* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Conversion disorder: understanding the
Aging brain wikipedia , lookup
Neuroplasticity wikipedia , lookup
Alzheimer's disease wikipedia , lookup
History of neuroimaging wikipedia , lookup
Limbic system wikipedia , lookup
Biology of depression wikipedia , lookup
Neuropsychology wikipedia , lookup
Neuropsychopharmacology wikipedia , lookup
Metastability in the brain wikipedia , lookup
Causes of mental disorders wikipedia , lookup
National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke wikipedia , lookup
Biochemistry of Alzheimer's disease wikipedia , lookup
Abnormal psychology wikipedia , lookup
Externalizing disorders wikipedia , lookup
Clinical neurochemistry wikipedia , lookup
Brain 2010: 133; 1295–1299 | 1295 BRAIN A JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGY SCIENTIFIC COMMENTARIES Conversion disorder: understanding the pathogenic links between emotion and motor systems in the brain When neurology and psychiatry moved apart from each other around the turn of the 20th century, casualties included the many patients with unexplained neurological disorders. Many labels have been applied to these patients. Some are descriptive (functional disorders, medically unexplained symptoms), while others refer to a presumed aetiology (psychogenic, hysteria, non-organic) or putative mechanism (dissociative or conversion disorder). Whatever is the label, for some physicians these patients are among the most interesting and challenging in the clinic. For others they are frustrating, using much time and resources for an often disappointing outcome. Whether you are an enthusiast or nihilist, it is clear that that neither neurology nor psychiatry alone have succeeded in sufficiently advancing our neurobiological understanding or management of these disorders. The slow progress is in stark contrast to the scale of the problem. Medically unexplained neurological symptoms account for 30% of referred neurology out-patients (Carson et al., 2000; Stone et al., 2009). Conversion disorder alone, explicitly associated with psychological stressors at the outset, accounts for 5% of referrals and is a stable, accurate diagnosis (Perkin, 1989; Stone et al., 2005, 2009). Psychogenic movement disorders are common in neurological practice, including tremor, dystonia, gait change and paralysis. Diagnostic criteria emphasize clinical observations such as inconsistency, distractibility and false neurological signs (Fahn and Williams, 1988). These can be both sensitive and specific, at least in the context of a movement disorder clinic (Shill and Gerber, 2006). Physiological criteria have also been developed, such as coherence of tremor oscillations distinguishing psychogenic from organic syndromes (McAuley and Rothwell, 2004). The emphasis in this ‘neurological’ approach is on the physical examination, supplemented by physiological tests. The psychological aspects of the disease take second place, e.g. requiring ‘obvious’ but not specified psychiatric or emotional disturbance (Fahn and Williams, 1988; Shill and Gerber, 2006). In contrast, the ‘psychiatric’ approach emphasizes features of the clinical interview, including psychological factors at outset and inferences of intent. This is not ideal as a diagnostic ‘gold standard’ for developing a biological science of conversion disorder but the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV) at least provides strict diagnostic criteria. The diagnosis requires the temporal association of psychological factors with the neurological symptoms (Criterion B: note that in the current revision of DSM, the aetiological relevance of the psychological factors has been downgraded from causal to association only). A further condition (Criterion C) is the lack of wilful simulation of symptoms (otherwise indicative of malingering, factitious disorders or ‘Munchausen syndrome’) but this is difficult to substantiate. The neurological approach to psychogenic movement disorders and the psychiatric approach to conversion movement disorder are therefore both problematic. Many patients move between neurological and psychiatric services, but a unified formulation may not emerge, contributing to an unsatisfactory outcome. Importantly, we lack mechanisms to explain the neurological manifestation of the conversion disorder, even when likely psychological aetiological factors are established. An objective, explanatory framework for conversion disorder is clearly required, one that brings together both psychological and physiological phenomena. Against this troubled background, in the current issue, Voon et al. (p 1526) use functional magnetic resonance imaging to study conversion movement disorders. The combination of psychiatrists and neurologists among the research team is important, and not just for the credibility of this particular study. It also signifies a trend towards a common understanding of conversion disorder, which is important for developing effective neuropsychiatric management strategies. Voon et al. are not the first to use neuroimaging methods to study conversion disorder or psychogenic movement disorders [see Nowak and Fink (2009) for a review of ‘psychogenic paralysis’]. However, previous studies often used motor tasks that are close to the functional deficit. For example, in studies of conversion/psychogenic paralysis, participants have been asked to try to move the affected limb, imagine moving the paralysed limb or feign immobility of the ‘good’ limb (Marshall et al., 1997; Halligan et al., 2000; Spence et al., 2000; Ward et al., 2003; de Lange et al., 2007). These studies indicate abnormalities outside the core motor network, including the prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex. The results support the hypothesis of abnormal ß The Author (2010). Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Guarantors of Brain. All rights reserved. For Permissions, please email: [email protected] 1296 | Brain 2010: 133; 1295–1299 inhibition of motor systems by limbic regions (Marshall et al., 1997; Halligan et al., 2000; Spence et al., 2000; Ward et al., 2003) or impairments of motor conceptualization (Burgmer et al., 2006; de Lange et al., 2007). However, imaging abnormalities identified from tasks that are based directly on the motor deficits can be confounded by differences in motor performance and sensory feedback (the latter studied directly by Vuilleumier et al., 2001). In addition, the interactions between limbic and motor areas are inferred, but have not been shown directly in the previous studies. A different strategy has been taken by Voon et al. They study patients using a task that they can perform normally, and one that is at first sight unrelated to the motor deficit: a gender discrimination task with face pictures. By manipulating the emotional expression of the faces, irrelevant to the task at hand, they are able to study a robust neurocognitive system for emotion processing and arousal, including the amygdala. This choice of task is very important, given the emotional events that may contribute to the initiation or maintenance of conversion disorder. A second interesting aspect of this study is the inclusion of a diverse patient population with tremor, gait disorder, dystonia or mixed syndromes. This contrasts with smaller series (median n = 4) of closely matched patients in previous neuroimaging studies (Marshall et al., 1997; Halligan et al., 2000; Spence et al., 2000; Ward et al., 2003; de Lange et al., 2007). Whereas clinical heterogeneity can reduce sensitivity to the neural correlates of individual phenotypes, it also means that results reflect the communalities, not differences, of conversion movement disorders. It is all the more interesting, therefore, that common abnormalities are seen in the amygdala and its interaction with the supplementary motor area. This provides direct support for a model of conversion disorder based on abnormal limbic–motor interactions. Is it sufficient to identify neural correlates of chronic conversion movement disorder? Several issues arise here. First, one must acknowledge that, in isolation, physiological correlations do not provide the cause or mechanisms of disease. However, they do support a change in the direction of research, striving for a neurobiological model. This should not discard the importance of psychological or psychodynamic factors, but they must lie within an evidence-based functional and causal (mechanistic) neuroanatomical model. Second, the search for a neural signature of conversion disorder implicitly challenges the historical ‘non-organic’ model. This problem is not unique to conversion disorder, but applies to other major psychiatric syndromes including depression or schizophrenia. The observed neural correlates of conversion disorder might also be due to late epiphenomena or comorbidities in chronically disabled patients, e.g. depression. In the study by Voon et al., the patients do not meet diagnostic criteria for major affective or psychotic disorders or substance misuse. However, they do have significantly higher rates of depression and anxiety symptoms than controls. In separate studies, depression, remitted depression or elevated depressive symptoms without depression have been shown to influence the activity of the amygdala (Drevets, 2000) and its response to salient stimuli (Taylor Tavares et al., 2008; Beesdo et al., 2009). Voon et al. argue cogently that their findings are independent of depressive or anxiety symptoms. However, they rely partly on a lack of significant correlation between Scientific Commentaries depression scores and amygdala activity (possible risk of type II error) and partly on interpretation of left versus right differences (possible risk of false lateralization arising in thresholded images). Nonetheless, the depression scores in their patients may have been elevated by somatic symptoms, a common feature of conversion disorder, rather than mood disturbance per se. A third important aspect of the study by Voon et al. is their analysis of the connectivity of the amygdala. It has previously been proposed that conversion disorder arises from abnormal interactions between limbic and motor regions. To test hypotheses of interacting neural systems calls for formal analyses of network connectivity, but this has been lacking in previous studies. Voon et al. use two methods to study connectivity. The first is psychophysiological interactions (Friston et al., 1997), revealing that patients viewing salient stimuli have abnormal connectivity between the amygdala and the supplementary motor area. The second method, Granger Causality Modelling (Roebroeck et al., 2009), is used to identify the directionality of this effect: amygdala activity is predictive of future changes in the supplementary motor area, but not vice versa. Future studies will no doubt refine task designs and analysis options to include more realistic biophysical models with anatomically defined networks (Friston, 2009). As a first step, however, the concordance between connectivity methods and their immediate relevance to the predicted limbic–motor interactions is impressive. There remain many unanswered questions. For example, firstly, why do even common and mild mood disorders or stressors lead to bizarre and disabling conversion disorders/psychogenic movement disorders? Secondary gain, or exposure to neurological illness in personal or professional life, may be relevant (Shill and Gerber, 2006), but they lack objective measures and are prone to recollection bias. Secondly, what determines the nature of the neurological deficit—tremor, paralysis, blindness, sensory loss? Here, at least the approach taken by Voon et al. provides clear testable hypotheses regarding interactions between amygdala and visual or sensory cortex in other syndromes. Thirdly, why do patients experience a sense of loss of control over their movements? This speaks to the broader neuroscience of volition (Haggard, 2008) and disturbed sense of ‘agency’ in neurological disorders (Moore et al., 2009). Answers to these questions will require further long-term integration of psychiatry and neurology during training, in the clinic and in the laboratory. With its high prevalence and disability, conversion disorder clearly requires higher priority for research, into both basic mechanisms and effective therapies. In the meantime, be inspired by the work of Voon et al., whose results are a significant step towards understanding the neurobiological basis of conversion disorder and developing better therapies. James B. Rowe Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK E-mail: [email protected] doi:10.1093/brain/awq096 Scientific Commentaries References Beesdo K, Lau JY, Guyer AE, McClure-Tone EB, Monk CS, Nelson EE, et al. Common and distinct amygdala-function perturbations in depressed vs anxious adolescents. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2009; 66: 275–85. Burgmer M, Konrad C, Jansen A, Kugel H, Sommer J, Heindel W, et al. Abnormal brain activation during movement observation in patients with conversion paralysis. Neuroimage 2006; 29: 1336–43. Carson AJ, Ringbauer B, Stone J, McKenzie L, Warlow C, Sharpe M. Do medically unexplained symptoms matter? A prospective cohort study of 300 new referrals to neurology outpatient clinics. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2000; 68: 207–10. de Lange FP, Roelofs K, Toni I. Increased self-monitoring during imagined movements in conversion paralysis. Neuropsychologia 2007; 45: 2051–8. Drevets WC. Neuroimaging studies of mood disorders. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 48: 813–29. Fahn S, Williams DT. Psychogenic dystonia. Adv Neurol 1988; 50: 431–55. Friston K. Dynamic causal modeling and Granger causality Comments on: the identification of interacting networks in the brain using fMRI: model selection, causality and deconvolution. Neuroimage 2009; Advance Access published on September 19, 2009, doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.031. Friston KJ, Buechel C, Fink GR, Morris J, Rolls E, Dolan RJ. Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging. Neuroimage 1997; 6: 218–29. Haggard P. Human volition: towards a neuroscience of will. Nat Rev Neurosci 2008; 9: 934–46. Halligan PW, Athwal BS, Oakley DA, Frackowiak RS. Imaging hypnotic paralysis: implications for conversion hysteria. Lancet 2000; 355: 986–7. Marshall JC, Halligan PW, Fink GR, Wade DT, Frackowiak RS. The functional anatomy of a hysterical paralysis. Cognition 1997; 64: B1–8. Brain 2010: 133; 1295–1299 | 1297 McAuley J, Rothwell J. Identification of psychogenic, dystonic, and other organic tremors by a coherence entrainment test. Mov Disord 2004; 19: 253–67. Moore JW, Wegner DM, Haggard P. Modulating the sense of agency with external cues. Conscious Cogn 2009; 18: 1056–64. Nowak DA, Fink GR. Psychogenic movement disorders: aetiology, phenomenology, neuroanatomical correlates and therapeutic approaches. Neuroimage 2009; 47: 1015–25. Perkin GD. An analysis of 7836 successive new outpatient referrals. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1989; 52: 447–8. Roebroeck A, Formisano E, Goebel R. The identification of interacting networks in the brain using fMRI: model selection, causality and deconvolution. Neuroimage 2009; Advance Access published on September 25, 2009, doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.036. Shill H, Gerber P. Evaluation of clinical diagnostic criteria for psychogenic movement disorders. Mov Disord 2006; 21: 1163–8. Spence SA, Crimlisk HL, Cope H, Ron MA, Grasby PM. Discrete neurophysiological correlates in prefrontal cortex during hysterical and feigned disorder of movement. Lancet 2000; 355: 1243–4. Stone J, Carson A, Duncan R, Coleman R, Roberts R, Warlow C, et al. Symptoms ’unexplained by organic disease’ in 1144 new neurology out-patients: how often does the diagnosis change at follow-up? Brain 2009; 132: 2878–88. Stone J, Smyth R, Carson A, Lewis S, Prescott R, Warlow C, et al. Systematic review of misdiagnosis of conversion symptoms and ‘hysteria’. BMJ 2005; 331: 989. Taylor Tavares JV, Clark L, Furey ML, Williams GB, Sahakian BJ, Drevets WC. Neural basis of abnormal response to negative feedback in unmedicated mood disorders. Neuroimage 2008; 42: 1118–26. Vuilleumier P, Chicherio C, Assal F, Schwartz S, Slosman D, Landis T. Functional neuroanatomical correlates of hysterical sensorimotor loss. Brain 2001; 124: 1077–90. Ward NS, Oakley DA, Frackowiak RS, Halligan PW. Differential brain activations during intentionally simulated and subjectively experienced paralysis. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 2003; 8: 295–312. Are we getting to grips with Alzheimer’s disease at last? Recent statistics from the Alzheimer Research Trust suggest that in the UK alone 820 000 people are affected by dementia with a cost to the economy of £23 billion/year. Similar figures apply to all developed countries and most others are catching up rapidly. With increasing life expectancy, age-related dementia is seen globally as an urgent public health priority. Histological examination of the brain is still regarded as the gold standard for diagnosis of the specific disease process underlying dementia, the commonest cause of which is Alzheimer’s disease. The main features of Alzheimer’s disease are extracellular accumulation of amyloid b-protein (Ab) in the form of plaques and in blood vessel walls as cerebral amyloid angiopathy; intraneuronal accumulation of tau protein forming tangles in neuronal cell bodies as well as in neuronal processes situated close to plaques (dystrophic neurites) and elsewhere (neuropil threads); activation of microglia and astrocytes; and neuronal and synaptic loss. However, histological assessment of the disease has several limitations including, almost by definition, observations made at only one time point in the disease, usually the end-stage of a neurodegenerative process that has been developing over many years. This means that using post-mortem neuropathology, we have little or no notion of the dynamics of the pathological processes involved, how they are interrelated in terms of cause and effect, and which feature, if any, best correlates with, or causes, the cognitive dysfunction. The principal hypothesis for the pathogenesis of Alzheimer’s disease for two decades or more has been built around amyloid, and known as the Ab cascade hypothesis, which states that Ab, either in the form of extracellular amyloid plaques or in soluble or oligomeric forms, has the key role in initiation of the disease. A powerful way to test the Ab hypothesis is to modify this aspect of the pathophysiology and observe any effects on other aspects of the pathology and on brain function. In this issue of Brain, Serrano-Pozo and colleagues (page 1312) have studied the effects of Ab immunization on neuronal and tau pathology in Alzheimer’s disease. Active immunization with