Download Week 3 Answers - Stephen P. van Vlack

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Neuroeconomics wikipedia , lookup

Verbal Behavior wikipedia , lookup

Behavior analysis of child development wikipedia , lookup

Insufficient justification wikipedia , lookup

Behaviorism wikipedia , lookup

Learning theory (education) wikipedia , lookup

Eyeblink conditioning wikipedia , lookup

Learning wikipedia , lookup

Psychophysics wikipedia , lookup

Psychological behaviorism wikipedia , lookup

Classical conditioning wikipedia , lookup

Operant conditioning wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Stephen van Vlack
Sookmyung Women’s University
Graduate School of TESOL
Human Learning and Cognition
Spring 2017
Week 3 – Answers
Lieberman (2004) Chapter 2 Foundations of classical conditioning
1. What is classical conditioning and why is this important?
Classical conditioning, otherwise known as Pavlovian conditioning, is when an organism
forms a simple association between two previously unrelated stimuli. This association is
claimed to be based on, or at least strongly affected by, contiguity.
To understand this we need to back up a bit and take a look at some more basic ideas that
are at play in the whole idea of classical conditioning.
These are the ideas of:
Stimulus – like the idea of input itself, this is something that we can expect people to react
to. A stimulus is something that elicits a reaction/response.
Response – the reaction to the stimulus/input
Habituation – when people get used to a stimulus over time and fail to react to it
Learning – when a response is seen to occur in reaction to a stimulus and this is not seen
to occur in the natural world. An organism has made an association between two
things on the basis of experience. That is the association is learned (not innate or
natural)
Non-learning - a situation in which things do happen to co-occur (are contiguous) in the
experience of the organism do not become associated. There is no change in the
organism’s behavior despite what occurs or is contiguous in the environment.
Pavlovian conditioning is important in the study of learning because it shows that organisms
can LEARN to form associations between two things that are not causally connected in the
natural world and, further, these associations are discernable though the behavior of the
organism. We start by looking at the different types of stimuli and the responses that can be
generated from them.
One of the stimuli is important to the organism and is called one of two names. The
general name for this type of response is an orienting stimulus. If this same orienting
stimulus is considered from the perspective of Pavlovian conditioning it is called an
unconditioned stimulus (US). This type of stimulus is often though of as being evolutionary or
natural because this type of stimulus evokes a response that may keep a person or animal
alive in a dangerous situation. For example, if the wind blows in your eyes you will react by
squinting/closing your eyes or turning your face away from the wind (to avoid getting dust in
your eyes). So the wind is an orienting or unconditioned stimulus. The response to an US (in
this case the wind) is seen as being natural and thus not learned from the perspective of
classical conditioning. It is called an orienting response or an Unconditioned Response (UR)
and it is not learned. It is simply natural based on our physiology.
The other type of stimulus is called a conditioned stimulus (CS). The association made
between a conditioned stimulus and a response, on this view, must be learned, because it is
not something that occurs naturally. Additionally, there is a demonstrable change in behavior
as a result of an association having been made. For example, a startle reaction to a tulip is
seen as being a conditioned response to a conditioned stimulus because in the natural
world this type of stimulus does not typically lead to this type of response. Tulips are not in
the normal order of things dangerous. The startle reaction to a tulip must have been learned
under a specific set of conditions.
In Pavlovian conditioning the association of a CS to a CR is made possible by a US. The
organism will have transferred the natural or unconditioned response to the unconditioned
stimulus to what is now seen as a conditioned stimulus even though prior to the
experimental trials using temporally controlled exposure they were not at all related. In a
nutshell, this is what classical conditioning is. Again, it is used to show that responses can
be learned and as a response to what is found incidentally in the environment.
2. What is extinction and how does it work?
Extinction is an important step in understanding learning because it shows us that learning is
not just a result of a simple association being made between two contiguous things.
Learning is not static - it changes. Extinction, then, is the eradication of the conditioned
response due to breaking the co-occurrence of the unconditioned stimulus and the
conditioned stimulus. An interesting thing about extinction (and this gives us valuable insight
on learning and forgetting in general) is that the effects of extinction are only temporary so
long as the pattern of co-occurrence is reinstated at a later date. This confirms what we
said last week about the theory that once learned things are never completely forgotten or
truly lost though they might go seemingly dormant for a period of time (for a number of
reasons). In any case, the potential for recovery is always there.
What probably actually happens in an extinction situation is that the costs of retaining or
even recovering the association simply becomes comes too high. This is based on the idea
of neuronal firing. We know that neuronal firing is a process in the brain by which electricity
is entered into a neuron and is disseminated to its synapses. Each synapse requires a
different amount of electricity to fire it, or make it work. The amount of electricity required is
called the threshold level. As a synapse is used more and more often its threshold level will
go down. A low threshold level means the nodes at the end of the neuron’s axon fire more
easily (using less electricity). This also leads to a higher activation level – which also means
the neuron will fire with less effort. So, to a large extent the threshold levels of a neuron’s
synapses are determined by their frequency. If synapses are not used frequently their
threshold levels are going to go up and the longer they are not used the more they will go up
and the more energy it will take to fire those synapses. Once they reach a certain level it
becomes hardly unlikely that these synapses will be fired because they simply cost too
much energy. The brain due to its plasticity will simply find another way around and threshold
levels will therefore continue to go up in what seems to be a rather vicious cycle. This
doesn't mean we can't fire a synapse that has a high threshold level due to lack of use, it
just means that we avoid doing it because it's not efficient. If we need at some point to
actually do that we can but it is going to cost a large amount of energy.
3. How is conditioning related to learning?
Classical conditioning as mentioned in the answer to question 1 is a simple example of what
is called associative learning. This is learning where one learns to associate two previously
unrelated concepts, ideas, and stimuli. It should be clear that we are expanding out from the
simple stimulus-response (s-r) model mentioned in Lieberman (2004) and are importantly
including a stimulus-stimulus (s-s) model here. The key difference between the two is that
the s-r model includes some sort of physical reaction (response) to a stimulus. The s-s
model takes us inside the brain and allows us, by extending out from the simplistic s-r model,
to posit associations that are not just physical but cognitive. In short, the s-s model allows
us to posit associations between concepts and this is something we need if we are to
extend the basic model of classical conditioning to the learning of systems like language.
Although for the purposes of trying to control the process and/or concepts of morality
classical conditioning experiments are usually carried out in a laboratory and with organisms
other than humans, it is possible to see how this can be applied to the outside world and to
the human world in general. Conditioning posits a basic type of learning that takes natural
association just one baby step further. In conditioning we are taking what was learned
through an UR and extending it on to another stimulus that just so happens to co-occur (is
contingent) in the world. Such a type of learning is obviously largely responsible for our
uncanny ability to survive for it means that we can extend our behaviors beyond a simple
flight or fight response initiated by the association of an US and an UR. It may also be the
very underpinning of our ability for reasoning and possibly even language as we shall see.
4. How can classical conditioning be used to explain some language learning phenomenon?
Classical conditioning as mentioned in the answer to question 1 is a simple example of what
is called associative learning. This is learning where one learns to associate two previously
unrelated concepts, ideas, and stimuli. It should be obvious that we are following the
stimulus-stimulus (s-s) model here. As mentioned above, it is the leap from a s-r model to a
s-s model that allows us to bridge into language learning.
The very defining feature of language is its arbitrariness as proposed by de Saussure
(1959); that is the arbitrariness of linguistic structures to their prescribed or adopted
meaning. We would then wonder, if there is no logical reason why some structural units
should be expected to be found adjacent to others (to think so would be ludicrous) then how
did they come to co-occur like they do? How are word forms associated with meanings?
How can a person learn these patterns of association? There must be a type of learning
which supports the rather quick learning of language. Association based on contiguity might
just be the missing link. Think about context. Context is the clue to all learning and those
that can see it and differentiate its parts are the good learners.
Lieberman (2004) Chapter 3 Principles and applications of classical conditioning
1. How does contiguity affect conditioning?
Contiguity refers to the timing of the conditioning. This relates to the intervals between the
presentation of the two stimuli as well as the order of the stimuli. In looking at contiguity the
biggest lesson we need to take from this is that a forward sequence is the most effective.
This probably because a forward sequence would allow the organism to effectively prepare
for the coming unconditioned stimulus (anticipatory priming). It is also very important to
remember that contiguity as a variable in conditioning is very much reliant on the task itself,
the species involved in the experiment, and the type of response expected. All this ties into
brain differences among species and among the different functions as related to the type of
response expected. Environmental differences have an effect. For humans it is also cultural
differences (prior knowledge) that affect the way we evaluate stimuli. If the stimulus is
primary visual in nature then the neurological structure of the optic nerve and how this is
connected to other parts of the brain becomes an important factor. Also, some experiences
are more intense than others, such as food poisoning, and this will have a large effect on
contiguity.
There are several other factors that have a strong effect on conditioning within and despite
contuguity. They are:
Prior exposure - familiarity,
Compound stimuli,
Surprise,
Relevance,
and Inhibition.
Prior exposure is a sword that cuts both ways. Prior exposure in which the two stimuli did cooccur will serve to reinforce or heighten the conditioned response. Prior exposure, however,
in which the two stimuli did not co-occur will increase the chances that a conditioned
response will not occur.
When compound conditioned stimuli are presented several different effects occur depending
on the timing and salience of the conditioned stimulus in relation the unconditioned stimulus.
Most of this is intuitive and, therefore, does not require further explanation here, as are the
effects of surprise and blocking when two or more conditioned stimuli are used.
The amount of relevancy of the two stimulus to each other seems to have a fairly large
effect on not only what will be associated but also on how quickly the association will take
place.
An inhibitory response is one in which the absence of the unconditioned response will cause
a conditioned response. What is interesting for us is that an inhibitory response can only be
generated based on an understanding (knowledge) of the components that go into making
the unconditioned stimulus. In order to know that something will not happen you need to be
able to predict under what conditions it would happen and to know about what elements are
present and what elements are missing.
2. What is instrumental learning and how does it differ from classical conditioning?
Instrumental learning is akin to the idea of preparedness as presented in Lieberman (2004).
Instrumental learning is learning in which a stimulus, response, and the consequences of the
action derived from the first two are associated. This is a based on the simple and
necessary extension of what we observed in classical conditioning to the real world of
consequences and rewards. In instrumental learning the outcome of the newly acquired
behavior (response) is evaluated against a series (possibly) of needs-based criteria. In this
way the consequences have some effect on the association between the first two elements
in the triumvirate (stimulus – response - consequences). While this new dimension brings
behavior theory closer to the real world of cause and effect relationships (trial and error
learning) it also adds a further dimension of complications and differing variables to the
behavior-based model.
The beauty of instrumental learning, and one of the main differences between the
instrumental learning and classical conditioning, is that of control. In instrumental learning
because the consequences are taken into consideration the organism is able to control
actions consciously. This means that once someone has made two types of associations
which are necessary in instrumental learning they will/may actually seek out situations and
engage the world in order to receive a consequence which they would like. This means that
they need to plan. In classical conditioning, on the other hand, the organism is really just a
passive sponge-like creature which observes what happens in the world and under the right
conditions manages unwittingly to associate things which happened to co-occur. There is no
volition or planning, just a response that has no rationale except for contiguity.
We may also take a moment here to make a connection between instrumental learning
and the concepts in socio-cultural theory of artifacts and tools. In fact, it seems that the
learning of artifacts as tools within sociocultural theory is a direct result of instrumental
learning. In essence, we learn what we can do with different things by examining and
evaluating the effect/consequences of the response. In this way we learn the repercussions
of our actions.
3. What is response learning and what is it importance?
The basic idea in response learning is that the specific stimulus is cut out (removed) and the
response exists unto itself and without a stimulus. In effect, what basically happens in
response learning is that the response becomes the stimulus in that that response can initial
new responses as response-response learning. For psychologists, response learning is quite
important because it allows the researcher to shape responses and in doing so they can
actually alter people's behavior particularly when there are problems which need to be fixed
in behavior, e.g. aversion theory. For us, however, response learning is interesting because it
ties in to language itself. Certainly in the real world when people use language they don't
have to wait for a particular linguistic stimulus in order to start talking. If this were the case
then nothing would ever get done because nobody would actually talk first. So, for us to
develop a theory of language learning that is based on basic learning concepts of neuronal
firing helped by behaviorist models then response learning becomes quite important. Again,
the bottom line in response learning is that people will actually respond without a stimulus.
So, for language this would mean people actually start speaking without having to wait for
some sort of linguistic stimulus. People do this because they think they know what the
consequence of their speech is going to be. This is, therefore extremely, basic and is
extremely important for us.
As was mentioned above, what is learned is the connection between not only the stimulus
and the response as we saw in classical conditioning but both of them with the
consequence(s). This is demonstrated by changes in behavior relative to the type and
strength of the consequence. What it really interesting here is that this three-way connection
can be broken. Thus, the stimulus might not be important all the time for response to occur.
Likewise people can respond without being assured of a particular consequence, so the
consequence will eventually be cut off from the whole scheme. By advocating this three-way
type of connection or association, but one which is also flexible, we can allow for a
tremendous amount of variation in behavior based on a very simple type of learning. This is
the type of thing which is necessary for us if we are really going to use this to describe how
language might be learned and actually works in the real world.
4. How do the contingencies of non-reward, punishment, and avoidance work in instrumental
conditioning?
We have already dealt with instrumental learning which is basically all about trial and error
learning in making associations between three different elements; the stimulus, response,
and consequence. Now, we are ready to start looking at us how we can use this information
to condition people (our students) to behave in the way that we want. This means that we
can use this knowledge to reinforce behavior that we like, such as studying and paying
attention in class, but we can also use this information to try to alter behavior that we do not
like, such as running around in class and not paying attention. In addition, there are also
certain linguistic elements or language learning elements which we can also further define
based on the type of information contained in theories of instrumental learning. This week in
particular we are going to focus on trying to alter unwanted behaviors and reinforce better
behaviors after having reprogrammed the unwanted behavior. This is done in response to
what we have already discussed which is basically about reinforcing good or wanted
behavior.
The three main contingencies available to us in the theory of instrumental conditioning in
order to try to change behavior are non-reward, punishment, and avoidance. Starting with the
idea of non-reward we see that it is actually a very simple idea. All this really entails is
simply taking the reward away (or really not providing a reward) for a particular type of
behavior. The expected effect would be that the behavior would stop. For example, I give
you stickers on your homework assignments to some extant as a type of reward for doing
the work. This is a reward because even if you don't do a great job you still get some sort of
sticker. If I were to stop giving you stickers based on your performance on the homework
the behavior of some students might change. You might be less motivated and your drive to
get a sticker would diminish because there simply are no stickers anymore. This seems very
simple and straightforward but in the real world it might not actually be so easy. To start with
we would need to know what the specific rewards are as a result of a particular behavior
and try to remove them. It is not always easy for teachers to see what might comprise an
award for a certain student. For some a punishment such as yelling at them or making them
stand in the corner might actually be a reward (attention, escaping from actual school work).
If the reward is coming from somewhere that's out of our control like some sort of internal
reward then there is actually very little we can do. Research on lab animals has also shown
that even when the reward is removed they still seem to perform the action because they
are used to it. It's a type of habituation where you get used to performing certain actions
regardless of whether the reward is there or not.
Punishment is about trying to stop a certain behavior by changing the consequence from a
positive one to a decidedly negative one. For example if a who child acts up in class is
somehow rewarded for this by getting attention from other students, by getting universal
laughter and an accompanying feeling of power then the teacher needs to find a punishment
which is stronger than the positive consequences. It has been claimed, most notably by
Skinner and Thorndike, that punishment doesn't work, at least not long-term but there have
been many studies which show that punishment does work provided it is given immediately,
consistently, and with a strong force (brutally, so as to eclipse any possible reward). What I
mean by brutally is that it has to be a very strong punishment immediately and always. If the
punishment starts off small and get stronger then the person will simply habituate to the
punishment and it won't have the same effect. There needs to be some element of surprise.
Although punishment does seem to work in some ways there is also a moral dilemma in
punishment. For punishment to work it must be brutal and strong. This often has the effect
of causing aggression among the punished group or person. Also punishment can also be
sending mixed signals in that people still might be getting rewards as their punishment and
some possible positive rewards become associated together. This was Skinner's main
argument. It is, therefore, still important that the punishment really does strongly outweigh
any kind of possible reward. Since we are not always available to punish students in every
instance of behavior and, therefore, cannot control all the rewards this becomes somewhat
of a tricky venture.
In avoidance, the third type of contingency, the behavior itself changes in order to avoid any
kind of consequence. In order for this to happen it would seem to reason of course that the
consequence would have to be negative. So, if we don't want something bad to happen we
do something in advance which will stop that negative consequence. This means that the
behavior itself which would cause the consequence doesn't exist (is simply avoided) and is
replaced by another type of behavior. As we said in class, the typical type of avoidance
behavior that we find in the English language classroom in Korea would simply be silence.
Students are silent because they are avoiding the punishment internal and external that
comes from making mistakes. This punishment could and probably did originally come from
the outside but it also comes from the inside with people that are highly sensitized to it. This
type of silence in behavior probably is also a very strong result of a feeling of helplessness
as we will discuss in the next question.
5. What are some of the main applications of these ideas for us as language teachers?
Punishment
Motivation
There are many applications of these ideas in instrumental learning which we can apply to
the language classroom. Most of these applications relate to the idea of motivation or to
put it into more behaviorist terms drive and incentive. We can regulate drive and incentive by
carefully trying to regulate punishment and reward using the principles presented here.
Based on some of the ideas presented here it would seem that a certain amount of reward
is necessary in order to give people drive, but at the same time we don't want this drive to
become satiated through an overuse of rewards. When the drive does become satiated then
people stop working so hard and drive reduces. That is the problem when we provide too
much of what is, in our familiar terminology, extrinsic motivation. Therefore it's important to
know when and how to reward and when and how to punish. The danger of punishment is
the people begin to associate the punishment with the response and will therefore avoid
responding in all possible cases and I think we see things like this actually happen in the
English language classroom in Korea. The evaluation system is a system of punishment not
a system of reward for everyone except those at the very top and people do associate
punishment with responses, in large part because their responses no matter how hard they
try are never quite good enough. What this means for us is that we need to reward but not
for reward's sake. We need to reward honestly and fairly and when it's applicable.
Punishment should be dished out the same way. There should be no irrelevant patterns of
punishment or reward.
An underlying presumption of this idea is that the students are actually responding. This
means that the students actually have to perform (respond) in some sort of way. We can
only get an idea of consequences or form associations with consequences when we
actually respond. This would seem to indicate that language classrooms must involve a lot
more real language use for the students. If language classrooms do not involve actual
performance, that is responses, by the students then none of these ideas are going to work.
As we move further and further into these behaviorist theories it is important to remember
how they might actually be linked to language use and eventually language learning.
We are interested in language as a specific type of behavior. This will involve language
as being in its primary sense; a type of behavior. Of course, for this behavior to actually
occur there has to be some underlying knowledge and language is a very complex system
of knowledge. This is what we're trying to work towards. The things we study this week
about instrumental learning are quite important because now we not only can explain to how
patterns of sounds, morphemes, words, phrases and clauses might actually enter the brain
but now we can actually begin to talk a little bit about behavior; how speakers use them. In
instrumental learning what people actually learn is to associate consequences with either
stimulus or responses or both. Knowing these consequences allows people to self-direct
behavior. This means that language, for our considerations in this course, has now moved
out of the realm of the brain and into the realm of real-world behavior. People can begin
conversations and engage in conversations based on somebody's very simple ideas here.
The fact, mentioned above, that this three-way association between stimulus response in
consequence can be broken in certain places allows people to extend the simple
generalization into many different areas, thus allowing language to actually occur as a type
of behavior. This is very interesting in importance step for us in our quest to try to determine
how language is actually learned.
References
de Saussure, F. (1959). Course in general linguistics. New York: McGraw-Hill.