* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download DanielBearMCB105 Research Proposal
Convolutional neural network wikipedia , lookup
Stereopsis recovery wikipedia , lookup
Eyeblink conditioning wikipedia , lookup
Computer vision wikipedia , lookup
Emotion perception wikipedia , lookup
Perception of infrasound wikipedia , lookup
Stimulus (physiology) wikipedia , lookup
Conditioned place preference wikipedia , lookup
Psychophysics wikipedia , lookup
Neuroesthetics wikipedia , lookup
Feature detection (nervous system) wikipedia , lookup
C1 and P1 (neuroscience) wikipedia , lookup
Visual servoing wikipedia , lookup
Classical conditioning wikipedia , lookup
Time perception wikipedia , lookup
Daniel Bear MCB 105 Research Proposal Background and Significance Binocular rivalry occurs in conscious humans and monkeys when different images are simultaneously presented to each eye (1, 2). If the distinct images cover a relatively small portion of the visual field, humans will usually see one image for several seconds, then the other, switching back and forth (1). This perceptual phenomenon was long thought to involve reciprocal inhibition between neurons with monocular visual receptive fields, and recent studies have found monocular neurons in the human lateral geniculate nucleus that only fire when the subject perceives one eye’s image (3); however, binocular rivalry must also involve image dominance and suppression at higher, eye-independent levels of perception, as switching the images displayed between the two eyes several times a second does not change the pattern of each image’s dominance and suppression times (4). Moreover, some of the binocular neurons in V1/V2, V4, and V5 (MT) fire only when a macaque monkey reports seeing one of the two rivaling images, and nearly all neurons recorded from in the inferior temporal cortex and superior temporal sulcus fire in response to a particular percept (2, 5). Thus, as early in the visual pathway as V1, neurons encode one psychophysical representation of visual space, not just the physical qualities of the two rivaling visual stimuli. MT, one of the areas with percept-selective neurons during binocular rivalry, can also influence the behavioral response to a visual stimulus. Some neurons in MT fire only when their receptive fields contain motion in a particular direction; when small groups of cells corresponding to a single direction are stimulated with an electrode, monkeys report seeing motion in that direction, even if a visual stimulus contains no overall directional motion (6). This suggests that even cells at this level of the visual system can characterize a perception enough to drive behavior. If the experience of binocular rivalry were driven by competition between such perception-encoding neurons, then these cells would be a likely target for other neural pathways that increase or decrease the dominance of one of the rivaling images. The predominance of an image is a measure of the relative amount of time the subject perceives that image alone during binocular rivalry. With two neutral, equivalent competing stimuli, such as right-slanted black lines and left-slanted black lines, the predominance of each image is zero as expected (i.e. neither image is perceived a larger percentage of the time). Many physical characteristics of the rivaling images, including contrast, color, and surrounding image, can affect the ratio of times spent perceiving each image (1). For example, using the images above, as the contrast of one set of lines increases, this image will be suppressed for less time relative to the fixed-contrast lines (4). While this shows that an image’s intrinsic qualities can increase its predominance during ambiguous visual stimulation, relatively few studies have carefully measured the roles of psychological factors and conditioning on an individual’s perception during binocular rivalry (7). Unlike selective attention paradigms, which show similar neuronal firing patterns to binocular rivalry, humans seem to have almost no voluntary control over the predominance of an image (5); whereas a human can, with practice, spend more time paying attention to one of two unambiguous stimuli, one cannot usually affect the predominance of a rivaling image by any behavioral trick (1). This raises the question of whether involuntary psychological factors can alter the perceptual pattern of binocular rivalry. Although it is implicitly assumed that state of Daniel Bear MCB 105 Research Proposal mind influences perception, the binocular rivalry paradigm offers a way to directly and quantitatively measure a controlled effect on well-characterized ambiguous stimuli. Note that the conflict in binocular rivalry occurs not only at the level of perception and interpretation, as in Gestalt figures, but also at the level of primary physical stimulus. While state of mind and prior learning clearly influence the perception of Gestalt figures, binocular rivalry appears to resist conscious efforts to see one image on command or indefinitely (1). If conditioning were able to affect the predominance of an image in binocular rivalry, it might allow an individual to alter his perception of information in visual and other sensory modalities or access stimuli that had been suppressed by a lifetime of unintentional conditioning. This might also allow researchers to investigate the neural mechanisms that strengthen and weaken percept-specific circuits in the visual system, such as the previously discussed neurons in MT. Classical and operant conditioning, as two of the simplest and most powerful mechanisms of altering behavior, may be able to affect the predominance of an image in binocular rivalry. In this scenario, perceiving one image (e.g. right-slanted black lines) would be the conditioned response or the reinforced “behavior,” in the cases of classical and operant conditioning, respectively. Some evidence suggests that when one of the two rivaling images is more emotional or natural than the other, such as a fearful or rightside-up face versus a neutral or upside-down face, the more “significant” one predominates (7). These studies may be confounded by the physical nature of the images, however, since the two faces might not have the same contrast or edge makeup, factors which can directly affect binocular rivalry. One recent study claims to have replicated this finding using faces with the same physical qualities (8); nevertheless, emotional faces have become strong classically conditioned stimuli well before the experiment, making it difficult to induce changes in binocular rivalry due to conditioning at the behavioral or neuronal level. Another study from the same group attempted to circumvent this problem by conditioning a completely neutral stimulus (vertical or horizontal black lines) with simultaneous shocks to the arm whenever it appeared (7). This technique very modestly but statistically significantly increased the predominance of the conditioned image, as well as its probability of being the first percept during rivaling stimulation. The methodology of this experiment, however, may not optimize predominance: this study treated the shock as unconditioned stimulus (UCS) and displaying one image as the paired conditioned stimulus (CS), so that the real unconditioned response and conditioned response (CR) should be “fear of pain” rather than “perception of the conditioned image.” As such, the slightly increased predominance may be the result of emotional priming rather than true classical conditioning. A more logical experiment would reverse the UCS and CS, so that displaying one image alone is the UCS, and a conditioned stimulus such as a shock predicts this image. Then, during binocular rivalry, a shock (the CS) might evoke a conditioned response, the perception of the selected image. (This will be discussed more in the proposed methods section; here it points out the flaw in the previous study and the need for more rational investigation of classical conditioning in binocular rivalry.) Daniel Bear MCB 105 Research Proposal Specific Aims The overall goals of the proposed research, then, are to clarify the effects of classical conditioning on image predominance and to explore the completely uncharacterized effects of operant conditioning in binocular rivalry. This research would also search for means to change and measure image predominance at the level of percept reporting (in humans) and neural activity (in macaque monkeys) as ways of studying the psychology and neurophysiology of perception. These goals are based on the observation that emotionally and behaviorally significant images tend to override less significant visual information (7, 8) and the hypothesis that percept-responsive neurons like those found in MT are susceptible to behavioral modulation. Because the primate brain can process and act on only a small fraction of available sensory input, conditioning at the level of perception itself may be a way for the brain to efficiently reconcile ambiguous stimuli. The proposed research would therefore be aimed toward answering the following questions: 1. Humans and monkeys—Can pairing a previously unrelated stimulus with a particular image cause that image to predominate over a physically equivalent image during binocular rivalry (classical conditioning)? As discussed, previous studies either have relied on the lifetime conditioning of specific emotional faces or have used questionable conditioning paradigms (7, 8). Since the goal is to induce measurable changes in binocular rivalry through classical conditioning, the proposed research would require the newly introduced stimuli to be associated with initially neutral images. Slanted lines with opposite direction would be ideal, even though classical conditioning in general may have a preference for evolutionarily significant stimuli (7). Since UCR = CR in the classical conditioning paradigm, the protocol should make “perception of left/right slanted lines” the conditioned response. This would make presentation of that image in a non-rivaling setting the unconditioned stimulus, for perception of this image is the natural response. During training, the UCS would be preceded by what would become the CS. If the conditioning were effective, presentation of the CS before or during binocular rivalry would cause the CR image to predominate. This approach has more natural logic than making the image itself the CS, since it normally evokes a perceptual without any conditioning. These experiments would determine how much prior pairing of stimuli, either intentionally or unintentionally, can bias perception of a truly ambiguous stimulus (i.e. one where there is no natural advantage of one percept over the other.) 2. Humans and monkeys—Is there a way to selective reinforce one percept during binocular rivalry so that it, like a behavior, begins to predominate over inconsequential percepts (operant conditioning)? Operant conditioning normally occurs when an individual learns that a specific behavior predicts, and therefore causes, a reward or a cessation of punishment. The subject will then perform the behavior more often than before conditioning. Binocular rivalry does not involve any motor or cognitive behavior; in fact, perceptual switching seems to be almost completely out of voluntary control (1). Nevertheless, if one percept were to predict a reward more often than the other, the brain might reinforce perception itself, without involving a traditional behavior. To test this, a Daniel Bear MCB 105 Research Proposal transient reward, such as a pleasing tactile stimulus, would be given to the subject only while he reported seeing one of the two images during binocular rivalry. Despite the lack of voluntary control over perception, this reward would signal to the brain that, when the eyes see two possible images, only one has meaningful consequences. This method would require rewards during binocular rivalry, as rewarding the same image under unambiguous conditions is not true operant conditioning: at the behavioral level, perceiving one of two possible images is different than simply looking at a fixed image; and at the cellular level, the two situations evoke different neuronal responses, with some neurons active when an image is displayed unambiguously but silent during rivaling perception of that same image (2). Moreover, rewarding an image first under unambiguous conditions is the same as the “classical conditioning” paradigm of (7), but using rewards instead of punishments. The hypothesis predicts that if one percept is rewarded during binocular rivalry, this image will increasingly predominate. (A special technique, to be discussed in the methods section, would be needed to assure that perception, not the motor method of reporting, were reinforced in monkeys.) This operant conditioning is predicted to have a more powerful effect than classical conditioning, since the former may not rely on evolutionarily relevant stimuli. A positive result would suggest that an ambiguous image can drive its own preferred perception if one image is somehow deemed important, even if this involves suppressing another entire set of visual information. 3. Monkeys only—Where in the visual system are conditioned changes in image predominance detectable by electrophysiological recording? If the proposed classical and operant conditioning paradigms cause one image to predominate over the other, this reinforcement must be encoded somewhere in the visual or perceptual systems. Since V1, V2, V4, MT, and other temporal areas contain percept-responsive neurons, these cell populations would be recorded by electrode before and after each type of conditioning. The goal would be to detect the earliest point at which conditioning had modulated activity in a percept-dependent fashion, as all downstream regions would be expected to follow similar patterns. This experiment would hint at where emotional and behavioral information directly influences the visual system to drive perceptual changes. It would also provide a list of potential neural targets to stimulate as a way to further condition a perception or alter a behavioral response. Data could help determine what is truly suppressed in binocular rivalry: percept-specific neurons, feature specific neurons, or some combination of both. The specific aims of the proposed research would elucidate the roles of learning and emotional memory in perceiving an ambiguous stimulus. While it is interesting that prior experience can influence perception of Gestalt figures, where the physical stimulus is unambiguous, it would be even more remarkable if learning could cause the brain to block out or promote entire channels of incoming sensory information, not just their higher-order representations. These experiments would also emphasize controlled changes in perception of neutral stimuli, thereby excluding a lifetime’s worth of emotional and physical confounding variables. Daniel Bear MCB 105 Research Proposal Research Design Aim 1: Determining the effect of classical conditioning on binocular rivalry. Humans: Participants with normal vision and psychological make-up will give informed consent to experiencing potentially aversive sounds, but no physical pain. Since prior mindset may affect the strength of conditioning, participants initially will be told only that they are involved in tests of visual concentration; the full details of the experiment will be disclosed afterward. A total of six participants will be tested: two unconditioned controls, two yoked controls, and two for the conditioning experiment. The rivaling stimuli for these experiments will be right-slanted and left-slanted black lines, which are assumed to be emotionally neutral. Participants will view a monitor through a mirror stereoscope as in (7) that will isolate the images presented to each eye. They will have 20 training trials, in which they will learn to press one key if they see right-slanted lines and another if they see left-slanted lines; during training, the same image will be presented to both eyes and the two images will alternate stochastically every few seconds so as to mimic the experience of binocular rivalry, for a total of 8 seconds. Participants will then proceed to the conditioning experiment. 10 initial 8 second rivalry trials will present one image to the left eye and the other to the right eye to induce binocular rivalry (the images will alternate eyes every trial). Participants will report percepts in order to calculate any baseline predominance of one image, given by (time spent perceiving RIGHT LINES – time spent perceiving LEFT LINES) / (total time perceiving a single image). During this and subsequent tests, the initial percept will also be recorded. After the baseline trials, the two unconditioned controls will have another 10 identical trials. The two participants designated for conditioning will have 10 trials for which the same images are presented to each eye in a pattern identical to the training trials. In these trials, however, the computer will be programmed to emit a short buzzer sound every time immediately before one of the two images (the designated UCS) appears. For one participant, the UCS will be right-slanted lines, for the other left-slanted lines. The buzzer will therefore act as the CS. The yoked controls will have 10 similar trials except that the computer will buzz at random times throughout each trial, uncorrelated with which image is about to appear. Finally, participants will be given 10 test trials during which rivaling images are presented to each eye, as in the baseline trials. Immediately before each trial begins, the computer will buzz (give the CS). Initial percepts and total time spent seeing each image will be recorded and calculated as predominance. The set of conditioning and test trials (20 total) will be repeated four times for a total of five experimental blocks. Participants will be debriefed and predominance analyzed. The hypothesized results are statistically significant increases in both initial percept and predominance of the conditioned image during the test trials for the conditioned group, but not the yoked group (which should not have either image associated with the buzzer). This paradigm is designed to maximize the CS-CR association during binocular rivalry, unlike the technique in (7) which treats the image itself as a conditioned stimulus and does not present the associated stimulus (an electric shock) during binocular rivalry. Daniel Bear MCB 105 Research Proposal Monkeys: Macaque monkeys will be trained to report each image with levers and tested for accurate reporting with catch trials as in (2). Three monkeys will be trained, one for each of the conditions in the human experiments. The protocol for conditioning and testing monkeys will be identical to the one used for humans, using a monkey-adapted apparatus. The same setup used in (2) will also allow extracellular recording from individual neurons in V1/V2, V4, MT, and other cortical areas before, during, and after conditioning (see design for Aim 3). Aim 2: Finding an effective operant conditioning paradigm in binocular rivalry. Humans: Six participants will give informed consent as in Aim 1, except that there will not be any aversive sounds. Two participants will be assigned to each of the control, yoked, and conditioning groups. The rivaling stimuli, training sessions, and baseline predominance trials will be identical to those in Aim 1. Every participant will then choose from a list of sustainable computer-produced sounds the one that is most rewarding or pleasing to him. Participants will then view rivaling stimuli in blocks of 20 8-second trials. The first 10 trials will differ between groups: for the control group, these trials will be identical to baseline trials in order to measure any effect of hearing a pleasant sound before binocular rivalry; for the conditioning group, the computer will emit the chosen sound with a probability of .5 whenever the participants report seeing one of the two images (the conditioned percept = right-slanted lines for one person, leftslanted for the other), for as long as this percept lasts; and for the yoked group, the computer will emit the sound with probability .25 whenever the participants report seeing either percept. The .5 probability in the conditioning group is thought to maximize the effect of operant conditioning (variable ratio reinforcement). The last 10 trials of each block will be identical to the baseline trials for all experimental groups. There will be 5 identical test blocks for a total of 100 trials, with image predominance being measured separately in the first 10 versus the last 10 trials of each block. The first 10 trials of each block will determine whether reinforcement during binocular rivalry can skew the predominance of each image, while the last 10 trials will measure any lasting perceptual “learning,” even in the absence of the original reinforcer. The hypothesized result is a significant increase in predominance of the reinforced image for the conditioned group, especially during the first 10 trials when this image is being actively reinforced. Monkeys: Monkeys will require a slightly different protocol from humans, as a reward during binocular rivalry might cause a monkey to errantly report the reinforced image more often, even if he does not actually perceive it. As in Aim 1, three monkeys will first be trained to report each of the two rivaling images. The monkeys will also undergo a second round of training in which they learn not to be distracted by sounds during binocular rivalry. This will be achieved by ceasing training for good whenever the monkey is distracted by background sounds, and rewarding the monkey with juice whenever he completes a “noisy” training session. Monkeys must further be tested to make sure that they are reporting accurately when there is noise present. This will be done using catch trials (as in (2)) with noise present and by comparing monkeys’ report to electrophysiological recording. Once cells are located in V4 and MT that respond binocularly to a particular image, experimenters will compare the cells’ firing pattern during binocular rivalry to the monkeys’ report. Since many cells are known to vary with perceptual patterns (2), these recordings can further validate accurate reporting when Daniel Bear MCB 105 Research Proposal sound is present. The monkeys will then choose a pleasing sound from several choices by pressing a different button to initiate each sound. A button pressed significantly more often than the others will be deemed pleasing. This sound will then be used in operant conditioning trials identical to the ones given to humans, with one monkey assigned to each of the control, conditioned, and yoked groups. Resulting image predominance will be calculated in the same way. Aim 3: Detecting electrophysiological changes in monkeys due to the reinforcement of a percept. Using the electrophysiological setup of (2), individual neurons in V1/V2, V4, MT, the inferior temporal lobe, and the superior temporal sulcus will be recorded from with extracellular electrodes while monkeys view unambiguous right- or left-slanted lines, one eye at a time. When a cell is found that shows a firing preference for one image over the other, the electrode will be held in place while the monkey undergoes either the classical or the operant conditioning protocol. During binocular rivalry, the electrode and resulting data analysis will measure this neuron’s correlation with reported percept, mean firing duration, and total percentage of the trial spent firing. The hypothesis is that if either classical or operant conditioning is successful at reinforcing one percept, the firing of some image-specific neurons must be potentiated. The neurons predicted to increase their firing rate and percentage are those which respond binocularly to a given percept, i.e. they fire regardless of which eye actually sees the image. These neurons are presumably at a higher level in visual processing than the monocular channels or percept-independent cells, and they represent the supposed cellular substrate for behavioral modification of perception. Changes in firing patterns will be measured in cells at all locations in the visual cortex. Further Directions Because previous research has shown that emotional images or patterns associated with aversive stimuli predominate in binocular rivalry, it would be surprising if this new classical conditioning paradigm had no effect (7, 8). The next question, then, would be how much conditioning can influence perception when the brain is presented with physically equivalent stimuli. With the proposed technique, the conditioned stimulus, a buzzer, might be substituted with a more evolutionarily relevant aversive stimulus, such as pain (as in (7)), a human voice, or an image of a predator preceding binocular rivalry. Even more natural would be pairing an evolutionary CS, such as a snake, with a logical image in binocular rivalry, such as a fearful face. Since one group has used faces with equivalent characteristics to study binocular rivalry, this would be possible without confounding physical variables (8). The next step would be to see if classical conditioning could actually override visual qualities that normally bias binocular rivalry toward one image, such as higher contrast (2). Since the proposed operant conditioning protocol is novel to the study of binocular rivalry, it is possible that a reinforcing stimulus would either have no effect on image predominance or would distract from image viewing altogether. If this were the case, the proposed research’s focus would shift to the classical conditioning experiments, possibly replacing aversive stimuli with rewards for perceiving an image before the onset Daniel Bear MCB 105 Research Proposal of binocular rivalry. Since the brain is normally capable of integrating many sensory stimuli, however, it seems more likely that a reward would potentiate relevant visual information. Other rewards could be tested, including pleasing tactile stimuli, food rewards, and even psychological encouragement (“you’re doing very well”). Punishments could also be tested using the same protocol for their ability to suppress one percept. The overall goal would be to study whether the brain treats involuntary perception as it would behavior, even when it means ignoring a coherent visual stimulus. Since both images are processed in binocular rivalry, the brain might alter perception at relatively early stages of the visual pathway under conditions where one image has proven itself important. If the electrophysiological experiments detected activity changes in V1/V2, for instance, they could help explain why image switching in binocular rivalry is completely involuntary, as opposed to selective attention or viewing Gestalt figures: the physical information of one image might be suppressed too early in visual processing for voluntary cognitive processes to affect it. Finally, if changes in V1/V2, V4, and MT neuronal activity accompany conditioned changes in perception as expected, the next step would be to find out which other brain areas are modulating the visual cortex. The amygdala has known inputs to the visual regions of the brain that would likely mediate the classical conditioning and emotional effects on binocular rivalry, so it would be important to determine whether this pathway is active at all times or only when the conditioned image is perceived during binocular rivalry. If it is active whenever the emotion-associated image is presented (i.e. always during binocular rivalry), the overall parameters of percept switching might be expected to change without one image predominating strongly; for instance, percept duration might increase for both competing images. On the other hand, if the emotional pathway only acts when that image is being perceived or only influences percept-specific neurons, that image might predominate dramatically as seen in (8). The neural pathways that could influence the proposed operant conditioning in perception are unknown; follow up experiments would test the midbrain dopamine system for any influence, possibly by direct electrical stimulation. This pathway is largely responsible for traditional operant conditioning, so it might act by a parallel mechanism on perception of ambiguous visual stimuli. References 1. R. Blake, N.K. Logothetis, Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 13 (2002). 2. D.A. Leopold, N.K. Logothetis, Nature 379, 549 (1996). 3. J.D. Haynes, R. Deichmann, G. Rees, Nature 438, 496 (2005). 4. N.K. Logothetis, D.A. Leopold, D.L. Sheinberg, Nature 380, 621 (1996). 5. D.L. Sheinberg, N.K. Logothetis, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 94, 3408 (1997). 6. C.D. Salzman, K.H. Britten, W.T. Newsome, Nature 346, 174 (1990). 7. G.W. Alpers et al., Int. J. Psychophys. 57, 25 (2005). 8. G.W. Alpers, A.B. Gerdes, Emotion 7, 495 (2007).