* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Using the Five-Factor Model to Represent the DSM-IV
Separation anxiety disorder wikipedia , lookup
Depersonalization disorder wikipedia , lookup
Autism spectrum wikipedia , lookup
Impulsivity wikipedia , lookup
Eating disorders and memory wikipedia , lookup
Generalized anxiety disorder wikipedia , lookup
Conversion disorder wikipedia , lookup
Schizoaffective disorder wikipedia , lookup
Comorbidity wikipedia , lookup
Asperger syndrome wikipedia , lookup
Eating disorder wikipedia , lookup
Obsessive–compulsive personality disorder wikipedia , lookup
Munchausen by Internet wikipedia , lookup
Conduct disorder wikipedia , lookup
Mental disorder wikipedia , lookup
Diagnosis of Asperger syndrome wikipedia , lookup
Causes of mental disorders wikipedia , lookup
Addictive personality wikipedia , lookup
Child psychopathology wikipedia , lookup
History of mental disorders wikipedia , lookup
Spectrum disorder wikipedia , lookup
Antisocial personality disorder wikipedia , lookup
Dissociative identity disorder wikipedia , lookup
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders wikipedia , lookup
Personality disorder wikipedia , lookup
Copyright 2001 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0021-843X/01/S5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0021-843X. 110.3.401 Journal of Abnormal Psychology 2001, Vol. 110, No. 3,401-412 Using the Five-Factor Model to Represent the DSM-IV Personality Disorders: An Expert Consensus Approach Donald R. Lynam and Thomas A. Widiger University of Kentucky This study sought to extend previous work on the five-factor dimensional model (FFM) of personality disorder (PD) by developing more comprehensive FFM descriptions of prototypic cases. Specifically, the authors asked experts in each of the 10 DSM-IV PDs to rate the prototypic case by using all 30 facets of the FFM. Aggregating across raters of the given disorder generated a prototype for each disorder. In general, there was good agreement among experts and with previous theoretical and empirical FFM translations of DSM diagnostic criteria. Furthermore, the ability of the FFM explanation to reproduce the high comorbidity rates among PDs was demonstrated. The authors concluded that, with the possible exception of schizotypal PD, the DSM PDs can be understood from the dimensional perspective of the FFM. Future directions for research, including the use of the present prototypes to "diagnose" personality disorder, are discussed. personality disorder categories can be seen as configurations of basic dimensions of personality. The five-factor model (FFM) of personality was derived originally from studies of the English language to identify the domains of personality functioning most important in describing the personality traits of oneself and other persons (Digman, 1990; John & Srivastava, 1999; Wiggins & Pincus, 1992). This lexical research has emphasized five broad domains of personality, identified as extraversion (surgency or positive affectivity), agreeableness (vs. antagonism), conscientiousness (or constraint), neuroticism (negative affectivity), and openness to experience (intellect or unconventionality; John & Srivastava, 1999). Each of these five domains can be further differentiated into underlying facets or components. Costa and McCrae (1995) proposed six facets within each domain on the basis of their research with the NEO Personality Inventory—Revised (NEO-PI-R; Costa & McCrae, 1992). For example, they suggested that the domain of agreeableness can be usefully differentiated into more specific facets of trust (vs. mistrust or skepticism), straightforwardness (vs. deception or manipulation), altruism (vs. egocentrism or exploitation), compliance (vs. oppositionalism or aggression), modesty (vs. arrogance), and tendermindedness (vs. toughmindedness or callousness). Empirical support for the construct validity of the FFM is extensive, at both the domain and the facet levels, including convergent and discriminant validation across self, peer, and spouse ratings (Costa & McCrae, 1988); temporal stability across 7-10 years (Costa & McCrae, 1994); cross-cultural replication (De Raad, Perugini, Hrebickova, & Szarota, 1998); and heritability (Jang, McCrae, Angleitner, Reimann, & Livesley, 1998; Plomin & Caspi, 1999). The FFM has been used as an integrative model of personality functioning for children (Halverson, Kohnstamm, & Martin, 1994), adults (McCrae & Costa, 1990), the elderly (Costa & McCrae, 1994), and even animal species (Gosling & John, 1999). Instructive and informative critiques of the FFM have been provided (e.g., Block, 1995; Westen, 1995), but there does appear to be sufficient empirical support for the "basicness" of the FFM The American Psychiatric Association's Diagnostic and Statistical Manual'of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994), provides personality disorder diagnostic categories. There is good evidence that these diagnoses identify clinically meaningful maladaptive personality traits; several diagnoses seem especially well-validated, including antisocial (see Stoff, Breiling, & Maser, 1997), borderline (see Clarkin, Marziali, & Munroe-Blum, 1992), schizotypal (Raine, Lencz, & Mednick, 1995), narcissistic (Ronningstam, 1998) and dependent (see Bornstein, 1992) personality disorders. These disorders have long clinical traditions, are related to cognitive and perceptual abnormalities, influence the life outcomes of individuals, have implications for psychological treatment, and show some genetic component (Clarkin & Lenzenweger, 1996; Livesley, 2001). Nonetheless, the DSM-IV personality disorders have been criticized for the fundamental assumption that "Personality Disorders represent qualitatively distinct clinical syndromes" (American Psychiatric Association, 1994, p. 633). A variety of studies and methodologies have raised compelling concerns regarding the validity of the categorical model (Clark, Livesley, & Morey, 1997; Livesley, 1998; Widiger, 1993) and have proposed alternative dimensional models for the personality disorders, including (but not limited to) the 15-factor model of Clark (1993), the interpersonal circumplex (Benjamin, 1993), the 7-factor model of Cloninger (Cloninger, Svrakic, Bay on, & Przybeck, 1999), the 18factor model of Livesley (1998), and the 5-factor model (Widiger & Costa, 1994). From these dimensional perspectives, the DSM Donald R. Lynam and Thomas A. Widiger, Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky. This research was supported in part by National Institute of Mental Health Grant MH60104. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Donald R. Lynam, University of Kentucky, Department of Psychology, 115 Kastle Hall, Lexington, Kentucky 40506-0044. Electronic mail may be sent to [email protected]. 401 402 LYNAM AND WIDIGER to consider it for use as a possible dimensional model of personality disorder symptomatology (Widiger & Costa, 1994). From the perspective of the FFM, personality disorders represent configurations of basic dimensions of personality. Much research is consistent with this possibility. The dimensions of the FFM are related to personality disorder symptomatology. Widiger and Costa (in press) have identified over 50 published studies that have shown relations between the FFM and personality disorder symptoms. Most of the studies have examined the relations between scores on FFM inventories and scores on measures of personality disorder symptoms. Almost all of the authors have concluded that their findings provided support for understanding personality disorder symptomatology from the perspective of the FFM. Many of these FFM studies addressed specific hypotheses put forth by Widiger, Trull, Clarkin, Sanderson, and Costa (1994), who provided five-factor translations of the DSM-III-R personality disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 1987). Besides serving as useful heuristics for studies concerned explicitly with DSM-III-R personality disorders, these translations suggest that the FFM possesses the language necessary for the description of the personality disorders and illustrate the beginnings of an important alternative approach to understanding the personality disorders in terms of the FFM. To the extent that the FFM can be used as a starting point in reproducing the PDs and the nomological nets that surround them, support is generated for the FFM account. In the present study, we seek to extend this previous work by using an alternative approach to the development of FFM personality disorder profiles, the use of expert consensus. This study examines the degree to which experts agree in their FFM descriptions of the personality disorders. In addition, we investigate how well the expert consensus approach agrees with theoretical and empirical approaches to profile development. Finally, this study examines how well the overlap among FFM prototypes can reproduce the high and differential comorbidity rates among the DSM personality disorders—an epidemiological fact that a theory of personality disorders should be able to explain but a fact with which the categorical model has had much difficulty (Clark et al., 1997; Lilienfeld, Waldman, & Israel, 1994; Livesley, 1998; Widiger, 1993). The expert consensus approach to be used in the present article was used previously by Miller, Lynam, Widiger, and Leukefeld (2001) and Lynam (in press) in their attempts to understand psychopathy from the perspective of the FFM. Miller et al. (2001) developed an FFM profile of psychopathy by compiling the descriptions obtained from 15 nationally recognized psychopathy researchers of a prototypic case of psychopathy with respect to each of the 30 Costa and McCrae (1995) facets of the FFM. This expert consensus FFM profile was then compared with Widiger and Lynam's (1998) FFM translation of the 20 items within Hare's (1991) Revised Psychology Checklist (PCL-R) in an attempt to provide convergent validity for the FFM conceptualization of psychopathy. The correlation between the expert consensus profile and the PCL-R FFM profile provided by Widiger and Lynam was .64, indicating good convergence but also suggesting some notable disagreements reflecting a lack of coverage of the PCL-R. In a comparison between the expert consensus prototype approach and the criteria translation approach, the consensus approach seems to enjoy some advantages. First, in the case of Miller et al. (2001), the expert consensus FFM profile was constructed by persons with varying degrees of familiarity with the FFM rather than by investigators with initial expectations of the profile that should be obtained. Thus, results should be more compelling when obtained from personality disorder researchers than from the FFM investigators themselves. Second, aggregation across multiple experts blunts idiosyncratic interpretations or judgments concerning the criterion and brings out in stark contrast the points of agreement. In these respects, the consensus approach of Miller et al. could be said to provide a more empirical, objective, and independent FFM profile. Third, the expert consensus methodology allows for the possibility of obtaining elevations on facets of the FFM not included explicitly within a particular criteria set. In the case of psychopathy, although the PCL-R is the most widely used and compelling measure of psychopathy, concerns have been raised with respect to the adequacy of its coverage and conceptualization of psychopathy (Lilienfeld, 1994; Lykken, 1995). Fourth, the consensus methodology allows for an evaluation of the internal consistency and interrater reliability of the FFM descriptions. If the FFM is to serve as a model of personality disorder symptomology, experts should agree in their FFM parsings of the symptoms. For example, Miller et al. obtained good agreement among the psychopathy researchers with respect to their FFM ratings. More than half of the 30 facets obtained standard deviations of less than .70, and only 17% had a standard deviation greater than 1.0; interrater reliability ranged from .61 to .84, with a mean of .75. Fifth and finally, the expert consensus approach provides a quantitative profile against which individuals' FFM profiles can be matched to determine the degree of similarity; individuals who are more similar to the prototype possess the disorder to a greater degree. Using data from a longitudinal community sample of 481 adults, Miller et al. found that individuals who more closely approximated the psychopathic profile had higher scores on a psychopathy inventory; more symptoms of antisocial personality disorder, substance abuse, and substance dependence; more frequent and varied antisocial activities; and fewer internalizing symptoms. All of these relations replicate those in previous studies using incarcerated, PCL-R defined psychopaths. Because of the advantages to this consensus approach, a purpose of the current study is to generate expert consensus FFM profiles for each of the DSM-FV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) personality disorders. Internal consistency and interrater reliability are examined along with a comparison to the profiles generated theoretically by Widiger et al. (1994) and empirically by Dyce and O'Connor (1998) to assess convergent validity, points of disagreement, and divergent coverage. In addition, the current study further explores the validity of the expert consensus FFM profiles by determining the extent to which the overlap among the profiles mirrors the covariation or co-occurrence among the personality disorder diagnostic categories. Comorbidity of personality disorder diagnoses is the norm rather than the exception, despite the hope or aspiration of DSM-IV that only one personality disorder diagnosis would be applicable for any particular patient (Gunderson, 1992; Gunderson, Links, & Reich, 1991). Several studies have demonstrated that most patients who meet diagnostic criteria for one personality disorder will meet the criteria for a number of other personality disorder diagnoses (e.g., Skodol, Rosnick, Kellman, Oldham, & Hyler, 1991; Widiger & Trull, 1998). As noted by Mineka, 403 FFM PD PROTOTYPES Watson, and Clark (1998), "the greatest challenge that the extensive comorbidity data pose to the current nosological system concerns the validity of the diagnostic categories themselves—do these disorders constitute distinct clinical entities?" (p. 380). Although high comorbidity presents a fundamental challenge to the validity of the categorical approach, it is easily accommodated within a dimensional model that views the categories as configurations of basic dimensions of personality (Clark et al., 1997; Livesley, 1998; Widiger, 1993). From this view, the apparent comorbidity among the personality disorders is understood as the co-occurrence of diagnoses that have overlapping constellations of maladaptive personality traits (Lilienfeld et al., 1994). Much of the co-occurrence among the DSM personality disorders might be explained by the domains and facets of the FFM; from the FFM perspective, disorders are expected to co-occur to the extent that they assess common FFM domains and facets. For example, the co-occurrence of the avoidant and schizoid personality disorders may reflect the involvement of introversion in both disorders (Widiger et al., 1994). Borderline personality disorder is the most commonly diagnosed and co-occurring personality disorder, consistent with its FFM conceptualization as being the personality disorder defined primarily by the facets of neuroticism (Clarkin, Hull, Cantor, & Sanderson, 1993; Widiger et al., 1994). Only one prior FFM personality disorder study has addressed personality disorder covariation (O'Connor & Dyce, 1998). Although the results of this study provided support for the FFM conceptualization of personality disorders, it was limited by not addressing comorbidity hypotheses with respect to individual personality disorders and by confining the analyses to the five broad domains of the FFM. Some differentiation among the personality disorders can occur with respect to the five domains, but more precise differentiation might be obtained at the level of the 30 facets (Axelrod, Widiger, Trull, & Corbitt, 1997; Dyce & O'Connor, 1998; Trull, Widiger, & Burr, 2001). The current study extends the findings of O'Connor and Dyce (1998) by determining the extent to which the diagnostic co-occurrence or covariation among personality disorders identified in prior studies can be recreated within the expert consensus FFM profiles. To the extent that this can happen, support is generated for the FFM account. Method Experts were identified through electronic searches of the psychological and psychiatric literature by using the personality disorders as search terms. To be included in the present research, an individual had to have published at least one article on the specific personality disorder (although most participants had published more than two articles). Between 25 and 30 experts were identified for most disorders. Address information was collected from the articles or through a search of the World Wide Web. A total of 197 experts were identified; the number of experts per disorder ranged from a low of 23 for schizotypal personality disorder to a high of 29 for histrionic and dependent personality disorders. Of these 197 experts, 20 were asked to rate three different personality disorders, 33 were asked to rate two different personality disorders, and 144 were asked to rate only one personality disorder. Experts were asked to rate prototypic cases of the various personality disorders with respect to the 30 facets of the FFM as described by the NEO-PI-R (Costa & McCrae, 1992). The identifying label"for each of the 30 facets was provided, along with two to four adjectives that described both poles of each of the facets. Adjectives were taken from the NEO-PI-R test manual and FFM adjective checklists (Costa & McCrae, 1992; Gold- berg, 1992; Saucier, 1994). For example, anxiousness was assessed with the following descriptors: fearful, apprehensive versus relaxed, unconcerned, cool. Order was assessed by using these descriptors: organized, methodical, ordered versus haphazard, disorganized, sloppy. For each of the 30 traits, experts were asked to rate the prototypic case of a specified personality disorder on a l-to-5 scale. Specifically, he or she was asked the following: In particular, we would like you to describe the prototypic case for [one, each of the two, or each of the three] personality disorders on a 1 to 5 point scale, where 1 indicates that the prototypic person would be extremely low on the trait (i.e., extremely lower than the average person), 2 indicates that the prototypic person would be low, 3 indicates that the person would be neither low nor high (i.e., does not differ from the average individual), 4 indicates that the prototypic person would be high on that trait, and 5 indicates that the prototypic person would be extremely high on that trait. For example, for the vulnerability trait dimension, a score of 1 would indicate that the prototypic case for that respective personality disorder would be extremely low in vulnerability (i.e., stalwart, brave, fearless, unflappable), whereas a score of 5 would indicate that the prototypic person with that respective personality disorder would be extremely high in vulnerability (i.e., fragile, helpless). Please rate the prototypic case for the respective personality disorder on each of the 30 trait dimensions. Experts were provided with one rating form for each disorder, a demographic questionnaire, and a return, postage-paid envelope with no means for the researchers to identify the respondent. Experts were told that the ratings they provided would be consolidated with ratings obtained from other selected experts, and they were assured that their individual ratings would be kept confidential and not be reported. We received 170 completed ratings from 120 experts. The average completion rate of 63% compares well with similar previous studies; Miller et al. (2001) obtained a completion rate of 69% in their FFM study of psychopathy, and Lynam (in press) obtained a completion rate of 57% in his study of fledgling psychopathy. The per-disorder completion rate ranged from a low of 38% for paranoid PD to over 85% for antisocial and borderline PD. A total of 93% of the 120 individuals completed the demographic questionnaire. Of these, 77% were male, 81% were Caucasian, and the median age was 47 years. Fully 96% had doctorate or medical degrees, 56% worked in an academic setting, and 22% worked in medical centers. Finally, on a scale ranging from 1 (not at all familiar) to 4 (very familiar), 98% of the experts said they were at least moderately familiar (3), and 77% indicated they were very familiar with the DSM-fV personality disorders. Results Personality Disorder Prototype Descriptions Table 1 provides the means and standard deviations for each of the 30 facets of the FFM for each of the 10 personality disorders included within the DSM-IV. As in Miller et al. (2001), taking any facet with a mean score of 2 and lower (low) or 4 and higher (high) as characteristic, it is possible to describe each of the personality disorders with the language of the FFM. For example, antisocial personality disorder was represented by low scores on all 6 facets of agreeableness, 3 of 6 facets of conscientiousness (dutifulness, self-discipline, and deliberation), and 2 facets of neuroticism (anxiousness and self-consciousness); it was also represented by high scores on 3 of the 6 facets of extraversion (assertiveness, activity, and excitement seeking) and 2 facets of neuroticism (angry hostility and impulsiveness). 404 LYNAM AND WIDIGER >— cr. CN -H <N ^- — en o en r- —- f N - H t N < N ' - - - ^ ' —1 — <u Q j : —' i n c e ^ o v e n o o a N O t ^ - ^ o i^a^ [rirn^^c^r^ "o s •' O" i—<" bo" r-" ^ •—' o — f oo" ^-* csf bo" oo'oooo'^oooooo—; o — oooo^oo^oo'o'oooooooo ) r- r- c>^^^ ' \Q oo oo oo r-- ON o> OOOOO'OOOOOOO'OOOO'O-HOOOO'OO'OOO'OOOOOOOO "2 O -— — r-ooir--'—< ^ ^( - ^ I O N —< TtTtr^ —<" rj — r- in oo I r- oo <n en , o, o, o, o^ o, o, o, o, o, o o, o, o, , ^ o, o, o, ^ o, o o, o, o, ^ o, o, o, o, o, o, o, o o, col en m r- •— <| r^ r-t oC en ^o in t-~ bo es" bo bo o r~- -— cs OOooOOOQOOin | » n O O O O O0 O pt^^'^.'O ^ lenen T3 'S § • g <u£ s s £- 11 s<- blJ .1? 1 g II !Q^ ^o < < •s •g s£ P en a " o T2 >" 8 S I (D J3r- -a B .2 -3 .a js .a > >^ e « ^o«i-t«^H'j3.t^rt> * -o 1 P 1 ! ! c e " aus a J|1S 3A ^ill-i -C S S 5?s S X) t**3 " a " =3 ^ n S .S § » 2 jS cfl 'aj '-C ^ ^2 g p-g S til •- §•» '5 -S S3 — s 1-s i|^l^2> w §,£«< al^ § FFM PD PROTOTYPES 405 Table 2 Measures of Agreement Among Experts for the 10 Personality Disorders Disorder Paranoid Schizoid Schizotypal Antisocial Borderline Histrionic Narcissistic Avoidant Dependent Compulsive No. of raters Average rWSa Average SDb Average interrater rc Average corrected item-total t* a for composite6 r with Widiger et al.f r with Dyce & O'Connor8 10 13 0.65 0.75 0.63 0.66 0.70 0.63 0.65 0.67 0.70 0.71 0.82 0.67 0.83 0.80 0.76 0.85 0.79 0.78 0.75 0.75 0.51 0.53 0.48 0.65 0.53 0.56 0.63 0.63 0.56 0.66 0.68 0.71 0.66 0.75 0.68 0.74 0.78 0.78 0.73 0.80 0.91 0.93 0.91 0.97 0.96 0.96 0.95 0.97 0.96 0.97 0.79 0.55 0.79 0.77 0.72 0.58 0.54 0.83 0.78 0.58 0.72 0.64 0.74 0.78 0.78 0.54 0.75 0.72 0.61 0.83 12 22 24 19 12 21 19 17 " rwg = the proportional reduction in error variance relative to a discrete uniform distribution. b Average SD = the arithmetic mean of the standard deviations. c Average interrater r = the average of the correlations between experts' ratings in which experts are treated as variables and facets as cases. d Average corrected item-total r = the average of the correlations between each rater's profile and the composite profile computed without that rating. e a for composite = coefficient alpha for the composite prototype in which experts are treated as variables and facets as cases; it does depend, in part, on the number of raters. f This is the correlation with the prototypes taken from Widiger et al. (1994). These prototypes were created by assigning a score, ranging from 4 (high) to —4 (low) for each facet according to the following: a 4 or —4 for facets that are high or low according to DSM-HI-R criteria; a 3 or - 3 for facets that are high or low on the basis of clinical literature; a 2 or - 2 for facets that are high or low on the basis of associated features in the DSM-IH-R; a 1 or -1 for facets that are somewhat high or low on the basis of the clinical literature; and a 0 for facets that are neither high nor low. g This is the correlation of the consensus prototype with the correlations between the five-factor model of personality and the measures of personality disorder obtained by Dyce and O'Connor (1998) in a sample of undergraduates. Agreement Among Experts Table 2 presents several measures of agreement among our experts. The second column provides information on the average within-group agreement, rwg, for the FFM facets for each personality disorder. This coefficient has been offered as a means of "assessing agreement among the judgements made by a single group of judges on a single variable in regard to a single target" (James, Demaree, & Wolf, 1984, as cited in James, Demaree, & Wolf, 1993) and represents the proportional reduction in error variance relative to a random process. On average, agreement at the facet level appeared fairly high; in all cases, the expert ratings achieved at least a 60% reduction in error. The third column of Table 2 provides information on agreement at the facet level through examination of the standard deviations of expert ratings. In general, the average standard deviations were good: all were below 0.90. For most disorders, fewer than 20% of the facets achieved standard deviations greater than one; this compares favorably to the 17% of facets with standard deviations greater than one in the Miller et al. (2001) study.1 According to these criteria (i.e., high rwg and low SD), agreement was best for schizoid, dependent, and compulsive PDs and worst for schizotypal and histrionic PDs. The next three columns of Table 2 provide information about agreement among raters across the entire profile. The fourth column provides the average interrater correlation (i.e., the average correlation of one rater's profile with every other rater's profile) for each personality disorder. These average interrater correlations range from a low of .48 for schizotypal personality disorder to a high of .66 for obsessive-compulsive personality disorder; all of these correlations are somewhat lower than the average of .75 reported by Miller et al. (2001). The fifth column provides the average correlation between an individual expert's rating and the composite made without his or her rating contributing to it; as such, it offers information on how well individual ratings agree with the overall composite. These average, corrected item-total correlations are all relatively high, ranging from a low of .66 for schizotypal personality disorder to a high of .80 for obsessivecompulsive personality disorder. The sixth column provides information on the reliability of the composite through calculation of coefficient alpha.2 These reliability estimates are all relatively high, ranging from .91 for paranoid and schizotypal personality disorders to .97 for antisocial, avoidant, and obsessivecompulsive personality disorders. All compare favorably to the .98 found for the psychopathy ratings. According to these three indices, agreement was best for compulsive, antisocial, and avoidant PDs and worst for schizotypal, schizoid, and paranoid PDs. Agreement With Hypothesized FFM-PD Relations (Widiger et al., 1994) The seventh column in Table 2 provides information on the convergence between the present expert-generated prototypes and a quantification of the hypotheses put forth by Widiger et al. (1994). The FFM descriptions of the personality disorders by Widiger et al. were hierarchical according to whether they concerned core features or associated features and whether they were based on the DSM-HI-R or the clinical literature. For the purposes of this study, hypotheses based on the diagnostic criteria were 1 The Miller et al. (2001) study on psychopathy provides a fairly good upper-bound comparison for the present results. We believe the Miller et al. study serves as an upper bound because of its concentration on psychopathy, which is (a) a disorder described predominantly in terms of personality traits rather than behaviors, which serve as the bases for many of the DSM criteria; and (b) studied by a relatively small, homogeneous group of investigators. Both of these factors can be expected to contribute to high agreement among raters. 2 For this analysis, raters served as variables and facets served as cases. 406 LYNAM AND WIDIGER Table 3 Examples of Hypothetical (Widiger et al, 1994) and Empirical (Dyce & O'Connor, 1998) FFM-PD Profiles Widiger et al.a Domain and facet Neuroticism Anxiousness Angry hostility Depressiveness Self-consciousness Impulsiveness Vulnerability Extraversion Warmth Gregariousness Assertiveness Activity Excitement seeking Positive emotions Openness Fantasy Aesthetics Feelings Actions Ideas Values Agreeableness Trust Straightforwardness Altruism Compliance Modesty Tendermindedness Conscientiousness Competence Order Dutifulness Achievement striving Self-discipline Deliberation Paranoid Dyce & O'Connorb Narcissistic 2 0 4 0 0 4 0 0 -2 4 0 Histrionic Compulsive -.29 -.11 -.32 -.36 .00 -.14 -.13 .01 -.30 -.15 .04 3 -.23 0 0 3 0 0 0 .48 .42 .43 .39 .36 0 4 ••y 0 0 0 0 0 .12 .11 .21 .19 .12 -2 -2 0 0 0 2 -2 -3 0 0 .42 -.01 -4 0 .18 -4 0 -4 -2 -2 -.11 ^ 2 0 0 0 0 0 .04 -.10 .00 .06 -.13 .02 -.28 .01 -.04 -.12 -.01 -.14 -4 .16 .12 .18 .23 0 -4 -4 -.05 .19 -.23 .06 .09 .11 .21 .38 .48 .45 .43 .51 .43 2 0 0 1 0 0 -.04 .02 .17 .14 -.14 Note. FFM = five-factor model of personality; PD = measures of personality disorder. a Numbers in these columns represent quantification of qualitative hypotheses regarding relations between FFM and PD put forth by Widiger et al. (1994). b The data in columns 3 and 4 are from Table 2 of "Personality Disorders and the Five-Factor Model: A Test of Facet-Level Predictions," by J. A. Dyce and B. P. O'Connor, 1998, Journal of Personality Disorders, 12, pp. 37-38. Copyright 1998 by Guilford Press. Reprinted with permission. Numbers in these columns represent empirical correlations between FFM and PD symptoms reported by Dyce and O'Connor (1998). given a rating of 4 or —4 (a high or low rating on a facet, respectively); hypotheses based on the core features in the clinical literature were given a 3 or —3; hypotheses based on associated features in the DSM-III-R were given a rating of 2 or -2; and hypotheses based on associated features in the clinical literature were given scores of 1 or — 1. A score of 0 was assigned if Widiger et al. provided no hypotheses for the facet in question.3 Table 3 provides examples of this quantification; the full quantification is available on request. To obtain the degree of convergence, each expert prototype was correlated with the respective Widiger et al. prototype in an analysis in which facets were treated as cases. It is evident from Table 2 that there was considerable convergence between our expert prototype ratings and the Widiger et al. (1994) hypotheses for 6 of the 10 personality disorders: paranoid, schizotypal, antisocial, borderline, avoidant, and dependent per- sonality disorders. For these disorders, the convergent validity correlations ranged from .77 (antisocial) to .83 (avoidant).4 These convergent validity coefficients for the DSM-TV personality disorders compare favorably with the .64 correlation obtained by Miller et al. (2001) for the FFM description of psychopathy. 3 Results change very little under alternative quantification schemes (e.g., including only facets based on DSM criteria or associated features, making no distinction between DSM criteria and clinical literature, or reversing the coding of clinical core features and DSM associated features). 4 Because these correlations are used descriptively rather than inferentially to index the convergence between our profiles and the Widiger et al. profiles, no statistical significance tests were performed. FFM PD PROTOTYPES Lower but still relatively large convergent relations were also obtained for the remaining four personality disorders: schizoid, histrionic, narcissistic, and obsessive-compulsive. For these disorders, correlations ranged from .54 (narcissistic) to .58 (obsessive-compulsive). Comparisons of the expert and hypothetical profiles provide information on the divergences that contributed to these relatively lower convergent validity coefficients. For example, both the schizoid PD researchers and Widiger et al. (1994) characterized the schizoid PD as being very low in gregariousness, positive emotionality, excitement seeking, warmth, and openness to feelings. However, Widiger et al. also described the schizoid PD as being low in selfconsciousness and high in hostility, whereas the schizoid expert consensus prototype was neither high nor low in any facets of neuroticism. In addition, the consensus prototype included low scores for schizoid PD on the two additional facets of extraversion (i.e., assertiveness and activity) and openness to actions. The experts and Widiger et al. (1994) characterized the histrionic personality disorder as involving facets of extraversion, including gregariousness, excitement seeking, activity, and positive emotionality; facets of openness to fantasy, feelings, and actions; and low self-discipline, a facet of conscientiousness. The divergence appeared to be driven by extreme differences in the ratings of self-consciousness (rated high by Widiger et al., 1994, but low by the experts); the characterization by Widiger et al. (1994) of histrionic PD as high in hostility; and by the inclusion in the expert consensus prototype of two additional facets representing impulsivity that were not included by Widiger et al. (1994): high impulsiveness and low deliberation. The greatest disagreement occurred for the narcissistic personality disorder. There was agreement with respect to several facets of antagonism, particularly low altruism, low modesty, and low tendermindedness, as well as the neuroticism facet of angry hostility and the extraversion facet of assertiveness. Similar to the case for histrionic PD, Widiger et al. (1994) characterized the narcissistic personality disorder as being very high in self-consciousness and vulnerability, whereas the expert raters described the prototypic narcissist as being very low in self-consciousness and did not believe that vulnerability was particularly defining. The expert raters also described the narcissistic individual as being very low in all facets of agreeableness (including trust, straightforwardness, and compliance), two facets of extraversion (warmth and excitement seeking), and openness to feelings, but high in openness to actions. The expert raters and Widiger et al. (1994) also disagreed in some respects in their descriptions of the obsessive-compulsive PD. Both descriptions included high scores on several facets of conscientiousness and low scores on warmth, openness to feelings, and actions. However, the prototype derived from obsessive-compulsive PD experts also included additional facets related to the extreme control of impulses (i.e., low scores on impulsiveness and excitement seeking, and high scores on self-discipline), low scores on openness to ideas and values, and high scores on anxiousness and competence. In addition, Widiger et al. (1994), but not the experts, indicated that obsessivecompulsive PD was characterized by low scores on two facets of agreeableness (altruism and compliance) and high scores on assertiveness. 407 Agreement With Empirical FFM-PD Relations (Dyce & O'Connor, 1998) The final column of Table 2 provides the convergence between the present expert-generated prototypes and an empirically derived profile taken from a study by Dyce and O'Connor (1998) in which scores on the NEO-PI-R were correlated with PD symptomology assessed with the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory—III (MilIon, Millon, & Davis, 1994). In this analysis, facets were again treated as cases and the FFM profiles were correlated with the correlations between the NEO-PI-R facets and personality disorder symptoms taken from Dyce and O'Connor (1998); Table 3 provides examples of the empirical FFM-PD profiles taken from Dyce and O'Connor. As can be seen in Table 2, convergence was again quite good. Correlations were above .70 for seven of the disorders: compulsive, antisocial, borderline, narcissistic, schizotypal, paranoid, and avoidant; agreement was not as strong for the remaining three, especially for histrionic PD. Five of the disorders that converged well with the Widiger et al. (1994) profiles also converged well with the Dyce and O'Connor profiles (paranoid, schizotypal, antisocial, borderline, and avoidant), whereas two converged moderately for both (schizoid and histrionic). As before, a comparison of the expert and empirical profiles provided information on the nature of the divergences. For schizoid PD, both sources (the experts and the empirical relations) agreed that this PD was characterized by low levels of all facets of extraversion and two facets of openness (feelings and actions). The sources diverged in that the empirical profile, but not the experts, indicated that schizoid PD was also characterized as high in two facets of neuroticism (depression and self-consciousness) and low in trust. Expert and empirical descriptions agreed in the characterization of histrionic PD as high in all facets of extraversion, two facets of openness (feelings and actions), and trust and as low in selfconsciousness and deliberation. The sources diverged in that experts, but not the empirical profile, characterized histrionic PD as high in impulsiveness and low in self-discipline. Conversely, the empirical profile, but not the experts, suggested that histrionic PD is characterized by low scores on additional facets of neuroticism (anxiety, depression, and vulnerability). Finally, there was agreement across sources in the characterization of dependent PD as high in most facets of neuroticism (except for angry hostility and impulsiveness) and as low in assertiveness. The divergence appears to be due to dependent PD being characterized as high in three facets of agreeableness (trust, compliance, and modesty) by the experts but not by the empirical profile. In addition, the empirical profile, but not the expert raters, suggested that dependent PD is characterized as low in competence. Using the FFM Prototypes to Reproduce DSM Comorbidities To examine how well the FFM model could reproduce the comorbidities among the personality disorders, we first correlated the FFM prototypes with one another to generate a matrix representing the conceptual overlap among the prototypes; this was accomplished through an analysis that treated facets as cases and treated different PDs as variables. Results are provided in Table 4. 408 LYNAM AND WIDIGER Table 4 Correlations Among Five-Factor Model Expert Prototypes 10 1. Paranoid 2. Schizoid 3. Schizotypal 4. Antisocial 5. Borderline 6. Histrionic 7. Narcissistic 8. Avoidant 9. Dependent 10. Compulsive .51 .48 .08 .27 -.36 .29 .41 -.23 .53 .64 -.49 •JC ,L~J -.69 -.32 .72 .33 .50 -.17 .34 -.20 -.15 .66 .28 .01 — .63 .67 .80 -.67 -.74 -.52 — .56 .39 -.03 -.18 -.49 — .51 -.57 -.28 -.75 — -.56 -.81 -.16 — .69 .43 — .12 — Note. These correlations were obtained through an analysis that treated facets as cases and personality disorders as variables. Correlations ranged from —.81 between dependent PD and narcissistic PD to .80 between antisocial PD and narcissistic PD.5 Next, we assessed the congruence between this matrix and two previously generated comorbidity matrices aggregated across multiple studies. This examination was accomplished by correlating the comorbidity estimates for a given PD taken from one matrix with those estimates taken from another matrix. For example, the numbers in the first column of Table 4 representing the overlap between the FFM paranoid profile and the other FFM profiles were correlated with comorbidity estimates for paranoid PD taken from one of the other comorbidity matrices to give an estimate of the congruence across matrices. To the extent that the .matrix generated by the expert consensus prototypes mirrors the observed comorbidities (expressed as high correlations between the estimates), evidence is generated for the FFM account. Matrices aggregated across multiple studies were used because of the low agreement between comorbidity estimates taken from any two single studies (Oldham et al., 1992).6 The first aggregate matrix of comorbidities was a correlation matrix reported previously by Widiger et al. (1991), who aggregated the covariation among the DSM-II1 personality disorder criterion sets provided in nine previously published studies. The second matrix came out of work by the DSM-IV Personality Disorders Work Group, who obtained data on the criterion sets for all of the DSM-IU-R personality disorders from six independent research sites. The co-occurrence rates among the DSM-III-R personality disorders were then aggregated across the data sets and reported by Widiger and Trull (1998) in the fourth volume of the DSM-IV Sourcebook. For each disorder, its association with the other 9 disorders in a given data set (FFM, aggregation of the 9 previously published DSM-HI studies, and the DSM-IV MacArthur comorbidities) was correlated with its degree of association with the other 9 disorders in each of the other two data sets. This correlation provides an index of how well the various comorbidity estimates for a given disorder compare across the data sets; the higher the correlation, the greater the congruence. Table 5 provides these results. The first two columns provide the correlations between the FFM prototype matrix and the comorbidities from the 9 previously published DSM-III studies and the 6 data sets from the DSM-IV MacArthur data. The third column provides the correlation between the two aggregated DSM comorbidity matrices. The convergences between the overlap from the FFM profiles and the DSM-III comorbidities ranged from a low of .58 for dependent PD to a high of .95 for narcissistic PD. Convergence was highest for antisocial and narcissistic PDs, with correlations greater than .90; five additional correlations were greater than .70 (schizoid, schizotypal, borderline, histrionic, and avoidant). Three disorders showed weaker convergence (paranoid, dependent, and compulsive), with correlations less than .70. Over half of these correlations equal or exceed those obtained from a comparison of the two aggregated matrices, which provides an important benchmark.7 The congruence between the association of FFM profiles and the comorbidity estimates from the DSM-IV MacArthur studies was generally less than what was observed for the DSM-III Comorbidities. These correlations ranged from a low of .09 for paranoid PD to a high of .89 for schizotypal PD. The three highest convergences were found for schizotypal, antisocial, and narcissistic PDs, with all correlations greater than .75. Somewhat lower convergence was found for schizoid, histrionic, avoidant, and compulsive PDs, with correlations ranging from .60 to .70. Finally, frankly poor convergence was obtained for dependent and paranoid PDs. Almost half of all correlations were as high or higher than the convergences obtained for the two aggregated matrices. Discussion This study sought to extend the five-factor model understanding of personality disorders as configurations of extreme scores on common dimensions of personality. First, it extended understand5 Note that negative correlations are expected from the FFM account, although they are infrequently observed in empirical comorbidity estimates and are actually impossible with co-occurrence data. We believe these differences are due to the fact the FFM allows for individuals to score at either pole of a trait but PD symptoms assess only one pole. For example, individuals can be either suspicious or gullible on the trust facet of agreeableness but only suspicious or not in the paranoid criteria. 6 For example, using data from a study of Oldham et al. (1992) that assessed the same patieats with two different structured interviews, we found that the correlations between the co-occurrences for a given disorder calculated from each interview ranged from .07 to .86 with a mean of .51. 7 Again, because these correlations are used descriptively rather than inferentially, no statistical significance tests were performed. 409 FFM PD PROTOTYPES Table 5 Similarity Among Comorbidity of FFM Prototype and Two Other DSM Comorbidity Matrices by Personality Disorder FFMa and nine DSM-HI studies6 FFM and DSM-IV MacArthur Project0 DSM-IV MacArthur Project and nine DSM-HI studies Paranoid Schizoid Schizotypal Antisocial Borderline Histrionic Narcissistic Avoidant Dependent Compulsive .60 .75 .09 .70 .06 .72 .83 .92 .89 .87 .70 .92 .75 .84 .95 .84 .50 .58 .65 .76 .63 .33 .60 .80 .85 .89 .84 AH personality disorders .76 .60 Personality disorder .68 .90 .84 .78 Note. FFM = five-factor model; DSM-111 and DSM-IV = Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd & 4th eds.; American Psychiatric Association, 1987, 1994). The numbers represent correlations between Comorbidity rates for each disorder taken from different sources. a "Comorbidities" for the FFM represent correlations among expert prototypes. b Comorbidities represent correlations among personality disorders averaged across nine studies reported in Widiger et al. (1991). c Comorbidities represent co-occurrence rates among personality disorders from the DSM-IV MacArthur Project reported in Widiger and Trull (1998). ing by developing comprehensive FFM descriptions of the personality disorders provided by published researchers. Agreement among the expert raters for almost all of the DSM-IV PDs was quite good; standard deviations were low, proportional reductions in error variance were high, and average interrater correlations, average corrected item-total correlations, and composite coefficient alphas were all high. Agreement is especially impressive in light of the fact that experts were rating prototypic cases from their own diverse experiences and not the same individual case. These results suggest that most of the DSM-IV PDs can be readily described, and usually with a high level of agreement, in terms of the 30 facets of the FFM. The lowest interrater and ratercomposite agreement were obtained for the schizotypal PD (.48 and .66, respectively), which has also obtained relatively weaker support as a maladaptive variant of common personality traits in studies correlating measures of the FFM to PD symptomatology (e.g., Trull, 1992). Although these results may reflect shortcomings in the FFM assessment of the cognitive-perceptual domain, they may also be due to the fact that schizotypal personality disorder might be better understood as a variant of schizophrenia rather than as a personality disorder (Siever & Davis, 1991; Widiger & Costa, 1994). The expert-generated prototypes converged very well with the FFM translations of DSM-IV symptoms provided by Widiger et al. (1994) and with empirical relations found between the FFM and personality disorders (Dyce & O'Connor, 1998). Although some of this convergence may have been due to previous familiarity with the Widiger et al. prototypes, we believe this was unlikely. Expert raters were selected on the basis of their familiarity with the personality disorders not with the FFM; they were instructed to rate prototypic cases, and there were interesting divergences that we believe were mostly due to the more comprehensive coverage of FFM facets provided by the expert consensus ratings. For example, obsessive-compulsive PD experts suggested that this personality disorder also involves a substantial anxiousness, a controlled restraint (low impulsiveness), and an excessive cautiousness (low excitement seeking) that are not recognized by the DSM-IV criteria for this disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). Although it is possible that our experts, rather than the DSM descriptions, were mistaken, our approach to aggregation requires that these mistakes or misconceptions be widely held by experts; if these misconceptions were held only by a few raters, they would have been blunted in the aggregated profile. Some disagreement also reflected points of dispute in the development of the diagnostic criteria. For example, there was considerable disagreement between the narcissistic PD researchers and the ZWM-based descriptions with respect to self-consciousness and vulnerability. Widiger et al. (1994) described the narcissistic PD as being high in self-consciousness and vulnerability, whereas the narcissistic PD researchers described the prototypic case of this disorder as being low in these traits. This disagreement parallels a difficulty the authors of the narcissistic diagnostic criterion sets have had with the apparently inconsistent response of these persons to praise, criticism, or rebuke from others (Gunderson, Ronningstam, & Smith, 1991; Ronningstam & Gunderson, 1990; Widiger, Frances, Spitzer, & Williams, 1988). Finally, we examined how well the FFM descriptions of the personality disorders could reproduce the very high Comorbidity rates reported in the literature. Although these high Comorbidities are nettlesome to the categorical approach (Clark et al., 1997; Lilienfeld et al., 1994; Livesley, 1998; Mineka et al., 1998), they are expected under a dimensional model that views the personality disorders as configurations of a finite number of basic personality dimensions. Under the FFM account, disorders appear comorbid to the extent that they are characterized by the same facets. Although our results cannot prove that this is the case, we believe that, to the extent the high rates of comorbidity among disorders can be reproduced when one begins with these trait dimensions rather 410 LYNAM AND WIDIGER than the disorders themselves, support is generated for the dimensional understanding. The conceptual overlap among FFM profiles reproduced well the covariation obtained for the schizoid, schizotypal, antisocial, borderline, histrionic, narcissistic, avoidant, and compulsive PDs aggregated across several sets of studies. The FFM predictions correlated as highly with the co-occurrence rates reported in nine previously published studies, as these studies correlated with the DSM-IV Mac Arthur co-occurrence rates for 8 of the 10 personality disorders. Poor results were obtained across both data sets for only one personality disorder, dependent PD. This may be due to the fact that the FFM profiles are constructed with bipolar descriptors in contrast to the DSM criterion sets, which assess only one pole of a dimension, resulting in much greater differentiation within the FFM profiles than within general clinical settings. This will be especially true when co-occurrence rates are used. The greatest differentiation among the personality disorders was, in fact, predicted for the dependent PD, with correlations of —.81 and — .74 with narcissistic and antisocial PDs, respectively, whereas the dependent PD was in fact reported to be somewhat comorbid with the antisocial and narcissistic PDs in most of the prior comorbidity studies (Widiger & Trull, 1998). The distinctiveness between PDs in the present study stands in contrast to results from a recent study by Morey, Gunderson, Quigley, and Lyons (2000), who concluded that the FFM was unsuccessful in providing much differentiation among the personality disorders because the FFM profile they obtained was essentially the same for each personality disorder. However, the primary reasons for the failure of Morey et al. to obtain adequate differentiation was the inadequate differentiation among the disorders themselves and their use of the NEO-FFI (Costa & McCrae, 1992), which only assesses the five domains of the FFM. In their study, each of the 11 DSM-III-R PDs met, on average, the diagnostic criteria for 3 other personality disorders; persons who met the histrionic and narcissistic criteria met the criteria for 5 other personality disorders on average. Similar FFM profiles will be obtained for different personality disorders if there is substantial co-occurrence among them because the same individuals will contribute to the ratings of each disorder. Moreover, more precise differentiation between disorders can be obtained at the level of the 30 facets (Axelrod et al., 1997). Several researchers (e.g., Clark et al., 1997; Widiger & Frances, 1994) have previously argued for the superiority of the dimensional approach; we believe the present results underscore this conclusion. The dimensional approach avoids dichotomous blackwhite decisions and the cognitive biases that come with such decisions (Cantor & Genero, 1986). Under the FFM account, an individual is described by using his or her standing on the 30 facets of the NEO-PI-R (which provides a very detailed and specific description using all available information) or by their degree of approximation to a prototype. Similarly, personality disorders are understood as constellations of basic traits and described by the relative standings of the prototypic case on all 30 facets. Information on all facets is preserved, and prototypic cases of different disorders may diverge in degree for a given facet. In addition, as noted previously, a dimensional model anticipates high rates of comorbidity among disorders; present results suggest that shared dimensions across disorders is sufficient to account for the high comorbidity. Finally, and often underappreciated, a dimensional model of personality ties applied research into a corpus of knowledge derived from basic research on personality. That is, to the extent that personality disorders are understood from a dimensional perspective, basic research in personality can be easily brought to bear on personality disorders. For example, research on the structure (John & Srivastava, 1999; Watson, Clark, & Harkness, 1994), genetics (Bouchard, Lykken, McGue, Segal, & Tellegen, 1990; Rutter et al., 1997), neurobiology (Depue, 1996; Zuckerman, 1994), and development (Caspi, 1997; Scarr & McCartney, 1983) of personality can be applied to research on personality disorders to extend the understanding of mechanisms and treatment and to generate theory. In sum, the current results provide support for the specific FFM account and the more general dimensional approach to understanding the personality disorders. Personality disorders do appear to involve constellations of maladaptive variants of the personality traits included within the more general FFM of personality functioning. We note, however, that simply because the personality disorders can be understood from a dimensional perspective does not render them useless as descriptions of configurations of traits. Antisocial personality disorder is an important construct because the particular combination of high antagonism; low deliberation, dutifulness, and self-discipline; low anxiety and self-consciousness; and high impulsiveness, excitement seeking, and angry hostility is so virulent (Widiger & Lynam, 1998). Where does one go from here? Up to this point, we have demonstrated that (a) the FFM can be used to describe DSM personality disorders, (b) experts can agree in their descriptions, (c) various approaches to this description agree with one another, and (d) the FFM can incorporate a finding with which the current categorical approach has difficulty—excessive comorbidity among the personality disorders. Next steps will involve further demonstrating the power of this approach by examining its ability to reproduce the nomological network surrounding the DSM personality disorders. For example, it is possible to ask whether personality disorders "diagnosed" using the present FFM prototypes behave similarly in terms of interpersonal, social, and occupational functioning as do traditional PD diagnoses. The prototypes generated here can be matched against an individual's NEOPI-R profile to yield a set of similarity scores. The more similar an individual is to a given prototype, the more he or she could be said to have that particular personality disorder. For example, in the Miller et al. (2001) study on psychopathy, we used an intraclass correlation coefficient to assess the degree of similarity between the FFM profile of the prototypic psychopath and an individual's NEO-PI-R FFM profile.8 This similarity index was then used as an index of psychopathy. We found that individuals who more closely approximated the prototype had higher scores on a psychopathy inventory; more symptoms of antisocial personality disorder, substance abuse, and substance dependence; more frequent and varied antisocial activities; and fewer internalizing symptoms. All of these relations replicated those in previous studies using incarcerated, PCL-R (Hare, 1991) defined psychopaths. When the FFM 8 The intraclass correlation was used because it takes into account not only similarity in shape but also similarity in height of the profiles. It is calculated as a double-entry correlation in which facets serve as participants and participants serve as variables. FFM PD PROTOTYPES profiles are shown to reproduce the nomological networks surrounding the existing diagnostic categories, then a strong argument might be made for replacing these diagnostic categories with the FFM. References American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Axelrod, S. R., Widiger, T. A., Trull, T. J., & Corbitt, E. M. (1997). Relation of five-factor model antagonism facets with personality disorder symptomatology. Journal of Personality Assessment, 69, 297-313. Benjamin, L. S. (1993). Interpersonal diagnosis and treatment of personality disorders. New York: Guilford Press. Block, J. (1995). A contrarian view of the five-factor approach to personality description. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 187-215. Bornstein, R. F. (1992). The dependent personality: Developmental, social, and clinical perspectives. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 3-23. Bouchard, T., Lykken, D., McGue, M., Segal, N., & Tellegen, A. (1990). Sources of human psychological differences: The Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart. Science, 250, 223-228. Cantor, N., & Genero, N. (1986). Psychiatric diagnosis and natural categorization: A close analogy. In T. Millon & G. Klerman (Eds.), Contemporary directions in psychopathology (pp. 233-256). New York: Guilford Press. Caspi, A. (1997). Personality development across the life course. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. 311-388). New York: Wiley. Clark, L. A. (1993). Manual for the Schedule for Nonadaptive and Adaptive Personality (SNAP). Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press. Clark, L. A., Livesley, W. J., & Morey, L. (1997). Personality disorder assessment: The challenge of construct validity. Journal of Personality Disorders, 11, 205-231. Clarkin, J. F., Hull, J. W., Cantor, J., & Sanderson, C. (1993). Borderline personality disorder and personality traits: A comparison of SCID-1I BPD and NEO-PI. Psychological Assessment, 5, 472-476. Clarkin, J. F., & Lenzenweger, M. F. (1996). Major theories of personality disorder. New York: Guilford Press. Clarkin, J. F., Marziali, E., & Munroe-Blum, H. (1992). Borderline personality disorder: Clinical and empirical perspectives. New York: Guilford Press. Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M., Bayon, C., & Przybeck, T. R. (1999). Measurement of psychopathology as variants of personality. In C. R. Cloninger (Ed.), Personality and psychopathology (pp. 33-65). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1988). Personality in adulthood: A six-year longitudinal study of self-reports and spouse ratings on the NEO Personality Inventory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 853-863. Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-P1-K) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFl) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1994). Set like plaster? Evidence for the stability of adult personality. In T. Heatherton & J. L. Weinberger (Eds.), Can personality change? (pp. 21-40). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Costa, P., & McCrae, R. (1995). Domains and facets: Hierarchical personality assessment using the Revised NEO Personality Inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment, 64, 21-50. Depue, R. A. (1996). A neurobiological framework for the structure of 411 personality and emotion: Implications for personality disorders. In J. F. Clarkin & M. F. Lenzenweger (Eds.), Major theories of personality disorder (pp. 347-390). New York: Guilford Press. De Raad, B., Perugini, M., Hrebickova, M., & Szarota, P. (1998). Lingua franca of personality: Taxonomies and structures based on the psycholexical approach. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 29, 212— 232. Digman, J. M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model. Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 417-440. Dyce, J. A., & O'Connor, B. P. (1998). Personality disorders and the five-factor model: A test of facet-level predictions. Journal of Personality Disorders, 12, 31-45. Goldberg, L. R. (1992). The development of markers of the Big Five factor structure. Psychological Assessment, 4, 26-42. Gosling, S. D., & John, O. P. (1999). Personality dimensions in nonhuman animals: A cross-species review. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 69-75. Gunderson, J. G. (1992). Diagnostic controversies. In A. Tasman & M. B. Riba (Eds.), Review of psychiatry (Vol. 11, pp. 9-24). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Gunderson, J. G., Links, P. S., & Reich, J. H. (1991). Competing models of personality disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 5, 60-68. Gunderson, J. G., Ronningstam, E., & Smith, L. (1991). Narcissistic personality disorder: A review of data on DSM—llI-R descriptions. Journal of Personality Disorders, 5, 167—177. Halverson, C. F., Kohnstamm, G. A., & Martin, R. P. (Eds.). (1994). The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Hare, R. D. (1991). The Revised Psychopathy Checklist. Toronto, Canada: Multi-Health Systems. James, L. R., Demaree, R. G., & Wolf, G. (1993). rwg: An assessment of within-group interrater agreement. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 306-309. Jang, K. L., McCrae, R. R., Angleitner, A., Riemann, R., & Livesley, W. J. (1998). Heritability of facet-level traits in a cross-cultural twin sample: Support for a hierarchical model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1556-1565. John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 102-138). New York: Guilford Press. Lilienfeld, S. O. (1994). Conceptual problems in the assessment of psychopathy. Clinical Psychology Review, 14, 17-38. Lilienfeld, S. O., Waldman, I. D., & Israel, A. C. (1994). A critical examination of the use of the term and concept of comorbidity in psychopathology research. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 1, 71-83. Livesley, W. J. (1998). Suggestions for a framework for an empirically based classification of personality disorder. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 43, 137-147. Livesley, W. J. (Ed.). (2001). Handbook of personality disorders: Theory, research, and treatment. New York: Guilford Press. Lykken, D. T. (1995). The antisocial personalities. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Lynam, D. R. (in press). Psychopathy from the perspective of the fivefactor model. In P. T. Costa Jr. & T. A. Widiger (Eds.), Personality disorders and the five-factor model of personality (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1990). Personality in adulthood. New York: Guilford Press. Miller, J. D., Lynam, D. R., Widiger, T. A., & Leukefeld, C. (2001). Personality disorders as extreme variants of common personality dimensions: Can the five-factor model adequately represent psychopathy? Journal of Personality, 69, 253-276. 412 LYNAM AND WIDIGER Millon, T., Millon, C., & Davis, R. (1994). MCM1-1I1 manual. Minneapolis, MN: National Computer Systems. Mineka, S., Watson, D., & Clark, L. E. A. (1998). Comorbidity of anxiety and unipolar mood disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 377412. Morey, L. C., Gunderson, J., Quigley, B. D., & Lyons, M. (2000). Dimensions and categories: The "Big Five" factors and the DSM personality disorders. Assessment, 7, 203-216. O'Connor, B. P., & Dyce, J. A. (1998). A test of models of personality disorder configuration. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 3-16. Oldham, J. M., Skodol, A. E., Kellman, H. D., Hyler, S. E., Rosnick, L., & Davies, M. (1992). Diagnosis of DSM-1II—R personality disorders by two semistructured interviews: Patterns of comorbidity. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 213-220. Plomin, R., & Caspi, A. (1999). Behavioral genetics and personality. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 251-276). New York: Guilford Press. Raine, A., Lencz, T., & Mednick, S. (Eds.). (1995). Schizotypalpersonality disorder. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Ronningstam, E. (Ed.). (1998). Disorders of narcissism: Diagnostic, clinical and empirical implications. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Ronningstam, E., & Gunderson, J. G. (1990). Identifying criteria for narcissistic personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 585-601. Rutter, M., Dunn, J., Plomin, R., Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Maughan, B., Ormel, J., Meyer, J., & Eaves, L. (1997). Integrating nature and nurture: Implications of person-environment correlations and interactions for developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 335-364. Saucier, G. (1994). Mini-markers: A brief version of Goldberg's unipolar Big Five markers. Journal of Personality Assessment, 63, 506-516. Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: A theory of genotype —> environment effects. Child Development, 54, 424-435. Siever, L. J., & Davis, K. L. (1991). A psychobiological perspective on the personality disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 1647-1658. Skodol, A. E., Rosnick, L., Kellman, H. D., Oldham, J. M., & Hyler, S. E. (1991). Development of a procedure for validating structured assessments of Axis II. In J. Oldham (Ed.), Personality disorders: New perspectives on diagnostic validity (pp. 41-70). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Stoff, D., Breiling, J., & Maser, J. D. (Eds.). (1997). Handbook of antisocial behavior. New York: Wiley. Trull, T. J. (1992). DSM-III-R personality disorders and the five-factor model of personality: An empirical comparison. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 553-560. Trull, T. J., Widiger, T. A., & Burr, R. (2001). A structured interview for the assessment of the five-factor model of personality: 2. Facet-level relations to the Axis II personality disorders. Journal of Personality, 69, 175-198. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Harkness, A. R. (1994). Structures of personality and their revelance to psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 18-31. Westen, D. (1995). A clinical-empirical model of personality. Life after the Mischelian ice age and the NEO-Iithic era. Journal of Personality, 63, 495-524. Widiger, T. A. (1993). The DSM-III-R categorical personality disorder diagnoses: A critique and an alternative. Psychological Inquiry, 4, 7590. Widiger, T. A., & Costa, P. T. (1994). Personality and personality disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 78-91. Widiger, T. A., & Costa, P. T. Jr. (in press). Five-factor model personality disorder research. In P. T. Costa Jr. & T. A. Widiger (Eds.), Personality disorders and the five-factor model of personality (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Widiger, T. A., & Frances, A. J. (1994). Toward a dimensional model for the personality disorders. In P. Costa Jr. & T. Widiger (Eds.), Personality disorders and the five-factor model of personality (pp. 19-39). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Widiger, T., Frances, A., Harris, M., Jacobsberg, L., Fyer, M., & Manning, D. (1991). Comorbidity among Axis II disorders. In J. Oldham (Ed.), Axis II: New perspectives on validity (pp. 163-194). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Widiger, T. A., Frances, A. J., Spitzer, R. L., & Williams, J. B. W. (1988). The DSM-III-R personality disorders: An overview. American Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 786-795. Widiger, T. A., & Lynam, D. R. (1998). Psychopathy from the perspective of the five-factor model of personality. In T. Millon, E. Simonsen, M. Birket-Smith, & R. D. Davis (Eds.), Psychopathy: Antisocial, criminal, and violent behaviors (pp. 171-187). New York: Guilford Press. Widiger, T. A., & Trull, T. J. (1998). Performance characteristics of the DSM-IH-R personality disorder criteria sets. In T. A. Widiger, A. J. Frances, H. A. Pincus, R. Ross, M. B. First, W. Davis, & M. Kline (Eds.), DSM-IV sourcebook (Vol. 4, pp. 357-373). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Widiger, T. A., Trull, T. J., Clarkin, J. F., Sanderson, C., & Costa, P. T. Jr. (1994). A description of the DSM-III-R and DSM-IV personality disorders with the five-factor model of personality. In P. T. Costa Jr. & T. A. Widiger (Eds.), Personality disorders and the five-factor model of personality (pp. 41-56). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Wiggins, J. S., & Pincus, A. L. (1992). Personality: Structure and assessment. Annual Review of Psychology, 43, 473—504. Zuckerman, M. (1994). Behavioral expressions and biosocial bases of sensation seeking. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Received August 24, 2000 Revision received January 10, 2001 Accepted February 25, 2001