knife mark, and the large white mark is a tear i n the section. Bar = 1 mm. 565 566 0 E a i L I I I I I Fig. 4a Reconstruction of the banding pattern (solid lines = pale bands), electrode tracks (arrows) and lesions marking the borders of ocular dominance columns (black dots). All twelve lesions, made in four penetrations, coincide with the pale bands. Note also that for much of the fourth penetration the electrode was running along a dark band, close to its edge (see text). Fig. 4b The same reconstruction, indicating the eye preference of columns crossed by the electrode. The left-eye (contralateral) ocular dominance columns have been shaded. Dashed line: 17-18 border. Bar = 5 mm. Posterior is down in this figure. TOPOGRAPHY OF OCULAR DOMINANCE COLUMNS a segregation according to eye of other fibers, particularly of the axon collaterals of the spiny stellate cells intrinsic to the fourth layer. In the visual cortex of kittens raised with a n artificial squint there is physiologically a more pronounced segregation of cells by ocular dominance than is found in the normal cortex (Hubel and Wiesel, ’65). Monkeys raised with squint were therefore examined in the present study in the hope that the columns might be revealed not only in the fourth layer but throughout the entire thickness of the cortex. In fact, no clear difference was found between the appearance of the columns in normal and in squinting monkeys. Mapping the ocular dominance system Reconstructions of the columnar system for most of the striate cortex were prepared from serial tangential sections stained with the reduced silver method. To make these reconstructions understandable it is necessary to describe the shape of the striate cortex and the manner in which the visual field is mapped onto it. We base the following summary of the topography on the findings of Talbot and Marshall (’41) and Daniel and Whitteridge (‘61). Much of the striate cortex lies on the smooth outer surface (operculum) of the occipital lobe (fig. 7a). This area contains the representation of the most central part of the contralateral visual field. The foveal representation lies laterally (fig. 7b), and the horizontal meridian runs lateromedially, reaching 8 ” of eccentricity at the midline, where it doubles back along the roof of the calcarine fissure. The vertical meridian is represented at the 17-18 border (interrupted lines in figs. 7a,b): below the fixation point it runs upward and medially a short and variable distance behind the lunate sulcus; above the fixation point it runs downwards and medially round the inferior rim of the operculum. The representation of the central part of the vertical meridian by a C-shaped curve on the cortical surface is in part a reflection of the greater magnification factor (the distance on the cortex representing one degree of visual field) for the central part of the field. This expansion in magnification is illustrated in figure 7b, in which the representations of the horizontal meridian and of 567 parallel horizontal lines one, three, and six degrees above and below the meridian are sketched onto the opercular surface. These can be seen to diverge as the vertical meridian is approached. At the midline, area 17 is folded back under itself to form the roof of the calcarine fissure, a triangular piece of cortex underlying the medial part of the operculum (fig. 7a, dotted line). Parasagittal sections (figs. 7c-e) show that at the superior and inferior margins of this triangular region the cortex curls under to form a third and even deeper level, occupying the same triangular area a s the second but consisting of two separate leaves. Where the two leaves meet, they turn anteriorly and form the banks of the “stem” of the calcarine fissure. Medially the roof of the fissure is broad and the stem short, but laterally the roof becomes progressively narrower and the stem longer (figs. 7c-e). The horizontal meridian runs laterally, roughly across the middle of the roof, and at its lateral end (representing about 14” of eccentricity according to Talbot and Marshall, ’41) it turns anteriorly and runs along the blind lateral end of the stem. Thus the temporal crescent lies entirely within the stem of the fissure. In order to reconstruct the columnar pattern over as large a n area as possible, the occipital lobe was sectioned in a plane tangential to the operculum. This plane (the plane of figure 7a) was tangential to three levels of cortex - the exposed surface, the roof of the calcarine fissure, and the two leaves joining the roof to the stem. The only parts of the cortex not mapped were the stem of the fissure, representing the most peripheral part of the field, and the regions of high curvature joining the operculum with the roof of the fissure medially, and joining the roof to the two leaves. Reconstructions of the ocular dominance columns for these three levels of cortex are shown in figure 8. The reconstructions are from a single animal. In another animal less perfect reconstructions were made for the same areas on both sides of the brain, and in four further animals partial reconstructions were made, confined to the operculum. Casual observations of the directions of the columns in various parts of the cortex have been made in a number of other animals. Though each pattern was 568 SIMON LEVAY, DAVID H. HUBEL AND TORSTEN N. WIESEL Fig. 5 High-power micrograph of a single pale band (running horizontally across the field between the arrows) and parts of the two flanking dark b a n d s (top and bottom of field), i n tangential section. The density of tangential fibers within the pale band is lower than i n the dark bands. The bundles of vertically-oriented fibers, seen a s dense clumps i n cross-section, a r e possibly also a little thinner (fewer fibers per bundle) in the pale bands. Bar = 100 u. TOPOGRAPHY OF OCULAR DOMINANCE COLUMNS 569 5 70 SIMON L E V A Y . DAVID H . H U B E L AND TORSTEN N . WIESEL b ' I n / L.S. e Fig. 7 Topography of striate cortex i n the macaque monkey. Fig. 7a Posterolateral view of the brain showing the 17-18 border (dashed line) and the extent of the roof of the buried calcarine fissure (dotted line). The three oblique lines indicate the levels of the parasagittal sections shown in figures 7c, d and e. Fig. 7 b The same view, with the representation of the horizontal meridian ( 0 ) and of horizontal lines 1 , 3 and 6 degrees above ( and below - ) the meridian. T h e fovea is represented laterally, where the horizontal meridian meets the 17-18 border (vertical meridian). Fig. 7c-e Parasagittal sections (medial to lateral) to show the extent of area 1 7 (black). There are three levels of cortex lying in parallel planes: (1) the operculum, (2) the roof of the calcarine fissure and ( 3 ) the leaves joining the roof to the stem. The stem of the fissure is oriented perpendicular to the other levels, and the columns have not been mapped for this area. L.S.: lunate sulcus. C.F.: calcarine fissure. Bar = 5 mm. + unique, in the sense that the fine details the islands, breaks and forks - were not repeated from one individual to the next, the overall direction of the columns was always the same for any particular part of the cortex. In the regions mapped, the columns met the 17-18 border at right angles and ended there abruptly. Within the opercular part of area 17 the columns converged from the border and turned medially, accommodating themselves to a smaller space by repeated fusions of columns. They then streamed over the lip of the calcarine fissure, and doubling back along the roof of the fissure continued laterally for 1-2 mm (fig. 8b). Here however, at a line representing roughly 9-10' of eccentricity, they met a second set of columns oriented parasagittally, at right angles to the first. The TOPOGRAPHY OF OCULAR DOMINANCE COLUMNS boundary between the two systems was not a complete discontinuity but a series of small-scale fusions, loops and turns. The parasagittally oriented columns in the more lateral part of the roof of the fissure were continued onto the floor of the superior and inferior limbs, which formed the third plane of reconstruction (fig. 8c). They then disappeared over the lips of the stem of the fissure, heading for the 17-18 border, which runs mediolaterally near the base of the stem. The first set of columns, situated on the operculum and extending a short distance onto the roof of the calcarine fissure, ran roughly along the cortical representation of horizontal lines in the visual field. This may be seen by comparing figure 8 a with figure 7b. Within the foveal area there were a few loops which were not continuous with the main system. The second set of columns, within the calcarine fissure, were oriented roughly along circumferences in the visual field. The width of the columns was remarkably constant. In particular there was no obvious systematic variation in different parts of the cortex: for example, it was similar for the foveal area as compared with the most peripheral area examined. There was some variation from brain to brain, the range being 250-350 p. This amount of variation might well be caused by varying degrees of shrinkage during processing of the tissue. In the brains containing electrode tracks the shrinkage could be estimated by comparing the distances between lesions with the same distances as read from the electrode advancer; this gave a value for the shrinkage of about 2 5 % , so that the width of the columns in life would be about 3 0 0 4 5 0 p . DISCUSSION It comes as a surprise that the ocular dominance system, which was originally a physiological concept, and which was later demonstrated anatomically by an elaborate technique of reconstructing terminal degeneration after laminar lesions of the geniculate, may be visualized and mapped by a method as simple and old-fashioned as a Cajal silver stain. The columns are visible in reduced silver preparations because a narrow band at the borders between adjacent columns contains a slightly lower 571 density of tangential fibers than is found within the columns proper. We attach no functional significance to the fact that the column boundaries, as seen in these preparations, have an appreciable width. We suggest that the geniculocortical axons themselves contribute to the banding pattern because Fink-Heimer preparations of the cortex after single-layer geniculate lesions show many coarse preterminal fibers running tangentially in layer 4C, and more of these fibers remain within single columns than cross columnar borders. This segregation might give rise to the pale bands if the coarser preterminal fibers began to thin out a short distance from the true border. After lesions involving two adjacent geniculate layers the granular deposit, supposed to represent degenerating boutons, forms a continuous band in the fourth layer without any interruptions (Hubel and Wiesel, '72). This indicates that the finest preterminal arborizations and the boutons themselves, belonging to two adjacent columns, are either immediately contiguous or overlap at the border. 1%nally, the physiology gives no suggestion of a no-man's-land between neighboring columns - the recordings indeed show an overlap of about 25-50 p , which may be real, but is probably produced by the use of electrodes whose uninsulated tip lengths are themselves 25-50 p, As was already indicated in the laminarlesion study, the width of the columns does not vary with eccentricity but remains remarkably constant at about 300 p . When the available space for stripes becomes reduced, as seems to occur during the streaming inward from the 17-18 border, there is no reduction in stripe width; instead the occasional stripe simply ends blindly while its neighbors to either side coalesce. Hubel and Wiesel ('74a) have found evidence for a similar constancy and independence of eccentricity in the width of columns subserving preferred stimulus orientation. The total width of a set of columns covering all 180" of orientation was estimated to be 0.5-1 mm, a value similar to the width of a set of ocular dominance columns (one leftright pair). The significance of this uniformity in cortical function has been discussed (Hubel and Wiesel, '74b). It is of considerable interest that the overall pattern formed by the ocular domi- 572 SIMON LEVAY. D A V I D H. H U B E L A N D TOKSTEN N . W I E S E L Fig. 8 Reconstructions of t h e ocular d o m i n a n c e columiis for ( a ) t h c ~o p c ~ c u l u m i, b ) roo! of thv c;ilc;irine fissure a n d ( c ) t h e Ir;ives ,joining t h e roof t o thv stem. T h e midlincs is t o thr left. a n d the l u n a t e s u l c u s t o the top of the d i a g r a m s ( c o m p a r e fig. 7). Dashed line: 17-18 border. T h e dotted lines represent t h e creases w h e r e t h e cortex i s folded back on itsctlfor ( i n t h e middle of fig. 8 c ) t u r n s o u t of t h e planc o f t h e section to form t h e stem of t h e fissure. T h e rcconstructions generally stop short of t h e dotted lines, hccariw th(, c u r v a t u r e becomes too great for t h e h a n d s to be visible. B a r = 5 mm. i- 8 C 574 S I M O N L L V A Y . DAVII) I 1 FIUBEL A N D TORSTEN N nance columns was similar in all the animals examined, the individual brains differing only in the minute details of the branching arrangements. The most orderly part o f t h e pattern was at the 17-18 border, which the columns always met more or less at right angles. This arrangement may be the most suitable one for the formation of connections from area 17 to area 18 in the immediate vicinity of the border, for it implies that cells in left and right eye columns, serving the same point in the visual field, are situated the same distance from the 17-18 border. This would not have been so if the columns had been arranged parallel to the border. Within area 17 the pattern formed by the columns fell into two parts. Over the smooth outer surface of the cortex, and extending a short distance onto the roof of the calcarine fissure, the columns ran along curves corresponding roughly to the representation of horizontal lines in the visual field. Of possible relevance to this is the observation that the fourth layer of the striate cortex carries a double representation of the visual field, one for the left eye columns and one for the right (Hubel et al., '74). This doubling of the field representation necessitates a corresponding halving of the magnification factor in a direction orthogonal to the columns, while the magnification along the columns remains the same as the overall magnification. We have been able to detect this anisotropy physiologically. The disposition of the columns along the representation of horizontal lines therefore results in horizontal components of images in the central part of the field being favored with a greater cortical magnification in the fourth layer than are vertical components. This arrangement could conceivably be useful for stereoscopic vision, since stereoscopy requires the detection of small horizontal disparities between images in the two monocular representations. Insofar as the relationship between column direction and the representation of horizontal lines is not completely rigid - in particular, the area of foveal representation has several columns running transversely across the horizontal meridian - it is hard to be sure whether the relationship has a real significance or not. I t would be useful in this connection to examine the cortex of WIESk.1 different species, to see which features of the pattern, if any, are shared by them. More peripherally, in the calcarine fissure, the columns were oriented parasagittally, that is, roughly along the representation of circumferential lines in the visual field. This may be the simplest arrangement of the columns in the peripheral area, since it means that if the cortex could be unfolded the columns would run directly from the 17-18 border on one bank of the stem of the fissure to the 17-18 border on the other bank. There are two areas in the calcarine fissure which should be free of columns. One is the monocular segment, which lies in the lateral part of the stem of the fissure, mostly on the superior bank. This has been demonstrated autoradiographically (Wiesel et al., '74), but it was not sectioned tangentially in the present study, and thus could not be mapped. The other area is the representation of the optic disc. This is probably situated at the lateral end of the roof o f t h e fissure, but we have not been able to locate it precisely. IIow is the columnar pattern arrived at during development? The segregation of the two sets of afferents into alternating bands might be understood as the consequence of two conflicting tendencies, the specification of the two sets to occupy the same cortical space according to a single topographic map, and an opposing tendency for grouping within each set, as if caused by mutual repulsion between members of the two sets. The first principle would tend to increase the degree of interpenetration of the two sets, that is, to reduce the maximum distance from any cortical point to the nearest column border. If this distance (which is about 150 p) is kept constant, then a repulsion between the two sets would tend to produce an arrangement with the smallest total length of interface. Alternating bands give a shorter interface than any other arrangement such as a checkerboard or circular islands in a matrix. But the constancy of the overall pattern from animal to animal suggests that the afferents are also guided by wide-field positional information. One is tempted to think that the 17-18 border may be of importance in organizing the pattern, because it is here that the pattern is most orderly. The developing cortex . TOPOGRAPHY OF OCULAR DOMINANCE COLUMNS is rather inaccessible to experimental intervention, but the means by which the pattern is formed may become clearer from studies of visually deprived animals. The simple method of demonstrating ocular dominance columns which has been described here promises to be useful in such studies. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We wish to thank Sarah Kennedy for histological assistance and Claire Wang for help with the diagrams. This work is supported by the Rowland Foundation, Inc., the Esther A. and Joseph Klingenstein Fund, Inc., and by NIH grants 5 ROI EYO 0605 and 5 ROI EYO 0606. LITERATURE CITED Daniel, P. M., and D. Whitteridge 1961 T h e representation of the visual field on the cerebral cortex in monkeys. J. Physiol., 159: 203-222. Hubel, D. H., and T. N. Wiesel 1965 Binocular interaction in striate cortex of kittens reared with artificial squint. J. Neurophysiol., 2 8 : 1041-1059. 575 1972 Laminar and columnar distribution of geniculo-cortical fibers in the macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neur., 1 4 6 : 4 2 1 4 5 0 . 1974a Sequence regularity and geometry of orientation columns in the monkey striate cortex. J. Comp. Neur., 1 5 8 : 267-294. 1974b Uniformity of monkey striate cortex: a parallel relationship between field size, scatter, and magnification factor. J . Comp. Neur., 158: 295-306. Hubel, D. H., T. N. Wiesel and S . LeVay 1974 Visual-field representation in layer IVC of monkey striate cortex. SOC. Neurosci.. 4th A n n u a l Meeting (abstracts). Jones, R. M. 1950 McClung’s Handbook of Microscopical Technique. Hafner, New York. Lund, J . S. 1973 Organization of neurons in the visual cortex, area 17, of the monkey (Mtrctrccr mztldtcr). J . Comp. Neur., 1 4 7 : 4 5 5 4 9 6 . Talbot, S . A,, and W. H. Marshall 1941 Physiological studies on neural mechanisms of visual localization and discrimination. Amer. J . Ophthalmol.. 2 4 ; 1255-1246. Wiesel, T. N., D. H. Hubel and D. Lam 1974 Autoradiographic demonstration of ocular domin a n c e columns in the monkey striate cortex by means of transsynaptic transport. Brain Res.. 79: 273-279.