Download Successful Aging: A Developmental Approach

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Environmental psychology wikipedia , lookup

Unilineal evolution wikipedia , lookup

History of the social sciences wikipedia , lookup

Cognitive psychology wikipedia , lookup

Leader development wikipedia , lookup

Social psychology wikipedia , lookup

Anthropology of development wikipedia , lookup

Positive disintegration wikipedia , lookup

Sociological theory wikipedia , lookup

Postdevelopment theory wikipedia , lookup

Erikson's stages of psychosocial development wikipedia , lookup

Social perception wikipedia , lookup

Development theory wikipedia , lookup

Political psychology wikipedia , lookup

Development economics wikipedia , lookup

Psychosexual development wikipedia , lookup

Personality psychology wikipedia , lookup

Bioecological model wikipedia , lookup

Life extension wikipedia , lookup

Developmental psychology wikipedia , lookup

Gerontology wikipedia , lookup

Successful aging wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
The unique challenges and growth processes of middle and late adulthood have not been
fully incorporated into previous studies of successful aging. This article examines earlier
conceptualizations of successful aging and calls for a reformulation that is more responsive
to developmental processes and theoretical guidance. Issues of operational definitions,
selective sampling, and stage theory are discussed, and the perspective is illustrated with
empirical research in the personality realm.
Successful Aging: A Developmental
Approach
Carol D. Ryff, PhD1
Like goodness, truth, and other human ideals, successful aging may appeal more than it illuminates. It
is an image that attracts human interest but defies easy
or consensual definition. As growing numbers of people anticipate a long old age, the meaning of successful aging takes on even greater importance. Formulations of the concept become standards by which individuals evaluate themselves and others, as well as
visions toward which to strive. In attempting to advance understanding of how these years might be fully
experienced, this article examines previous perspectives on successful aging and presents a new alternative. The aim is to illustrate how a developmental
orientation adds new challenges and dimensions to
the task of growing old successfully.
has been investigated in terms of happiness, adjustment, morale, health, survival, subjective well-being,
and the balance between aspirations and achievements (Cutler, 1979; George, 1979; Larson, 1978; Palmore, 1979). These dimensions are accompanied by a
multiplicity of measures and empirical findings, 30
years of which have been recently reviewed by Larson
(1978). The key question integrating this wide array of
work is what variable or variables best predict (correlate with) life satisfaction, happiness, morale, etc. Factors investigated as possible clues to the mystery of
growing old successfully include health, socioeconomic status, age, sex, race, employment, marital status, availability of transportation, residence, activity,
and social interaction (Larson, 1978; Lohman, 1977;
Palmore, 1979). Among the prominent predictors are
social activity, health, marital status, and socioeconomic status, although even in combination these
variables leave the greatest proportion of variance
in subjective well-being unexplained (Larson, 1978).
Cutler (1979), perhaps in response to this showing, has
recently called for a reexamination of the life satisfaction construct, contending that it is a multidimensional
rather than a unidimensional system and that the
separate components must be clearly delineated. In
addition, Cutler provided empirical evidence that the
particular dimensions comprising life satisfaction vary
across age groups.
A further variant on the topic of successful aging
warranting mention is the perspective of Fozard and
Popkin (1978). Focusing on intervention by way of
environmental engineering and individual counseling,
their objective is to optimize adults' abilities to adjust
to various aging processs in vision, memory, and learning spheres, as well as to maximize individual choices
in realms of work, health, and leisure. The optimization analysis presented stresses the changing interplay
between the individual and the environment at different phases of the life course. Thus, in contrast to
the life satisfaction orientation wherein subjective selfperceptions are paramount, this approach emphasizes
the significance of environmental factors and external
elements in the attainment of optimal functioning.
Previous Approaches to Successful Aging
Four dimensions of successful aging were proposed
in the Kansas City Studies of Adult Life: the amount of
activity in which the individual engaged, the ability to
disengage, satisfaction with life, and maturity or integration of personality. Williams and Wirths (1965) later
derived two additional views of successful aging: a
balanced exchange of energy between the individual
and the social system, and a stable social system. Of
these various formulations, life satisfaction became
the most frequently investigated dimension of successful aging. Life satisfaction included the components of zest versus apathy, resolution and fortitude,
relationships between desired goals and achieved
goals, self-concept, and mood tone (Neugarten, Havighurst, & Tobin, 1961). A vast body of gerontological
research was generated by this early formulation, so
much so that life satisfaction has been described as
the most widely studied variable in the field of aging
(Maddox & Wiley, 1976). The abundance of empirical
work elaborated and differentiated the concept of life
satisfaction far beyond its initial definition. Thus, in
addition to the original dimensions, successful aging
1
Assistant Professor, Department of Psychology, Fordham University,
Bronx, NY 10458.
Vol. 22, No. 2, 1982
209
and knowledge application (Baltes & Willis, 1977).
That is, theoretical guidance will advance research
beyond exploratory stages into more formalized, hypothesis-testing endeavors.
Danish and D'Augelli (1980) have summarized, in a
generic fashion, the characteristics of the optimization
model being advocated. These include statements
about desirable goals or end-states of behavior, a focus
on sequential change, emphasis on techniques of optimization, consideration of the individual as an integrative biopsychosocial unit, and a view of development occurring in a changing biocultural context. Such
a model of progressive growth processes will generate
knowledge critical from the interventive standpoint
wherein the traditional emphasis on alleviative, afterthe-problem strategies is being challenged by a concern for preventive or enhancement strategies. Thus,
rather than focus on the identification of conditions
leading to problems, the optimization model focuses
on identification of conditions that lead to growth.
The aim is to help individuals encounter the future so
that events become opportunities for self-enhancement. The implementation of such a model is, however, contingent on a well-grounded knowledge of
adaptive functioning. The optimization, growth-oriented theories to be presented are viewed as mechanisms by which such knowledge could accumulate.
Personality, defined as the enduring dimensions or
distinctive characteristics of the individual (Staub,
1980), is a useful forum for illustrating the merit of a
developmental approach to successful aging. To begin
with, personality does not have the built-in inevitabilities of decline characteristic of physiologically linked
processes such as memory, vision, or cognition. It
might even be speculated that personality is the realm
compensating for loss in other spheres. Additionally,
personality as a realm of study can persuasively illustrate a theory-guided approach because it possesses
the necessary optimization theories. These theories
have, for the most part, been neglected in the formulation of relevant research. Renewed attention to the
richness of available theory and heightened commitment to refining and advancing such theory represent
a major objective of the present perspective.
When examining the above perspectives on successful aging, two notable points emerge. First, theoretical guidance in the related investigations is lacking.
The research in general represents exploratory ventures wherein particular measures of well-being are
correlated with various indices of context (e.g., marital
status, socioeconomic status, health). A priori theoretical accounts of why certain variables are likely to be
correlated with life satisfaction are missing, and obtained findings are left in a sort of explanatory ambiguity. Even the recent causal model of life satisfaction
proposed by Markides and Martin (1979) was formulated more on the basis of psychometric explorations
than the development of an explicit theoretical rationale. From the standpoint of environmental engineering
(Fozard & Popkin, 1978), the lack of theory makes it
additionally difficult to define what constitutes optimal vision, memory, or learning among older adults.
Second, and most important, research on successful
aging has focused largely on dependent variables that
could characterize optimal functioning at any age
period. Happiness, adjustment, and morale are more
measures of successful living, regardless of age, than
they are indicants of the unique challenges of growing
old effectively. The possibility that successful aging
may involve the resolution of issues separate from
other age periods is intimated in Cutler's (1979) differentiation of life satisfaction according to age groups,
as well as Fozard and Popkin's (1978) emphasis on the
unique environmental demands and adaptive capacities at various phases of adulthood. It is also indicated
by the adaptive tasks of aging formulated by Clark
and Anderson (1967). These included having a perception of aging and a sense of instrumental limitations,
redefining one's physical and social life space, substituting alternative sources of need satisfaction, reassessing the criteria for evaluation of self, and reintegrating values and life goals. The identification of the
unique qualities of successful living in adulthood and
aging, along with the concern for an interpretive theoretical framework, constitute key elements of the
alternative approach to successful aging being advocated herein.
Successful Aging from a Theory-Guided
Developmental Perspective
Revitalizing Neglected Theory
The newness of the present approach to successful
aging is reflected in its joint focus on developmental
processes and theoretical guidance. The developmental aspect underscores the aim of identifying the
uniquely positive characteristics and challenges of
adulthood and old age. That is, a developmental orientation searches for the higher, more differentiated
growth processes that occur with aging rather than
examining essentially nondevelopmental dimensions
(e.g., life satisfaction) that could be used to investigate
subjective well-being in any age period. These growthoriented developmental changes will be pursued
through application of existing theoretical models, the
second major element of a new approach to successful
aging. The emphasis on theory responds to a concern
for a systematic approach to knowledge generation
The psychosocial stage model of ego development
formulated by Erikson (1959) is perhaps the most wellknown exemplar of life-span developmental theory.
With regard to adulthood, the primary crises for the
expanding ego are the tasks of intimacy (young adulthood), generativity (middle adulthood), and integrity
(old age). Empirical research has generally employed
these concepts in an ex post facto fashion: They are
presented in discussion sections to interpret obtained
findings rather than relied on in the formation of
research designs or measurement instruments. There
are notable exceptions, studies in which the intent has
been to operationalize the concepts themselves
(Baker, 1971; Boyd & Koskela, 1970; Constantinople,
1969; Marcia, 1966; Whitbourne & Waterman, 1979),
although these have focused primarily on the identity
210
The Gerontologist
issues of adolescence and the intimacy concerns of
young adulthood.
The ego challenge of middle adulthood, generativity
versus stagnation, is largely unexplored empirically. It
is expressed theoretically through a concern for establishing and guiding the next generation and showing
an emphasis on productivity and creativity. These theoretical ideas suggest possibilities for growth for the
middle-aged individual—the opportunity to move beyond the self-directed concerns of identity or the
interpersonal dimensions of intimacy into a phase
wherein one's skill and knowledge are shared with
younger individuals and one assumes leadership and
decision-making roles.
The final stage of ego development, integrity versus
despair, is perhaps the most enticing and elusive of all
stages. Many aspects of integrity have been identified
by Erikson (1959): emotional integration, accepting
one's life cycle as something that had to be, feeling a
comradeship with the ways of distant times, having
adapted oneself to the triumphs and disappointments
of being, possessing a love of humankind rather than
self, and finally, achieving a spiritual sense that eliminates the fear of death. Surely one who evidences
these qualities would be deemed to have aged successfully. More important is the possibility that these
achievements are distinct to old age and are intrinsic
to reaching the end of a long life. As expressed by
Jung (1933):
inner-world orientation (Gutmann, 1964; Neugarten,
1968), provide additional clarity to the unique personality demands of middle and late adulthood.
Perhaps least attended to among available optimization theories is Buhler's (Buhler, 1935; Buhler &
Massarik, 1968) formulation of the basic life tendencies
that work toward the fulfillment of life. Buhler, in
response to the pervasive Freudian view of the 1930's,
asserted that homeostasis was not a goal in itself but
merely a favorable condition of functioning. She stated
that the organism's goal was to be active and productive in a reality that allowed for accomplishments.
Thus, Buhler proposed need satisfaction, self-limiting
adaptation, creative expansion, and upholding internal order as basic life tendencies.
Their implicit ultimate intent is self-development, the
establishment of contacts, the mastering of reality, the fulfillment of life through integrated actualization of the individual's potentials (Buhler & Massarik, 1968, p. 923).
Need satisfaction (the pursuit of tension-reducing satisfiers) and self-limiting adaptation (adapting one's
behavior to that of others in order to belong and
participate) are the prominent tendencies or goals of
infancy and later childhood. Creative expansion, the
eminent tendency of adulthood, refers to goals of
advancing in the world and changing it creatively
through physical or mental activities. It stresses leadership, influence, productivity, and even aggressive
behavior. Upholding internal order, the most complex
of the basic life tendencies prevails during the climacteric age (a phrase used symbolically by Buhler to refer
to the lengthy period of time following reproductive
capacity). This tendency comprises different ordering
principles that work toward the unity of personality
and behavior. In later life such principles are found in
the integrating operations of goals, ideals, and selfassessments. In combination, Buhler's life tendencies
of adulthood and old age hold merit for the investigation of successful aging. They both highlight the
unique challenges of various life periods and provide
guidelines as to the type of behaviors that will evidence successful resolution of the challenge.
When viewed in concert, the above works warrant
three summary points. First, there is no single form of
successful development whether in middle or old age.
The theories reflect a multiplicity of characteristics by
which one might be deemed as developing optimally.
Hence, the dilemma of value judgments inherent in
defining successful aging is mitigated by the variety of
behaviors reflective of growth. Second, although the
present treatment has stressed the positive, bright side
of change, it is relevant to note that many of the above
theorists provided telling descriptions of what might
be considered unsuccessful aging. These are further
helpful in differentiating the concept of optimal development and useful in identifying impediments to
its attainment. Finally, it must be acknowledged that
the theoretical ideas proffered exist in crude form;
they are a blend of enticing words that for the most
part lack explicit formulations or interlocking statements of laws. They are, in short, prototheories (Baltes
& Willis, 1977) and represent the mere beginnings of
A human being would certainly not grow to be seventy or
eighty years old if this longevity had no meaning for the
species to which he belonged. The afternoon of human life
must also have a significance of its own and cannot be
merely a pitiful appendage to life's morning (p. 109).
Thus, whether manifest through ego integrity or Jung's
concept of self-illumination, the unique significance
of old age emerges and the unique growth processes
are further identified.
Neugarten (1968, 1973), through a combination of
theoretical and empirical endeavors, has also clarified
the specific personality challenges of middle and old
age. With regard to the former, she has formulated the
"executive processes" of middle age: self-awareness,
selectivity, manipulation and control of the environment, mastery, and competence. She has described
the "incredible complexity" shown in the behavior of
the middle-aged business executive who manages and
schedules a demanding professional life, differentiates
between responsibilities to be delegated to others and
those to be completed by oneself, and further manages to satisfy a multiplicity of emotional, sexual, and
aesthetic needs. In contrast to the 47 year old's view
of the environment as rewarding risk-taking and boldness, Neugarten described the 60 year old as being
more conforming and accommodative to outer-world
demands. This inward turning of ego functions, known
as the process of interiority, represents a key personality issue of old age. It is perhaps through such inward
turning that one manages to achieve the emotional
integration described by Erikson. The above processes,
further described as the active-to-passive mastery sequence and the change from an outer-world to an
Vol. 22, No. 2, 1982
211
theoretical precision. From the standpoint of successful aging, the starting points are sufficiently rich to
merit efforts toward theoretical clarification, refinement, and extension.
Explaining the Theoretical Neglect
The rich dimensions suggested by the above theoretical formulations have not had a broad impact on
personality research in adulthood and old age. One
explanation for the neglect stems from the failure to
operationalize the proposed constructs. Ego integrity
has appealed to the interests of many gerontological
researchers, but few have attempted to translate its
meaning to strategies of empirical investigation. An
additional and more current reason for inattention to
these developmental formulations is related to broadranging attacks on stage theory. Criticisms of stage
theory have occurred within cognitive (Labouvie-Vief
& Chandler, 1978) and life-span developmental domains (Baltes, 1979; Brim & Kagan, 1980). Stage theory,
having its origin in biological growth models of development, is seen as representing an inflexible formulation, one requiring developmental progressions
that are unidirectional, irreversible, hierarchical, invariant, and universal. Such rigid models have been
soundly derided for ignoring a multiplicity of influences surrounding development including cohort effects, situational variability, cultural diversity, and individual differences.
In order that existing stage theories of adult personality development be meaningfully revitalized, the
above criticisms must be addressed. With regard to
inflexible developmental models, it is important to
recognize that biological-growth-oriented models represent but one approach to stage theory. Loevinger's
(1966,1976) theory of ego development, for example,
does not advocate universality but rather that, if development does occur, it will follow certain directions.
Similarly, Emmerich (1968), in his insightful discussion
of the structural aspects of personality development,
clarified Erikson's theory as combining classical developmental orientation (read stage theory) with differential analysis, which considers how in the course of
development individuals become sorted into differentiated subgroups. That is, Erikson's psychosocial
crises emerge in a fixed developmental order although
the resolution of each crisis is conceptualized in bipolar terms representing differing outcomes and
thereby incorporating individual differences within
the model. Runyan (1980) has also outlined a model
that offers the possibility of tracing different individual
pathways through a sequence of common stages. With
regard to hierarchical aspects of stage theory, Emmerich further distinguished between varying perspectives, from the most radical case in which higher levels
displace lower levels of development to the more
flexible form in which highest levels displace lower
levels, but the latter remain latent and reemerge if
circumstances warrant. Emmerich concluded that
"personality research has not yet clarified which of
these alternatives is most applicable" (p. 647).
Hence, the rigid inflexibility of classical stage theory
212
is not an appropriate characterization of a stage approach within the personality domain. Available theories capitalize on the positive aspects of stage theory
by formulating optimal development, but they do so
without the straitjacket imposed by biological growth
models. Parenthetically, it is worth noting that stage
theories of personality development have always been
more attentive to the unique issues of adulthood and
aging than stage theories in other domains such as
cognitive development in which final developmental
changes were postulated to occur in adolescence (Piaget, 1970). Generativity, integrity, interiority, creative
expansion, and inner order were all formulated to
capture the unique aspects of adulthood and aging.
Personality theories have, in sum, embodied the best
of life-span thinking by formulating new opportunities
for growth and development at all phases of life.
Beyond Surface Appeal
The task of revitalizing interest in optimization theories of adult personality development is not one of
making a persuasive case on a verbal level—the related
theories have long held sufficient surface appeal. The
real test is to translate these theoretical constructs to
research strategies so as to enable empirical advancement. Thus far, the clinical domain has made the most
strides in this direction as illustrated by the works of
Gould (1978), Levinson (1978), and Vaillant (1977).
These applications have been largely idiographic, reflecting clinical case studies, and have focused primarily on young adult and middle-aged men. With
regard to the growth processes of old age, there is a
beginning of empirical work. Boylin et al. (1976) examined the relationship between reminiscence and
ego integrity in institutionalized men. Subscales for
the last three of Erikson's stages were developed in the
study although reliability and validity assessments
were not made. Also focused on ego integrity is the
research of Walaskay and Whitbourne (1980). Working with community-dwelling men and women and
using semistructured interviews, they have investigated variations in the process of resolving ego integrity. Four integrity statuses have been formulated:
integrity achieving, dissonant (in crisis), foreclosed
(avoiding crisis), and despairing. These statuses have,
in turn, been related to reminiscence activity, death
attitudes and preparation, psychological well-being,
and the last three of Erikson's stages with preliminary
evidence of construct validity.
Personality development in the transition from middle age to old age has been examined in the research
of Ryff. A first study (Ryff & Baltes, 1976) attempted to
operationalize the shift from the "executive processes"
of middle age to the inwardness of old age, using a
distinction between instrumental and terminal values
(Rokeach, 1973). Data from a sample of middle-aged
and old-aged women supported the prediction of an
emphasis on instrumental values in middle age and a
preference for terminal values in old age. Ryff (1982)
replicated this finding and extended the investigation
to include personality dimensions. A distinction was
made between developmental dimensions of personThe Gerontologist
ality (i.e., those predicted to show perceived change)
and nondevelopmental dimensions (i.e., those predicted to show perceived stability). The former were
selected to parallel many of the previously described
theoretical processes. Findings from a sample of middle-aged and old-aged men and women supported
perceived change on the developmental dimensions
for middle-aged men and perceived stability for the
entire sample. As both of the above studies were
conducted with measures that only approximated the
theoretical processes of interest, Ryff and Heincke
(1981) constructed personality scales to measure the
specific dimensions of generativity, integrity, complexity (drawn from Neugarten's executive processes perspective), and interiority. The scales were developed
using structured assessment procedures, and the findings on a sample of 240 young adult, middle-aged,
and old-aged men and women revealed the predicted
emphasis on generativity in middle age, integrity in
old age, and partial support for interiority as a prominent aspect of self-perception in old age.
Research on successful aging will be advanced by
attentiveness to conceptualizations of new dimensions of growth in adulthood and aging. Whereas the
present perspective relied largely on developmental
theories, it is possible that insight will be gained from
the clinical literature. Allport (1961), for example, has
written extensively on maturity as a characteristic of
adulthood. Numerous aspects of maturity are defined
including emotional security, warm relating to others,
an extension of self, realistic perception, and a unifying
philosophy of life. Roger's fully functioning person
(Rogers & Dymond, 1954) also reflects a concern with
the psychological growth of the individual. Defining
features include self-direction, allowing oneself to be
complex in feelings and viewpoints, openness to experience, trusting oneself, and accepting the values
and experiences of other persons. It is interesting to
note that openness to experience has been investigated as a personality dimension in adulthood and
aging (McCrae & Costa, 1980). Age trends have been
documented in this dimension, and it has been linked
to measures of ego development. Finally, Maslow's
(1971) characteristics of self-actualizing people might
also provide direction for formulating growth processes in the second half of life. Lengthy descriptions
of the characteristics of self-actualized individuals
have been provided. From the present perspective, the
issue is whether these characteristics show developmental features. That is, does the process of self-actualization (or becoming fully functioning, or reaching
maturity) relate to one's stage in life and one's previous life experiences?
temporary aged are expected to show deftness in the
use of leisure time. In remaining open to such variation, optimization theories will avoid the strictures of
previous stage orientations, and yet will provide a
framework for understanding growth processes. Finally, research on successful aging requires that the
rationale for studying select adulthood and aging populations be articulated and implemented. Schaie
(1973) has argued that, if one wants to study optimal
intellectual functioning, one should work with a sample of active, intelligent, and intact aged. Personality
researchers must be equally willing to investigate with
select samples not simply to develop a body of elitest
empirical literature, but to learn whether growth processes do, in fact, occur during these age periods and
to identify the factors involved in the attainment of
higher levels of functioning. Such knowledge is vital
from multiple perspectives including education (e.g.,
countering the view that aging is synonymous with
decline and loss), intervention (e.g., identifying and
facilitating environments that promote growth), and
public policy (e.g., fostering a societal outlook that is
not just benevolent to elderly persons but sees them
as possessing insight unknown to other age periods).
Jung (1933), in discussing the uniqueness of old age,
described the art of living as the most distinguished
and rare of all the arts. He added that only a few
people are artists in life. It is in the study of these
artists that research stands to transform the meaning
and significance of old age.
References
Allport, C.W. Pattern and growth in personality. Holt, Rinehart, and Winston,
New York, 1961.
Baker, F. Measures of ego identity: A multitrait-multimethod validation.
Educational and Psychological Measurement, 1971, 31,165-174.
Bakes, P. B. Life-span developmental psychology: Some converging observations on history and theory. In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim, ]r. (Eds.), Life-span
development and behavior (Vol. 2). Academic Press, New York, 1979.
Baltes, P. B., & Willis, S. L. Toward psychological theories of aging and
development. In ). E. Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the
psychology of aging. Van Nostrand Reinhold, New York, 1977.
Boyd, R. D., & Koskela, R. N. A test of Erikson's theory of ego-stage development by means of a self-report instrument, journal of Experimental Education, 1970, 38,1-14.
Boylin, W., Gordon, S. K., & Nehrke, M. F. Reminiscing and ego integrity in
institutionalized elderly males. Cerontologist, 1976, 76,118-124.
Brim, O. G., )r., & Kagan, J. (Eds.). Constancy and change in human development. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, 1980.
Buhler, C. The curve of life as studied in biographies. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 1935, 19, 405-409.
Buhler, C, & Massarik, F. (Eds.). The course of human life. Springer, New York,
1968.
Clark, M., & Anderson, B. G. Culture and aging. Charles C Thomas, New York,
1967.
Constantinople, A. An Eriksonian measure of personality development in
college students. Developmental Psychology, 1969, 1, 357-372.
Cutler, N. E. Age variations in the dimensionality of life satisfaction. Journal
of Gerontology, 1979, 34, 573-578.
Danish, S. J., & D'Augelli, A. R. Promoting competence and enhancing
development through life development intervention. In L. A. Boyd & J. C.
Rosen (Eds.), Primary prevention of psychopathology (Vol. 4). University
Press of New England, Hanover, NH, 1980.
Emmerich, W. Personality development and concepts of structure. Child
Development, 1968, 39, 671-690.
Erikson, E. Identity and the life cycle. Psychological Issues, 1959, 1,1-171.
Fozard, J. L, & Popkin, S. ]. Optimizing adult development: Ends and means
of an applied psychology of aging. American Psychologist, 1978, 33, 975989.
George, L. K. The happiness syndrome: Methodological and substantive issues
in the study of social-psychological well-being in adulthood. Cerontologist,
1979, 79,210-216.
Gould, R. L. Transformations. Simon & Schuster, New York, 1978.
Gutmann, D. L. An exploration of ego configurations in middle and later life.
Empirical research following from these views of the
growing person would do well to include a concern
for individual differences. For example, variation in
the patterning and timing of growth processes may
occur as a function of sex, educational level, or ethnicity. Sensitivity to historical and cultural variation in
the behaviors deemed optimal is also necessary. Miller
(1968), for example, has noted that during the preindustrial era elderly persons were evaluated in terms of
their ability to persist at physical labor, whereas conVol. 22, No. 2, 1982
213
In B. L. Neugarten & associates (Eds.), Personality in middle and later life.
Atherton, New York, 1964.
)ung, C. C. Modern man in search of a soul. Harcourt Brace and World, Inc.,
New York, 1933.
Labouvie-Vief, C , & Chandler, M. J. Cognitive development and life-span
developmental theory: Idealistic versus contextual perspectives. In P. B.
Bakes (Ed), Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 1). Academic Press,
New York, 1978.
Larson, R. Thirty years of research on the subjective well-being of older
Americans. Journal of Gerontology, 1978, 33,109-125.
Levinson, D. ). The seasons of a man's life. Alfred A. Knopf, Inc., New York,
1978.
Loevinger, J. The meaning and measurement of ego development. American
Psychologist, 1966, 21,195-206.
Loevinger, J. Ego development. )ossey-Bass Publishers, San Francisco, 1976.
Lohman, N. Correlations of life satisfaction, morale, and adjustment measures.
Journal of Gerontology, 1977, 32, 73-75.
Maddox, C. L., & Wiley, J. Scope, concepts, and methods in the study of aging.
In R. Binstock & E. Shanas (Eds.), Handbook of aging and the social sciences.
Van Nostrand Reinhold, New York, 1976.
Marcia, j . E. Development and validation of ego-identity status. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 1966, 3, 551-558.
Markides, K. S., & Martin, H. W. A causal model of life satisfaction among the
elderly. Journal of Gerontology, 1979, 34, 86-93.
Maslow, A. H. The farther reaches of human nature. The Viking Press, New
York, 1971.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. Openness to experience and ego level in
Loevinger's Sentence Completion Test: Dispositional contributions to developmental models of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1980, 39,1179-1190.
Miller, S. ]. The social dilemma of the aging leisure participant. In B. L.
Neugarten (Ed.), Middle age and aging. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, 1968.
Neugarten, B. L. The awareness of middle age. In B. L. Neugarten (Ed.), Middle
age and aging. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, 1968.
Neugarten, B. L. Personality change in late life: A developmental perspective.
In C. Eisdorfer& M. P. Lawton (Eds.), The psychology of adult development
and aging. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, 1973.
Neugarten, B. L., Havighurst, R., & Tobin, S. The measurement of life satisfaction, Journal of Gerontology, 1961, 16, 134-143.
Palmore, E. Predictors of successful aging. Gerontologist, 1979, 19, 427-431.
Piaget, J. Piaget's theory. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael's manual of child
psychology (3rd ed.). Wiley, New York, 1970.
Rogers, C. R., & Dymond, R. (Ed.), Psychotherapy and personality change.
University of Chicago Press, Chicago, 1954.
Rokeach, M. L. The nature of human values. The Free Press, New York, 1973.
Runyan, W. M. A stage-state analysis of the life course. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 1980, 38, 951-962.
Ryff, C. D. Self-perceived personality change in adulthood and aging. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 1982, in press.
Ryff, C. D., & Baltes, P. B. Value transitions and adult development in women:
The instrumentality-terminality sequence hypothesis. Developmental Psychology, 1976, 12, 567-568.
Ryff, C. D., & Heincke, S. G. The subjective organization of personality in
adulthood and aging. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child
Development Meetings, Boston, April 1981.
Schaie, K. W. Methodological problems in descriptive developmental research
on adulthood and aging. In |. R. Nesselroade and H. W. Reese (Ed.), Lifespan developmental psychology. Methodological issues. Academic Press,
New York, 1973.
Staub, E. The nature and study of human personality. In E. Staub (Ed.),
Personality. Basic aspects and current research. Prentice-Hall, Inc., Englewood Cliffs, N), 1980.
Vaillant, C. E. Adaptation to life. Boston: Little, Brown, and Co., 1977.
Walaskay, M., & Whitbourne, S. K. Construction and validation of an ego
integrity status interview. Paper presented at the Cerontological Society
Meeting, San Diego, November 1980.
Whitbourne, S. K., & Waterman, A. S. Psychological development during the
adult years: Age and cohort comparisons. Developmental Psychology, 1979,
15, 373-378.
Williams, R. H., & Wirths, C. G. Lives through the years: Styles of life and
successful aging. Atherton Press, New York, 1965.
University Microfilms
International
300 North Zeeb Road
Dept.RR.
Ann Arbor, Mi. 48106
USA.
30-32 Mortimer Street
Dept. P.R.
London WIN 7RA
England
214
The Gerontologist