* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Loyalty - Spears School of Business
Guerrilla marketing wikipedia , lookup
Street marketing wikipedia , lookup
Target audience wikipedia , lookup
Product placement wikipedia , lookup
Multicultural marketing wikipedia , lookup
Direct marketing wikipedia , lookup
Marketing research wikipedia , lookup
Viral marketing wikipedia , lookup
Green marketing wikipedia , lookup
Food marketing wikipedia , lookup
Celebrity branding wikipedia , lookup
Marketing communications wikipedia , lookup
Advertising wikipedia , lookup
Digital marketing wikipedia , lookup
Marketing channel wikipedia , lookup
Integrated marketing communications wikipedia , lookup
Visual merchandising wikipedia , lookup
Product planning wikipedia , lookup
Customer engagement wikipedia , lookup
Global marketing wikipedia , lookup
Advertising management wikipedia , lookup
Marketing mix modeling wikipedia , lookup
Targeted advertising wikipedia , lookup
Consumer behaviour wikipedia , lookup
Advertising campaign wikipedia , lookup
Youth marketing wikipedia , lookup
Neuromarketing wikipedia , lookup
Brand awareness wikipedia , lookup
Brand equity wikipedia , lookup
Brand loyalty wikipedia , lookup
Personal branding wikipedia , lookup
Journal of Advertising, 43(1), 73–84 C 2014, American Academy of Advertising Copyright ISSN: 0091-3367 print / 1557-7805 online DOI: 10.1080/00913367.2013.815110 Exploring the Boundaries of Nostalgic Advertising Effects: A Consideration of Childhood Brand Exposure and Attachment on Consumers’ Responses to Nostalgia-Themed Advertisements Darrel D. Muehling and David E. Sprott Washington State University, Pullman, Washington, USA Abdullah J. Sultan Kuwait University, Safat, Kuwait images from the past (Vasquez 2012). In addition, nostalgia has become a popular trend in social media, with the recent introduction of BuzzFeed’s “Time Machine” (Parekh 2012) and Twitter’s “Throwback Thursdays,” where images and memories of the past are incorporated in company websites and posted on personal web pages with the intent to evoke nostalgic thoughts and feelings. Given its pervasiveness in the marketplace, it is not surprising that nostalgia has captured the interest of scholars who have investigated the phenomenon in a variety of research contexts, including self-concept (Belk 1988; Davis 1979), brand loyalty (Olsen 1993), brand meaning (Brown, Kozinets, and Sherry 2003), charitable giving (Merchant, Ford, and Rose 2011; Zhou et al. 2012), retail atmospherics (Orth and Bourrain 2008), literacy criticism (Stern 1992), and emotions (Bambauer-Sachse and Gierl 2009a; Holak and Havlena 1992, 1998). In an advertising context, a growing body of empirical research suggests that the effects of nostalgia appear to be rather robust—signaling to marketers and advertisers that nostalgia may be an effective means of appealing to a variety of audiences (Marchegiani and Phau 2010). In particular, recent research has demonstrated the positive effects of nostalgia-themed advertising on consumers’ responses to the brand sponsor (e.g., see Marchegiani and Phau 2010; Muehling and Pascal 2011; Pascal, Sprott, and Muehling 2002; Reisenwitz, Iyer, and Cutler 2004). These researchers and others (e.g., Baker and Kennedy 1994; Holak and Havlena 1998; Muehling and Sprott 2004) have provided empirical support to the claim that an advertisement with a nostalgic theme may be capable of prompting nostalgic thoughts and pleasant memories in consumers and resulting in more favorable brand attitudes and purchase intentions. Given the relative importance of such outcomes, one might assume that consumers’ brand evaluations may simply be enhanced by exposure to nostalgia-themed ads. However, while the This study examines empirically the differential influence of past brand associations (specifically, in-home childhood brand exposure and past personal attachment) on consumers’ reactions to nostalgia-themed advertisements. Results support the expectation that the effect of nostalgia on brand-focused outcomes (purchase intentions and brand attitude) will be most pronounced for those who have had some past personal association with the advertised brand. However, this effect was not observed for ad-focused responses (ad involvement and attitude toward the ad); as hypothesized, the nostalgic ad produced more favorable responses than the non-nostalgic ad, irrespective of participants’ past association with the brand. Nostalgia—“one’s sentimental longing for the past”—is a popular theme often used in the advertising campaigns and product designs of today’s marketers. Firms such as General Mills, McDonald’s, GE, Coca-Cola, Target, Volkswagen, and Unilever, to name a few, have used nostalgic appeals with the hope of strengthening consumers’ attitudes toward their brands and enhancing the likelihood of product purchase (Elliott 2009; Naughton and Vlasic 1998; Schultz 2012; White 2002). Furthermore, the popularity of nostalgia does not appear to be waning; during the airing of Super Bowl XLVI, audiences were exposed to a barrage of ads featuring nostalgic references and Address correspondence to Darrel D. Muehling, Washington State University, 367C Todd Addition, Pullman, WA 99164-4730. E-mail: [email protected] Darrel D. Muehling (PhD, University of Nebraska–Lincoln) is a professor of marketing, College of Business, Washington State University. David E. Sprott (PhD, University of South Carolina) is a professor of marketing, College of Business, Washington State University. Abdullah J. Sultan (PhD, Washington State University) is an assistant professor, Department of Management and Marketing, Kuwait University. 73 74 D. D. MUEHLING ET AL. main effect of nostalgia on brand evaluations has been shown, surprisingly little (if any) research has empirically accounted for boundary conditions in the context of nostalgia advertising effects. Examining such conditions is not only important for firms considering the use of nostalgia as a basis for their marketing and advertising campaigns but also serves to expand understanding of this construct. Investigating such effects is designed to enhance what is currently known with regard to how and when nostalgia “works.” In an empirical study, we address this gap in the nostalgia literature by exploring a simple yet currently overlooked factor that may differentially influence consumers’ responses to nostalgia-themed advertising. Given that nostalgia is often characterized as an intensely personal experience (Baker and Kennedy 1994; Davis 1979), we contend that the effects of nostalgic advertisements should be most pronounced for those whom the advertised brand has had some personal association or meaningful connection to their past (Belk 1990; Holak and Havlena 1992; Merchant and Ford 2008). To date, no empirical research (that we are aware of) has examined consumers’ past (childhood) in-home brand exposure or personal attachment as explanations for nostalgia advertising’s effects, despite suggestions of their potential influence (e.g., see Merchant and Ford 2008; Muehling and Sprott 2004). In the sections that follow, we begin with a brief discussion of nostalgia as a sociopsychological construct and examine the underlying assumptions related to its origins, types, and proposed effects on consumer behavior. Then a theoretical rationale for the hypothesized relationships between in-home childhood brand exposure, past attachment, and responses to nostalgiathemed advertising is offered. Following this is a discussion of the methods employed and findings obtained from our empirical investigation. The article concludes with a general discussion of the theoretical and practical implications of the findings and directions for future research. THE NOSTALGIA CONSTRUCT Although the origins of nostalgia as a theoretical construct can be traced as far back as Hofer’s (1688) dissertation, its consideration within a consumer psychological context is more recent and was most likely prompted by Davis’s (1979) seminal work on the subject (Rutherford and Shaw 2011). Several definitions and descriptions of nostalgia have emerged in the marketing literature over the past decades. Some (e.g., Davis 1979) make reference to the source of the positive emotion evoked by the past, characterizing nostalgia as a “positively toned evocation of a lived past in the context of some negative feeling toward the present or impending circumstance” (p. 18). Others include some reference to actual stimuli (objects, scenes, smells, etc.) that may prompt a nostalgic response (e.g., Belk 1990; Divard and Robert-Demontrond 1997). Still others (e.g., Holbrook and Schindler, 1991, p. 330) view nostalgia as a time-based preference, more specifically, “a preference (general liking, positive attitude, or favorable affect) toward objects (people, places, or things) that were common (popular, fashionable, or widely circulated) when one was younger (in early adulthood, in adolescence, in childhood, or even before birth).” Despite these differences, most conceptualizations of nostalgia view it primarily as an affective state, mood, or emotion that is somehow triggered by one’s thoughts of the past. In the current investigation, we consider “the past” as a time from one’s own youth or childhood and specifically the time spent “at home.” Home is often considered a key element in reference to nostalgia, as are family and friends (Holak and Havlena 1992). For example, Hofer’s (1688) dissertation focused on the symptoms of home-sickness. Moreover, Davis (1979) pointed out that “Nostalgia is from the Greek nostos, to return home, and algia, a painful condition—thus, a painful yearning to return home” (p. 1; see also Rutherford and Shaw 2011). Thus, the notion of home (and returning to said, via mental reflection) is clearly an integral component of nostalgia. Although often conceptualized as “bittersweet,” nostalgic reflections have been hypothesized and found to be predominantly positive, in other words, as seen through “rose-tinted glasses” (Belk 1990; Havlena and Holak 1991; Holak and Havlena 1992; Muehling and Sprott 2004; Stern 1992; Wildschut et al. 2006). For example, based on empirical evidence, Leboe and Ansons (2006) concluded that many instances of nostalgic experiences are distorted perceptions that lead to a positively biased, idealized recollection of one’s own past. Unlike autobiographical memories (literal memories of the past; see Sujan, Bettman, and Baumgartner 1993), nostalgic memories tend to be skewed toward the positive. This notion is akin to what McAdams and colleagues (2001) refer to as a redemption sequence, whereby memory of a negative life experience progresses over time to become positive or triumphant. Therefore, as Davis (1979) noted, “The nostalgic feeling is infused with imputations of past beauty, pleasure, joy, satisfaction, goodness, happiness, love, and the like, in sum, any or several of the positive affects of being” (p. 14). Further, Stern (1992) and others (Baker and Kennedy 1994; Marchegiani and Phau 2005; Merchant and Ford 2008; Muehling 2013; Muehling and Pascal 2011) have made a distinction between personal nostalgia (characterized as the recollection of good times from one’s personally lived past) and historical nostalgia (which takes people back to a time before their births and therefore is not and cannot be personally experienced). Whether the past is personally experienced or historical in nature (e.g., through books, films, or other narratives, outside of one’s living memory; Goulding 2001; Holbrook 1993b; Stern 1992), the resulting nostalgic feeling is expected to be strong enough to influence brand attitudes and purchase intentions when employed in an advertising campaign. Recent research has observed differential effects when personal versus historical nostalgic themes are employed, with more favorable EXPLORING NOSTALGIC ADVERTISING EFFECTS outcomes usually associated with the more personal type of nostalgia (Merchant and Ford 2008; Muehling 2013; Muehling and Pascal 2011). As will be discussed further in the section to follow, we are most interested in determining how varying levels of one’s exposure and past attachment to a brand while growing up at home differentially influence consumers’ present-day responses to brands featured in a nostalgia-themed advertisement. Thus, our focus is on personal nostalgia. This is not to suggest that individuals cannot personally experience a product or brand that existed prior to their birth (e.g., a 20-year-old driving a 1950s’ vintage automobile) or that they cannot be nostalgic for a bygone era (see Goulding 2001). However, and consistent with the majority of published research (e.g., Merchant, Ford, and Rose 2011; Wildschut et al. 2006), our treatment of nostalgia here is from the perspective of one’s own realized and experienced past with a branded product. NOSTALGIA AND BRAND ADVERTISING Nostalgia has been linked to the self as a distinctive way of relating one’s past to the present and future (Belk 1990; BraunLaTour 2007; Stern 1992) and is believed to be influenced by personal experience (Davis 1979; Merchant and Ford 2008). As Braun-LaTour (2007) noted, “The feeling of remembering a past event, particularly from early childhood, is quite powerful” (p. 403) and “It is no wonder, then, that advertisers have sought to use this emotional response to their advantage through referencing past childhood events in their advertising” (p. 404). Consistent with Braun-LaTour’s (2007) generalizations, brain research has shown that when retrieving information from one’s past, the hippocampus (the emotion center of the brain) is activated, with heart rate and blood pressure being increased (Kotre 1995; Schacter 1996). Such findings offer advertisers some hope that carefully crafted nostalgic advertising campaigns may evoke powerful responses and desired outcomes among consumers. The connection of self to the past is not limited to consumer research. For example, Routledge and colleagues (2011) tested and found support for the notion that nostalgia is capable of serving an existential function, in that it is positively associated with one’s sense of meaning in life. In the current investigation, we contend that brands can play an important role in defining a consumer’s past and (just as with other stimuli, such as music, scents, and photographs) can serve as a catalyst for generating nostalgic responses. As such, we test the propositions that inhome childhood exposure and past attachment to a brand are likely to prompt differential consumer responses to nostalgiathemed brand advertising. Over time, consumers are likely to develop unique personal connections with brands that are associated with particular prior experiences in their lives and stored in long-term memory (Braun 1999; Keller 1993). For example, a consumer might be exposed to a brand during earlier, simpler life expe- 75 riences at home (e.g., helping one’s mom pour Tide detergent into the washing machine or witnessing one’s dad’s use of a Coleman brand cook stove during family camping trips). From a memory-based perspective, one’s association with the past creates a network of connections to the person that may help facilitate a nostalgic response (Holak and Havlena 1998). In other words, a nostalgic response is more likely to occur for those whose connections to the past (e.g., childhood associations with the brand) are more abundant. Consistent with memory-based research (Craik and Lockhart 1972), we reason that the more inhome exposure children have to a brand, the more likely these associations will be stored in long-term memory and will be available for later retrieval. Relatedly, brand associations may have formed due to meaningful prior experiences with a brand, such as sharing a certain snack food with a childhood friend or family member, or consuming the brand with loved ones as part of a holiday or special family event (Keller 1993; Merchant and Ford 2008). In a study of autobiographical memory, Grysman and Hudson (2011) found that the “self-focused” written narratives of their participants included more statements related to personal meaning and emotional value of a recalled event (akin to personal nostalgia), as opposed to specific details of the narrative (i.e., episodic memory or the “who, what, when, and where” of the event). Barrett and colleagues (2010) contend that the emotional components of autobiographical memories often aid in the storage and later retrieval of nostalgic reflections. Therefore, it is reasonable to suggest that triggers of nostalgia at the time of ad exposure may be the result of associations between the brand and past events, as well as the emotions associated with those events (what we refer to here as past brand attachment). We contend that when consumers come into contact with brands in either of these two ways (via in-home childhood brand exposure or meaningful past personal experiences), their recall of the brand manifests itself as positive (most likely nostalgic) memories of the past. In keeping with this notion, Bartier (2011) contends that consumers are likely to remember “lived” past memories more so than vicarious, “learned” ones, because lived memories are deemed more relevant and memorable. Moreover, she contends that these lived memories are often traceable to events in consumers’ youth or childhood. Stern (1992) posited, but did not empirically test, the notion that nostalgia marketing is capable of stimulating an imaginative re-creation of the past, which can be positively associated with an advertised brand. Research focusing on autobiographical memory is helpful in explaining the influence of such memories on brand evaluations. Autobiographical memories (similar to personal nostalgic memories) have been characterized as emotional, affect-laden memories of past personal experiences. When advertisements encourage the retrieval of these memories, consumers’ judgment processes can be influenced (Sujan, Bettman, and Baumgartner 1993). Using 76 D. D. MUEHLING ET AL. affect transfer as their theoretical base (Burke and Edell 1989; MacKenzie, Lutz, and Belch 1986), Sujan, Bettman, and Baumgartner (1993) observed that the transfer of autobiographical affect to the advertised brand was dependent on the linkage between the brand and personal memory and provided evidence suggesting that a nostalgic ad is likely to generate more favorable brand evaluations (by reminding consumers of past pleasant experiences); as the researchers noted, however, “for nostalgia to really impact brand judgment, direct links need to be forged between the nostalgic experience and the brand” (p. 434). Importantly, as Braun-LaTour and colleagues have pointed out (Braun-LaTour 2007; Braun-LaTour, LaTour, and Zinkhan 2007), prompting memories of childhood experiences is instrumental in evoking emotional responses that subsequently shape brand preferences and consumption decisions. Furthermore, when mental images of past consumption experiences are intense (emotionally charged), as nostalgic responses have been shown to be (Baumgartner 1992), consumers’ attitudes toward the advertised brand are likely to be more favorable (Bambauer-Sachse and Gierl 2009b). This, in part, is due to nostalgic thoughts prompting higher identification with the advertised message, which enhances the ad’s persuasiveness (Braun, Ellis, and Loftus 2002). Such findings are consistent with the branding literature, which has shown positive relationships between self-brand connections, brand evaluations, and attitude strength (Moore and Homer 2004, 2008). Taken together, this body of work suggests that the effects of nostalgia on consumers’ brand attitudes and purchase intentions should be influenced by one’s past associations with (i.e., in-home childhood exposure and past personal attachment to) the advertised brand. As such, we propose the following effects of nostalgia on consumers’ brand-related judgments: H1: Exposure to a nostalgia-themed advertisement (as opposed to a nonnostalgic ad) is more likely to produce favorable brand attitudes for those persons (a) who were exposed to the brand in their homes as they were growing up and (b) who formed a personal attachment to the brand during their childhoods. H2: Exposure to a nostalgia-themed advertisement (as opposed to a nonnostalgic ad) is more likely to produce greater purchase intentions for those persons (a) who were exposed to the brand in their homes as they were growing up and (b) who formed a personal attachment to the brand during their childhoods. As noted earlier, advertising-induced nostalgia is viewed primarily as a positive affective state generated by one’s reflection on the past (Muehling and Sprott 2004). A growing body of research has supported the notion that nostalgic ads (as compared to nonnostalgic ones) are effective in generating interest and evoking emotional responses that lead to more favorable ad-related outcomes (Bambauer-Sachse and Gierl 2009b; Marchegiani and Phau 2005; Muehling and Sprott 2004; Muehling and Pascal 2011; Pascal, Sprott, and Muehling 2002). Researchers have also provided some empirical evidence that nostalgic cues embedded in brand advertisements may enhance viewers’ attention to the ad, as well as result in more favorable responses toward the ad in general (Pascal, Sprott, and Muehling 2002; Reisenwitz, Iyer, and Cutler 2004). Likewise, Muehling and Pascal (2012) observed that nostalgic ads (especially those with a “personal” nostalgic theme) are capable of inducing greater levels of advertising involvement (as compared to nonnostalgic ads). These findings are consistent with traditional advertising research, which has shown that positive feelings and emotions generated by advertising are robust and predictive of consumers’ engagement with and attitudes toward the ad (Burke and Edell 1989; Derbaix 1995; Holbrook and Batra 1987; Moore and Hutchinson 1985). While we expect the positive effects of nostalgia-themed advertising on brand-related judgments to be a function (at least in part) of consumers’ prior exposure and attachment to the brand (hypothesis 1 and hypothesis 2), we do not expect these factors to have a differential impact on responses to the advertisement itself. Advertising involvement and attitude toward the ad are ad-focused outcomes that capture consumers’ responses to the ad (e.g., reactions to the ad’s contents, its executional features, and the like; Muehling and McCann 1993). Although often predictive of brand attitudes, studies examining attitude toward the ad tend to view it as independent from the product and brand itself (e.g., MacKenzie, Lutz, and Belch 1986). Most important to the current investigation, a meta-analysis of the attitude toward the ad construct conducted by Brown and Stayman (1992) found that the effects of ad-generated feelings and cognitions were not strongly impacted by one’s prior brand knowledge or experience. Further, while not directly tested, findings from Muehling and his colleagues indicate that nostalgia-themed advertisements do not generate a disproportionate number of brand-related thoughts. In particular, these researchers demonstrated that the number of brand/message-related thoughts in response to nostalgic ads was no greater than (Muehling and Sprott 2004) or, in fact, was sometimes less than the number in response to nonnostalgic ads (Muehling and Pascal 2011). Further, Muehling and Sprott (2004) noted that few instances were found where research participants made a direct linkage between nostalgia and the advertised brand or product. Instead, most thoughts focused on “friends, neighborhoods, and memories of days gone by, rather than how the use of the advertised product may help them relive a nostalgic moment” (p. 32). Consistent with our line of reasoning, these findings suggest that participants may have not drawn from brand-related memory when assessing the qualities and likability of the ads. As a result, we contend that a consumer’s involvement with or attitude toward a nostalgic (versus nonnostalgic) ad should not be differentially influenced by one’s past exposure and attachment to the brand. More specifically, we hypothesize the following ad-related outcomes as a result of exposure to a nostalgiathemed ad: EXPLORING NOSTALGIC ADVERTISING EFFECTS H3: Exposure to a nostalgia-themed advertisement (as opposed to a nonnostalgic ad) will produce greater ad involvement, regardless of whether persons (a) were exposed to the brand in their homes as they were growing up or (b) formed a personal attachment to the brand during their childhoods. H4: Exposure to a nostalgia-themed advertisement (as opposed to a nonnostalgic ad) will produce more favorable ad attitudes, regardless of whether persons (a) were exposed to the brand in their homes as they were growing up or (b) formed a personal attachment to the brand during their childhoods. STUDY Method Brand and product choice and advertising stimuli. Research has shown effects of nostalgia with products such as snack items (Chou and Lien 2010), cameras (Muehling and Pascal 2011), and photographic film (Pascal, Sprott, and Muehling 2002). In the current study, we exposed participants to advertisements for a product category (toothpaste) that is not typically associated with nostalgic advertising themes. This was done to help reduce the possibility that the advertised product category would in and of itself encourage nostalgic reflection. However, to assess the effects of past personal associations with a brand, it was necessary for the ads to feature a well-known brand of toothpaste (Crest), which was likely to exhibit varying levels of past in-home exposure and personal attachment across study participants. Experimenter-generated stimuli were created for the Crest toothpaste brand. We constructed print ads similar in layout and executional elements, except for those features (e.g., headline, ad copy, and year referenced) designed to evoke nostalgia (see appendix). In addition, the nostalgic ad featured a sepia-colored background (a color often associated with aging photographs and nostalgia) and showed children on a beach (a visual reference to childhood), whereas the nonnostalgic ad featured a more contemporary blue background with adults shown in a similar beach setting (a visual reference to the present day). Worth noting, the “life-focused” theme of both ads (“Life was great back then,” nostalgic ad; versus “Life is interesting,” nonnostalgic ad) was consistent with the current advertising tagline of Crest (i.e., “Crest . . . healthy, beautiful smiles for life”). Participants, measures, and procedures. A total of 180 individuals participated in the study. The sample was obtained from an online panel provider (Amazon Mechanical Turk, or “MTurk”), and the study was conducted via the Internet. Males represented 45.6% of our sample (54.4% were female), with participants’ ages ranging from 18 to 35 (mean age = 22.1 years). We purposely restricted study participants’ ages to 35 years or younger for the time frame referenced in the nostalgia ad treatment condition (the 1990s) to have been personally experienced during the participants’ youth or childhood. Research participants were randomly assigned to an experimental (nostalgic) or control (nonnostalgic) ad treatment condition (n = 91 and 89, respectively). Utilizing the services of 77 the online provider, participants first completed an online questionnaire containing items focusing on their childhood exposure and past attachment to the Crest brand. As one of our two moderating variables, we were interested in capturing participants’ in-home exposure to the brand during their childhood years. It is this personal exposure at home that we surmised may prompt thoughts of the past and consequently influences participants’ responses to nostalgic ads. To assess past (childhood home) brand exposure, participants were asked to respond to an open-ended question. Specifically, they were asked: “What one brand of toothpaste did your family most often use while you were growing up at home?” In addition to the measure noted, participants responded to five 11-point “past brand attachment” items adapted from those recently appearing in the brand equity literature (Park et al. 2010; Thomson, MacInnis and Park 2005). The items—with endpoints Not at all (0) and Completely (10)—included questions such as “To what extent was Crest part of you and who you were when you were younger?”; “To what extent did you feel personally connected to Crest when you were younger?”; and “To what extent did you feel emotionally bonded with Crest when you were younger?” The Cronbach’s alpha for the five items (α = .98) supported the formation of an index of past brand attachment. A median-split of this 11-point index was subsequently performed for testing the hypothesized pattern of relationships; participants were placed into “high” (values > 6.3; n = 90) and “low” (values ≤ 6.3; n = 90) past brand attachment groups. Participants were also asked to respond to a forced-choice question, selecting one of four statements that best describes their current and past attachment to the Crest brand (i.e., “I would say that I am very attached to Crest currently, and I was very attached as well when I was younger”; “I would say that I was very attached to Crest when I was younger, but I am not very attached currently”; “I would say that I am very attached to Crest currently, but I was not very attached when I was younger”; and “I would say that I am not very attached to Crest currently and I was not very attached when I was younger”). Not surprisingly (perhaps due to the mundane nature of the toothpaste category in general), only 36 (51.4%) of the 70 individuals who indicated that Crest was the one brand of toothpaste most often used at home while growing up selected one of the two categories corresponding to “high” past brand attachment. Therefore, in the findings reported here (and consistent with our statement of hypotheses), we performed two separate sets of analyses—one utilizing the measure of participants’ in-home childhood brand exposure and the other utilizing a median-split of the past brand attachment scale. After completing the past brand exposure and attachment measures, participants were then given the opportunity to view a screenshot of either the nostalgic or nonnostalgic ad stimulus for approximately 30 seconds. It should be noted that random assignment to ad-treatment conditions occurred prior to any past brand exposure/attachment categorization, thereby contributing to the observed uneven cell sizes (see Tables 1 and 2). After 78 D. D. MUEHLING ET AL. TABLE 1 Descriptive Statistics (Using Past Brand Exposure Measure) Ad treatment condition Group Dependent variable Past brand exposure in childhood home AB PI INV AAd NOST Little/no past brand exposure in childhood home AB PI INV AAd NOST a Nostalgic Nonnostalgic (n = 38) 6.28 6.11 4.90 5.03 4.96 (n = 53) 5.64 5.16 5.17 5.23 5.59 (n = 32) 5.59 5.56 3.74 3.80 3.18 (n = 57) 5.58 5.23 4.25 4.36 3.50 t value 2.79a 1.57c 2.33b 2.53a 3.95a .25 −.23 2.62a 2.40a 6.64a Significant at p < .01 (one-tailed). bSignificant at p < .05 (one-tailed). cMarginally significant at p < .065 (one-tailed). ad exposure, participants were asked to complete items corresponding to the dependent variables, nostalgia manipulation check, and demographics. Dependent measures. Brand-related dependent measures included attitudes toward the advertised Crest brand (AB ) (bad/good, dislike very much/like very much, unfavorable/favorable, worthless/valuable; Cronbach’s alpha = .95) and purchase intentions (PI) (unlikely/likely, improbable/probable, impossible/possible they would purchase Crest toothpaste the next time they purchased toothpaste; Cronbach’s alpha = .93). These measures employed 7-point scales, with larger numbers representing more favorable responses, and were patterned af- ter measures reported in the nostalgia advertising literature (e.g., Pascal, Sprott, and Muehling 2002). Ad-related dependent measures included attitude toward the ad (AAd ) (bad/good, unfavorable/favorable, negative/positive, unpleasant/pleasant; Cronbach’s alpha = .97) and self-reported ad involvement (INV) (“To what extent did you find the ad to be interesting, involving, and personally relevant to you?” with scale endpoints Not at all and Very; Cronbach’s alpha = .94). These measures have been reported in the nostalgia advertising literature (e.g., Muehling and Pascal 2012). Indices were created from the 7-point scale items, with larger numbers representing more favorable responses. TABLE 2 Descriptive Statistics (Using Past Brand Attachment Scale Median-Split) Ad treatment condition Group Dependent variable High past brand attachment AB PI INV AAd NOST Low past brand attachment AB PI INV AAd NOST a Significant at p < .01 (one-tailed). bSignificant at p < .05 (one-tailed). Nostalgic Nonnostalgic (n = 50) 6.31 6.06 5.76 5.03 5.82 (n = 41) 5.43 4.94 4.20 4.32 4.73 (n = 40) 5.90 5.61 4.89 3.80 4.32 (n = 49) 5.32 5.14 3.39 3.49 2.62 t value 2.30b 1.96b 2.49a 2.47a 4.79a .34 −.49 1.99b 1.98b 5.75a EXPLORING NOSTALGIC ADVERTISING EFFECTS Manipulation check. In the final section of the survey, participants completed 10 items of an evoked nostalgia scale (NOST; Cronbach’s alpha = .98), adapted from Pascal, Sprott, and Muehling (2002) as a manipulation check. Sample items (anchored with 7-point Strongly disagree and Strongly agree endpoints) included: “The ad . . . reminded me of the past”; “helped me recall pleasant memories”; and “made me feel nostalgic.” Supporting our manipulation, participants in the nostalgic ad condition reported significantly higher levels of adevoked nostalgia (M Nost = 5.33) as compared to those in the nonnostalgia condition (M Nonnost = 3.39; t (178) = 11.52, p < .01). This finding was obtained regardless of participants’ reported prior in-home exposure or past personal attachment with the brand (see NOST values in Tables 1 and 2). Results Brand attitudes and purchase intentions (hypotheses 1 and 2). Table 1 reports the findings of the brand-focused hypothesis tests when participants were grouped according to their in-home childhood exposure (some past brand exposure versus little/no past brand exposure). In support of hypothesis 1a and hypothesis 2a, the nostalgic ad (as compared to the nonnostalgic ad) prompted more favorable brand attitudes (M Nost = 6.28, M Nonnost = 5.59, t (68) = 2.79, p < .01) and stronger intentions to purchase the advertised brand, (M Nost = 6.11, M Nonnost = 5.56, t (68) = 1.57, p < .065) for those participants noting some “past brand” exposure. However, and consistent with our hypothesized expectations, the nostalgic ad (as compared to the nonnostalgic ad) had no greater effect on AB or PI for those participants with little/no reported exposure to the brand in their childhood homes (both p values > .10; refer to Table 1). When a median-split of the past brand attachment index was utilized as our grouping variable, a similar pattern of results to those reported emerged. More specifically, and in support of hypothesis 1b and hypothesis 2b, the nostalgic ad again produced more favorable AB (M Nost = 6.31, M Nonnost = 5.90,) and greater PI (M Nost = 6.06, M Nonnost = 5.61, t (88) = 1.96, p < .05) than did the nonnostalgic ad, but only in the high past brand attachment condition. As expected, there were no statistically significant differences in means for AB or PI across nostalgic and nonnostalgic ad treatment conditions for those in the low past brand attachment condition (both p values > .10; refer to Table 2). Ad involvement and attitude toward the ad (hypotheses 3 and 4). Consistent with hypothesis 3a and hypothesis 4a expectations, the nostalgic ad produced more favorable ad-related responses as compared to the nonnostalgic ad, irrespective of participants’ level of prior brand exposure in their childhood homes. More specifically, greater ad involvement (M Nost = 4.90, M Nonnost = 3.74; t (68) = 2.33, p < .05) and more favorable attitudes toward the ad (M Nost = 5.03, M Nonnost = 3.80; t (68) = 2.53, p < .01) were observed in the nostalgia ad condition when participants reported having prior brand exposure in their child- 79 hood homes. A similar pattern of results was obtained for those participants who reported little or no exposure to the brand in their childhood homes (M Nost = 5.17, M Nonnost = 4.25; t (68) = 2.62, p < .01, for ad involvement; M Nost = 5.23, M Nonnost = 4.36; t (68) = 2.40, p < .01, for attitude toward the ad). See Table 1. A nostalgia advertising advantage was also observed for measures of INV and AAd when a median-split of the past brand attachment measure was employed. Consistent with our hypothesized expectations (hypothesis 3b and hypothesis 4b) and with the results reported here, the nostalgic ad (as compared to the nonnostalgic ad) produced heightened INV and more favorable AAd , irrespective of whether participants were placed in the “high” or “low” past brand attachment group (all p values < .05 or better). Refer to Table 2 for descriptive statistics. DISCUSSION A growing body of research has shown that nostalgia-themed advertising is capable of producing positive brand-related outcomes (e.g., Bambauer-Sachse and Gierl 2009b; Marchegiani and Phau 2005; Muehling and Sprott 2004; Muehling and Pascal 2011; Pascal, Sprott, and Muehling 2002). Nonetheless, while published findings are potentially important for marketers who use nostalgia to promote their brands (a relatively common practice in today’s marketplace; e.g., Elliott 2009; Schultz 2012), little if any research has examined boundary conditions for the effects of nostalgia in an advertising context. Building upon the extant literature on self-brand connections (e.g., Moore and Homer 2008) and autobiographical memory (e.g., BraunLaTour, LaTour, and Zinkhan 2007), we explore how in-home childhood exposure and past personal attachment to a brand influences responses to nostalgia-themed brand advertisements. Our expectation was that brand attitudes and purchase intentions would be most favorable and strongest for those viewing a nostalgic ad and for whom some type of meaningful in-home exposure or personal attachment to the advertised brand had existed during childhood (hypothesis 1 and hypothesis 2), but that prior brand experiences and associations would not differentially influence responses to the advertisements themselves (hypothesis 3 and hypothesis 4). These hypotheses were tested and supported in our empirical study. In particular, results of our study indicate that a nostalgiathemed advertisement is most likely to yield favorable brand attitudes and purchase intentions for consumers who have had some prior in-home exposure or personal attachment to the brand, with no appreciable effect of nostalgia being observed for those who lacked such childhood brand experiences or associations. These results were obtained for a more traditional brand of toothpaste (Crest) that is not typically associated with nostalgic advertising. The findings are noteworthy, given our expectation (confirmed by our data) that participants would not likely possess high levels of attachment with the brand. As such, it appears that prior association with the brand does 80 D. D. MUEHLING ET AL. not require extensive exposure and self-defining brand attachment over an extended period of time. One’s relatively simple, but memorable, past associations with the brand (especially in the context of one’s childhood home) may effectively influence how consumers react to a company or brand in a current advertising setting (Braun-LaTour, LaTour, and Zinkhan 2007). Taken together, our findings suggest that prior in-home brand exposure and attachment are meaningful boundary conditions to consider in regard to understanding and explaining consumers’ brand-related responses to nostalgia-themed advertising. Building on research on attitude toward the ad (e.g., Brown and Stayman 1992) and nostalgia advertising (e.g., Muehling and Pascal 2012), we predicted that consumers’ ad-related responses to a nostalgia-themed advertisement, however, would be independent from the nostalgic reflection that may be triggered by the brand itself. In other words, past associations with the brand were not expected to differentially influence nostalgia’s impact on ad involvement and attitude toward the ad. Our findings support such a notion. We found that prior brand experiences and past brand attachment did not play important roles in differentially influencing participants’ attitudes toward the nostalgic (versus the nonnostalgic) ad or enhancing their involvement with the ads. Irrespective of participants’ past associations with the advertised brand, we found the nostalgic ad generated more favorable attitudes toward and greater involvement with the advertisement. This pattern of effects, in part, may be attributed to the fact that consumers are capable of experiencing nostalgic thoughts and emotions even without meaningful prior brand/product experiences. In support of this notion, Muehling and Pascal (2011) used ad stimuli featuring a fictitious brand of camera, thereby making it impossible for their study participants to have reported prior associations with the advertised brand. Nonetheless, the authors observed a nostalgia advantage (i.e., the nostalgic ad produced more favorable attitudes toward the ad than did the nonnostalgic ad)—similar to the findings observed in the current study. Such a finding suggests that nostalgic advertising may be capable of evoking positive affect and emotions that at times are not directly connected to the affect and emotions associated with a particular brand. This is not to suggest that brand names do not play an important role when considered in a nostalgia context. Exposure to an ad for a brand often associated with nostalgia may prompt reader/viewers to produce more nostalgic thoughts than would normally be expected. As such, one should not overlook the very real possibility that some brands or products alone may be capable of evoking nostalgia (Loveland, Smeesters, and Mandel 2010), thereby further enhancing the effects of an advertising campaign. While more research is certainly in order, we speculate that when iconic brands (e.g., Coca-Cola soft drinks or Campbell’s soup) are featured in traditional advertising executions, some nostalgic thoughts may be generated in response to the brand itself. These nostalgic thoughts, in turn, may have some positive affective influence on further enhancing the advertisement’s effect on consumers’ responses.1 To date, the nostalgia literature has not taken into account the influence that past band associations may have on consumers’ responses to advertising. The findings reported here suggest that for measures of brand-focused responses (specifically, brand attitudes and purchase intentions) the extent to which a consumer associates an advertised brand with his or her past does indeed moderate the effects of nostalgia-themed advertising. Practical Implications From a practitioner’s viewpoint, our findings suggest that the use of nostalgia in marketing programs should not yield universally beneficial results, irrespective of the target audience. Instead, our studies indicate that firms wishing to obtain the greatest impact from nostalgia-based marketing campaigns must take into consideration their target audience’s prior association with the advertised brand. As Braun-LaTour (2007) noted, “Key to understanding what a brand means to consumers today is unlocking what they remember about their usage of the brand or product in the past” (p. 404). Therefore, in the current context, the task for advertisers is to identify and target consumer segments that may have had such meaningful brand experiences in their earlier years. This could be accomplished in a number of ways. For example, “reminders” of consumers’ past brand experiences could be featured on company websites or Facebook pages, along with current advertising messaging. Rather than simply touting the brand’s current features and benefits, these webpages could remind consumers that the company and brand have been staples throughout their lives, dating back to childhood. In addition, select media vehicles that tend to cater to nostalgia-prone individuals (e.g., Nostalgia Magazine, the TV Land channel, and/or syndicated reruns of Casey Kasem’s “American Top 40” radio show) should be considered a vital part of a firm’s media mix, with promotional messages relating the brand to the consumer’s past. To reach younger segments (e.g., ages 18 to 35), firms may also benefit by utilizing similar nostalgic advertising themes in social media outlets such as Twitter’s “Throwback Thursdays” (outlets that are popular with younger generations). It should also be noted that as children we may “experience” a variety of brands and products for which we have some nostalgic connection (e.g., Tide laundry detergent, Chevrolet automobiles, Budweiser beer), but for which we were never the primary “user” when we were younger. This suggests to advertisers that focusing on references to past brand usage may not be the only avenue worth pursuing in nostalgic ad executions. References to past in-home exposure and attachment to a brand (“Do you remember when?”) may also prove to be effective catalysts for prompting brand-based nostalgic reflection. Furthermore, even if a firm’s target market has not had meaningful prior experiences with the brand, one should not conclude that brand-focused nostalgic reflections cannot be activated in EXPLORING NOSTALGIC ADVERTISING EFFECTS consumers’ minds. In an intriguing study on the effects of autobiographical memory, Braun, Ellis, and Loftus (2002) found that autobiographical referencing in advertising can cause people to believe they actually had prior brand experiences, even when they had not. This phenomenon is explained by the reconstructive nature of memory (Braun 1999; Schacter 1996) and is consistent with the biased retrieval of memories inherent in nostalgic reflection (Leboe and Ansons 2006). As Braun, Ellis, and Loftus (2002) noted, “A consumer’s past is constantly being updated to fit one’s changing self-knowledge and social contexts” (p. 3). Therefore, in the context of the present investigation, repeated exposure to nostalgic advertising campaigns that reference past usage of or exposure to the brand may prompt consumers to believe they have actually had positive brand experiences in the past. If such is the case, we would expect the findings obtained in our studies to be observed for those who did not have prior brand experiences but nonetheless believed they had. Also worth noting, nostalgic reflection (the elicitation of nostalgic thoughts) is considered to be relatively effortless (i.e., with few cognitive demands placed on the reader/viewer at the time of ad exposure). Research by Janata, Tomic, and Rakowki (2007) has shown, for example, that the autobiographical memories evoked by music are often associated with nostalgia and specifically elicit a memory of a general life period or specific life event from the past with a positive affective component attached to it (see also Barrett et al. 2010). Using MRI data, Janata (2009) observed these results with tasks requiring low memory retrieval demands. Such findings suggest that the types of nostalgic advertising campaigns we are proposing here may be effectively used in “low-involvement” processing conditions—conditions that are typical of how many consumers respond to advertising these days—thereby broadening their appeal to potential advertisers. Limitations and Future Research As with any research, the present investigation has limitations that should be kept in mind when interpreting its findings and could be addressed in future research. First, our study was based on a sample of younger individuals (age ≤ 35),and as such may not have captured the full extent of consumers’ nostalgic memories and the impact they have on responses to nostalgia-themed ads. Although some researchers have indicated that older adults may be more likely to experience nostalgia than younger people (Batcho 1995; Davis 1979; Holbrook and Schindler 1996; Holak and Havlena 1992; Sierra and McQuitty 2007), empirical studies on the topic have found no significant correlation between age and nostalgic responses (Holbrook 1993a; Holbrook and Schindler 1991). Further, a number of researchers have successfully employed younger (i.e., college-aged student) samples to demonstrate the effects of nostalgia on marketing outcomes (e.g., Baker and Kennedy 1994; Batcho 2007; Chou and Lien 2010; Evans et al. 2010; Holbrook 1993a; Muehling and Sprott 2004; Pascal, Sprott, and Muehling 2002; Praxmarer and Gierl 81 2009; Wildschut et al. 2010). The general position of these researchers is that people of most any age (beyond adolescence) will have had prior experiences about which they may become nostalgic. Marketers apparently agree that younger consumers may respond in a nostalgic manner, given that nostalgic themes are often used for products marketed to younger generations. Nonetheless, previous research has suggested that a relationship may exist between age and proneness to nostalgia, with nostalgia proneness increasing as one gets older (Chou and Lien 2010; Reisenwitz, Iyer, and Cutler 2004). Therefore, one might expect that the effects observed in our study may have been even more pronounced had a more mature adult sample been used (Kusumi, Matsuda, and Sugimori 2010). Of course, to have further confidence in the stability and applicability of these findings, additional replications are encouraged that employ groups varying in age and (perhaps) culture. Second, our experiment was restricted to one brand and product category. Therefore, we cannot ensure that similar findings would emerge for brands representing other product categories. Our expectation is that these findings should hold for most brands where consumers have meaningful, past exposure and prior personal attachment with the brand. Nonetheless, future researchers are advised to replicate and extend our findings using additional brands (and products). When choosing brands, researchers may wish to select brands that vary along relevant theoretical dimensions. For example, brands that vary in terms of attachment (e.g., Park et al. 2010) or associations in memory (e.g., Keller 1993) might provide a deeper understanding of how prior brand experiences differentially influence responses to nostalgia-themed advertising. Furthermore, our study’s use of an online panel did not allow us to take measures of participants’ past Crest brand associations (in-home childhood exposure and past attachment) that were separated in time from when the experimental ad exposure occurred and when the measures of the dependent variables were taken. As such, interpretations of our study’s findings should take into account the possibility that measuring past brand associations immediately before being exposed to the ad treatments and measures of the dependent variables may have had some impact on the results. It should be relatively apparent that the current investigation focused on personal nostalgia (i.e., nostalgia generated by reflecting upon times from one’s own experienced past) as we were most interested in how prior, personal experiences with a brand influences responses to nostalgia-themed ads. Prior research, however, has also considered historical nostalgia, which is generated by reflecting upon a time before one’s birth (e.g., Baker and Kennedy 1994; Muehling and Pascal 2011; Stern 1992); as well as vicarious nostalgia, which may be evoked when consumers attempt to reconstruct or relive an event from a bygone era (Goulding 2001). In keeping with this notion, future researchers may also wish to explore prior brand association effects by using various types of nostalgia-themed ad treatments. Based on extant research, our expectation is that historical and 82 D. D. MUEHLING ET AL. vicarious nostalgia ad treatments would produce similar, but likely weaker, results than what is reported herein (Merchant and Ford 2008; Muehling 2013). Moreover, in comparison to personal nostalgia for established brands (such as Crest), the use of these other forms of nostalgia may be an ideal approach for new brands with which consumers have had no prior personally lived experiences but for which some vicarious/historical relationship with the brand exists. Finally, it should be noted that the current study employed ad- and brand-related measures—an approach that is common in the consumer behavior literature. Future researchers should consider testing a more extensive range of nostalgia’s effects on behavioral, as well as other cognitive and affective, response measures. In particular, prior research has suggested that nostalgic thoughts may interfere with viewers’ focusing on and processing message claims (e.g., Muehling and Pascal 2011; Sujan, Bettman, and Baumgartner 1993). From this perspective, collecting measures of brand beliefs after exposure to a nostalgia-themed advertisement seems a reasonable next step in further developing our understanding of nostalgia’s effects. In addition to incorporating other dependent variables, future research may wish to further explore alternate ad stimuli that use auditory or olfactory cues to activate nostalgic reflections. Given the relationship between music and nostalgia (Barrett et al. 2010) and the heavy use of music within advertising, examining the impact of music-based nostalgia manipulations could be particularly interesting and potentially effective for advertising and marketing managers. NOTE 1. Findings of a prestudy conducted by the authors (not reported in this article) support such a notion. When ads featuring the iconic Coca-Cola brand were employed, measures of ad-evoked nostalgia indicated that although the nostalgic ad treatment produced significantly more nostalgic thoughts than did the nonnostalgic ad, the mean value for the nonnostalgic ad was above the scale midpoint. REFERENCES Baker, Stacy Menzel, and Patricia F. Kennedy (1994), “Death by Nostalgia: A Diagnosis of Context-Specific Cases,” in Advances in Consumer Research, vol. 21, Chris Allen and Deborah Roedder Johns, eds., Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 169–74. Bambauer-Sachse, Silke, and Heribert Gierl (2009a), “Can a Positive Mood Counterbalance Weak Arguments in Personal Sales Conversations?,” Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 16 (6), 190–96. ——— and ——— (2009b), “Effects of Nostalgic Advertising through Emotions and the Intensity of the Evoked Mental Images,” in Advances in Consumer Research, vol. 36, Ann L. McGill and Sharon Shavitt, eds., Duluth, MN: Association for Consumer Research, 391–98. Barrett, Frederick, Kevin J. Grimm, Richard W. Robins, Tim Wildschut, Constantine Sedikides, and Petr Janata (2010), “Music-Evoked Nostalgia: Affect, Memory, and Personality,” Emotion, 10 (3), 390–403. Bartier, Anne-Laure (2011), “‘Things Were Better Before’: What Is the Power of Nostalgia Toward the Brand?,” in Louvain School of Management Working Paper Series, Per J. Agrell, ed., Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium: Louvain School of Management Research Institute, 1–22. Batcho, Krystine I. (1995), “Nostalgia: A Psychological Perspective,” Perceptual and Motor Skills, 80 (1), 131–43. ——— (2007), “Nostalgia and the Emotional Tone and Content of Song Lyrics,” American Journal of Psychology, 120 (3), 361–81. Baumgartner, Hans (1992), “Remembrance of Things Past: Music, Autobiographical Memory, and Emotion,” in Advances in Consumer Research, vol. 19, John F. Sherry, Jr., and Brian Sternthal, eds., Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 613–20. Belk, Russell W. (1988), “Possessions and the Extended Self,” Journal of Consumer Research, 15 (2), 139–68. ——— (1990), “The Role of Possessions in Constructing and Maintaining a Sense of Past,” in Advances in Consumer Research, vol. 17, Marvin E. Goldberg, Gerald Gorn, and Richard W. Pollay, eds., Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 669–76. Braun, Kathryn A. (1999), “Postexperience Advertising Effects on Consumer Memory,” Journal of Consumer Research, 25 (4), 319–34. ———, Rhiannon Ellis, and Elizabeth F. Loftus (2002), “Make My Memory: How Advertising Can Change Our Memories of the Past,” Psychology and Marketing, 19 (1), 1–23. Braun-LaTour, Kathryn A. (2007), “Beyond Nostalgia: Using Childhood Memories as a Means to Emotionally Connect with Consumers,” International Journal of Advertising, 26 (3), 402–05. ———, Michael S. LaTour, and George M. Zinkhan (2007), “Using Childhood Memories to Gain Insight into Brand Meaning,” Journal of Marketing, 71 (2), 45–60. Brown, Stephen, Robert V. Kozinets, and John F. Sherry, Jr. (2003), “Teaching Old Brands New Tricks: Retro Branding and the Revival of Brand Meaning,” Journal of Marketing, 67 (3), 19–33. Brown, Steven P., and Douglas M. Stayman (1992), “Antecedents and Consequences of Attitude toward the Ad: A Meta-Analysis,” Journal of Consumer Research, 19 (1), 34–51. Burke, Marian C., and Julie A. Edell (1989), “The Impact of Feelings on Ad-Based Affect and Cognition,” Journal of Marketing Research, 26 (1), 69–83. Chou, Hsuan-Yi, and Nai-Hwa Lien (2010), “Advertising Effects of Songs’ Nostalgia and Lyrics’ Relevance,” Asia Pacific Journal of Marketing and Logistics, 22 (3), 314–29. Craik, Fergus I. M., and Robert S. Lockhart (1972), “Levels of Processing: A Framework for Memory Research,” Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11 (6), 671–84. Davis, Fred (1979), Yearning for Yesterday: A Sociology of Nostalgia, New York: Free Press. Derbaix, Christian M. (1995), “The Impact of Affective Reactions on Attitude toward the Advertisement and the Brand: A Step toward Ecological Validity,” Journal of Marketing Research, 32 (4), 470–79. Divard, Ronan, and Philippe Robert-Demontrond (1997), “La Nostalgie: Un Thème Récent Dans la Recherche Marketing,” Recherche et Applications en Marketing, 12 (4), 41–62. Elliott, Stuart (2009), “Warm and Fuzzy Makes a Comeback,” New York Times, April 6, B3. Evans, Robert D., Philip M. Hart, John E. Cicala, and Dan L. Sherrell (2010), “Elvis: Dead and Loving It—The Influence of Attraction, Nostalgia, and Risk in Dead Celebrity Attitude Formation,” Journal of Management and Marketing Research, 3 (1), 1–13. Goulding, Christina (2001), “Romancing the Past: Heritage Visiting and the Nostalgic Consumer,” Psychology and Marketing, 18 (6), 565–92. Grysman, Azriel, and Judith A. Hudson (2011), “The Self in Autobiographical Memory: Effects of Self-Salience on Narrative Content and Structure,” Memory, 19 (5), 501–13. Havlena, William J., and Susan L. Holak (1991), “The Good Old Days: Observations on Nostalgia and Its Role in Consumer Behavior,” in Advances in Consumer Research, vol. 18, Rebecca H. Holman and Michael R. Solomon, eds., Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 323–29. EXPLORING NOSTALGIC ADVERTISING EFFECTS Hofer, Johannes (1688), “Medical Dissertation on Nostalgia,” Carolyn Kiser Anspach, trans., Bulletin of the History of Medicine (1934), 2, 376–91. Holak, Susan L., and William J. Havlena (1992), “Nostalgia: An Exploratory Study of Themes and Emotions in the Nostalgic Experience,” in Advances in Consumer Research, vol. 19, John F. Sherry, Jr., and Brian Sternthal, eds., Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 380–87. ———, and ——— (1998), “Feelings, Fantasies, and Memories: An Examination of the Emotional Components of Nostalgia,” Journal of Business Research, 42 (3), 217–26. Holbrook, Morris B. (1993a), “Nostalgia and Consumption Preferences: Some Emerging Patterns of Consumer Tastes,” Journal of Consumer Research, 20 (2), 245–56. ——— (1993b), “On the New Nostalgia: ‘These Foolish Things’ and Echoes of the Dear Departed Past,” in Continuities in Popular Culture: The Present in the Past and the Past in the Present and Future, Ray B. Browne and Ronald J. Ambrosetti, eds., Bowling Green, OH: Bowling Green State University Popular Press, 74–120. ———, and Rajeev Batra (1987), “Assessing the Role of Emotions as Mediators of Consumer Responses to Advertising,” Journal of Consumer Research, 14 (3), 404–20. ———, and Robert M. Schindler (1991), “Echoes of the Dear Departed Past: Some Work in Progress on Nostalgia,” in Advances in Consumer Research, vol. 18, Rebecca H. Holman and Michael R. Solomon, eds., Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 330–33. ———, and ——— (1996), “Market Segmentation Based on Age and Attitude toward the Past: Concepts, Methods, and Findings Concerning Nostalgic Influences on Customers Tastes,” Journal of Business Research, 37 (1), 27–39. Janata, Petr (2009), “The Neural Architecture of Music-Evoked Autobiographical Memories,” Cerebral Cortex, 19 (11), 2579–94. ———, Stefan T. Tomic, and Sonja K. Rakowski (2007), “Characterisation of Music-Evoked Autobiographical Memories,” Memory, 15 (8), 845–60. Keller, Kevin L. (1993), “Conceptualizing, Measuring, and Managing Customer-Based Brand Equity,” Journal of Marketing, 57 (1), 1–22. Kotre, John (1995), White Gloves: How We Create Ourselves through Memory, New York, NY: Free Press. Kusumi, Takashi, Ken Matsuda, and Eriko Sugimori (2010), “The Effects of Aging on Nostalgia in Consumers’ Advertisement Processing,” Japanese Psychological Research, 52 (3), 150–62. Leboe, Jason P., and Tamara L. Ansons (2006), “On Misattributing Good Remembering to a Happy Past: An Investigation in to the Cognitive Roots of Nostalgia,” Emotion, 6 (4), 596–610. Loveland, Katherine E., Dirk Smeesters, and Naomi Mandel (2010), “Still Preoccupied with 1995: The Need to Belong and Preference for Nostalgic Products,” Journal of Consumer Research, 37 (3), 393–408. MacKenzie, Scott B., Richard J. Lutz, and George E. Belch (1986), “The Role of Attitude Toward the Ad as a Mediator of Advertising Effectiveness: A Test of Competing Explanations,” Journal of Marketing Research, 23 (2), 130–43. Marchegiani, Christopher A., and Ian Phau (2005), “Dimensionalising Nostalgic Appeals in Broadcast Advertisements: Effects of Personal Nostalgia, Historical Nostalgia and Period Oriented Music,” in Broadening the Boundaries, Sharon Purchase, ed., Perth, Western Australia: The University of Western Australia, 109–16. ———, and ——— (2010), “Effect of Personal Nostalgic Response Intensity on Cognitions, Attitudes, and Intentions,” Journal of Research in Interactive Marketing, 4 (3), 241–56. McAdams, Dan P., Jeffrey Reynolds, Martha Lewis, Allison H. Patten, and Phillip J. Bowman (2001), “When Bad Things Turn Good and Good Things Turn Bad: Sequences of Redemption and Contamination in Life Narrative and Their Relation to Psychosocial Adaptation in Midlife Adults and in Students,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27 (4), 474–85. 83 Merchant, Altaf, and John B. Ford (2008), “Nostalgia and Giving to Charity: A Conceptual Framework for Discussion and Research,” International Journal of Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Marketing, 13 (1), 13–30. ———, ———, and Gregory Rose (2011), “How Personal Nostalgia Influences Giving to Charity,” Journal of Business Research, 64 (6), 610–16. Moore, Danny L., and J. Wesley Hutchinson (1985), “The Influence of Affective Reactions to Advertising: Direct and Indirect Mechanisms of Attitude Change,” in Psychological Processes and Advertising Effects: Theory, Research, and Application, Linda F. Alwitt and Andrew A. Mitchell, eds., Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 65–87. Moore, David J., and Pamela M. Homer (2004), “Self-Brand Connections: An Exploratory Study into Construct Validity and Gender Effects,” in Advances in Consumer Research, vol. 31, Barbara E. Kahn and Mary Frances Luce, eds., Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 196–97. ———, and ——— (2008), “Self-Brand Connections: The Role of Attitude Strength and Autobiographical Memory Primes,” Journal of Business Research, 61 (7), 707–14. Muehling, Darrel D. (2013), “The Relative Influence of Advertising-Evoked Personal and Historical Nostalgic Thoughts on Consumers’ Brand Attitudes,” Journal of Marketing Communications, 19 (2), 98–113. ———, and Michelle McCann (1993), “Attitude toward the Ad: A Review,” Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 15 (2), 25–58. ———, and Vincent J. Pascal (2011), “An Empirical Investigation of the Differential Effects of Personal, Historical, and Non-Nostalgic Advertising on Consumer Responses,” Journal of Advertising, 40 (2), 107–22. ———, and ——— (2012), “An Involvement Explanation for Nostalgia Advertising Effects,” Journal of Promotion Management, 18 (1), 100–18. ———, and David E. Sprott (2004), “The Power of Reflection: An Empirical Examination of Nostalgia Advertising Effects,” Journal of Advertising, 33 (3), 25–36. Naughton, Keith, and Bill Vlasic (1998), “The Nostalgia Boom,” Business Week, March 23, 58–64. Olsen, Barbara (1993), “Brand Loyalty and Lineage: Exploring New Dimensions for Research,” in Advances in Consumer Research, vol. 20, Leigh McAlister and Michael L. Rothschild, eds., Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 575–79. Orth, Ulrich R. and Aurelie Bourrain (2008), “The Influence of Nostalgic Memories on Consumer Exploratory Tendencies: Echoes from Scents Past,” Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 15 (4), 277–87. Parekh, Rupal (2012), “GE Gets Nostalgic with BuzzFeed Sponsorship, Social-Media Push,” Ad Age Media News, September 17, http://adage.com/article/media/ge-nostalgic-buzzfeed-sponsorship-socialmedia-push/237238/ Park, C. Whan, Deborah J. MacInnis, Joseph Priester, Andreas B. Eisingerich, and Dawn Iacobucci (2010), “Brand Attachment and Brand Attitude Strength: Conceptual and Empirical Differentiation of Two Critical Brand Equity Drivers,” Journal of Marketing, 74 (6), 1–17. Pascal, Vincent J., David E. Sprott, and Darrel D. Muehling (2002), “The Influence of Evoked Nostalgia on Consumers’ Responses to Advertising: and Exploratory Study,” Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 24 (1), 39–49. Praxmarer, Sandra, and Heribert Gierl (2009), “The Effect of Positive and Negative Ad-Evoked Associations on Brand Attitude,” Asia Pacific Journal of Marketing and Logistics, 21 (4), 507–20. Reisenwitz, Tim H., Rajesh Iyer, and Bob Cutler (2004), “Nostalgia Advertising and the Influence of Nostalgia Proneness,” Marketing Management Journal, 14 (2), 55–66. Routledge, Clay, Jamie Arndt, Tim Wildschut, Constantine Sedikides, Claire Hart, Jacob Juhl, A.J. Vingerhoets, and Wolff Scholtz (2011), “The Past Makes the Present Meaningful: Nostalgia as an Existential Resource,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101 (3), 638–52. Rutherford, Jana, and Eric H. Shaw (2011), “What Was Old Is New Again: The History of Nostalgia as a Buying Motive in Consumption Behavior,” 84 D. D. MUEHLING ET AL. Marketing History in the New World, Leighann C. Neilson, ed., New York: CHARM Association, 157–66. Schacter, Daniel L. (1996), Searching for Memory, New York, NY: Basic Books. Schultz, E.J. (2012), “General Mills Brings Back Green Giant, Cheerios Kid in Nostalgic Appeal,” Advertising Age, September 17, http://adage.com/ article/news/general-mills-brings-back-green-giant-cheerios-kid-nostalgicappeal/237230/ Sierra, Jeremy J., and Shaun McQuitty (2007), “Attitudes and Emotions as Determinants of Nostalgia Purchases: An Application of Social Identity Theory,” Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, 15 (2), 99–112. Stern, Barbara B. (1992), “Historical and Personal Nostalgia in Advertising Text: The Fin de Siecle Effect,” Journal of Advertising, 21 (4), 11–22. Sujan, Mita, James R. Bettman, and Hans Baumgartner (1993), “Influencing Consumer Judgments Using Autobiographical Memories—A Self-Referencing Perspective,” Journal of Marketing Research, 30 (4), 422–36. Thomson, Mathew, Deborah J. MacInnis, and C. Whan Park (2005), “The Ties That Bind: Measuring the Strength of Consumers’ Emotional Attachments to Brands,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 15 (1), 77–91. Vasquez, Diego (2012), “Big Thing in Super Bowl Ads: Our Past,” Media Life Magazine, February 6, http://www.medialifemagazine.com/big-thingin-super-bowl-ads-our-past/ White, Roy B. (2002), “Why It’s Cool to Troll through Time,” Time, April 8, 16. Wildschut, Tim, Constantine Sedikides, Jamie Arndt, and Clay Routledge (2006), “Nostalgia: Content, Riggers, Functions,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97 (5), 975–93. ———, ———, Clay Routledge, Jamie Arndt, and Flippo Cordaro (2010), “Nostalgia as a Repository of Social Connectedness: The Role of Attachment-Related Avoidance,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98 (4), 573–86. Zhou, Xinyue, Tim Wildschut, Constantine Sedikides, Kan Shi, and Cong Feng (2012), “Nostalgia: The Gift That Keeps on Giving,” Journal of Consumer Research, 39 (1), 39–50. APPENDIX Ad Stimuli Nostalgic Ad. (Color figure available online). Nonnostalgic Ad. (Color figure available online). Copyright of Journal of Advertising is the property of Routledge and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.