* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Ordinal Position of Neurons in Cat Striate Cortex
Extracellular matrix wikipedia , lookup
Cell growth wikipedia , lookup
Cytokinesis wikipedia , lookup
Tissue engineering wikipedia , lookup
Cell culture wikipedia , lookup
Cellular differentiation wikipedia , lookup
Organ-on-a-chip wikipedia , lookup
Cell encapsulation wikipedia , lookup
J~URNALOINEUROPHYSIOLOGY Vol. 42, No. 5, September 1979. Yrinted in U.S.A. Ordinal Position of Neurons in Cat Striate Cortex JEAN BULLIER AND GEOFFREY H. HENRY Dqm-tment oj*Physiology, John Curtin School oj’ ikkdicul Nutionul University, Cunhurru, A. C. T. 2601, Austruliu SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS I. The ordinal, or serial, position of different types of striate neurons was assessed from their latencies to electrical stimulation of the optic radiations in paralyzed cats anesthetized with N,O/O, (70/30%). 2. The distribution histogram for response latencies of striate neurons to electrical stimulation in the optic radiations shows three broad peaks. These peaks become sharper if the stimulating site is moved from a low point (OR,) to a high point (OR,) in the optic radiations. It is argued that each peak represents a population of cells lying at a different ordinal, or serial, position within the cortical pathway, i.e., receiving a mono-, di-, or polysynaptic excitatory input from the thalamus. 3. The duration separating the peaks, of the order of 1.0 ms, is then taken as the transmission time for conduction of the nervous impulse from one cortical cell to the next. Confirmatory evidence for this value of the cell-to-cell transmission time comes from the measurement of interspike intervals in cortical cells that responded with multiple spikes to electrical stimulation. 4. From the peaks in the distribution of OR, latencies, boundaries have been established to arrive at an ordinal position for 131 cells classified as S, Sh, C, B, or cells with nonoriented or concentric receptive field (N-O and cone), following the terminology of Henry (14). Four ordinal groups were distinguished: group M comprised cells receiving an exclusive monosynaptic input from the thalamus, group C1+,iconsisted of cells exhibiting a convergence of excitatory inputs, one of which was monosynaptic; group D was composed of cells in which the lowest order input was disynaptic. Cells were placed in group P if they were driven from the thalamus through three or more 0022-3077/79/0000-OOOO$O 1.25 Copyright 0 1979 The Rcscurc~h, Austruliun synapses. Examination of the distribution of different cell types for these various ordinal groups revealed that no particular cell type was exclusively associated with a single ordinal position. In particular, a large proportion of S-, Sh-, C-, and N-O and cone cells showed a monosynaptic input from the thalamus (groups M and Cl+,)). 5. For the three classes of cell, S, Sh, and C, with sufficient numbers in groups MY cl+n~ and D, a search was made for differences in response properties associated with the various ordinal groups. The properties examined included ocular dominance, direction selectivity, orientation specificity, and receptive-field size. With the exception of the C-cell, which showed a difference of direction selectivity in groups M and Cl+,), we observed no systematic changes in the response properties with differences in ordinal position. 6. For most cell types in cat striate cortex there are examples of neurons receiving a monosynaptic input. It is difficult to reconcile these findings with a hierarchical model for the processing of visual information. It is proposed that these cells are first-order elements of different streams running in parallel within the cortex and that this multiplicity of streams may be associated with the diversity of outputs leaving the striate cortex. INTRODUCTION By 1965, the pioneering studies of Hubel and Wiesel (20-22) had led to the formulation of a model, later described as a hierarchical scheme, to explain the stages followed in the processing of information in the visual cortex of the cat. In its original form, simple cells of area 17, representing the first stage in the processing sequence, received a direct input from the dorsal lat- American Physiological Society 1251 1252 J. BULLIER AND era1 geniculate nucleus (dLGN) and, in turn, provided the input to complex cells. From area 17 the axons of complex cells carried the information into areas 18 and 19, where they terminated on hypercomplex cells. Since 1965, additional classes or subclasses have been recognized in the functional cell types found in area 17 of the cat. Current attempts at modeling should allow for the possibility that these new classes contribute to the function of area 17. In the briefest summary, new subclasses have been suggested for simple cells (3, 13) and complex cells (13, 16, 28), a new variety of hypercomplex cell has been found in area 17 (3, S), and cells with nonoriented or concentric receptive fields have also been described in area 17 (15). In the present study we have reexamined the sequence of information processing in the cat striate cortex. After allowing for a greater diversity of cell types than envisaged in the hierarchical scheme, an attempt was made to class striate neurons according to their ordinal or serial position in the processing sequence. The latency of orthodromic activation by electrical stimulation at various sites along the retinostriate pathway was employed to assess the ordinal position of striate neurons. This method has been used previously by a number of authors, but generally their analysis was restricted to a decision on whether the cell under examination was receiving a direct or indirect input from the optic radiations. Even so, there is diversity in the conclusions drawn from their results; Denney, Baumgartner, and Adorjani (7) saw their results as supporting the hierarchical model, whereas others (15, 19, 29, 30, 32) have shown that a proportion of both simple and complex cells are potential recipients of a monosynaptic input. The restriction of the earlier studies to the examination of monosynaptically driven cells came from the difficulty in assigning ordinal positions from latency distributions where the peaks representing each ordinal group were broad and overlapping. Only the monosynaptically driven cells responding with short latency to electrical stimulation could be separated, while groups of cells of higher ordinal position could not be identified with confidence. We have attempted to G. H. HENRY overcome this problem through a reduction of the scatter in the latency distribution. This was done by shortening the signal-conduction time in the optic radiations by placing the stimulating electrode as close as possible to the cortex. METHODS General preparution visual stimulation und A detailed account of the methods have been published elsewhere (15) and only a brief description is presented here. Experiments were performed on adult cats. Under halothane anesthesia, cannulas were inserted into the trachea, the cephalic vein, and the femoral artery. Bilateral cervical sympathectomy was performed to diminish eye movements (25). At the end of the surgical preparation and after injecting the wounds with long-lasting local anesthetic, the animals were paralyzed and artificially respired with a mixture of nitrous oxide and oxygen (70/ 30). The blood gasses were monitored during the experiment and sodium bicarbonate was given to compensate for metabolic acidosis, which was frequently found to be present. The level of ventilation was adjusted to keep the partial pressure of Co, in the blood at 30 mm Hg (10, 17). The corneas were protected with plastic contact lenses and correcting lenses, determined by retinoscopy, were added to focus the eyes on a tangent screen at 1 m. Artificial pupils were centered on the visual axis with the aid of an indirect ophthalmoscope. Single units were isolated with tungsten-in-glass microelectrodes (23). The properties of the receptive field of each isolated unit were qualitatively assessed by using bars or spots of light of variable size and a variety of black targets. For each unit, the minimum discharge field (1, 3) was plotted from the responses to light and dark edges moved in either direction and the response to flashing light was studied by flashing small spots or long bars of light in different regions of the receptive field. The size of the receptive field was measured by taking the total area of the minimum discharge field. The ocular-dominance grouping followed the ratings established by Hubel and Wiesel (21); direction selectivity was estimated by listening to the response of the cell to an optimal stimulus moved in both directions and was rated from 50 (nondirection selective) to 100 (direction selective). The orientation selectivity was recorded as the range of effective stimulus orientations for moving stimuli. A pulse counter measured the spontaneous activity and the upper stimulus velocity at which the cell ceased to fire (cutoff point) was graded on a scale from 1 to 5. ORDINAL POSITION Neurons were classified as S, Sh, C, B, or as cells with nonoriented (N-O) or concentric (cone) receptive fields according to criteria laid down previously (14, 15). In summary, a cell was classed as possessing a nonoriented receptive field if light and dark bars, set at any orientation, produced similar responses when moved across the receptive field. These cells also responded with an on/off discharge to small flashing spots positioned anywhere within the receptive field. Cortical cells with concentric receptive fields had similar response properties to cells with concentric receptive fields found in the dLGN and the retina. The cortical cells were differentiated from dLGN axons only by the fact that they could be transynaptically activated from stimulating electrodes in the optic radiations. The classification of neurons showing orientation specificity (S, Sh, C, B) was based primarily on the spatial disposition of discharge regions to moving and flashing stimuli. For S-cells the discharge regions for light and dark moving edges did not coincide spatially, although some degree of overlap was’ often observed. If the cell was not direction selective this feature held for both directions of movement. When the flashing light response was reliable enough, a similar displacement was also observed in the region of onand off-discharge. By contrast, the discharge regions for moving edges in the receptive fields of B- and C-cells overlapped exactly and the on- and off-discharge regions to stationary flashing bars also coincided spatially. B-cells were distinguished from C-cells by their smaller receptive fields, lower spontaneous activity, more regular discharge to moving bars, and preference for more slowly moving stimuli (for further details see Ref. 16). Cells with the suffix h displayed the hypercomplex property so that &-cells had the same basic properties as S-cells but differed in that the elongation of a moving bar stimulus beyond its optimal length resulted, on most presentations, in the total suppression of the response. Electrical stimulation Three pairs of stimulating electrodes were positioned, one in the optic chiasm and two in the optic radiations. The electrical activation was provided by current pulses (O-10 mA) of variable duration (50-200 ps). The stimulating electrodes were made of tungsten wire isolated with glass, with approximately 1 mm of wire protruding from the glass. The optic chiasm electrode was positioned at Horsley-Clarke AP 13.0- 15.0; ML 1.0 (approximate depth 23.0-24.0 mm). The final depth was adjusted according to the strength of the evoked potential elicited by repeated flashes of light and recorded through the stimulating electrode. OF STRIATE NEURONS 1253 L 4 FIG. 1. Groups of spikes elicited by electrical stimulation at the site OR,, demonstrating the variability in the latency of firing in 20 stimulus repetitions. Cell with nonoriented receptive field in lamina 4. Scale: vertical bar, 0.75 mV, negative upward; horizontal bar, 1.0 ms. Arrow marks stimulus onset. The first pair of stimulating electrodes in the optic radiations (OR,) was positioned just above the dLGN (H-C AP 5-6; ML 10; approximate depth, 10 mm). The second pair of optic radiation electrodes (OR,), with a longer protruding length of tungsten wire (4 mm), was positioned close to the cortex, a few millimeters from the recording electrode. The positions of the stimulating electrodes in the optic chiasm and at OR, were checked by gross dissection and the position of the OR, electrode was verified in the histological slides prepared for electrode reconstruction. The criteria for distinguishing antidromic from orthodromic activation were conventional (2, 1 I), and particular emphasis was placed on the collision test to determine antidromic activation. In a few cases, collision was observed in cells that showed some jitter in their latency to electrical stimulation (of the order of 0.3-0.4 ms). This amount of jitter is unusual in antidromic activation with short latencies (less than 2 ms) (31) and these cases were, therefore, eliminated from the data because of the uncertainty of the type of activation. Geniculate axons isolated in the cortex were easily distinguished from cortical cells by the constancy of their latency and the aptitude to follow high frequencies of repetitive stimulation (up to 100 Hz). The activation by electrical stimulation was presumed to be orthodromic when the unit failed to satisfy the conditions for antidromic or axontype activation. Typically, the jitter in orthodromic latency was of the order of 0.1-0.5 ms for the first spike but was much larger for later spikes (Fig. 1). The latency measures used J. BULLIER AND G. H. HENRY throughout were the minimum latency between the stimulus artifact and the foot of the A potential (2). In early experiments the latency was measured by examining the response pattern on a conventional oscilloscope, but in later experiments a Polaroid camera or a storage oscilloscope was used in making the measurement. No difference was observed among the measurements obtained with camera, storage oscilloscope, or with conventional oscilloscope, and we pooled the data obtained using all three methods. The minimal latency was obs&d to vary by as much as 0.2 ms, depending on the strength of stimulation or whether the elicited spike was preceded by a spontaneously occurring spike. In consequence,the precision of the latency measurement cannot be expected to be better than 0.2 ms and this value is certainly higher in the case of polysynaptic pathways. first-, second-, and third-order neurons. From the positions of these peaks one could deduce characteristic values of latencies for different ordinal positions and use these values to assign an ordinal position to each cell in the striate cortex. However, as shown in Fig. 2A, the histogram of latencies to electrical stimulation from site OR, early in the optic radiations does not show narrow, but rather broad peaks with a large number of values between each peak. The variability in conduction times for different dLGN axons can be estimated from the latencies to OR, stimulation of geniculate axon terminals isolated by the recording electrode in the cortex. The distribution of OR, latenties for these axons is shown in Fig. 2B where each axon was classified as BS (brisk sustained) or BT (brisk transient) according to its receptive-field properties (6). The scatter of latencies is similar to the interval between each peak in the histogram of Fig. 2A and the variability of conduction time for different axons in the optic radiations appears to be an important contributing factor in broadening the peaks in Fig. 2A. We have attempted to reduce the effects of variability in the conduction time of the optic radiations by using a second stimulating electrode (OR,) positioned high in the optic radiations, just below the striate cortex. Because of the shorter distance, the RESULTS In order to determine the ordinal position of a cortical cell with respect to its thalamic input, it would first seem appropriate to generate a frequency histogram of response latencies to electrical stimulation at a site close to the thalamus. If the conduction time in the optic radiations and the transmission time from one cell to the next showed a small degree of scatter, the histogram would show sharp peaks representing populations of A 3Or q q BT axon BS axon I 4 OR, 2. stimulation FIG. Histogram of latencies for all spikes elicited at site OR ,I. BT: brisk transi ent; BS: brisk latency ( ms ) in cortical su stained. cells (A) and in dLGN axons (B) by electrical ORDINAL POSITION OF STRIATE variability of the conduction time from OR, to the cortex is smaller than that from OR,. This reduced variability is apparent in Fig. 3, which presents the scatter plot of OR, latencies versus OR, latencies for nine geniculate axons. The scatter is significantly smaller along the OR, (0.25 ms) than the OR, (0.8 ms) axis. The regression line fitted to the data points in Fig. 3 has a slope of 0.30 (95% confidence interval, 0.09-0.52) and intercepts the OR, axis at 0.21 ms (95% confidence interval, o-0.42). The value of the slope is consistent with the ratio of the distances of the OR, and OR, electrodes from the cortex (i.e., approximately 4 mm from OR, and 14 mm for OR,). By contrast, the deviation of the intercept of the regression line away from the point of origin does not carry statistical significance and, therefore, does not require interpretation. The reduced variability observed in the latencies of dLGN axons to OR, stimulation has led us to adopt this measure for assessing the ordinal position of cortical cells. However, in doing so, there are two possible sources of error. The first is that the OR, stimulating electrode may activate excitatory afferents to the cortex other than those arising from the thalamus; and second, for cells with a convergence of mono- and polysynaptic inputs, there is the possibility that the two radiation electrodes (OR, and OR,) activate the cell through different synaptic pathways. The likelihood of these errors was reduced by selecting only the cells for which similar spike patterns were elicited by both OR, and OR, stimulation. Also, cells were rejected if the latency of homologous spikes (from OR, and OR,) differed by more than 1 ms. This led to a slight reduction in 1.07 x BT axon c 0 BS axon - I 0.5 OR, I 1.0 latency I 1.5 J 2.0 ( ms ) FLG. 3. Scattergram of OR, latency versus OR, latency for dLGN axons isolated in striate cortex. Regression line has slope of 0.3 * 0.22 ms and intercept at 0.21 5 0.21 ms (95% conf intervals; Y = 0.78). NEURONS 1255 -7 n r 30 =m 82 % b $1 2 r0 r, 0 1 2 OR2 latency 3 4 5 ( ms ) FIG. 4. Histogram of latencies for all spikes elicited in cortical cells by electrical stimulation at OR, site. Arrows mark criterion levels for assigning ordinal position (see Table 1). the sample sizes. Of the 166 cells that could be activated from OR1, only 27 failed to be activated from OR2, and among the 139 cells that were activated from both OR, and ORz, in only 4 units did the OR, and OR, latenties differ by more than 1 ms. Figure 4 presents the histogram of the spikes elicited from OR, stimulation for cells that satisfied the conditions stated above. The reduced variability in conduction time results in sharper peaks in Fig. 4 than the equivalent histogram of OR, latencies (Fig. 2A). The modes of these peaks occur at 1.2-1.4,2.2-2.6, and 3.8-4.0 ms. The presence of low troughs between the peaks suggests that each peak is composed of a homogeneous group representing a specific ordinal position but, in order to strengthen this proposition, we have sought to obtain an independent estimate of the transmission time from one cell to the next. Trunsmission time To obtain an estimate of the cell-to-cell transmission time, we have examined the interspike intervals of the spikes generated by electrical stimulation when a single shock elicited several spikes in the cell’s response (Fig. 1). If each of the multiple spikes arises from a signal passing through a different sequence of cortical cells, then the interspike interval should be an integer multiple of the cell-to-cell transmission time. To demonstrate first that the multiple spikes arose from different inputs to the cell, we varied the strength of electrical stimulation. Apart from some minor variation in latencies for each spike as the strength of suprathreshold 1256 J. BULLIER AND G. H. HENRY dence of convergence of direct inputs we concluded that, in general, such convergence is not manifested in the occurrence of multiple spikes. Hence, in preparing the distribution of interspike intervals in Fig. 6, we included all cells that responded with multiple spikes to stimulation at OR1, irrespective of whether a similar pattern of firing could be elicited from stimulation at ORz. Peaking in Fig. 6 occurs at l-O/l .2 ms and at 2.0 ms, suggesting that 1.0 ms is an appropriate estimate for cell-to-cell transmission time. In arriving at these values, however, some caution should be exercised in interpreting the maximum point of the first peak in the distributions in Fig. 6 since the refractory state of the cell immediately followInterspike interval from OR, ( ms ) ing the first spike is likely to lead to truncation of the peak at intervals less than 0.8 ms. FIG. 5. Scattergram of interspike intervals for mulThe value of 1 ms for the cell-to-cell transtiple spikes elicited in cortical cells by stimulation at mission time drawn from Fig. 6 agrees well sites OR, and OR,. Line of unit slope represents constant interspike intervals from both stimulation sites. with the value of interpeak separation (1.0 ms between peaks 1 and 2) in the histogram of OR* latencies shown in Fig. 4. This stimulation was changed, the general pat- strengthens our claim that each peak in the tern of response was independent of the histogram corresponds to a different ordinal strength of stimulation. Then we also dem- position. It is interesting to note that the onstrated that the multiple spikes did not re- cell-to-cell transmission time appears to be sult from the convergence of fast and slowly longer than the transmission time between conducting afferents onto the cell. This was the dLGN axons and the cells represented done by showing that the interspike interval in the first peak of the histogram in Fig. 4. remained the same for stimulation either at This axon-to-cell transmission time can be OR, or OR,. A comparison of interspike in- estimated by taking the difference between tervals for stimulation at OR, and OR, is the mode of the first peak (an OR, value of presented in Fig. 5. For the interspike inter1.2- 1.4 ms) and the mean OR, latency for val to be invariant with the site of stimuladLGN axons (0.54 ms; SD. 0.1 ms; y2 = 9). tion, the points should fall on the line of unit This gives a value of approximately 0.75 ms slope passing through the point of origin. (0.66-0.86 ms), which is clearly smaller Most of the data points in Fig. 5 are within than the 1 ms estimated for the cell-to-cell 0.2 ms (the estimated error of latency meas- transmission time. urement) of the reference line and the greatest deviation is 0.4 ms. In the case of convergence of direct inputs on these cells, the interspike interval would be diminished by much more than 0.4 ms in going from stimulation at OR, to stimulation at ORz. With the exclusion of the convergence of direct inputs, the most likely explanation for multiplicity is that each spike comes through a different synaptic pathway. If so, the cellInterspike interval from OR, ( ms ) to-cell transmission time may be derived from the separation of peaks in the distribuFIG. 6. Histogram of interspike intervals for spikes tion of interspike intervals. Since the sample elicited in cortical cells by electrical stimulation at site of points plotted in Fig. 5 produced no evi- OR,. ORDINALPOSITION OFSTRIATENEURONS 1257 Derivation oj* ordinal position from TABLE 1. Summary oj’ criteriu for OR, latency rating cells The next step in the analysis is to arrive No. of Spikes, Criteria, at the cutoff points on the OR, latency axis to OR, for Cells With0 < OR, (Fig. 4) marking the boundaries for the Group Stimulation - OR, G 1.0 ms groups of mono-, di- and polysynaptically driven cells. A monosynaptic input from the M 1 only OR, s 1.6 ms or OR, < 1.8 ms thalamus is likely if the OR, latency is less than 1.6 ms. This figure is based on the arguC I+n Several 1 spike from M group, ment that the minimum time for disynaptic other spikes from D or P groups activation would equal the minimum OR, latency for cells (0.8 ms from Fig. 4) plus D 1 or several For first spike: the minimum cell-to-cell transmission time 2.0 s OR, s 2.8 ms (0.8 ms from Fig. 6). Similarly, monosynapP 1 or several For first spike: tic activation is unlikely for OR, latencies 0 R.,u 2 3.2 ms longer than 2 ms. The reasoning here is that the longest OR2 latency for an axon, showing an OR,-OR, latency difference of less We classed the se cells as bei ng no ndriven (ND), with the implicat ion th at the y could than 1 ms, will be smaller than 0.8 ms (Fig. 3). For a spike to be monosynaptically ac- not be driven electrically from the thalamic tivated at 2.0 ms, the axon-to-cell transmis- region. Among the 166 cells that could be sion time would need to be 1.2 ms, a dura- driven from OR1, 35 failed to meet the retion almost double the estimated value. This quirements laid down in Table 1 and could appears unlikely in view of the sharpness of not be ascribed an ordinal rating. Thus, 131 cells were given an ordinal rating and Fig. 7 the first peak in the OR2 distribution. By carrying through the reasoning applied shows how the various types of cells are disabove, it may be concluded with a high de- tributed among the four ordinal groups. The gree of probability that spikes with OR, la- histograms in Fig. 7 record the percentage tencies I) of less than 1.6 ms correspond to of each ordinal type (including the ND a monosynaptic activation, 2) between 2 and group) for the various classes of cell. The different classes of cell (S, Sh, C, B, cells 2.8 ms correspond to disynaptic activation, with nonoriented and concentric receptive 3) greater than 3.2 ms are polysnaptically (more than 2 synapses) driven. In Table 1 we fields) were grouped from their receptivefield properties as described in METHODS. have drawn up a summary of the criteria selected for giving each cell one of four or- Of the population of 223 cells (driven or not dinal ratings: M (monosynaptic input), Cl+,) driven from OR,), 13 cells of uncertain clas(convergence of monosynaptic and other in- sification were not included in Fig. 7. The puts), D (disynaptic input for the first spike), responses of 69 cells were too unreliable to and P (polysynaptic input for the first spike). allow classification, and these are referred In the case of seven cells, the conditions for to as NF (no field) cells and are included in inclusion in the M group were relaxed be- Fig. 7. The high percentage of NF cells (69/ cause, although activation did not occur 258) possibly results from a decision to from ORz, the OR, latency was clearly con- place cells in this category whenever a lack sistent with monosynaptic activation (OR, of responsiveness created doubt about their correct designation. The functional implicalatency < 1.8 ms). tion of a group of celis with a low level of Ordincrl position oj’ dijferen t types oj’ responsiveness is considered in the followstriate neuron ing paper (5). In all, 258 cells in 16 electrode penetraFigure 7 shows clearly that no single class tions in 12 cats were studied for the re- of cell receives exclusively a mono-, di- or sponses to visual and electrical stimulation. POlY s ynaptic input from the thalamus . InOf these, 92 could not be driven with OR, stead , a varie tyofe xampl es were found with stimulation although, in some instances, different ordinal ratings-in each class of cell. cells of this group could be driven from OR,. With the exception that instances of poly- 1258 J. BULLIER AND G. H. HENRY Sh NF N=2A N=69 c N=25 FIG. 7. Percentage distribution among different ordinal groups (Table 1) for cortical cell types. N-O + cone, cells with nonoriented or concentric receptive fields. S, Sh, C, B as described in METHODS. NF, cells not driven bv visual stimulation. ND, Cells not driven by electrical stimulation at OR, site. N indtcates the sample size for each cell type. synaptic input were not registered for S-cells and for cells with nonoriented or concentric receptive fields, there was a full spectrum of ordinal ratings to be found in the various cell classes. However, in some groups-S, C, and the nonoriented or concentric-there is a clear preponderance of monosynaptically driven cells, whereas in the B, S,,, and NF groups, cells with a multisynaptic drive predominate. Almost all cells with concentric or nonoriented receptive fields may be regarded as first-order cortical neurons in that they belong either to the M or C l+n grow. Possibly associated with their first-order character, all cells with concentric and nonoriented receptive fields could be driven electrically (see Fig. 7). In sharp contrast, only 14% of the cells that could not be visually driven (NF cells) were driven by electrical stimulation. In general terms, classes with a high incidence of monosynaptic drive also have a high proportion of cells that can be driven electrically (e.g., S, C, cells with nonoriented or concentric receptive fields), whereas classes with a low incidence of monosynaptic drive (B and S, cells) have a higher proportion of cells that were nonresponsive to electrical stimulation. Even the visually unresponsive cells (NF class) may conform to this pattern since there is some evidence that they are higher order cortical neurons belonging to laminae 2 and 3, which do not receive a direct thalamic input (5). Injlurnce oj’ ordinal position on response properties within each cluss For three classes of cell-S, Sh, and Cthere was a large enough sample to search for within-class variations that might be dependent on the ordinal position of the cell. For this comparison of response properties, cells of each class were subdivided into M, C I+,,, and D subgroupings. OCULAR DOMINANCE. In none of the three classes of cell was the pattern of ocular dominance (21) found to vary with ordinal position. In particular, a growth in binocularity (ocular dominance groups 3-5) was not observed to be associated with an increase in the ordinal position of the cell. DIRECTION SELECTIVITY. Within the S and S,, classes there was a similar proportion of direction-selective units in all ordinal groups. However, in the C category a decrease in the direction-selective character of the response was observed in going from cells receiving a purely monosynaptic input (M) to those ORDINAL POSITION OF STRIATE receiving a convergence of mono- and polysynaptic inputs (C,,,). This variation is apparent in the distribution histograms in Fig. 8 where direction selectivity is plotted on a scale ranging from 50 (not direction selective) to 100 (totally direction selective). Within each ORIENTATION SPECIFICITY. cell class no significant difference was observed in orientation specificity, recorded by assessing the range of effective orientations, for cells of different ordinal position. RECEPTIVE-FIELD SIZE. Similarly no significant size difference was apparent among the ordinal groupings within each cell class when the excitatory region of the receptive field was plotted as a minimum response field. SPATIAL DISPOSITION OF LIGHTEDGE DISCHARGE REGIONS. AND i M Group Cl+” Group D FIG. 9. Distribution of S-cells of different ordinal types of groups CM. C,+n, D: Table 1) among different recepttve-field organization. *, direction-selective discharge; s, not direction-selective discharge; L, lightedge discharge; D, dark-edge discharge: L,D, hghtedge discharge encountered ahead of dark-edge dtscharge: 0, miscellaneous combination of light- and dark-edge discharge. M “r <Group Grow DARK- For S cells in the striate cortex the mapping of the receptive field is complicated by the fact that the spatial disposition of discharge regions varies when the moving stimulus is changed from a light to a dark edge. The hypothesis has been advanced, for the monkey at least, that classes of cells based on the disposition of light- and dark-edge discharge regions represent different ordinal positions in the striate cortex (27). To test this possibility in the cat, S-cells have been grouped into variGroup 1259 NEURONS Cl+, “1 ous combinations of light- (L) and dark- (D) edge discharge region disposition for the preparation of the frequency histograms for M, G+n> and D groups in Fig. 9. The most striking feature of the results in Fig. 9 is the occurrence of examples of all subgroups of S-cell in the monosynaptically driven (M) group. In consequence, grouping based on the spatial disposition of receptive-field discharge regions does not appear to reflect differences in the ordinal position of the cell. Weight is added to this conclusion in that S-cells belonging to the C,,, and D groups are also distributed among several subclasses in Fig. 9. DISCUSSION ’ 8. Distribution of C-cells of different ordinal groups (M and C,,,,) among the direction selectivity groups: 50, not direction selective; 100, totally direction selective. FIG. Ordinul position and intrrcellular trunsmission time In interpreting the three peaks in the frequency distribution histogram of OR, laten- 1260 J. BULLIER AND ties in Fig. 4, we have concluded that each peak represents a group of cells occurring at a different ordinal position in the intracortical pathway. This interpretation gained support from multiple-spike recordings with the finding of a cell-to-cell transmission time of 1.0 to 1.2 ms -a duration similar to that of the interpeak interval in Fig. 4. However, this time for cell-to-cell transmission is appreciably longer than the average synaptic delay (0.4 ms) in the mammalian nervous system (9) and one may question our estimate on the grounds that it is, in reality, composed of transmission through several synapses. Any doubt of this nature seems to be dispelled, however, by the findings of Toyama et al. (33). Recording intracellularly from cat striate cortex, Toyama and his colleagues distinguished two types of EPSP -an early and a late one-evoked in cortical cells by electrical stimulation in the optic radiations. After stimulation in the dLGN the difference between the mean latencies of the two types of EPSP (early and late) was approximately 1.1 ms-a value very similar to our estimate of the cell-tocell transmission time. Moreover, if a delay of 0.2-0.3 ms is allowed for the generation of the spike after the onset of an EPSP, then the mean value of the OR, latency for monosynaptically (M and C,,, groups: mean 1.6 ms; SD 0.24 ms; n = 92) and disynaptically (D group: mean 2.81 ms; SD 0.31 ms; n = 31) induced spikes in our results agrees almost exactly with the mean latency for the early and late EPSPs in Toyama et al’s study. A value of 1.0 ms, therefore, appears to be an accurate estimate of the time for cell-to-cell transmission within the striate cortex. The discrepancy between the cell-to-cell transmission time and the time for synaptic delay presumably reflects the fact that the transmission time consists of other elements in addition to synaptic delay. Components such as conduction time of the impulse along the presynaptic axon and also the interval in the second neuron between the onset of the EPSP and the generation of the spike must contribute to the total transmission time. There are reasons to believe that the conduction time in the axon makes a major contribution to the transmission time. Cortical cells have axons that are generally unmyelinated and are of a fine caliber, equal G. H. HENRY to or less than 1 ,urn in diameter in Golgi preparations (unpublished observations). Assuming a conduction velocity of 1 m/s in unmyelinated fibers of 1 pm diameter (12, 18, 26), an impulse would take 0.5 ms to travel 500 pm, a realistic distance for intralaminar conduction in the axons and axon collaterals of striate neurons (15, 16). Such a value together with a synaptic delay of 0.4 ms would account for most of the cell-to-cell transmission time. Our observation that the mean axon-tocell (from optic radiation to first-order neuron) transmission time is shorter than the mean cell-to-cell transmission time may be related to the comparatively small distance traveled by the unmyelinated terminal twigs of dLGN axons. For most of the dLGN axons infiltrated with horseradish peroxidase in a companion study (5), myelination ceased when axon diameters reached approximately 1 pm within the lamina of termination and in close proximity (within 250 pm) to the receiving cell. Interpretdon of response patterns The accuracy of our results depends on the exclusion of errors that may arise from the misinterpretation of response patterns. In particular, to avoid distortion of the histogram of OR, latencies in Fig. 4 it is essential to know if every spike elicited by OR, or OR, stimulation corresponds to an excitatory input from the thalamus. This would not be the case if the spike resulted from antidromic activation traveling either directly along the cell’s axon or indirectly via a collateral from the efferent axon of a neighboring cell. Direct antidromic activation was systematically excluded by applying the collision test (11) when latency measurements showed a variability of less than 0.2 ms. To rule out the possibility of activation of collaterals of efferent axons of other cortical cells, the cell was tested for orthodromic activation from the optic chiasm. Signals induced from the optic chiasm only activated dLGN cells and left unstimulated the efferent axons arising from the cortex. This test was applied to 86 of the 95 cells apparently receiving a monosynaptic input from the thalamus (i.e., groups M and C,,,). In every case among these 86 units, the OX latency was consistent with disynaptic activation from the optic chiasm (one synapse ORDINAL POSITION OF STRIATE in dLGN and one in the cortex). We therefore concluded that the signal from OR, or OR, seldom, if ever, passed to the cell via the excitatory collaterals of efferent axons. The apparent rarity of cell activation via axon collaterals seems to indicate that the excitatory action of collaterals is too weak to generate a spike in the postsynaptic cell and, therefore, the axon collaterals of pyramidal cells in laminae 5 and 6 may not provide a path for sequential processing. Another possible source of distortion of the peaks in the histogram presented in Fig. 4 comes from the wide range of conduction velocities of dLGN axons. Although we have demonstrated that the use of OR, electrode reduces the scatter due to conduction time in the optic radiations, it is worthwhile to consider how much variability in the distribution of OR2 latencies can still be attributed to conduction in the optic radiations. A scattergram of OR, latency versus OR,OR, latency difference for cells receiving a monosynaptic (groups M and C,,,,) input prepared to test this point, showed little sign of correlation between OR, and ORI-ORZ, suggesting that the OR, latency is largely independent of the conduction velocity within the optic radiations. Ordinul position und cdl types Examination of the distribution of different cell types at various ordinal stages in the processing sequence (Fig. 7) reveals that no particular cell type is exclusively associated with a single ordinal position. On the contrary, for almost all cell types, there are examples of cells with almost identical response properties occurring at all ordinal positions. This was a surprising result since we had anticipated modifications in the receptive-field properties with progression along the processing sequence. We were prompted, therefore, to look for possible sources of error in allocating the ordinal position. First, had the cell failed to respond with a monosynaptic spike because the electrode was inappropriately positioned? In this case a monosynaptic input would be overlooked and an incorrect classification given to the cell if it otherwise met the criteria for disynaptic activation. However, it is unlikely that both OR, and OR2 electrodes would be out of position and that a spike absent from stimulation at one site NEURONS 1261 would also be absent from the other. Of the 139 cells that could be activated from both OR, and ORB, only one cell showed an indication, in the form of an additional spike in going from stimulation at OR, to ORz, that it should be regarded as having a lower ordinal position. Indeed, the majority of cells- 119 of the 139-showed exactly the same pattern of spikes when stimulated at OR, and OR,. Then we also considered the possibility that a monosynaptic excitatory input was masked by inhibition traveling in a more rapidly conducting afferent pathway than excitation. However, intracellular recordings of cortical cells suggest that they receive inhibitory activation disynaptically from the thalamus (24, 33, 34). As a result, inhibition is unlikely to precede monosynaptic excitation unless it travels much faster over a long afferent pathway. By diminishing the distance of the afferent pathway with the positioning of the OR, electrode, we have excluded this possibility. While the reasoning above eliminates the chance of overlooking a monosynaptic input, there is some likelihood that a polysynaptic input may have been missed for cells placed in the M or monosynaptic category. The disynaptically activated inhibition, referred to above, may arrive soon enough to mask excitation taking a di- or polysynaptic path to the cell. In consequence, in cells with evidence of inhibitory activity in their visual responses, such as Sand &-cells, there may be more convergence of mono- with di- and polysynaptic inputs (Cl+n) than indicated in our results. Finally, in reviewing the effectiveness of our methods, consideration should be given to a shortcoming associated with electrical stimulation. There is no indication, particularly in cells receiving a convergence of inputs, of the functional significance of one input relative to another. For example, for cells receiving a mono- and disynaptic input in the C,,,, group, a principal excitatory input cannot be recognized from a modulating one and it should not be presumed that the monosynaptic input is the key determinant of the cell’s excitatory response. Bearing in mind the points made above there still can be little question that, for a number of cell types (S, Sh, and C), there are some examples of cells with a monosynaptic drive while there were others with a di- J. BULLIER AND synaptic input. In those instances where groups of cells receiving a mono- or disynaptic input existed within a single cell type, we could not find any evidence of systematic differences in the receptive-field properties associated with an increase in ordinal position. This is contrary to the central theme of the hierarchical model, which proposes that receptive-field properties increase in the specificity of their stimulus requirements with progression along the processing sequence. If the similarity in receptive-field properties for cells in different ordinal position does not result simply from an inadequate assessment, then it is necessary to review the role of striate neurons as relaying elements in a chain of excitatory events. It is possible, for example, that a disynaptitally activated cell replicates the response of a monosynaptically driven cell because it is acting as an interneuron about to invert the excitatory response into an inhibitory one. It is also possible that replication occurs when, for reference purposes, the response pattern of a monosynaptically driven cell is reproduced in cells, perhaps in other laminae, lying off the main path taken by the processing sequence. Under these circumstances the role of the disynaptically driven cell remains uncertain since we cannot tell if it lies on the main processing pathway or along a secondary side band. From our results, another difficulty for the concept of serial processing arises in the large number of cell types that appear to receive a monosynaptic input from the thalamus. We believe this diversity of monosynaptically driven neurons cannot be ascribed to an inappropriate classification based on differences in receptive-field properties. There is now sufficient evidence to suggest that B- and C-cells, at least, are performing different functional roles from Sand &,-cells and from cells with nonoriented and concentric receptive fields. It is possible, as mentioned above, that the mono- G. H. HENRY synaptic drive does not come from the principal input to the cell but from one which exerts only a modulating influence. However, this seems unlikely in the majority of S-, &-cells, and cells with nonoriented or concentric receptive field in laminae 4 or 6 where the principal drive would come as a direct input from the dLGN. It is more difficult to exclude the possibility that a monosynaptic input to C-cells has only a modulating influence since the majority of C-cells lie in lamina 5 (15), which does not receive a direct input from the dLGN and commonly receive a convergence of inputs. Even allowing for such a contingency, we are still faced with a diversity of first-order neurons in the cat striate cortex. This finding may not be so surprising when consideration is given to the multiplicity of efferents leaving the striate cortex. When a structure like the dLGN appears to send at least three efferent streams (X, Y, and W) to a single destination, the striate cortex, we can begin to envisage the multiplicity of streaming that must occur within the striate cortex itself to generate projections to a variety of cortical and subcortical centers. In light of these considerations and because of the wide variety of monosynaptically driven striate neurons, we have undertaken two additional studies in this series. The first examines the relationship that cortical processing streams may have with the different divisions of dLGN input (4) and the second, the laminar distribution of cells occurring at different levels of processing in different cortical streams (5). ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We express our thanks to Ken Collins for technical assistance, Bryson Easton for histological preparations, and Jan Livingstone and Josephine Anderson for secretarial assistance. Received 26 December 2 April 1979. 1978; accepted in final form REFERENCES 1. BARLOW, H. B., BLAKEMORE, C., AND PETTIJ. D. The neural mechanism of binocular depth discrimination. J. Yhysiol. Lo~~don 193: 327342, 1967. 2. BISHOP, P. O., BURKE, W., AND DAVIS, R. Single unit recording from antidromically activated optic GREW, radiation neurones. J. Yhysiol. London 162: 432450, 1962. 3. BISHOP, P. 0. AND HENRY, G. H. Striate neurons: receptive field concepts. Itwust. Ophthalmol. 11: 346-354, 1972. 4. BULLIER, J. AND HENRY, G. H. Neural path taken ORDINAL POSITION OF STRIATE by afferent streams in striate cortex of the cat. J. Newophysiol. 42: 1264- 1270, 1979. 5. BULLIER, J. AND HENRY, G. H. Laminar distribution of first-order neurons and afferent terminals in cat striate cortex. J. Newophysiol. 42: 1271- 128 1, 1979. 6. CLELAND, B. G., DUBIN, M. W., AND LEVICK, W. R. Sustained and transient neurones in the cat’s retina and lateral geniculate nucleus. J. Physiol. London 217: 473-496, 1971. 7. DENNEY, D., BAUMGARTNER, G., AND ADORJANI, C. Responses of cortical neurones to stimulation of the visual afferent radiations. Exp. Bruin Rccs. 6: 265-272, 21. B. Hypercomplex cells in the cat’s striate Itwest. Ophthulmol. 11: 355-356, 1972. 9. ECCLES, J. C. The Physiology oj’Synup.ws. Berlin: Springer, 1964, p. 37-43. 10. FINK, B. R. AND SCHOOLMAN, A. Arterial blood acid-base balance in unrestrained waking cats. Proc. SW. Exp. Biol. Med. 112: 328330, 1963. 11. FULLER, J. H. AND SCHLAG, J. D. Determination of antidromic excitation by the collision test: problems of interpretation. Bruin Rus. 112: 283-298, 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 1976. 12. GASSER, H. S. Unmedullated dorsal root ganglia. J. Gen. fibers Physiol. originating 33: 651-690, in 1950. C. Laminar differences in receptive field 13. GILBER-I‘, properties of cells in cat primary visual cortex. J. Physiol. London 268: 391-421, 1977. 14. HENRY, G. H. Receptive field classes of cells in the striate cortex of the cat. Bruin Rcs. 133: l-28, 1977. 15. HENRY, G. H., HARVEY, A. R., AND LUND, J. S. The afferent connections and laminar distribution of cells in the cat striate cortex. J. Comp. Ncwrol. In press. 16. HENRY, G. H., LUND, J. S., AND HARVEY, A. R. Cells of the striate cortex projecting to the ClareBishop area of the cat. Bruin Rus. 151: 154-158, 1978. D. A. AND MITCHELI-, R. A. Blood gas tensions and acid-base balance in awake cats. J. Appl. Physiol. 30: 434-436, 1971. 18. HODGKIN, A. L. A note on conduction velocity. J. Phy.siol. London 125: 221-224, 1954. 19. HOFFMANN, K.-P. AND SI‘ONE, J. Conduction velocity of afferents to cat visual cortex: a correlation with cortical receptive field properties. Bruin Rcs. 32: 460-466, 1971. 20. HUBEL, D. H. AND WIESEL, T. N. Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat’s striate cortex. J. Phvsiol. London 148: 574-591. 1959. 17. HERBERT, HUBEL, binocular the cat’s 1968. 8. DREHER, cortex. NEURONS 28. D. H. AND WIESEL, T. N. Receptive fields, interaction and functional architecture in visual cortex. J. Physiol. London 160: 106-154, HUBEL, 1962. 229-289, LEVICK, 1965. D. H. AND WIESEL, T. N. Receptive and functional architecture in two nonstriate areas ( 18 and 19) of the cat. J. Ncurophysiol. fields visual 28: W. R. Another tungsten microelectrode. Med. Biol. Eng. 10: 510-515, 1972. LI, C. L., ORTIZ-GOLVIN, A., CHOU, S. N., AND HOWARD, S. Y. Cortical intracellular potentials in response to stimulation of lateral geniculate body. J. Ncwophysiol. 23: 592-601, 1960. RODIECK, R. W., PETTIGREW, J. D., BISHOP, P. O., AND NIKARA, T. Residual eye movements in receptive-field studies of paralyzed cats. Vision Res. 7: 107-I 10, 1967. RUSHTON, W. A. H. A theory of the effects of fibre size in medullated nerve. J. Physiol. London 115: 101-122, 1951. SCHILLER, P. H., FINLAY, B. L., ANDVOLMAN, S. F. Quantitative studies of single-cell properties in monkey striate cortex. I. Spatiotemporal organization of receptive fields. J. Neurophysiol. 39: 1288-1319, SILLITO, 1976. A. M. Inhibitory processes underlying the directional specificity of simple, complex and hypercomplex cells in the cat’s visual cortex. J. Physiol. London 271: 699-720, 1977. W., TRETTER, F., AND CYNADER, M. Or29. SINGER, ganization of cat striate cortex: a correlation of receptive-field properties with afferent and efferent connections. J. Neurophysiol. 38: lOSO- 1098, 1975. 30. STONE, J. AND DREHER, B. Projection of X- and Ycells of the cat’s lateral geniculate nucleus to areas 17 and 18 of visual cortex. J. New-ophysiol. 36: 551-567, 1973. 31. SWADLOW, H. A., WAXMAN, S. G., AND ROSENE, D. L. Latency variability and the identification of antidromically activated neurons in mammalian brain. Exp. Bruin Rcs. 32: 439-443, 1978. 32 TOYAMA, K., MAEKAWA, K., ANDTAKEDA, T. An analysis of neuronal circuitry for two types of visual cortical neurones classified on the basis of their responses to photic stimuli. Bruin Res. 61: 33 395-399, TOYAMA, TOKASHIKI, organization 21: 45-66, WATANABE, 1973. K., MATSUNAMI, K., OHNO, T., AND S. An intracellular study of neuronal in the visual cortex. Exp. Bruin Rcs. 1974. S., KONISHI, M., AND CREU-I‘ZFELDT, 0. Postsynaptic potentials in the cat’s visual cortex following electrical stimulation of afferent pathways. Exp. Bruin. Res. 1: 272-282, 1966.