* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download EFFECTS OF HABITUATION TO AN UNCONDITIONED STIMULUS
Learning theory (education) wikipedia , lookup
Self-affirmation wikipedia , lookup
Behaviorism wikipedia , lookup
Confirmation bias wikipedia , lookup
Vladimir J. Konečni wikipedia , lookup
Psychological behaviorism wikipedia , lookup
Eyeblink conditioning wikipedia , lookup
Psychophysics wikipedia , lookup
1 Running head: EFFECTS OF HABITUATION TO AN UNCONDITIONED STIMULUS Effects of Habituation to an Unconditioned Stimulus Jessica Ivison Algoma University EFFECTS OF HABITUATION TO AN UNCONDITIONED STIMULUS 2 Abstract Psychologists have long argued over whether classical conditioning involves a stimulus-stimulus (SS) connection or a stimulus-response (SR) connection. In 1973 Rescorla found a SS connection in rats. The present study examines whether classical conditioning in humans involves a SS or a SR connection. Using a computer, participants learned that a flash of light signaled that a loud noise was about to occur. After training, half of the participants were presented with the noise alone (to break that noise + response connection; habituation). Finally, all participants were presented with just the flash of light. If a SS connection exists, participants that were habituated to the noise should not show a startle response following presentation of the flash of light (as the noise + response connection has been broken). Neither group showed a change in responding, suggesting that conditioning may not have occurred in the first phase. Further research could be directed towards using a more aversive stimulus and forming a stronger level of initial conditioning. Keywords: classical conditioning, habituation, suppression, conditioned stimulus, unconditioned stimulus, unconditioned response EFFECTS OF HABITUATION TO AN UNCONDITIONED STIMULUS 3 Effects of Habituation to an Unconditioned Stimulus Classical conditioning is an association between two stimuli, quite often understood as an association between an unconditioned stimulus and a conditioned stimulus (Domjan, 2010). When a stimulus consistently produces an unlearned response it is called an unconditioned stimulus (US). Through pairing it can cause a second stimulus, a conditioned stimulus (CS), to produce that same response (Domjan, 2010). This was accidentally discovered by Ivan Pavlov (1928) while he was studying the digestive processes of dogs. A buzzer was presented (the CS) just before presenting the dog with food (the US). After repeated trials of this the dog began to salivate (the response) after hearing the buzzer, even without the presence of food (Pavlov, 1928). A question that developed from this finding is whether the learning involved in classical conditioning is due to an association between the stimulus and its response (SR), or an association between the two stimuli (SS) (Byrne, 2003). This question has caused learning theorists to debate which association is actually taking place (Byrne, 2003). Hull (1943) and Spence (1950) were two supporters of the SR approach to learning. Spence (1950) indicates that a main difference between the two learning theories is that one suggests an association between a sensory process and a motor process (the SR approach), while the other suggests an association between two sensory processes (the SS approach). He also acknowledges that it is possible for both types of learning to co-exist, but that he is sure that all types of learning are not just SS connections. One of the leading theorists of the SS approach was Tolman (Byrne, 2003). He argued that learning is more cognitive than the SR theorists were accounting for (Tolman, 1932). Tolman (1948) compares the SS theory of learning to a map and the SR theory of learning to a telephone switchboard. The SS theory is like a map because it indicates that as an individual encounters stimuli in his environment he establishes a relationship EFFECTS OF HABITUATION TO AN UNCONDITIONED STIMULUS 4 between each stimulus. Consequently, he creates a cognitive map that he can use to navigate the environment (Tolman, 1948). The SR theory is analogous to a telephone switchboard because when the individual encounters a stimulus (incoming calls), a response is elicited (outgoing calls). In simply reacting to stimuli the individual is moving through the environment as if he or she has no purpose or direction (Tolman, 1948). SS and SR learning theorists often went back and forth in this way, however neither side had evidence that could prove their theory while simultaneously disproving the other’s theory (Byrne, 2003; Spence, 1950). Rescorla (1973) designed a study to provide insight to this debate through examining the relationship between a US and a CS. Using rats as subjects Rescorla studied the effects of habituation to a US following conditioning to a CS. Before the experiment began the rats were trained to press a bar for food. The level of conditioning was measured throughout the experiment by counting the number of times each rat pressed the bar. In the first phase of this experiment the rats were conditioned to the light. All rats were placed in a Skinner box and presented with a light (the CS) followed by a noise (the US). In the second phase the rats were split into two groups and each group received a different treatment; Group E was habituated to the US through repeated presentations of the noise, Group C was placed in the Skinner box with no presentations of the noise. By the end of this phase Group E no longer startled when presented with the noise, showing that they have become habituated to the noise. In the third phase both groups were presented with the light only. This was to test each rat’s level of suppression to the CS. The suppression ratio compares the number of bar presses that occurred before the CS, spanning the same amount of time as the CS, with the number of bar presses that occurred during the CS. In Group C the suppression ratio shows the level of conditioning to the light. The less the rats press the bar during the CS, the closer the suppression ratio is to 0. A suppression ratio of 0 EFFECTS OF HABITUATION TO AN UNCONDITIONED STIMULUS 5 indicates that the rats have been conditioned to expect to the CS (Rescorla, 1973). Likewise, the more the rats press the bar during the CS, the closer the suppression ratio is to .5. A suppression ratio of .5 indicates no conditioning to the CS (Rescorla, 1973). In Group E the suppression ratio shows whether the rats are making a SS connection or a SR connection. A suppression ratio of 0 indicates that the rats are making a SR connection. A suppression ratio of .5 indicates that the rats are making a SS connection. The results of this experiment were indicative of a SS connection because Group C had a suppression ratio closer to 0, indicating that conditioning had occurred, and Group E had a suppression ratio closer to .5, indicating that a SS connection was being made by the rats because the presence of the light did not affect the rate of bar pressing (Rescorla, 1973). This conclusion was supported in similar studies by Taylor (1956) and Rescorla (1974). They found that changes to a US after conditioning altered the response produced by the CS, which indicates a connection between the CS and the US (Taylor, 1956; Rescorla, 1974). While non-humans are typically the focus of classical conditioning experiments, conditioning in humans has not been studied as thoroughly. This leaves in question how important human awareness is to learning. Lovibond and Shanks (2002) examined many different studies concerning the connection between awareness and conditioning. The general consensus was that awareness is needed for conditioning to occur (Lovibond & Shanks, 2002). However, Lovibond and Shanks (2002) also came across evidence demonstrating that awareness is not always needed for conditioning to occur. They suggest that the role of human awareness in conditioning needs to be further examined, and that an effective way to do this would be to present a CS and assess how expectant the subject is of the US (Lovibond & Shanks, 2002). The purpose of the present study is to examine the role that human awareness plays in classical EFFECTS OF HABITUATION TO AN UNCONDITIONED STIMULUS 6 conditioning, and more specifically whether classical conditioning in humans is a connection between two stimuli or between a stimulus and a response. Since classical conditioning and habituation are similar in human and non-human animals (Byrne, 2003), it was hypothesized that participants would make a SS connection. Methods Participants and Design Ten adults participated in this study. They were randomly assigned to either the habituation group or the control group. Materials A pc-based computer, a computer program, and headphones were the only materials used for this study. The computer program was created by Jon Natale, a computer science major at Algoma University, for this study. The opening screen of the computer program listed the options to input how long the program would run (in seconds), the individual times at which each flash of light would occur (in seconds), and the individual times each noise would occur (in seconds). The flash of light was made by changing the screen from a continuous black screen to a white screen for 0.5 seconds and then changing back to black. The noise was a white noise created by the program designer which lasted 0.4 seconds. The noise had a delay on it so that it would overlap with the flash by 0.2 seconds instead of occurring at the exact same time (e.g. if the flash and the noise were both set to occur at 15 seconds, the flash would occur at 15.0 seconds while the noise would occur at 15.4 seconds). Once the times were inputted into the program a start button appeared. Once the start button was pressed the program would begin recording the time of each space bar press. After the set amount of time expired a done button EFFECTS OF HABITUATION TO AN UNCONDITIONED STIMULUS 7 would appear. When the done button was pressed it would display the time of each bar press, as well as the interval of time between each bar press. Procedure There were three phases to this study. Participants were asked to wear headphones and press the space bar throughout all three phases. In phase 1 each participant received ten presentations of the flash and noise together, set at random intervals. Phase 1 lasted 10-min. At the end of phase 1 participants were given a 5-min break. In phase 2 participants were randomly assigned to one of two groups: habituation or control. The habituation group received ten presentations of the noise at random intervals. The control group received no presentations of the noise or the flash. Phase 2 lasted 10-min. At the end of phase 2 participants were given a 5-min break. In phase 3 both groups received ten presentations of the flash set at random intervals. Phase 3 lasted 10-min. Participants were verbally debriefed following completion of phase 3. Results There were 5 participants in the control group, and 5 participants in the habituation group. A suppression ratio was used to measure the difference between the number of bar presses before the CS, spanning the same amount of time as the CS, and the number of bar presses during the CS for each participant. Then the average suppression ratio was found for each group. In phase 1 both the control group and the habituation group had a suppression ratio of .5 (SD = 0). In phase 2 both groups had a suppression ratio of .5 (SD = 0). In phase 3 both groups had a suppression EFFECTS OF HABITUATION TO AN UNCONDITIONED STIMULUS 8 ratio of .5 (SD = 0). This shows there was no change in bar pressing due to condition, and that the rate of bar pressing did not change when either stimuli were presented. Discussion Previous research on the association made in classical conditioning has demonstrated that rats form a SS connection in associative learning (Rescorla, 1973; Taylor, 1956; Rescorla, 1974). Since classical conditioning and habituation work similarly in humans and non-human animals (Byrne, 2003), the present study tested the hypothesis that humans would also make a SS connection in classical conditioning. The results of this study indicated that participants may not have been conditioned to the light because the number of bar presses did not change for either group when the light was presented. Therefore, it cannot be determined whether participants were making a SS or a SR connection. In order to identify which connection participants were making there must first be evidence that the participants had been conditioned to the light; i.e. there needs to be evidence that participants had learned that the light predicts the noise. There was no evidence that participants had learned this because if they had the control group would have had a different rate of bar pressing during the CS than before the CS. The results of this study do not confirm or deny the hypothesis. The lack of conditioning may have occurred due to phase 1 being too short to establish an association between the noise and the light. Participants were conditioned to the light for ten minutes with ten presentations of the light and noise together. This may not have been enough time or enough pairings of the stimuli to establish a strong level of classical conditioning. Rescorla (1973) presented the rats with the noise-light pairing over two sessions in two days. EFFECTS OF HABITUATION TO AN UNCONDITIONED STIMULUS 9 Perhaps increasing the length of phase 1 or having multiple sessions of phase 1 is needed to establish conditioning to the light. Alternatively, the noise the participants heard may not have been aversive enough to startle participants and cause them to change their rate of bar pressing. If this were true, participants may have learned that the light predicts the noise, however since the noise did not cause them to startle their learning could not be observed because there was no change in bar pressing when the light was presented. Consequently, even if learning did occur there is no way to tell from the data collected in this study if participants had been conditioned. Similarly, the participants may have been expecting a sound to come through the headphones due to the knowledge that it is common for sound to come through headphones. Expecting a sound to occur most likely made the sound less startling to the participants. A sound that is more unexpected may be needed to produce the startle response from the participants so that there is a way to measure their learning. In future studies, a longer period of conditioning to the light with more pairings of the two stimuli may be needed to establish conditioning. Also, a more startling noise could be used and presented to participants in a way that makes the noise unexpected. For example, the sound could come through hidden speakers placed near the participant rather than through headphones. The current study indicates that, possibly due to human awareness, it may be necessary to set up a situation where the participants are not expecting the stimuli that are going to be presented to them. EFFECTS OF HABITUATION TO AN UNCONDITIONED STIMULUS 10 References Byrne, J. H. (Ed.). (2003). Learning and memory (2nd ed.). New York: Macmillan. Domjan, M. (2010). The principles of learning and behavior (7th ed.). Stamford, CT: Cengage Learning. Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior: An introduction to behavior theory. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Lovibond, P. F. & Shanks, D. R. (2002). The role of awareness in pavlovian conditioning: Empirical evidence and theoretical implications. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 28 (1), 3-26. Pavlov, I. P. (1928). Lectures on conditioned reflexes: Twenty-five years of objective study of the higher nervous activity (behaviour) of animals. New York: International Publisher. Rescorla, R. A. (1973). Effect of US habituation following conditioning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 82 (1), 137-143. Rescorla, R. A. (1974) Effect of inflation of the unconditioned stimulus value following conditioning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 86 (1), 101-106. Spence, K. W. (1950). Cognitive versus stimulus-response theories of learning. Psychological Review, 57 (3), 159-172. Taylor, J. A. (1956). Level of conditioning and intensity of the adapting stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51, 127-130. Tolman, E. C. (1932). Purposive behavior in animals and men. New York: Appleton-Century. EFFECTS OF HABITUATION TO AN UNCONDITIONED STIMULUS Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review, 55 (4), 1-11. 11