Download The Influence of Social Norms in Consumer Behavior

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Behavioral modernity wikipedia , lookup

Normality (behavior) wikipedia , lookup

Sociological theory wikipedia , lookup

Social theory wikipedia , lookup

Unilineal evolution wikipedia , lookup

Criminology wikipedia , lookup

Social network (sociolinguistics) wikipedia , lookup

Social history wikipedia , lookup

Thin-slicing wikipedia , lookup

Sociology of terrorism wikipedia , lookup

Neuroeconomics wikipedia , lookup

Labeling theory wikipedia , lookup

Behaviorism wikipedia , lookup

Social Bonding and Nurture Kinship wikipedia , lookup

History of the social sciences wikipedia , lookup

Social group wikipedia , lookup

Value-action gap wikipedia , lookup

Social psychology wikipedia , lookup

Social norm wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
1
The Influence of Social Norms in Consumer Behavior:
A Meta-Analysis
Vladimir Melnyk
Erica van Herpen
Hans C. M. van Trijp*
* Vladimir Melnyk is a doctoral candidate at the Marketing and Consumer Behavior Group,
Wageningen University, 6706 KN Wageningen, The Netherlands (e-mail:
[email protected], Phone: +31 317 484348, Fax: +31 317 484361).
Erica van Herpen is assistant professor, Marketing and Consumer Behavior Group, Wageningen
University, 6706 KN Wageningen, The Netherlands (e-mail: [email protected], Phone:
+31 317 483385, Fax: +31 317 484361).
Hans C. M. van Trijp is a professor of Marketing and Consumer Behavior, Wageningen
University, 6706 KN Wageningen, The Netherlands (e-mail: [email protected], Phone: +31
317 483385, Fax: +31 317 484361).
2
Social norms are major drivers of human behavior and crucial in consumer decision
making. Consumers often take expectations and behavior of others into consideration when they
decide what is appropriate and social norms thus profoundly influence their preferences and
behavior (Cialdini, Reno, and Kallgren 1990). How much to drink at a party, whether to
subscribe to a fitness club and how much to eat are all decisions that are at least partly guided by
social norms. Although social norms can substantially influence consumer decision making,
understanding of how the specification of the norm determines its effect is limited. Despite a
large body of research on social norms, empirical findings about their effect in consumer
decision making are not consistent (Schultz et al. 2007). For example, Sheeran, Abraham, and
Orbell (1999), in their meta-analysis of the willingness to use condoms (121 studies out of which
21 include social norms) find that subjective norms are weak predictors of intentions (r = .26),
whereas Rivis and Sheeran (2003) in their meta-analysis of the theory of planned behavior (21
studies) find a more substantial correlation between norms and intentions (r = .44).
This meta-analysis examines the association between social norms on the one hand and
consumer’s attitudes, behavioral intentions, and behavior on the other hand, while accounting for
study characteristics (study domain, gender and age of participants, type of culture) and
methodological factors. We expect that several aspects of norm specification will influence the
strength of these associations. First, norms can be specified in a descriptive or prescriptive (i.e.,
injunctive) way, a distinction which has previously been shown to determine their influence on
consumer decision making (Cialdini et al. 1990). Injunctive norms focus consumers on what is
approved and may activate the typical attitudes associated with the group, whereas descriptive
norms specify the behavior of others, and consumers may follow such norms without giving it
much thought. Thus we expect descriptive norms have a larger effect on behavior but a smaller
effect on attitudes than an injunctive norms. Second, the influence of social norms may depend
on the concreteness with which the required behavior, the consequences of following or
deviating from the norm, and the target person are specified (Shaffer 1983). Concrete
information is generally more engaging and memorable than abstract information, and could
therefore be more persuasive. Third, norms may be more relevant, and hence more influential,
when these come from persons with whom the consumer can easily identify. Fourth, because
public behavior is noticed and corrected by others, norms relating to public behavior should have
more influence than norms relating to private behavior.
The sample consisted of 200 studies, producing 659 effect sizes. As a measure for effect
sizes, we chose the Pearson correlation coefficient, converted to the normally distributed Fisher's
z scores. Because attitudes are both strongly correlated with norms and have a strong influence
on intentions (behavior), the total correlation between norms and intentions (behavior) may
contain some of the effect of attitudes and thus overestimate the effect of social norms. To
account for this, we also examined partial correlations between norms and intentions (behavior),
controlling for attitudes.
Results showed that effect sizes obtained from partial correlations were significantly
lower than effect sizes obtained from total correlations for both behavioral intentions and
behavior. This implies that indeed a substantial part of the effect of social norms on intentions
and behavior can be accounted for by the covariation between attitudes and social norms.
Descriptive norms were shown to have a larger effect on behavior than injunctive norms,
whereas injunctive norms have a larger effect on attitudes than descriptive norms. Hence, for
changing attitudes, injunctive norms may be more effective, whereas for changing behavior,
descriptive norms are more appropriate. Effects on behavior are also stronger when norms come
from close and concrete sources (vs. authority figures or abstract others) and when the behavior
is public (vs. private). No effects were found for specifications of the expected behavior, the
consequences, or the target person. In addition, we examined interaction effects between
3
independent variables and no significant effects were found.
The study also demonstrated that the effect of social norms differed across domains.
Compared with decisions related to healthy lifestyle, social responsibility, or sex, everyday
consumption decisions such as choices between food, drinks, and leisure time activities showed a
high consistency between social norms and attitudes. Interestingly, social norms had a relatively
large effect on social responsible behaviors. These are behaviors where societal benefits are
involved, and where social norms should drive human behavior to prevent free-riding problems.
Our study showed that social norms indeed are relatively influential for consumer behavior in
this domain.
Our study has several implications. The meta-analysis reveals a high association between
social norms and attitudes, and one possible extension to theoretical models that include
normative components is to examine this relationship. It has recently been posited that social
norms influence attitudes (Terry et al. 2000), and our study shows that the strength of this
influence depends on the specification of norm aspects.
Additionally, consumers respond differently to injunctive versus descriptive norms, and,
more importantly, an injunctive specification of a norm leads to stronger effects on attitudes but
weaker effects on behavior than a descriptive formulation. This implies that an investigation of
the effect of social norms which examines only attitudes or only behavior does not provide a
complete picture of the effect of social norms. To truly understand the effect of social norms,
attitudes, behavioral intentions, and behavior need all be examined. In addition, further research
could examine other aspects of social norms, such as group size, or uncertainty, which could
influence their effectiveness. This meta-analysis is obviously bound to prior research that has
been conducted, but more aspects remain to be investigated. We hope that our study presents a
stepping stone towards a deeper understanding of when and how social norms drive consumer
attitudes, intentions, and behavior.
References
Albarracín, Dolores, Blair T. Johnson, Martin Fishbein, and Paige A. Muellerleile (2001),
"Theories of Reasoned Action and Planned Behavior as Models of Condom Use: A MetaAnalysis," Psychological Bulletin, 127 (1), 142-61.
Armitage, Christopher J. and Mark Connor (2001), "Efficacy of the Theory of Planned
Behaviour: A Meta-Analytic Review," British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 471-99.
Bijmolt, Tammo H. A. and Rik G. M. Pieters (2001), "Meta-Analysis in Marketing When
Studies Contain Multiple Measurements," Marketing Letters, 12 (2), 157-69.
Cialdini, Robert B., Raymond. R. Reno, and Carl A. Kallgren (1990), "A Focus Theory of
Normative Conduct: Recycling the Concept of Norms to Reduce Littering in Public
Places," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58 (6), 1015-26.
Reno, Raymond R., Robert B. Cialdini, and Carl A. Kallgren (1993), "The Transsituational
Influence of Social Norms," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64 (1), 10412.
Rivis, Armanda and Paschal Sheeran (2003), "Descriptive Norms as an Additional Predictor in
the Theory of Planned Behaviour: A Meta-Analysis," Current Psychology, 22 (3), 21833.
Schultz, P. Wesley, Jessica M. Nolan, Robert B. Cialdini, Noah J. Goldstein, and Vlades
Griskevicius (2007), "The Constructive, Destructive and Reconstructive Power of Social
Norms," Psychological Science, 18 (5), 429-34.
4
Shaffer, Leigh S. (1983), "Toward Pepitone’s Vision of a Normative Social Psychology: What Is
a Social Norm?," Journal of Mind and Behavior, 4 (2), 275-93.
Sheeran, Paschal, Charles Abraham, and Sheina Orbell (1999), "Psychosocial Correlates of
Heterosexual Condom Use: A Meta-Analysis," Psychological Bulletin, 125 (1), 90-132.
Terry, Deborah J., Michael A. Hogg, and Katherine M. White (2000b), "Attitude-Behavior
Relations: Social Identity and Group Membership " in Attitudes, Behavior, and Social
Context: The Role of Norms and Group Membership, ed. Deborah J. Terry and Michael
A. Hogg, London: Lawrence Erlbaum 67-94.