Download The Muscarinic Receptor Agonist Xanomeline Has an Antipsychotic

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Discovery and development of angiotensin receptor blockers wikipedia , lookup

Effect size wikipedia , lookup

5-HT2C receptor agonist wikipedia , lookup

Pharmacogenomics wikipedia , lookup

Pharmacokinetics wikipedia , lookup

Cannabinoid receptor antagonist wikipedia , lookup

Hormesis wikipedia , lookup

Chlorpromazine wikipedia , lookup

Nicotinic agonist wikipedia , lookup

NK1 receptor antagonist wikipedia , lookup

Polysubstance dependence wikipedia , lookup

Stimulant wikipedia , lookup

Pharmacognosy wikipedia , lookup

Drug interaction wikipedia , lookup

Atypical antipsychotic wikipedia , lookup

Neuropsychopharmacology wikipedia , lookup

Antipsychotic wikipedia , lookup

Dydrogesterone wikipedia , lookup

Theralizumab wikipedia , lookup

Neuropharmacology wikipedia , lookup

Psychopharmacology wikipedia , lookup

Bilastine wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
0022-3565/01/2992-782–792$3.00
THE JOURNAL OF PHARMACOLOGY AND EXPERIMENTAL THERAPEUTICS
Copyright © 2001 by The American Society for Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics
JPET 299:782–792, 2001
Vol. 299, No. 2
4170/941086
Printed in U.S.A.
The Muscarinic Receptor Agonist Xanomeline Has an
Antipsychotic-Like Profile in the Rat
K. J. STANHOPE, N. R. MIRZA, M. J. BICKERDIKE, J. L. BRIGHT, N. R. HARRINGTON, M. B. HESSELINK,
G. A. KENNETT, S. LIGHTOWLER, M. J. SHEARDOWN, R. SYED, R. L. UPTON, G. WADSWORTH, S. M. WEISS, and
A. WYATT
Vernalis Research Limited, Winnersh, Wokingham, United Kingdom
Received May 23, 2001; accepted August 1, 2001
This paper is available online at http://jpet.aspetjournals.org
attenuated amphetamine-induced hyperactivity at doses that
had no effect on spontaneous activity, possibly indicating a
separation between attenuation of limbic hyperdopaminergic
function and the induction of hypolocomotion. Haloperidol and
clozapine also reversed amphetamine-induced hyperlocomotion, but at similar doses to those that reduced spontaneous
locomotion. Clozapine, but not haloperidol had an anxiolyticlike effect in the CER test. The effects of xanomeline in the CER
test were similar to those of clozapine, although at the anxiolytic dose it tended to disrupt baseline levels of lever pressing.
Finally, haloperidol, clozapine, pilocarpine, and xanomeline, all
induced an increase in dopamine turnover in medial prefrontal
cortex. The antipsychotic-like effects of xanomeline in the animal models used here suggest that it may be a useful treatment for psychosis.
Dysregulation of dopamine is generally considered to be
central to the symptoms of schizophrenia. Over the past 40
years, however, accumulating data indicate that the cholinergic system may also play an important role. Key early
findings include the discovery that compounds that nonselectively block muscarinic cholinergic receptors induce a form of
psychosis in normal humans and exacerbate the symptoms of
schizophrenia (Neubauer et al., 1966a,b). Moreover, antimuscarinic agents were also reported to block the effects of
typical antipsychotics on positive symptoms (Johnstone et
al., 1983).
Although muscarinic receptor agonists have not been
tested in controlled trials with schizophrenic patients, muscarinic receptor agonists and acetylcholinesterase (AChE)
inhibitors both appear to be effective in the treatment of
psychotic symptoms in neurodegenerative disorders. Bodick
et al. (1997) reported that the functionally selective M1/M4
muscarinic receptor agonist xanomeline dose dependently
reduced hallucinations, agitation, and delusions in a 6-month
randomized double-blind, placebo-controlled parallel group
trial with Alzheimer’s patients. Hutchinson and Fazzini
(1996) found that the AChE inhibitor tacrine reduces or
abolishes hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease, and Kaufer
(1998) found that metrifonate and other AChE inhibitors
alleviated neuropsychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer patients,
including a reduction in hallucinations in a multicenter, 26week, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Although it
seems likely that the neural basis for the psychotic symptoms
in neurodegenerative disorders such as Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease differ from that occurring in schizophrenia,
a recent report claiming that the antipsychotic agent olanzapine was effective in the treatment of psychotic symptoms in
Alzheimer’s disease gives some credence to the suggestion
that compounds that treat psychotic symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease may also be effective in schizophrenia (Street et
al., 1999).
ABBREVIATIONS: AChE, acetylcholinesterase; 5-HT, 5-hydroxytryptamine; PPI, prepulse inhibition; CER, conditioned emotional response;
DOPAC, 3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid; HVA, homovanillic acid; dur, rate of lever pressing during each light presentation; pre, interval preceding
each light presentation; CDP, chlordiazepoxide; ANOVA, analysis of variance; CAR, conditioned avoidance response; mPFC, medial prefrontal
cortex.
782
Downloaded from jpet.aspetjournals.org at ASPET Journals on May 2, 2017
ABSTRACT
The muscarinic receptor agonist xanomeline was examined
and compared with the antipsychotics clozapine and/or haloperidol in the following in vivo rat models: apomorphine-induced disruption of prepulse inhibition (PPI), amphetamineinduced hyperlocomotion, and the conditioned emotional
response (CER) test. The effects of xanomeline were also assessed ex vivo on dopamine turnover in the rat medial prefrontal cortex. Under conditions of varying dose and prepulse intensity, xanomeline, like haloperidol, had no effect on PPI. In
contrast, the muscarinic receptor antagonist scopolamine and
the muscarinic receptor agonist pilocarpine both induced significant dose-dependent deficits in PPI. Haloperidol and xanomeline, but not pilocarpine, dose dependently reversed apomorphine-induced disruption of PPI. Thus, xanomeline induced
a clear antipsychotic-like effect in PPI, whereas pilocarpine
appeared to induce a psychotomimetic-like effect. Xanomeline
Antipsychotic-Like Effects of Xanomeline
Materials and Methods
Animals and Maintenance Conditions
Adult male rats were used. Lister hooded rats were obtained from
Harlan Olac (Bicester, Oxon, UK) and Sprague-Dawley rats were
obtained from Charles River (Margate, Kent, UK). All experiments
were conducted between 9:00 AM and 5:00 PM. Rats were grouped
housed under a 12-h light/dark cycle (lights on at 8:00 AM), in a
temperature- (20.5 ⫾ 2.5°C) and humidity-(55 ⫾ 15%) controlled
environment. Unless otherwise mentioned food and water were
freely available. All animal husbandry and experimental procedures
were carried out in accordance with the UK Animals (Scientific
Procedure) Act 1986 governing the health and welfare of laboratory
animals.
Drugs
Haloperidol (Baker Norton, Harlow Essex, UK), xanomeline tartrate (Medicinal Chemistry Department, Vernalis Research Ltd.,
Wokingham, UK), d-amphetamine sulfate, apomorphine, scopolamine HBr, methylscopolamine, clozapine (Sigma-Aldrich, Poole,
Dorset, UK), and pilocarpine (Tocris Cookson, St. Louis, MO) were
used. All dose levels are expressed as free base. The pretreatment
time, route of administration, vehicle, and dosing volume varied
between experiments as outlined below.
PPI
Sprague-Dawley rats (300 –350 g) were tested in four startle
chambers (SR-LAB, San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA). Each
chamber consisted of a Plexiglas cylinder (8.8 cm in diameter)
mounted on a frame and secured to a base unit by four metal pin legs.
Movement of the rat within the cylinder was detected by a piezoelectric accelerometer attached below the frame. A loudspeaker (Radio
Shack Supertweeter) was mounted 24 cm above the cylinder and
provided background white noise and acoustic stimuli (pulse and
prepulses). The whole apparatus was housed in a ventilated and lit
chamber (39 ⫻ 38 ⫻ 58 cm, length ⫻ width ⫻ height). Presentation
of the acoustic stimuli was controlled by SR-LAB software and interface, which also digitized (0 – 4095), rectified, and recorded the
responses from the accelerometer. Mean startle amplitude, the dependent measure, was determined by averaging 150, 1-ms readings
taken at the beginning of the pulse stimulus onset. Sound levels of
the auditory stimuli were calibrated using a noisemeter (model number 1422C; Dawe Instruments, Instrotech, St. Albans, UK).
On the day before PPI testing, rats were placed into the startle
apparatus for a habituation session. After a period of 1 min during
which rats were exposed to background noise (70 dB), the session
began with rats being exposed to different auditory stimuli. The
session began with 10 consecutive startle stimulus trials (120 dB),
which lasted 40 ms. This was followed by 12 of each of two trial types:
1) startle stimulus (120 dB, 40 ms), and 2) startle stimulus preceded
100 ms by a prepulse 3 dB above background noise (70 dB) and
lasting 20 ms. These two trial types were interspersed across six
consecutive blocks, with two of each occurring in each block. The
intertrial interval was variable between 20 and 40 s and sessions
lasted approximately 18 min.
Test Session. After a habituation period of 1 min during which
rats were exposed to background noise (70 dB), the session began
with rats being exposed to different auditory stimuli. The session
began with 10 consecutive startle stimulus trials (120 dB), each of
which lasted 40 ms. This was followed by 12 of each of the following
four trial types: startle stimulus (120 dB, 40 ms), and the startle
stimulus preceded 100 ms by prepulses of 3, 6, or 12 dB above
background noise (70 dB) and lasting 20 ms. These four trial types
occurred across six consecutive blocks, with each trial type occurring
twice within each block. Within each of these six blocks there was
also one null stimulus that was the same as background noise (70 dB,
40 ms). The intertrial interval was variable between 20 and 40 s and
sessions lasted approximately 35 min.
Drugs. All drugs were administered s.c. at a dose volume of 1
ml/kg. Scopolamine HBr, methylscopolamine, haloperidol, pilocarpine, and xanomeline were all dissolved in 0.9% NaCl. Apomorphine was dissolved in a solution of 0.1% ascorbic acid in 0.9% saline.
In study 1, haloperidol (n ⫽ 8), pilocarpine (n ⫽ 9), xanomeline (n ⫽
10), scopolamine HBr (n ⫽ 10 –11), or methylscopolamine (n ⫽ 8)
were tested alone to ascertain their effects on the startle response
and PPI. In this study, all compounds were administered 30 min
before rats were placed in the startle chambers, except pilocarpine,
which was administered 20 min pretest. Study 2 assessed the ability
of haloperidol (n ⫽ 8), pilocarpine (n ⫽ 8), and xanomeline (n ⫽ 9 –10)
Downloaded from jpet.aspetjournals.org at ASPET Journals on May 2, 2017
Xanomeline, as well as other muscarinic receptor agonists,
have been shown to have effects resembling atypical antipsychotics in a number of animal models of schizophrenia,
notably dopamine hyperactivity models (see below). Because
xanomeline does not have appreciable affinity for various
other neurotransmitter receptors, including dopamine receptors, these findings implicate muscarinic receptors as potential targets for modulation of dopaminergic activity (Shannon
et al., 1994; Bymaster et al., 1994). Hagan et al. (1987)
showed that muscarinic receptor agonists and the anticholinesterase inhibitor physostigmine blocked amphetamineinduced ipsilateral rotations in rats with unilateral 6-hydroxydopamine lesions to the nigrostriatal system. In
contrast, muscarinic receptor antagonists increased amphetamine-induced rotations. Muscarinic receptor agonists have
also been shown to decrease apomorphine-induced climbing
(Shannon et al., 2000). In addition, various muscarinic receptor agonists resemble antipsychotics in inhibiting firing of
A10 dopamine neurons, blocking amphetamine-induced c-fos
expression in the nucleus accumbens, and inhibiting conditioned avoidance behavior in the rat (Bymaster et al., 1998a,
1999; Shannon et al., 1999, 2000). Interestingly, the inhibition of A10 firing by muscarinic receptor agonists is immediate, whereas chronic administration of the dopamine and
5-hydroxytryptamine (5-HT) receptor antagonist clozapine is
necessary for this effect, coincident with its delayed therapeutic effect as an antipsychotic agent (Bymaster et al.,
1998a; Shannon et al., 2000). Finally, antipsychotics and
muscarinic receptor agonists have been shown to improve
prepulse inhibition (PPI) disrupted by dopamine receptor
agonists or muscarinic receptor antagonists, respectively
(Swerdlow et al., 1994; Jones and Shannon, 2000a,b).
In the present study we have examined the effects of xanomeline in additional rat models of psychosis. These include
the reversal of amphetamine-induced hyperlocomotion (Arnt,
1995) and apomorphine-induced disruption of prepulse inhibition (Swerdlow et al., 1994). We also examined the effects of
xanomeline on dopamine turnover in the prefrontal cortex as
measured ex vivo. Finally, the effects of xanomeline were
examined and compared with the effects of antipsychotic
agents in the conditioned emotional response (CER) test.
Unlike the other tests, this latter test is used to detect anxiolytic potential as opposed to antipsychotic potential of compounds (Davis, 1990). However, atypical, but not typical,
antipsychotics have an anxiolytic-like profile in conflict models of anxiety in animals (Moore et al., 1994). Thus, this test
was included to determine whether the effects of a muscarinic receptor agonist would resemble those of either typical
or atypical antipsychotics.
783
784
Stanhope et al.
to reverse the effects of apomorphine on PPI. In this interaction
study, haloperidol, pilocarpine, and xanomeline were administered
30 min pretest and apomorphine was administered 10 min pretest.
Control rats were administered with the appropriate vehicle at the
appropriate pretreatment times. Thus, all animals in study 2 received either apomorphine or its vehicle 10 min before the session
and the target drug and its vehicle 30 min before the session.
Amphetamine-Induced Hyperlocomotion
Dopamine Turnover in Rat Prefrontal Cortex
Sprague-Dawley rats (300 –350 g) were weighed before s.c. administration of drug or vehicle control (1 ml/kg, n ⫽ 7– 8 depending upon
study). In study 1, rats were administered either vehicle control (8%
Tween in saline), haloperidol (0.1 or 1 mg/kg), or clozapine (1 or 10
mg/kg). In studies 2 and 3, rats were administered either vehicle
control (8% Tween in saline, study 2; or saline, study 3) or pilocarpine (1 or 3 mg/kg, study 2; or 3 or 10 mg/kg, study 3). In the final
study, rats were administered either saline control or xanomeline (1,
3, 10, or 30 mg/kg). Drug treatments were coded and administration
to each animal determined by a Latin square, with tissue dissection
conducted blind to treatment. In all experiments, animals were sacrificed 60 min postinjection and the prefrontal cortex dissected on
ice. Tissues were then snap frozen on dry ice and stored at ⫺80°C
before analysis.
Prefrontal cortex tissue content of dopamine and its metabolites
3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid (DOPAC) and homovanillic acid
(HVA) were determined by high-performance liquid chromatography
as described below. Dopamine turnover (T/O) was then defined by
the equation T/O ⫽ ([DOPAC] ⫹ [HVA])/[dopamine].
Tissue samples were homogenized in Eppendorf tubes in ice-cold
0.1 M perchloric acid (10 ␮ml/mg of tissue) and centrifuged at 10,000
rpm for 10 min. The resultant supernatant was then spun through
Acrodisc 0.45-␮m filters and finally diluted either 1:5 or 1:10 in
high-performance liquid chromatography grade water (consistent
within each experiment). Samples were autoinjected onto a Hypersil
3 ␮m C18 ODS reverse phase column (100 ⫻ 3 mm; Presearch,
Hitchin, UK). The mobile phase used to separate dopamine, DOPAC,
and HVA consisted of a phosphate buffer (0.15 M NaH2PO4), 10%
methanol, 0.1 mM EDTA, and 0.6 mM sodium octyl sulfate (ion-pair
reagent), at pH 3.62. Dopamine and its acid metabolites were detected electrochemically using a glossy-carbon Antec Decade cell
(Antec, Leiden, The Netherlands) set at ⫹0.65 V versus a Ag/AgCl
reference and compared with the peak areas obtained from injected
standards (10⫺8 M dopamine, DOPAC, and HVA).
CER Test
Hooded Lister rats (initial weights 250 –300 g) that were fooddeprived to approximately 90% of their free feeding weight were
trained in 24 operant chambers (Coulbourn Instruments, Allentown,
PA) with associated Coulbourn pellet dispensers and operant levers.
These chambers were housed in outer sound- and light-attenuating
shells equipped with a ventilation fan that also helped to mask
Downloaded from jpet.aspetjournals.org at ASPET Journals on May 2, 2017
Sprague-Dawley rats weighing 350 to 550 g were used. The rats
were placed in the experimental room on the day of the procedure.
Animals were administered the test compound or saline s.c. 40 min
pretest (1 ml/kg) and were returned to their home cages (n ⫽ 7– 8).
Immediately before testing, the animals were administered s.c. with
d-amphetamine (0.5 mg/kg) or saline (1 ml/kg). The animals were
then placed individually in an activity monitor cage (48 ⫻ 27 ⫻ 20
cm, length ⫻ width ⫻ height) and locomotor activity was measured
for 1 h (Activity Monitor AM1052; Benwick Electronics, Linton Instrumentation, Norfolk, UK). The activity monitor cage was positioned in a frame equipped with infrared light sources and photocells
in a 7 by 4 matrix. The light beams crossed the cage 3 cm above its
base. Locomotor activity was assessed by counting the total number
of beam breaks that occurred during a 1-h test period.
external noise. A single operant lever was positioned on the left side
of the front panel of the operant chamber, 2 cm above the grid floor.
Coulbourn animal test cage grid floor shockers were used to deliver
shock (0.4 mA, 0.5 s) to the grid floors (model E-10-10SF). Formula P
Noyes food pellets (45 mg) could be delivered to the food magazine
centered on the front wall of the chamber approximately 2 cm above
the floor. A houselight (4.0 mA, 28 V) was positioned on the front wall
of the chamber, above the food magazine, approximately 1.0 cm
below the ceiling. The operant chambers were controlled and the
number of lever presses recorded by an Acorn computer programmed
in Paul Fray Arachnid software (Cambridge, UK).
By using the methods described by Stanhope and Dourish (1996),
hungry rats were trained in darkened chambers to lever press for
food pellets on a random interval 90-s schedule. Rats then received
training during which presentation of a houselight was sometimes
paired with mild electric footshock. These sessions were approximately 60 min in length and usually conducted 5 days/week. During
these sessions, food pellets continued to be available on a random
interval 90-s schedule as before. However, the rats were now presented with two, 2-min-long periods of houselight illumination. The
first light presentation occurred approximately 20 min after the start
of the session (range 15–25 min) and the second light presentation
occurred approximately 20 min after the first light presentation
(range 15–25 min). Light presentations were occasionally followed by
a 0.5-s, 0.4-mA footshock. In a given session, footshock could follow
neither, one, or both of the light presentations. The rate of lever
pressing during each of the light presentations (the “dur” period) and
the rate of lever pressing in the 2-min interval preceding each light
presentation (the “pre” period) was recorded. These response rates
were used to calculate suppression ratios using the following formula: response rate during light/[(response rate dur light) ⫹ (response rate pre light)]. A suppression ratio of 0 indicates that the
light has evoked conditioned fear and has completely suppressed
lever pressing, whereas a suppression ratio of 0.5 indicates the
response rate is unchanged by the light presentation, i.e., the complete absence of fear.
Once a reliable suppression ratio was observed, occasional test
sessions occurred. Test sessions were identical to training sessions
except that footshock was never delivered after the first light presentation and treatment with either vehicle or a test compound
occurred before the session began. Drug tests occurred no more
frequently than once per week and a baseline training session was
always carried out before each test session.
Drugs. Before drug test sessions, the rats were semirandomly
assigned to groups such that the groups were matched for the rate of
lever pressing and the level of conditioned suppression displayed
during baseline training. Drug treatment in each study was orthogonal to drug treatment administered to the animals in the previous
test. Rats with suppression ratios of greater than 0.15 or that did not
lever press in both pre periods in the previous baseline session were
excluded a priori from experiments.
In each drug test, a group of rats was treated with a benzodiazepine receptor agonist, either chlordiazepoxide (CDP) or diazepam,
which served as a positive control within each experiment. Due to
different predose times, routes of administration and vehicle between the positive control compound and the target compounds, in
the clozapine and the haloperidol experiments each drug treatment
group received two injections. In the clozapine experiment, there
were five groups of rats (n ⫽ 7–9) given the following pair of treatments 1) vehicle/vehicle, 2) vehicle/2.5 mg/kg clozapine, 3) vehicle/5.0
mg/kg clozapine, 4) vehicle/10 mg/kg clozapine, and 5) 10 mg/kg
CDP/vehicle. The first injection (CDP or its vehicle of 1% methylcellulose) was administered p.o. in a dosing volume of 2 ml/kg 40 min
before the rat was placed into the operant chamber. The second
injection [clozapine or its vehicle of 20% polyethylene glycol 400 in
acidified ␤b-cyclodextrin (25 mM citric acid solution in 10% ␤bcyclodextrin, w/v)] was administered s.c. in a dosing volume of 2
ml/kg 10 min before the rat entered the chamber.
Antipsychotic-Like Effects of Xanomeline
In the haloperidol experiment, five groups of rats (n ⫽ 10 –11)
were given pairs of injections with the first injection being either
diazepam or its vehicle and the second injection of either haloperidol
or its vehicle as follows: 1) vehicle/vehicle, 2) vehicle/0.01 mg/kg
haloperidol, 3) vehicle/0.03 mg/kg haloperidol, 4) vehicle/0.1 mg/kg
haloperidol, and 5) 4.0 mg/kg diazepam/vehicle. Haloperidol was
purchased in an aqueous solution of 5 mg/ml in Serenac ampoules.
This solution of haloperidol was diluted to the appropriate concentrations by using 0.9% saline vehicle. In this experiment and in the
following xanomeline study, diazepam was dissolved in 8% Tween 80
and administered at a dosing volume of 1 ml/kg. Both injections were
s.c. and occurred approximately 30 min before the rats being placed
into the operant chambers.
In the xanomeline study (n ⫽ 7–9), both xanomeline and the
positive control compound diazepam were administered using the
same vehicle, dosing volume, route, and pretreatment time as used
for diazepam in the preceding haloperidol study. Therefore, the animals in the xanomeline study received only one injection of vehicle;
2.5, 5.0, 10, or 20 mg/kg xanomeline; or 4.0 mg/kg diazepam.
or 2) d-amphetamine. Significant main effects in the separate oneway ANOVAs were followed by post hoc Dunnett’s t tests.
Dopamine Turnover in Rat Prefrontal Cortex. Drug-induced
changes in dopamine turnover were assessed for statistical significance by one-way ANOVA followed post hoc by Dunnett’s t tests,
where appropriate.
CER. A mixed within-subjects (trials) and between-subjects (drug
treatment) ANOVA was conducted separately on the rate of lever
pressing during the pre and dur periods and on the suppression
ratios. Significant interactions were followed by one-way analysis of
variance on the data collected for each trial. Significant main effects
of drug treatment were analyzed using post hoc Dunnett’s t test. In
experiments in which significant drug treatment ⫻ trial interactions
were identified, the data for each trial are presented separately.
Otherwise, the data for each experiment are presented collapsed
across the two test trials.
Suppression ratios could not be calculated if no lever presses
occurred during the pre light periods. Thus, animals that failed to
respond during either the first or the second light presentation were
excluded from the statistical analysis.
Results
Prepulse Inhibition
Study 1: Effect of Haloperidol, Scopolamine, Pilocarpine, and Xanomeline. Haloperidol. Haloperidol induced a significant dose-dependent decrease in startle amplitude (F[4,35] ⫽ 2.6, P ⬍ 0.05) with a minimum effective
dose of 0.3 mg/kg, compared with vehicle (Fig. 1A). In contrast, haloperidol had no significant effect on percentage of
PPI (F[4,35] ⫽ 1.4; Fig. 1B). The treatment by prepulse
intensity interaction on percentage of PPI also failed to reach
statistical significance (F ⬍ 1; data not shown).
Fig. 1. Effect of haloperidol (s.c.) on mean startle amplitude to a 120-dB,
40-ms noise burst (A) and percentage of prepulse inhibition collapsed for
the three prepulse intensities (3, 6, and 9 dB) (B). Values represent
mean ⫾ S.E.M. for each dose. *, significantly different relative to the
vehicle (Veh)-treated group (n ⫽ 8/group).
Downloaded from jpet.aspetjournals.org at ASPET Journals on May 2, 2017
Data Analysis
PPI. The dependent measures in the PPI studies were startle
amplitude after the 120-dB startle stimulus (measured in arbitrary
units) and percentage of PPI. The latter was calculated according to
the following formula: [100 ⫺ (startle amplitude on prepulse-pulse
trials/startle amplitude on pulse alone trials) ⫻ 100]. For study 1, the
main effect of treatment on startle reactivity was analyzed by oneway analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by Newman-Keuls test.
Percentage of PPI was analyzed using two-way ANOVA with the
factors drug treatment and prepulse intensity (three levels: 3, 6, and
9 dB). A main effect of treatment was followed by Newman-Keuls
test. If, however, there was an interaction between treatment and
prepulse intensity then a one-way ANOVA was performed on each
prepulse-pulse trial separately followed by Newman-Keuls test,
where appropriate.
Apomorphine (0.5 mg/kg) routinely increases startle amplitude
and impairs PPI in our hands. Therefore, this was the dose chosen to
disrupt PPI in the drug interaction experiments in study 2. Before
conducting statistical analysis on the target drug effects in study 2,
the predicted effects of apomorphine on both the startle response and
percentage of PPI were confirmed. A t test was used to determine
whether apomorphine affected the magnitude of the startle response
to the 120-dB stimulus in the interaction studies (i.e., the apomorphine/vehicle group were compared with the vehicle/vehicle-treated
group). For the PPI measure in the drug interaction experiments, a
mixed two-way analysis of variance, including the vehicle/vehicletreated group and the vehicle/apomorphine-treated group as a between-subjects factor and prepulse intensity as a within-subjects
factor, was conducted to confirm that apomorphine significantly
disrupted PPI. Once the expected disruption in PPI was confirmed, a
two-way analysis of variance (with drug treatment as the betweensubjects factor and prepulse intensity as the within-subjects factor)
was conducted to determine whether any dose of the test compound
significantly reversed the disruptive effects of apomorphine on PPI.
Only the groups treated with apomorphine were included in this
analysis. One-way ANOVA with only the apomorphine-treated
groups was also used to determine whether the target drug in combination with apomorphine had any effects on the startle response to
the 120-dB stimulus. Significant effects of drug treatment were
followed by post hoc multiple group comparisons with a NewmanKeuls test.
Amphetamine-Induced Hyperlocomotion. Two-way ANOVA
was used to assess whether there was a significant effect of amphetamine and a possible interaction between the test compound and
d-amphetamine. If a significant interaction was detected, separate
one-way ANOVAs were conducted to determine whether significant
drug effects were induced in the animals pretreated with 1) vehicle
785
786
Stanhope et al.
Pilocarpine. A strong nonlinear tendency for pilocarpine to
decrease startle amplitude at the 3.0-mg/kg dose (Fig. 2A)
was observed at the doses tested. However, this effect did not
achieve statistical significance (F[3,32] ⫽ 1.4). In contrast,
pilocarpine induced a significant and dose-dependent impairment in percentage of PPI (F[3,32] ⫽ 8.7; P ⬍ 0.01), with 10
mg/kg pilocarpine significantly impairing PPI relative to vehicle (Fig. 2B). This effect of pilocarpine on percentage of PPI
was not influenced by prepulse intensity (F ⬍ 1 for the
interaction; data not shown).
Xanomeline. Xanomeline significantly and dose dependently decreased startle amplitude with a minimum effective
dose of 30 mg/kg (F[3,36] ⫽ 3.2; P ⬍ 0.04; Fig. 2C). Xanomeline had no significant effects on percentage of PPI at the
doses used (F[3,36] ⫽ 1.9; Fig. 2D) nor was there an interaction between xanomeline treatment and prepulse intensity
(F[6,72] ⫽ 1.9; data not shown).
Scopolamine. Scopolamine was tested as both the methyl
and HBr salts within the same experiment. Neither salt form
of this drug significantly affected startle amplitude at the
doses tested (F[4,46] ⫽ 2.1; P ⬍ 0.1; Fig. 3A). However, there
was a strong tendency toward a decrease, most notably after
0.1 mg/kg of the methyl salt form of scopolamine. In contrast,
there was a pronounced and significant effect of drug treatment on percentage of PPI (F[4,46] ⫽ 2.9; P ⬍ 0.03; Fig. 3B).
Scopolamine HBr dose dependently reduced percentage of
PPI, and this was significant compared with vehicle after the
0.1-mg/kg dose. The group administered 0.1 mg/kg methylscopolamine did not differ from vehicle on the percentage of
PPI measure. There was no significant interaction between
treatment and prepulse intensity on percentage of PPI measure (F ⬍ 1; data not shown).
Study 2: Effect of Haloperidol, Pilocarpine, and Xanomeline on Apomorphine-Induced Deficits in PPI.
Haloperidol. In this study, apomorphine when administered
alone, had no significant effects on mean startle amplitude to
Fig. 3. Effect of scopolamine HBr or methylscopolamine (Me-Scop, 0.1
mg/kg, right) administered s.c., on mean startle amplitude to 120-dB,
40-ms noise burst (A) and percentage of prepulse inhibition collapsed for
the three prepulse intensities (3, 6, and 9 dB) (B). Values represent
mean ⫾ S.E.M. for each treatment. *, significantly different relative to the
vehicle (Veh)-treated group (n ⫽ 8 –11/group).
the 120-dB stimulus [t(14) ⫽ 0.025; P ⫽ 0.81; Fig. 4A]. As
predicted, however, apomorphine significantly impaired percentage of PPI (F[1,14] ⫽ 20; P ⬍ 0.001; Fig. 4B); this effect
did not interact with prepulse intensity (F ⬍ 1). In rats
Downloaded from jpet.aspetjournals.org at ASPET Journals on May 2, 2017
Fig. 2. Effect of pilocarpine
(left) and xanomeline (right)
administered s.c., on mean
startle amplitude to 120-dB,
40-ms noise burst (A and C)
and percentage of prepulse inhibition (B and D) collapsed for
the three prepulse intensities
(3, 6, and 9 dB). Values represent mean ⫾ S.E.M. for each
treatment. ⴱ, significantly different relative to the vehicle
(Veh)-treated group (n ⫽ 9 and
10/group in the two studies, respectively).
Antipsychotic-Like Effects of Xanomeline
pretreated with haloperidol, ANOVA revealed no main effect
of drug treatment on the startle amplitude to the 120-dB
stimulus (F ⬍ 1). In contrast, haloperidol pretreatment reversed the apomorphine-induced deficit in percentage of PPI
(F[4,35] ⫽ 5.3; P ⬍ 0.01), and this effect did not interact with
prepulse intensity (F ⬍ 1; data not shown). Post hoc analysis
showed that there was a significant difference between rats
pretreated with 0.03 to 0.3 mg/kg haloperidol compared with
rats pretreated with vehicle, before apomorphine.
Xanomeline. In the experiment assessing the effects of
xanomeline on apomorphine-induced changes in PPI, apomorphine alone significantly increased startle amplitude
[t(18) ⫽ 2.7; P ⫽ 0.01; Fig. 5A]. Xanomeline pretreatment did
not affect this apomorphine-induced increase in startle amplitude (F[4,41] ⫽ 1.7), although as can be seen in Fig. 5A,
the 3-mg/kg dose produced a nonsignificant trend toward a
reversal of the apomorphine-induced increase in startle amplitude. As in the haloperidol experiment, apomorphine significantly reduced percentage of PPI compared with vehicle
treatment (F[1,18] ⫽ 6.2; P ⬍ 0.02; Fig. 5B). This effect of
apomorphine did not significantly interact with prepulse intensity (F[2,36] ⫽ 3.1; P ⫽ 0.06; data not shown). Xanomeline
pretreatment reversed the apomorphine-induced deficit in
PPI (F[4,41] ⫽ 5.3; P ⫽ 0.001; Fig. 5B), and this effect did not
interact with prepulse intensity (F[8,82] ⫽ 1.2; data not
shown). Post hoc analysis demonstrated that rats pretreated
with the highest dose of xanomeline (30 mg/kg) differed significantly from the group given vehicle before apomorphine.
Pilocarpine. As with the xanomeline experiment, there was
a significant increase in startle amplitude after apomorphine
[t(14) ⫽ 2.53; P ⫽ 0.02; Fig. 5C]. Pilocarpine pretreatment
significantly reversed this apomorphine-induced increase in
startle amplitude (F[4,35] ⫽ 4.7; P ⬍ 0.01). Post hoc analysis
showed that rats pretreated with 10 mg/kg pilocarpine differed significantly from rats administered vehicle before apomorphine treatment. As in the previous two studies, apomorphine induced a robust deficit in percentage of PPI (F[1,14] ⫽
6.6; P ⬍ 0.02; Fig. 5D). This effect did not interact with
prepulse intensity (F ⬍ 1; data not shown). However, in
contrast to haloperidol and xanomeline, pilocarpine did not
affect the apomorphine-induced deficit in PPI. There was no
significant effect of pilocarpine pretreatment (F ⬍ 1) nor an
interaction between pilocarpine treatment and prepulse intensity (F ⬍ 1; data not shown).
Fig. 5. Effect of xanomeline
(left) and pilocarpine (right) administered s.c. on apomorphineinduced (s.c.) effects on mean
startle amplitude to 120-dB,
40-ms noise burst (A and C) and
percentage of prepulse inhibition (B and D) collapsed for the
three prepulse intensities (3, 6,
and 9 dB). Veh and Apo represent the groups administered vehicle-vehicle and vehicle-apomorphine, respectively. Values
represent mean ⫾ S.E.M. for
each treatment. ⴱ, significantly
different relative to the Veh
group. ⫹, significantly different
compared with Apo (n ⫽ 8 and
9–10/group in the two studies,
respectively).
Downloaded from jpet.aspetjournals.org at ASPET Journals on May 2, 2017
Fig. 4. Effect of haloperidol (s.c.) on apomorphine-induced (s.c.) effects on
mean startle amplitude to 120-dB, 40-ms noise burst (A) and percentage
of prepulse inhibition collapsed for the three prepulse intensities (3, 6,
and 9 dB) (B). Veh and Apo represent the groups administered vehiclevehicle and vehicle-apomorphine, respectively. Values represent mean ⫾
S.E.M. for each treatment. ⴱ, significantly different relative to the Veh
group. ⫹, significantly different compared with Apo (n ⫽ 8/group).
787
788
Stanhope et al.
Fig. 6. Effect of haloperidol (s.c.) on locomotor activity in rats coadministered vehicle (0.9% saline s.c.; 䡺) or amphetamine (0.5 mg/kg s.c.; f)
during a 1-h test. Mean ⫾ S.E.M. ⴱ/$, significantly different from corresponding nonhaloperidol-treated control group (n ⫽ 8/group).
Fig. 7. Effect of clozapine (s.c.) on locomotor activity in rats coadministered vehicle (0.9% saline s.c.; 䡺) or amphetamine (0.5 mg/kg s.c.; f)
during a 1-h test. Mean ⫾ S.E.M. ⴱ/$, significantly different from corresponding nonclozapine-treated control group (n ⫽ 7– 8/group).
mine, DOPAC, and HVA content in vehicle-treated animals,
from which dopamine turnover was calculated, are shown in
Table 1A.
The antipsychotic agents haloperidol and clozapine both
dose dependently and significantly increased dopamine turnover in rat prefrontal cortex (Table 1B, study 1). Thus, at 0.1
and 1.0 mg/kg haloperidol increased turnover by 34% (P ⬍
0.05; Dunnett’s test) and 55% (P ⬍ 0.01), respectively, above
the control ratio. Clozapine was less potent, its administration leading to an increase in dopamine turnover of 19%
(nonsignificant) and 47% (P ⬍ 0.01) above the control ratio
after 1 and 10 mg/kg, respectively.
Pilocarpine had no effect on prefrontal cortex dopamine
turnover at 1 mg/kg but did significantly increase turnover at
3 mg/kg, by 67% compared with the control ratio (P ⬍ 0.01;
Table 1B, study 2). The effect at 3 mg/kg observed in study 2
was replicated in study 3 (55% increase above control ratio;
P ⬍ 0.05) in which the effect of higher doses of pilocarpine
were also investigated. Higher doses of the muscarinic agonist (10 and 30 mg/kg) had no significant effect on the same
neurochemical measure in this study, thereby producing a
“bell-shaped” dose-response curve with a narrow window of
efficacy.
Xanomeline dose dependently increased dopamine turnover with a minimum significantly effective dose of 30 mg/kg
(51% above control ratio; P ⬍ 0.01; Table 1B, study 4). Administration of 10 mg/kg xanomeline resulted in a raised
dopamine turnover ratio compared with control (24% increase), which fell just short of statistical significance.
Conditioned Emotional Response. Clozapine. Clozapine administered at 5 but not 10 mg/kg significantly decreased rates of lever pressing during the pre light period
(F[4,35] ⫽ 2.7; P ⬍ 0.05; Fig. 9A). Although this effect tended
to be observed on the second trial but not the first trial, the
treatment ⫻ trial interaction did not achieve statistical significance (F[4,35] ⫽ 1.5; P ⬎ 0.05). In contrast, there was a
significant interaction between drug treatment and trial on
the rates of lever pressing during the light stimulus (F[4,35]
⫽ 3.9; P ⬍ 0.01), but the source of this interaction could not
be determined. Separate one-way ANOVAs on the response
rates during the light for each trial showed a significant
effect of drug treatment on both trial 1 and trial 2 (F values[4,35] ⫽ 6.0 and 2.8, respectively; P values ⬍ 0.05). In
both cases post hoc Dunnett’s t test showed that CDP but not
clozapine significantly increased lever pressing during the
Downloaded from jpet.aspetjournals.org at ASPET Journals on May 2, 2017
Amphetamine-induced hyperlocomotion. When administered in the absence of a test compound, 0.5 mg/kg d-amphetamine consistently increased locomotor activity from a baseline of approximately 1200 to approximately 3700 beam
breaks (Figs. 6– 8). This induction of hyperlocomotion was
confirmed by the finding of a significant main effect of amphetamine compared with vehicle treatment in all three
studies (F[1,17] ⫽ 90.33, F[1,69] ⫽ 77.48 and F[1,56] ⫽
209.31; all P values ⬍ 0.0001).
At a dose of 0.1 mg/kg, haloperidol significantly reduced
baseline levels of locomotor activity (F[3,28] ⫽ 12.7; P ⬍
0.01). Both 0.03 and 0.1 mg/kg haloperidol significantly reduced the hyperlocomotion induced by d-amphetamine
(F[3,28] ⫽ 36.7; P ⬍ 0.01; Fig. 6).
In rats that were pretreated with vehicle, clozapine significantly reduced photobeam breaks with a minimum effective
dose of 1 mg/kg, although the effects of 3 mg/kg just failed to
reach statistical significance (F[4,35] ⫽ 2.8; P ⬍ 0.05). Clozapine significantly reversed the effects of amphetamine pretreatment with an minimum effective dose of 1 mg/kg
(F[4,34] ⫽ 7.4; P ⬍ 0.01; Fig. 7).
Xanomeline had no significant effects on the baseline levels of locomotor activity (F[4,35] ⫽ 1.9) but significantly
reduced d-amphetamine-induced hyperactivity at a minimum effective dose of 1 mg/kg (F[4,35] ⫽ 31.9; P ⬍ 0.01; Fig.
8).
Dopamine turnover in rat prefrontal cortex. Basal dopa-
Fig. 8. Effect of xanomeline (s.c.) on locomotor activity in rats coadministered vehicle (0.9% saline s.c.; 䡺) or amphetamine (0.5 mg/kg s.c.; f)
during a 1-h test. Mean ⫾ S.E.M. ⴱ/$, significantly different from corresponding nonxanomeline treated control group (n ⫽ 8/group).
Antipsychotic-Like Effects of Xanomeline
789
TABLE 1
Basal dopamine, DOPAC, and HVA content (ng/g of tissue) and dopamine turnover ([DOPAC] ⫹ [HVA])/[dopamine] expressed as percentage of
control dopamine turnover after haloperidol, clozapine, pilocarpine, and xanomeline by ex vivo analysis of rat prefrontal cortical tissue.
Study
Dopamine
DOPAC
HVA
Turnover ratio
1
2
3
4
417
281
172
351
503
88
125
125
374
200
418
126
2.10
1.02
3.16
0.72
Drug Dose (s.c.) 60 min before Sacrifice
Study
1
1
2
3
4
a
b
Drug (mg/kg)
Haloperidol
Clozapine
Pilocarpine
Pilocarpine
Xanomeline
ANOVA
0
0.1
1
100 ⫾ 7
100 ⫾ 7
100 ⫾ 14
100 ⫾ 13
100 ⫾ 10
134 ⫾ 7a
155 ⫾ 6b
119 ⫾ 7
122 ⫾ 4
100 ⫾ 10
3
167 ⫾ 12
155 ⫾ 9a
93 ⫾ 8
10
a
30
147 ⫾ 14b
108 ⫾ 13
124 ⫾ 7
82 ⫾ 12
151 ⫾ 7b
F(4,35) ⫽ 7.83
F(4,35) ⫽ 7.83
F(2,21) ⫽ 12.3
F(3,28) ⫽ 7.36
F(4,34) ⫽ 7.86
Significantly different to baseline, P ⬍ 0.05.
Significantly different from baseline, P ⬍ 0.01.
Fig. 9. Effects of clozapine (s.c.) and CDP (p.o.) on mean lever press rates
on trial 1 (䡺) and trial 2 (f): before (A) and during (B) the light presentation, and on the calculated (C) suppression ratios. Mean ⫾ S.E.M. ⴱ,
significantly different relative to the vehicle-treated control group (n ⫽
7–9/group).
light period compared with vehicle (Fig. 9B). Although the
effects of clozapine on light response rates were not statistically significant, both 10.0 mg/kg clozapine and CDP significantly increased suppression ratios compared with vehicle
(F[4,35] ⫽ 5.1; P ⬍ 0.01; Fig. 9C). The trial by drug treatment
interaction for suppression ratios was not statistically significant (F ⬍ 1).
Haloperidol. Only two of the 11 rats in the group treated
with the highest dose of 0.1 mg/kg haloperidol were able to
respond during the pre period of both trials. For the purpose
of presentation, the results of these two animals are included
in the figure, although their results are excluded from the
statistical analyses. Up to a dose of 0.03 mg/kg, haloperidol
did not significantly affect pre light rates of lever pressing
(F ⬍ 1; Fig. 10A). However, there was a significant effect of
drug treatment on lever pressing during the light (F[3,37] ⫽
15; P ⬍ 0.01). Post hoc Dunnett’s t test demonstrated a
significant increase in lever pressing during the light after
diazepam relative to vehicle (Fig. 10A). A significant effect of
drug treatment on suppression ratios (F[3,37] ⫽ 31; P ⬍ 0.01)
was again due to a significant difference between the diazepam and vehicle group (Fig. 10B). There were no interactions
between drug treatment and trial on any of the measures in
this experiment (F values ⬍ 1).
Xanomeline. After 10 and 20 mg/kg xanomeline the number of rats reaching the criteria of at least one response in
Downloaded from jpet.aspetjournals.org at ASPET Journals on May 2, 2017
Fig. 10. Effects of haloperidol (s.c.) and diazepam (DZM, s.c.) on mean
lever press rates (A) before (䡺) and during (f) the light presentations,
and on the calculated (B) suppression ratios (bottom). Mean ⫾ S.E.M. ⴱ,
significantly different relative to the vehicle-treated control group (n ⫽
10 –11/group).
790
Stanhope et al.
both pre periods was reduced to two and three, respectively.
Therefore, these treatment groups were excluded from the
statistical analyses. There was a significant effect of drug
treatment on pre light responses (F[3,25] ⫽ 3.2; P ⫽ 0.04;
Fig. 11A); however, the basis for this effect could not be
identified with the post hoc test. Although both 2.5 and 5.0
mg/kg xanomeline tended to decrease the rate of lever pressing during the pre light period (Fig. 11A), no drug treatment
groups were found to significantly differ from the vehicletreated group. Although there was a tendency for xanomeline
treatment to increase the rate of lever pressing during the
light, ANOVA followed by post hoc testing found that only
diazepam significantly increased responding relative to the
vehicle treatment (F[3,25] ⫽ 13; P ⬍ 0.01; Fig. 11A). There
was no effect of trial (F[1,25] ⫽ 1.3) nor a treatment ⫻ trial
interaction (F ⬍ 1) for either pre or dur light responses.
However, both xanomeline (5 mg/kg) and diazepam were
found to significantly increase suppression ratios relative to
vehicle treatment (F[3,25] ⫽ 15; P ⬍ 0.01; Fig. 11B). There
was no significant treatment by trial interaction on suppression ratios (F ⬍ 1).
Discussion
Xanomeline had similar effects to haloperidol and/or clozapine in all of the models used in the present study. Like
haloperidol and clozapine, xanomeline enhanced dopamine
turnover in the prefrontal cortex and reversed amphetamineinduced hyperactivity at doses not disrupting locomotor activity alone. Xanomeline also reversed the disruptive effects
of apomorphine in the prepulse inhibition model, which is
Downloaded from jpet.aspetjournals.org at ASPET Journals on May 2, 2017
Fig. 11. Effects of xanomeline (s.c.) and diazepam (DZM s.c.) on mean
lever press rates before (A; 䡺) and during (shaded bars) the light presentations, and on the calculated (B) suppression ratios. Mean ⫾ S.E.M. ⴱ,
significantly different relative to the vehicle-treated control group (n ⫽
7–9/group).
claimed to have both face and construct validity vis a vis
schizophrenia symptomology (Swerdlow et al., 1994). Like
the atypical antipsychotic clozapine, xanomeline reduced
conditioned suppression in the CER test. These findings and
published data suggest that xanomeline may be an effective
antipsychotic agent in humans.
Haloperidol alone had no effect on PPI but reversed the
PPI deficit induced by apomorphine. Others have also demonstrated this effect of haloperidol, although the minimum
effective dose of haloperidol is higher in some studies (Swerdlow et al., 1994; Varty and Higgins, 1995). Haloperidol may
also enhance PPI in normal rats, but procedural changes are
used to generate low baseline PPI values (Depoortere et al.,
1997). An enhancement of PPI in normal rats by an antipsychotic drug would not be expected using the parameters used
here.
Whereas xanomeline (30 mg/kg) reversed the apomorphine-induced deficit in PPI, pilocarpine up to a dose of 10
mg/kg had no effect. It could be argued that a higher dose of
pilocarpine could reverse the disruption in PPI induced by
apomorphine. This seems unlikely, however, given that at 10
mg/kg pilocarpine alone completely abolished PPI in untreated rats (Fig. 2B), an effect that is typical of a psychotomimetic as opposed to an antipsychotic action of drugs (see
below).
Shannon et al. (1999, 2000) have shown that at 30 mg/kg
xanomeline also impairs conditioned avoidance response
(CAR) in rats and that this effect can be blocked by scopolamine. However, these authors showed that at 10 mg/kg
pilocarpine also decreases CARs. The difference in the profile
of pilocarpine in the PPI and CAR models is unclear. However, in this study we specifically manipulated the dopamine
system, whereas the pharmacological basis for the CAR procedure may be more complex (Arnt, 1982; Ellenbroek, 1993).
We have also demonstrated that scopolamine HBr impairs
PPI (0.1 mg/kg) with no effect on startle. In contrast, at the
dose tested, methylscopolamine, which acts mainly in the
periphery, tended to reduce startle without affecting PPI.
Thus, scopolamine may decrease startle and impair PPI via
peripheral and central mechanisms, respectively. The decrease in startle with scopolamine concurs with some previous studies (Davis, 1980). However, Fendt and Koch (1999)
showed that scopolamine injections into the caudal pontine
reticular nucleus increased startle. Thus, scopolamine acting
at peripheral and central muscarinic receptors may have
opposite effects on startle. Fendt and Koch (1999) also
showed that scopolamine injected into the pontine reticular
nucleus significantly impaired PPI, although the effect was
not dose-related. In contrast, we saw a dose-related reduction
in PPI with scopolamine HBr. These findings with scopolamine concur with clinical data indicating that anticholinergics are psychotomimetic (Neubauer et al., 1966a,b).
Xanomeline, like classical and atypical antipsychotics, attenuated amphetamine-induced hyperactivity (Arnt, 1995).
This result is in keeping with previous findings for pilocarpine (Proctor et al., 1967). Whether reversal of amphetamine-induced hyperactivity is a predictor of antipsychotic
activity is unknown. However, the effect of drugs on locomotor hyperactivity induced by dopaminergic agonists has been
validated and used routinely as an animal model to assess
limbic dopaminergic function (Costall et al., 1980; Arnt et al.,
1994). Thus, attenuation of limbic dopaminergic function via
Antipsychotic-Like Effects of Xanomeline
3 mg/kg, but not at higher doses) nor does it reverse apomorphine-induced deficits in PPI. It is interesting that pilocarpine actually produces a PPI deficit in its own right at 10
mg/kg, the dose at which the drug no longer increases dopamine turnover. It seems likely that other nonspecific effects
of the drug are engaged at this dose, resulting in the behavioral and neurochemical profiles observed. The less specific
pharmacology of pilocarpine may also underlie the absence of
an effect on PPI at doses around 3 mg/kg, which increases
mPFC dopamine turnover. Interestingly, however, this is the
same dose that resulted in a clear tendency to reduce startle.
Other aspects of xanomeline’s pharmacology could account
for its antipsychotic profile in PPI and set it apart from
pilocarpine. Xanomeline is an agonist at 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B
receptors and an antagonist at 5-HT2 receptors (Watson et
al., 1998). However, these effects are unlikely to account for
xanomeline’s effects in PPI because agonist actions at 5-HT1A
or 5-HT1B receptors are reported to disrupt PPI and the
5-HT2A receptor antagonist MDL 100907 does not reverse
apomorphine-induced disruption of PPI (Sipes and Geyer,
1994; Geyer et al., 1999). It seems more likely that differences in selectivity and/or efficacy for muscarinic receptor
subtypes (e.g., M1, M2, or M4) may account for the differences between pilocarpine and xanomeline (Bymaster et al.,
1998b; Felder et al., 2000; Shannon et al., 2000).
In the CER model of anxiety clozapine (10 mg/kg) and
xanomeline (5.0 mg/kg), but not haloperidol, had anxiolyticlike profiles as demonstrated by an increase in the suppression ratio. These effects were demonstrable under conditions
where the clinically effective anxiolytics chlordiazepoxide or
diazepam also induced anxiolysis. However, the effects of
xanomeline appear to be less robust than those of chlordiazepoxide, diazepam, and clozapine, which increased suppressed responding without reducing baseline response
rates. In contrast, xanomeline nonsignificantly reduced baseline responding at the same dose that tended to increase
suppressed responding, and it is this combination of effects
that have resulted in a significant change in the suppression
ratio. Although this would suggest a cautious interpretation,
the tendency to increase responding in the conditioned stimulus period at the same doses that reduced baseline responding is not a profile commensurate with a general nonspecific
reduction in lever pressing such as that seen with haloperidol. Xanomeline’s profile is similar to that obtained with high
doses of ipsapirone (10 mg/kg) or diazepam (10 –30 mg/kg;
Stanhope and Dourish, 1996). The lack of an anxiolytic-like
effect with haloperidol concurs with its lack of effect in conflict tasks (Moore et al., 1994).
In summary, xanomeline dose dependently reverses apomorphine-induced deficits in PPI, and over the same doses and time
course increases dopamine turnover in rat PFC. In addition, it
reverses amphetamine-induced hyperlocomotion. These data
taken together provide further evidence that xanomeline may
have antipsychotic potential. The profile observed in the CER
model, moreover, suggests that its neuroleptic action may be
more similar to that of clozapine than haloperidol. The ability of
xanomeline to induce these effects in rats is consistent with
recent clinical data from patients with Alzheimer’s disease
(Bodick et al., 1997). The predictive validity of these data for the
antipsychotic effects of muscarinic receptor agonists in schizophrenic patients awaits the results of clinical trials.
Downloaded from jpet.aspetjournals.org at ASPET Journals on May 2, 2017
muscarinic receptor activation may reduce the reported hyperdopaminergic state observed in schizophrenic patients
and represent an alternative class of drug to existing therapies.
Interestingly, clozapine attenuated amphetamine-induced
hyperactivity only at doses that also reduced spontaneous locomotor activity. In contrast, haloperidol attenuated amphetamine-induced hyperactivity at a dose 3-fold lower than that
reducing spontaneous activity. This is in contrast with a previous study, which although finding a similar dose separation for
haloperidol, demonstrated that clozapine attenuated amphetamine-induced hyperactivity at a dose 6.5-fold lower than that
reducing spontaneous locomotor activity (Arnt, 1995). Procedural differences between our study and that of Arnt (1995)
may explain this discrepancy. In particular, in the study of Arnt
(1995) the effects of clozapine on spontaneous locomotor activity
and amphetamine-induced hyperactivity were assessed in different experiments published 4 years apart (Sanchez et al.,
1991; Arnt, 1995), whereas currently the comparison was made
in a single experiment.
Although xanomeline attenuated amphetamine-induced
hyperactivity at doses of 1 to 10 mg/kg, the drug had no effect
on spontaneous activity over the same dose range, indicating
the possibility of a wide separation between attenuation of
limbic hyperdopaminergic function and the onset of sedation.
The antipsychotics haloperidol and clozapine, and the muscarinic receptor agonists pilocarpine and xanomeline, all induced an increase in medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) dopamine turnover, assessed ex vivo. The increase in dopamine
turnover observed is likely to result from an increase in
mPFC dopamine release. For clozapine, an increase in dopamine turnover is consistent with previous microdialysis studies, in which the atypical neuroleptics clozapine and olanzapine potentiated dopamine release in rat mPFC (Kuroki et
al., 1999). Interestingly, haloperidol had either no effect or a
mild effect on mPFC dopamine release in these studies, in
agreement with intracortical infusion studies (Pehek and
Yamamoto, 1994). Thus, clozapine and olanzapine have a
more pronounced effect on dopamine function in cortical than
subcortical areas, whereas haloperidol has the opposite profile, leading to the suggestion that this is representative of an
atypical antipsychotic action. However, other studies, including those described here, have reported an enhancement by
haloperidol of mPFC dopamine metabolism or release
(Karoum and Egan, 1992). Thus, conclusions regarding the
“typical” or “atypical” nature of the muscarinic receptor agonists cannot be drawn from the present study. However, the
recent finding that xanomeline preferentially increases extracellular dopamine and expression of certain immediate
early genes in the PFC/nucleus accumbens relative to the
striatum is consistent with an atypical antipsychotic action
(Perry et al., 2001).
The efficacious dose of xanomeline that increased dopamine turnover in rat mPFC (30 mg/kg) correlates well with
the effects of the drug in PPI. This is the same dose required
to reduce startle in normal rats and is also the minimum
efficacious dose for the drug to reverse an apomorphineinduced deficit in PPI. This correlation supports the contention that an increase in mPFC dopamine function may contribute to an antipsychotic-like effect of the drug.
In contrast, pilocarpine had neither a clear dose-dependent
effect on dopamine turnover (increasing this measure at only
791
792
Stanhope et al.
References
other cholinesterase inhibitors on neuropsychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 9:8 –14.
Karoum F and Egan MF (1992) Dopamine release and metabolism in the rat frontal
cortex, nucleus accumbens, and striatum: a comparison of acute clozapine and
haloperidol. Br J Pharmacol 105:703–707.
Kuroki T, Meltzer HY, and Ichikawa J (1999) Effects of antipsychotic drugs on
extracellular dopamine levels in rat medial prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 288:774 –781.
Moore NA, Rees G, Sanger G, and Tye NC (1994) Effects of olanzapine and other
antipsychotic agents on responding maintained by a conflict schedule. Behav
Pharmacol 5:196 –202.
Neubauer H, Sundland DM, and Gershon S (1966a) Sernyl, ditran, and their antagonists: succinate and THA. Int J Neuropsych 2:216 –222.
Neubauer H, Sundland D, and Gershon S (1966b) Ditran and its antagonists in a
mixed psychiatric population. J Nerv Ment Dis 142:265–277.
Pehek EA and Yamamoto BK (1994) Differential effects of locally administered
clozapine and haloperidol on dopamine efflux in the rat prefrontal cortex and
caudate-putamen. J Neurochem 63:2118 –2124.
Perry KW, Nisenbaum LK, George CA, Shannon HE, Felder CC, and Bymaster FP
(2001) The muscarinic agonist xanomeline increases monoamine release and immediate early gene expression in the rat prefrontal cortex. Biol Psych 49:716 –725.
Proctor CD, Potts JL, Ashley LG, and Denefield BA (1967) Pilocarpine reversal of
d-amphetamine induced increase in mouse exploratory locomotor activity. Arch Int
Pharmacodyn 167:61– 67.
Sanchez C, Arnt J, Dragsted N, Hyttel J, Lembel HL, Meier E, Perregard J, and
Skarsfeldt T (1991) Neurochemical and in vivo pharmacological profile of sertindole, a limbic-selective neuroleptic compound. Drug Dev Res 22:239 –250.
Shannon HE, Bymaster FP, Calligaro DO, Greenwood B, Mitch CH, Sawyer BD,
Ward JS, Wong DT, Olesen PH, Sheardown MJ, et al. (1994) Xanomeline: a novel
muscarinic receptor agonist with functional selectivity for M1 receptors. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 269:271–281.
Shannon HE, Hatyt JC, Bymaster FP, Calligaro DO, Delapp NW, Mitch CH, Ward
JS, Fink-Jensen A, Sauerberg P, Jeppesen L, et al. (1999) Muscarinic receptor
agonists, like dopamine receptor antagonist antipsychotics, inhibit conditioned
avoidance response in rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 290:901–907.
Shannon HE, Rasmussen K, Bymaster FP, Hart JC, Peters SC, Swedberg MD,
Jeppesen L, Sheardown MJ, Sauerberg P, and Fink-Jensen A (2000) Xanomeline,
an M(1)/M(4) preferring muscarinic cholinergic receptor agonist, produces antipsychotic-like activity in rats and mice. Schizophr Res 42:249 –259.
Sipes TA and Geyer MA (1994) Multiple serotonin receptor subtypes modulate
prepulse inhibition of the startle response in rats. Neuropharmacology 33:441–
448.
Stanhope KJ and Dourish CT (1996) Effects of 5-HT1A receptor agonists, partial
agonists and a silent antagonist on the performance of the conditioned emotional
response test in the rat. Psychopharmacology 128:293–303.
Street J, Tamura R, Kadam D, Sanger T, Gannon KS, and Tollefson GD (1999)
Olanzapine in the treatment of psychiatric disturbances associated with Alzheimer’s disease. Poster presented at the Annual meeting of the American Academy
of Neurology, Toronto, Canada April 17–24.
Swerdlow NR, Braff DL, Taaid N, and Geyer MA (1994) Assessing the validity of an
animal model of deficient sensorimotor gating in schizophrenic patients. Arch Gen
Psych 51:139 –154.
Varty GB and Higgins GA (1995) Examination of drug-induced and isolation-induced
disruptions of Prepulse inhibition as models to screen antipsychotic drugs. Psychopharmacology 122:15–26.
Watson J, Brough S, Coldwell MC, Gager T, Ho M, Hunter AJ, Jerman J, Middlemiss
DN, Riley GJ, and Brown AM (1998) Functional effects of the muscarinic receptor
agonist, xanomeline, at 5-HT1 and 5-HT2 receptors. Br J Pharmacol 125:1413–
1420.
Address correspondence to: Dr. N. R. Mirza, Head of Pharmacology, NeuroSearch A/S, 93 Pederstrupvej, DK-2750, Ballerup, Denmark. E-mail:
[email protected]
Downloaded from jpet.aspetjournals.org at ASPET Journals on May 2, 2017
Arnt J (1982) Pharmacological specificity of conditioned avoidance response inhibition in rats: inhibition by neuroleptics and correlation to dopamine receptor
blockade. Acta Pharmacol Toxicol 51:321–329.
Arnt J (1995) Differential effects of classical and newer antipsychotics on the hypermotility induced by two dose levels of d-amphetamine. Eur J Pharmacol 283:55–
62.
Arnt JC, Sanchez C, and Skarsfeldt T (1994) The so-called atypical antipsychotics – how
similar are they? Neuropsychopharmacology 10:S469.
Bodick NC, Offen WW, Shannon HE, Satterwhite J, Lucas R, van Lier R, and Paul
SM (1997) Effects of xanomeline, a selective muscarinic receptor agonist, on
cognitive function and behavioral symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Neurol
54:465– 473.
Bymaster FP, Wong DT, Mitch CH, Ward JS, Calligaro DO, Schoepp DD, Shannon
HE, Sheardown MJ, Olesen PH, Suzdak PD, et al. (1994) Neurochemical effects of
the M1 muscarinic agonist xanomeline (LY246708/NNC11– 0232). J Pharmacol
Exp Ther 269:282–289.
Bymaster FP, Shannon HE, Rasmussen K, Delapp NW, Mitch CH, Ward JS, Calligaro DO, Ludvigsen TS, Sheardown MJ, Olesen PH, et al. (1998a) Unexpected
antipsychotic-like activity with the muscarinic receptor ligand (5R,6R)6-(3propylthio-1,2,5-thiadiazol-4-yl)-1-azabicyclo[3.2.1]octane. Eur J Pharmacol 356:
109 –119.
Bymaster FP, Carter PA, Peters SC, Zhang W, Ward JS, Mitch CH, Calligaro DO,
Whitesitt CA, DeLapp N, Shannon HE, et al. (1998b) Xanomeline compared to
other muscarinic agents on stimulation of phosphoinositide hydrolysis in vivo and
other cholinomimetic effects. Brain Res 795:179 –190.
Bymaster FP, Shannon HE, Rasmussen K, DeLapp NW, Ward JS, Calligaro DO,
Mitch CH, Whitesitt C, Ludvigsen TS, Sheardown M, et al. (1999) Potential role of
muscarinic receptors in schizophrenia. Life Sci 64:527–534.
Costall B, Fortune DH, Hui SC, and Naylor RJ (1980) Neuroleptic antagonism of the
motor inhibitory effects of apomorphine within the nucleus accumbens: drug
interaction at presynaptic receptors? Eur J Pharmacol 63:347–358.
Davis M (1980) Neurochemical modulation of sensory-motor reactivity: acoustic and
tactile startle reflexes. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 4:241–263.
Davis M (1990) Animal models of anxiety based on classical conditioning: the conditioned emotional response (CER) and the fear-potential startle. Pharmacol Ther
47:147–165.
Depoortere R, Perrault GH, and Sanger DJ (1997) Some, but not all, antipsychotic
drugs potentiate a low level of prepulse inhibition shown by rats of the Wistar
strain. Behav Pharmacol 8:364 –372.
Ellenbroek BA (1993) Treatment of schizophrenia: a clinical and preclinical evaluation of neuroleptic drugs. Pharmacol Ther 57:1–78.
Felder CC, Bymaster FP, Ward J, and DeLapp N (2000) Therapeutic opportunities
for muscarinic receptors in the central nervous system. J Med Chem 43:4333–
4353.
Fendt M and Koch M (1999) Cholinergic modulation of the acoustic startle response
in the caudal pontine reticular nucleus of the rat. Eur J Pharmacol 370:101–107.
Geyer MA, Krebs-Thomson K, and Varty GB (1999) The effects of M100907 in
pharmacological and developmental animal models of pre-pulse inhibition deficits
in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 21:S134 –S142.
Hagan JJ, Tonnaer JADM, Rikj H, Broekkamp CLE, and van Delft AML (1987)
Facilitation of amphetamine-induced rotation by muscarinic antagonists is correlated with M2 receptor affinity. Brain Res 410:69 –73.
Hutchinson M and Fazzini E (1996) Cholinesterase inhibition in Parkinson’s disease.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 61:324 –325.
Johnstone EC, Crow TJ, Ferrier IN, Frith CD, Owens DG, Bourne RC, and Gamble
SJ (1983) Adverse effects of anticholinergic medication on positive schizophrenic
symptoms. Psych Med 13:513–527.
Jones CK and Shannon HE (2000a) Muscarinic cholinergic modulation of prepulse
inhibition of the acoustic startle reflex. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 294:1017–1023.
Jones CK and Shannon HE (2000b) Effects of scopolamine in comparison with
apomorphine and phencyclidine on prepulse inhibition in rats. Eur J Pharmacol
391:105–112.
Kaufer D (1998) Beyond the cholinergic hypothesis: the effect of metrifonate and