Download Explaining Behaviorism

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Behavioral modernity wikipedia , lookup

Abnormal psychology wikipedia , lookup

Insufficient justification wikipedia , lookup

Thin-slicing wikipedia , lookup

Applied behavior analysis wikipedia , lookup

Theory of planned behavior wikipedia , lookup

Impression formation wikipedia , lookup

Theory of reasoned action wikipedia , lookup

Adherence management coaching wikipedia , lookup

Neuroeconomics wikipedia , lookup

Social perception wikipedia , lookup

Psychophysics wikipedia , lookup

Behavior analysis of child development wikipedia , lookup

Verbal Behavior wikipedia , lookup

Attribution (psychology) wikipedia , lookup

Descriptive psychology wikipedia , lookup

Psychological behaviorism wikipedia , lookup

Behaviorism wikipedia , lookup

Classical conditioning wikipedia , lookup

Operant conditioning wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Explaining Behaviorism: Operant &
Classical Conditioning
Simple and easy to digest explanations of behaviorism, take 1.
Post published by Eric Charles Ph.D on Feb 28, 2014 in Fixing Psychology
There are many explanations that can be used to help people understand the Behaviorist
Point of View. Some are very factual, others argue towards practical concerns, and still
others are highly philosophical. This is the first in a series of posts trying to show these
styles of explanation in compact and easy-to-digest form. Feedback is welcome. Because of
a guest lecture I must give soon, the first post will focus on outlining operant and classical
conditioning. The order is not meant to imply that this should be the first thing you tell
someone about behaviorism, nor to imply that it is the most convincing line of explanation.
How to Explain Behaviorism, version 1: Operant and Classical Conditioning
Operant and classical conditioning are two different ways in which organisms come to reflect
the order of the environment around them. They are not perfect processes and they certainly
cannot explain facet of human and non-human behavior. That said, they are surprisingly
reliable processes, and they can explain much, much, more about human and non-human
behavior than anyone would have thought before extensive study of those processes began.
It is probably best to think about operant and classical conditioning as offering two different
types of developmental stories. They are not stories about what a behavior is, now, but
rather stories about how that behavior got to be that way. Classical conditioning stories are
about things happening around the animal, no matter what the animal does. Operant
conditioning stories involve consequences of the animal's action, i.e., what happens when
the animal operates upon the world as an active agent. There is some debate about whether
we need two types of stories. There are good reasons to go either way, including some
recent genetic evidence
(link is external)
that they can be disentangled. None of that really matters here; all that matters is that you
understand the two types of stories and their consequences for future behavior.
Note below that "stimulus" can refer to any object, event, or situation that an organism could
potentially respond to. Note also that "response" can be anything the organism does. For
now a "response" could be an overt action (such as jumping up and down), a covert action
(such as tensing your leg without moving it), or even a thinking or feeling, so long as we
conceiving of those as active, rather than passive.
Operant Conditioning
Operant conditioning stories involve an animal doing something that changes the world in a
way that produces, crudely speaking, a good or a bad outcome. When an organism does
something that is followed by a good outcome, that behavior will become more likely in the
future. When an organism does something that is followed by a bad outcome, that behavior
will become less likely in the future. The action and outcome could coincide because of
natural laws or social conventions, because someone purposely set it up that way, or it could
be that the events followed due to random chance in this animals life history. For example, in
pretty much any animal's it is good to stop touching overly-hot objects (natural law), in some
worlds telling a parent you love them results in good outcomes (social convention), and in
some worlds tapping a baseball bat five times on the left corner of the mound is followed by
a home run (random chance).
Operant conditioning stories require that the outcome be reinforcing or punishing to the
particular animal in question. (There are ways to specify that so it does not involve circular
reasoning, but we don't need to go that deep.) For example, candy might reinforce one
person, but not another; some might find a graphic kill-sequence in a violent video game
punishing, while others find it reinforcing; etc.
Over time, the story goes, if a certain type of outcome consistently follows a particular
behavior, this will affect the rate of future behaviors.
Example Traditional Story: A cat is put in a "puzzle box". It performs a wide range of
behaviors, because cats don't like to be in cages. Eventually one of its flailing limbs pulls a
lever that opens the cage door. This happens many times, and each time the lever gets
pulled a little bit quicker (there is no "a ha!" moment).
Tradition vs. Necessity: Traditionally operant conditioning stories start with a relatively
"random" behavior, but they could start with any behavior. Traditionally the story then
introduces an arbitrary consequence, but in real-life situations we usually care about
socially-mediated consequences. Traditionally many cycles for the consequence to make big
changes in the frequency of future behavior, but sometimes the changes can be quite quick
and others it can take a very long time. In the traditional story the
consequence always follows the behavior, but there are many cool affects that we know
about when it does not the consequence is intermittent (i.e., the "schedule of reinforcement").
Traditionally the consequence has to be immediately following the behavior, though there
are some exceptions, you probably want to stick with the traditional version here.
Enhanced Traditional Story: Often operant conditioning stories are enhanced by adding a
"discriminative stimuli", which indicates that a particular contingency (a particular connection
between action and outcome) is in effect. For example, an experimenter working with rats
might have a light that, when on, means that lever pressing will result in food. Similarly, a
special education instructor might have a picture of a hat that, when held up, means that
saying "hat" will result in an M&M.
Other Classical Conditioning Stuff: You can do amazing things with discriminative stimuli.
You can train people to respond to very specific stimuli, or to very general "categories" of
stimuli. For example, we can get pigeons to discriminate early Monet's paintings from
Picasso's. Also, by drawing out the "schedule" of reinforcement, you can also train animals
to respond for many, many times without getting reinforced. For example, we can get people
to pull slot machine levers scores of times without a win.
After Conditioning: After the events of an Operant Conditioning story, a behavior either has
an increased or decreased rate of occurrence. Often there is a big increase or decrease
specifically when a particular stimulus is present. So, if you know the world that a person has
lived in before, you know something about why they respond now in certain ways in the
presence of certain objects, events, or situations.
Classical Conditioning
Classical conditioning stories involve (at least) two things that coincide "out there" in an
animal's world. Those things could coincide because they are causally related due to natural
laws or social conventions, or it could be that the events occur at random in relation to each
other and this animal just happens to be the animal that experiences them together. For
example, in pretty-much any animal's world lightning is followed by thunder (natural law), in
some worlds hearing "say cheese" might be followed by a camera flash (social convention),
and in some worlds eating lamb dinners could coincide with hearing bad news from loved
ones (random chance).
Classical conditioning stories also require that the organism already have a developed
response to one of the two events. For example, thunder could make you flinch, a bright
flash could make you wince, and bad news from loved ones could make you cry.
Over time, the story goes, if two things are repeatedly paired together out there in the world,
the organism will come to respond to one as they already respond to the other.
Example Traditional Story: When Mary was a child her father liked to take many pictures of
her. He he always said "Say Cheese" before he took the picture, and he always used a flash.
Every time the flash hit Mary, she winced slightly. Now, whenever she hears "Say cheese"
she winces.
Tradition vs. Necessity: Traditionally classical conditioning stories start with a response
that seems unlearned (an Unconditioned Response to an Unconditioned Stimulus), but they
could start with any response the animal already has. Traditionally the story then introduces
something the animal has no existing response to (a Neutral Stimulus), but it usually still
works for stimuli that already elicit some response. Traditionally the neutral stimulus comes
to evoke the response associated with unconditioned stimulus after several pairings (thus
becoming a Conditioned Stimulus), but sometimes only a single pairings is required, and
sometimes neutral stimuli fail to convert to conditioned stimuli even after many, many
pairings. Traditionally the stimuli have to be very close together in time, but sometimes you
can create conditioned stimuli when the pairings are far apart.
In many cases, where the traditional story does not hold, there has been a lot of research
into the exceptions, and we have very good understandings of why such exceptions should
exist. For example, after a single event many animals will learn to avoid novel tastes that
were associated with becoming sick quite a bit later. This makes a lot of evolutionary sense;
poison food present a big risk, and one dose not normally experience the full effects until
quite a bit after ingestion. On the other hand, when dealing with fairly arbitrary pairings of
stimuli, as we get all the time in our modern world, the structure of the traditional story holds.
For example, why should anyone ever have become excited by hearing a computerized
voice say "You've got mail!"? Because of several pairings, that's why.
Other Classical Conditioning Stuff: You can do amazing things here with generalization
and discrimination training, and there are many other interesting phenomenon that scientists
have discovered.
After Conditioning: After the events of a Classical Conditioning story, the presence of a
conditioned stimulus elicits a conditioned response. So, if you know the world that a person
has lived in before, you know something about why they respond to certain things in certain
ways now.
A Bit of Light Theory
Philosophical behaviorism can be very deep. In this context, all I will say is that most
behaviorists believe we can explain a great deal about human behavior using the types of
stories above. That is, the preferred style to a run of the mill "Why did he do that?!?"
question will begin with "Well, in the past history of that person, doing that behavior resulted
in...."
Because these explanations are all about the way the world around the person works, and
the person's past history in that world, you don't need to include traditional "mental"
explanations. That doesn't mean that traditional "mental" stuff doesn't exist, but it does
suggest that we can explain an awful lot about human behavior before we would need to
start talking about them.