* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Chapter 13
Peer pressure wikipedia , lookup
Interpersonal attraction wikipedia , lookup
Attitude (psychology) wikipedia , lookup
Relational aggression wikipedia , lookup
Attitude change wikipedia , lookup
Albert Bandura wikipedia , lookup
Implicit attitude wikipedia , lookup
Victim blaming wikipedia , lookup
Group dynamics wikipedia , lookup
Unpopularity wikipedia , lookup
Solomon Asch wikipedia , lookup
Memory conformity wikipedia , lookup
Social perception wikipedia , lookup
False consensus effect wikipedia , lookup
Vladimir J. Konečni wikipedia , lookup
Compliance (psychology) wikipedia , lookup
Social tuning wikipedia , lookup
Experimental Psychology PSY 433 Chapter 13 Social Psychology Social Psychology Social Cognition -- how we perceive others: Stereotypes, prejudice, attraction, liking. Attitudes and beliefs, identity, sense of self, and how these are changed. Social Influence -- how others influence our behavior: Conformity, compliance, and obedience. Aggression, violence, altruism, cooperation. Conformity Conformity Sherif’s (1935) work on social norms using the autokinetic effect. Autokinetic effect – a stationary spot of light in a dark room appears to move. What others say affects an observer’s perceptions –it appears to move in an arc if other people saw it move in an arc. Conformity Asch’s (1951, 1956, 1958) work on conformity using line judgments. Subjects were told the study was on visual discrimination, but it was actually on conformity. The task – identify which of 3 lines matches a standard. Asch expected that people would follow the evidence of their own eyes – but they didn’t. Standard A B C Asch’s Paradigm Six confederates & 1 subject Each responded out loud Experimental manipulation: Confederates respond correctly on 6 trials & incorrectly on 12 Most subjects conformed on 1 or more of the 12 incorrect trials Control: Confederates always responded correctly (only 5% of subjects erred). Compliance and Obedience Milgram (1963, 1964, 1965) obedience task Paid subjects volunteered for a study of the effects of punishment on learning/memory. Involved 3 people: Authority – the experimenter Victim – the “learner” (a confederate) Subject -- the “teacher” Milgram’s Shock Panel 15 30 45 60 75 90 105 120 135 150 165 180 195 210 225 240 255 270 285 300 Slight Shock Moderate Shock Strong Shock Very Strong Shock 315 330 345 360 375 390 405 420 435 450 ExtremeIntensity Shock Danger: Severe Shock XXX Intense Shock Learner Responses 15 30 45 60 75 90 105 120 135 150 165 180 195 210 225 240 255 270 285 300 Slight Shock Moderate Shock Grunts & Moans Strong Shock “Let me out!” Very Strong Shock “I can’t stand the pain!” Intense Shock “I refuse to answer!” Conditions Affecting Obedience The setting – did Yale foster obedience because it was well-known, Ivy League? Replication in a sleazy part of Bridgeport 48% gave max shock, compared to 65% Presence/absence of peers also showing defiance or conformity: Conforming peers encouraged greater shock. Proximity to the “victim”: 74% when hear victim, 40% when see victim, 30% when touch victim Interpreting Conformity Results Perhaps subjects trusted that no harm would really come to the subjects – treated the context as “make believe”. Perhaps results underestimate conformity, since the experimenter truly has no authority over the subject. Obedience is not necessarily bad – society would not function if people ignored laws and persons in authority. Dependent Variables Questionnaires measuring belief, attitude, preference (liking). Rating scales Behavioral measures: Aggression measured by shock given. Attraction measured by how long a man talks to a woman, smiles at her, whether he asks her out. Converging measures are better. Independent Variables Characteristics of a social situation or of people (demographic variables). Factors believed to affect behavior are manipulated: Persuasiveness – manipulate number or type of arguments used. Aggression – manipulate temperature in a room to test whether heat affects behavior. Conformity – manipulate number of people who agree or disagree. Experimenter Bias Subtle influences that experimenters may unknowingly exert on their outcomes. Tone of voice and emphasis may change across conditions. Expectancies may affect subject behavior. Blind and Double-blind procedures are not always possible. Replication guards against both experimenter bias and outright fraud. Demand Characteristics Are subjects acting normally in an experiment, or are they just doing what they think they are expected to do? Did Milgram’s subjects give shock because the experimental context demanded it? Placebo effect occurs when subjects are told they are taking medication and show effects because of their expectation. All experiments communicate an expectation explicitly and implicitly. Orne’s Experiments Orne sought an experiment so meaningless than subjects would refuse to do it: 2000 sheets with random digits that subjects were to add up – impossible yet no one refused. Told to tear up the sheets, subjects persisted for hours, saying there must be a good reason Orne and Evans (1965) examined demand characteristics in a hypnosis study. Is behavior due to hypnosis or demand characteristics? Hypnosis Demos http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=Lmgptd8bXfA http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=hn171z-CPLs Orne’s Results Hypnosis is not necessarily responsible for the behavior. Demand characteristics are sufficient to explain the behavior. Implicit Measures People may be unaware of their own prejudices or misreport them to be consistent with social norms against racism. Implicit attitude measures allow observation of correlates of prejudice without explicitly asking subjects about it. The IAT (Implicit Association Test) is one such measure – used in lab this week. Payne’s Priming Study On each trial a black or a white face was flashed so quickly subjects were unaware of seeing it (200 ms). Next either a weapon or a tool was presented. Subjects had to quickly identify whether it was a tool or a weapon. Tools were misidentified as guns more often after seeing the black face, suggesting a reliance on racial stereotypes. The Bystander Studies Several incidents pre-1970 got researchers interested in another area of social influence: The mere presence of other people The bystander effect -- the more people who observe a crisis, the less likely any one of them is to help the victim. Is this true in every situation? Outside of a Small Circle of Friends http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=Y4bSqSdto5g Kitty Genovese (1964) Darley and Latane (1968) Over an intercom, subjects discussed problems in college life with 1, 2, or 5 others. IV: Number of bystanders (0, 1, or 4) DV: whether subject responded & elapsed sec The more bystanders, the less likely subjects were to respond and the longer it took when they did respond Note: typo in Kantowitz Table 13-2. Darley & Latane’s Results Diffusion of Responsibility Piliavin et al. (1969) manipulated: Race of the victim simulating a crisis. Whether victim appeared ill or drunk. They recorded race of helper, number of helpers, racial composition of bystanders. Results: Help offered more readily to ill (95%) than drunk (50%). Race only mattered for drunk victims. Number of bystanders didn’t matter. Where Did the Effect Go? Piliavin et al.’s study was done in the field not in the lab. Maybe other factors were present. If people are made to feel responsible for a situation they are more likely to help, regardless of bystanders. Milgram’s subjects were told that the experimenter was responsible. People may be reluctant to intervene due to potential embarrassment, loss of poise. Stereotypes and Prejudice People may be unaware of their prejudices or misreport them in order to be consistent with social norms. Implicit memory tests and attitude measures permit observation of hypothesized correlates of prejudice without self-report. Implicit attitudes may influence behavior in real life, outside experimental contexts too.