Download We are What We Drive: A SRM Analysis of Human

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

James M. Honeycutt wikipedia , lookup

False consensus effect wikipedia , lookup

Group dynamics wikipedia , lookup

Social tuning wikipedia , lookup

Communication in small groups wikipedia , lookup

Zero-acquaintance personality judgments wikipedia , lookup

Team composition wikipedia , lookup

Impression formation wikipedia , lookup

Social perception wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
We are What We Drive: A SRM Analysis of Human-Brand Personality
Associations
Maxim Polonsky
University of Connecticut
Marketing Department
2100 Hillside Road, U-1041
Storrs, CT 06269-1041
Phone: (860) 486-2098
Email: [email protected]
Robin Coulter
University of Connecticut
Marketing Department
2100 Hillside Road, U-1041
Storrs, CT 06269-1041
Phone: (860) 486-2889
Email: [email protected]
We are What We Drive: A SRM Analysis of Human-Brand Personality
Associations
We draw upon self-congruity, symbolic interactionism, and interpersonal perception theories to
model brand/human personality interdependence. We identified ten automobile brands for which
consumers have some shared agreement about brand personality, and conducted a round-robin
experiment with twenty three/four person limited acquaintance groups. Group members rated
themselves and each other on seven personality characteristics, and answered: “How likely do
you see yourself driving [brand]?” “How likely do you see [group member] driving [brand]?”
and “How likely do you think that [group member] sees you driving [brand]” Using the Social
Relations Model, we assess the extent to which consumers perceive brands and consumers as
sharing personality characteristics.
Nearly a half-century ago, Levy (1959) advanced the idea that brands are
reflective symbols of the self. More recent work has considered how consumers use
brands to create and communicate identity (Belk 1988; Escalas and Bettman 2005;
Kleine, Kleine and Kerrnan 1993; Kleine, Kleine, and Allen 1995; Sirgy 1982; Solomon
1983), as well as to reinforce attitudes toward self (Escalas and Bettman 2003;
McCracken 1988). Aaker’s seminal work (1997) on the association of human personality
characteristics to brands has been extremely influential in spurring the dialogue about
how companies and customers anthropomorphize their brands. In the ensuing years,
marketers and consumer researchers have focused on leveraging and understanding brand
personality, that is, the set of human characteristics associated with a brand (Aaker 1999;
Aaker, Benet-Martinez, and Garolera 2001; Johar, Sengupta, Aaker 2005). As a
consequence, we are lead to contemplate questions about consumers’ brands and their
personality characteristics, such as: “Do you infer an individual’s personality based on
his/her ownership of specific brands? Similarly, when you observe an individual’s
personality, do you associate that individual with a specific brand? Finally, are your
opinions unique, or are they shared by others?
Several theories of the self, individually and in a social context, are important to
contemplate in the context of human-brand personality work. Briefly, self-congruity
theory argues that consumers prefer brands with a symbolic function that is congruous
with their self-identity (Sirgy 1982; Swann et al. 1992); symbolic interaction theory
focuses on an individual’s beliefs about how he is perceived by others (Cooley 1902,
Mead, 1934; Solomon 1983); and interpersonal perception theory suggests that person
perception consists of interdependent levels, but can be decomposed to understand an
individual’s perceptions of self, of another individual, and perceptions of how others see
him (Malloy and Kenny 1986; Kenny 1994). In this paper, we draw upon these three
theoretical perspectives, as well as the Social Relations Model (SRM; Kenny 1993, 1994;
Kenny and Albright 1987; Kenny, Kashy, and Cook 2006) to further explicate and model
brand and human personality interdependence. The SRM approach, using dyadic data,
enables us to address questions such as: If Jane is seen to be extraverted, is she also
associated with an extraverted brand? If Jane sees herself as being extraverted and prefers
extraverted brands, do others also see her as extraverted and preferring those brands? If
Jane sees herself using extraverted brands, does she see others using those brands?
Our research involved three data collections. As a basis for our experiment which
focuses on the intersection of human and brand personalities, we engaged in two
preliminary studies to understand consumers’ associations with automobile brands and
personality characteristics. Our work focuses on the automobile product category because
research in marketing and consumer behavior has documented that this product category
includes brands that are associated with personality characteristics by groups of
consumers (Bagozzi and Dholakia 2006; Muniz and O’Guinn 2001). We identified 23
brands and had 70 undergraduate students evaluate the brands on 34 semantic differential
items with regard to the Big Five personality characteristics – Agreeableness,
Extraversion, Conscientiousness, Emotional Stability, and Openness (Goldberg 1990)
and two additional brand personality characteristics, Sophistication and Ruggedness
(Aaker 1997). Additionally, we provided 40 MBA students with a definition of the seven
personality characteristics and via a free association task asked participants to name one
automobile brand that they associated with each personality characteristic. Based on these
data collections, we identified ten brands (Jeep, Volvo, Toyota, Hummer, Jaguar,
Mercedes, BMW, Audi, Honda, and VW) which had shared meaning for our main
experiment.
Our focal study employs a round-robin experiment using 20 three or four person
limited acquaintance groups, that is, groups in which the members were unfamiliar with
one another. After a short (seven minute) introduction period, group members rated
themselves and each group member on seven human-brand personality characteristics,
including conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, emotional stability, openness to
experience, sophistication, and ruggedness (Aaker 1997). Then, participants answered the
following brand use questions: 1) “How likely do you see yourself using brand X?” 2)
“How likely do you see [group member] using the brand X?” and 3) “How likely do you
think that [group member] sees you using brand X?” These data were analyzed using the
Social Relations Model (Kenny 1988; Kenny et al. 2006) to assess the extent to which
consumers perceive individual brands and consumers as sharing personality
characteristics, and to determine if the Devil really wears Prada.
Our work demonstrates that even short interaction encounters enable participants
to come to consensual conclusions regarding individual traits associated with specific
brands. Moreover, for some brands we found perceiver-target accuracy so the ratings of
participants correlated with target self-ratings. Both, consensus and accuracy speak to the
existence of the socially shared meanings among individuals, as well as between
personality traits and brands. Meta-accuracy further validates the symbolic-interactionism
perspective: an individual thinking that others see her driving a Jaguar and being accurate
in her perceptions indicates that a person is capable of understanding how others perceive
her (table 3). The symbolic-interactionism perspective posits that people perceive and
internalize others’ attitudes to form the concept of self (Depaulo et al. 1987). Solomon
(1983, 320) argued that people define themselves and social reality via product
symbolism, relying “upon the social information inherent in products to shape self-image
and to maximize the quality of role performance.”
Our research on brands and their personalities provides evidence of symbolic
interactionism in the context of consumption. We illustrate how the Social Relations
Model decomposes the multiple levels of social interaction as related to brands, their
users, and observers. In conclusion, our work demonstrates that consumer and brand
personalities are interdependent and points to a shared meaning system that is used to
make inferences about individuals and products. Shared assumptions about physical
appearance and shared interpretations of behaviors enable perceivers to come to
consensual judgments about targets’ personalities and brand use after seven-minute
interaction periods. Moreover, such judgments can be accurate, reflecting targets’ selfviews.
KEY REFERENCES
Aaker, Jennifer (1997), “Dimensions of Brand Personality,” Journal of Marketing
Research, 34 (8), 347–56.
Ambady, Nalini, and Rosenthal, Robert (1992), “Thin Slices of Expressive Behavior as
Predictors of Interpersonal Consequences: A Meta-Analysis,” Psychological
Bulletin, 111, 256–74.
Escalas, Jennifer Edson and James R. Bettman (23), “You Are What They Eat: The
Influence of Reference Groups on Consumer Connections to Brands,” Journal of
Consumer Psychology, 13 (3), 339-48.
Fournier, Susan (1998), “Consumers and Their Brands: Developing Relationship Theory
in Consumer Research,” Journal of Consumer Research, 4 (March), 343–73.
Keller, Kevin L. (1993), “Conceptualizing, Measuring, and Managing Customer-Based
Brand Equity,” Journal of Marketing, 57 (1), 1–22.
Kenny, David A. (1993), “A Coming-of-Age for Research on Interpersonal Perception,”
Journal of Personality, 61, 789-87.
Kenny, David A., Linda Albright, Thomas E. Malloy, and Deborah A. Kashy, (1994),
“Consensus in Interpersonal Perception: Acquaintance and the Big Five,”
Psychological Bulletin, 116 (2), 245–58.
Kenny, David A., Cynthia D. Mohr, and Maurice J. Levesque (21), “A Social Relations
Variance Partitioning of Dyadic Behavior,” Psychological Bulletin, 127(1), 128–
41.
Kleine, Robert E. III, Susan Schultz-Kleine, and Jerome B. Kernan (1992), “Mundane
Everyday Consumption and the Self: A Conceptual Orientation and Prospects for
Consumer Research,” Advances in Consumer Research, 19 (1), 411–15.
McCracken, Grant (1988), Culture and Consumption: New Approaches to the Symbolic
Character of Consumer Goods and Activities, Bloomington, IN: Indiana
University Press.
Sirgy, Joseph M. (1982), “Self-Concept in Consumer Behavior: A Critical Review,”
Journal of Consumer Research, 9 (December), 287–3.
Solomon, Michael R. (1983), “The Role of Products as Social Stimuli: A Symbolic
Interactionism Perspective,” The Journal of Consumer Research, 1 (December),
319 29.
Walker, Rob (28), Buying In: The Secret Dialogue between What We Buy and Who We
Are. Random House.