* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Student Questions/Comments - Psychology and Neuroscience
Communication in small groups wikipedia , lookup
Group polarization wikipedia , lookup
Group dynamics wikipedia , lookup
Carolyn Sherif wikipedia , lookup
False consensus effect wikipedia , lookup
Vested interest (communication theory) wikipedia , lookup
Social perception wikipedia , lookup
Social tuning wikipedia , lookup
Impression formation wikipedia , lookup
Elaboration likelihood model wikipedia , lookup
Self-perception theory wikipedia , lookup
Implicit attitude wikipedia , lookup
Attitudes ProSem Week 2/5 Comments/Questions First, some historical reading, if you’re so inclined: Bob Zajonc http://www.psychologicalscience.org/index.php/publications/observer/2009/april-09/memories-ofrobert-b-zajonc.html Questions: Chris Mellinger 1. It seems like Gawronski et al. and Jones et al. are tackling different stages of attitude formation, even though they are both studying the way in which evaluative information is “attached” to or integrated with a stimulus representation. One interesting thing about both of these papers is that they are relevant to the question of when and how information gaps are filled in. EC shows that evaluation occurs based on very subtle manipulations of situational valence. The context manipulation in Gawronski et al. is interesting in that it tries to illuminate one way in which conflicting valence information is dealt with. On the other hand, it doesn’t feel to me like the background color of a face image is a particularly ecological context. It’s good to know that situational factors are important (background color or proximal objects), but I’m curious about how both of these might extend to more realistic contexts. 2. It seemed to me (correct me if I’m wrong) that the consensus last week was that attitudes are more related to representations (always valenced), where evaluations are measurements extracted from (or based on) those representations. If that’s true, then it seems like attitude formation is basically the construction of representation. This assumption certainly seems to underlie the empirical research we read this week. The account of renewal and occasion setting is explicitly representational. The misattribution explanation of EC also seems to hint at representation, at least in the operationalization of evaluation (direct comparison of conditioned stimuli). One of the things that I hope attitude research does for us is to understand the effects of social category membership in attitude formation, both in terms of forming attitudes toward categories and in forming attitudes toward individuals who are members of categories. The research here is ongoing, but I’d love to hear thoughts on the role of categories in forming representations of exemplars. If I learn information about an exemplar, and then learn that s/he is a member of a category, which information wins? It seems that categories are tools for quickly and efficiently enriching a representation of an object, but individualized information can be in conflict with category representations: then what? To some extent these are empirical issues, but it might be fun to think about. 3. Is evaluation always driven by affect? When does one occur independently of the other, if ever? Zajonc tries to distinguish between feeling and thinking systems. The other authors avoid the debate, however their results are dependent on some kind of evaluation. Clearly, some evaluations can be more motivational than others, but I’m curious to know what others think of the idea that some evaluations might be relatively free of motivation/affect. Perhaps motivation is dependent on context, or the personal relevance of the object under consideration. 1/8 Attitudes ProSem Week 2/5 Comments/Questions 4. Maybe more interestingly, different evaluations can have dramatically different motivational implications. In the social neuroscience class, we had a discussion concerning evidence that social exclusion provokes a pattern of neural activation that resembles the neural “pain signature.” However, pain and social exclusion have dramatically different response implications; pain motivates us to avoid, but social exclusion motivates at least some approach. Thus, in addition to relatively different motivational strength, it seems like evaluations can also carry qualitatively different motivational content. (I acknowledge that in situations of social exclusion the target of evaluation is far more ambiguous, but at least in some cases the argument could be made that it is the excluder; in this case, I think the question stands). Sharaya Jones 1) Jones et al (2009) ~ Evaluative conditioning occurs when an object is paired with a positively or negatively valence stimuli. Supposedly this change in attitudes can occur as a result of mere association, which suggests that attitudes can change without cognition. Zajonc (1980), similarly notes that affect does not require cognition, such that we can like something and not necessarily know why. Jones et al. attempt to show through eye tracking that evaluative conditioning occurs more often when there is source confusion. How valid is the length of an individual’s gaze in explaining attitude shifts? Do these findings actually show attitude development, or do they simply show increased recognition? Are recognition and attitudes different? Without any measure of valence or affect, how can we call this behavior ‘attitude change’ or ‘attitude development’? 2) Zajonc (1980) discusses the role of affect in attitudes and notes that additional information does not change attitudes, because the induced affect remains, despite the addition of new information. Gawronski et al (2010), on the other hand, look to find what happens when individuals are exposed to information that is incongruent with the initially acquired information. In such situations, individuals search for contextual factors that might explain this discrepancy. The authors note that they do not use a direct measure for attention, which is a critical mediator. Depending on the weighting an individual gives to one piece of information over another could drastically impact an individual’s attitude change, or lack there of. 3) Zajonc (1980) discusses recognition and notes that affective reactions to a stimulus can be acquired by virtue of experience with that stimulus (similar to Jones et al). Extrapolating on this, repeatedly doing something might cause you to develop an attitude towards it. Further, Zajonc discusses the primacy of impression. If an initial response is negative, will additional experience with the stimulus enhance the negative attitude (by reinforcing it) or is there a possibility that familiarity with the stimulus will dim the initial negative response? Further, can you force yourself to like something by doing it over and over again? Brianne Eby 1. On page 154, Zajonc says, “…it is entirely possible that the very first stage of the organism’s reaction to stimuli and the very first elements in retrieval are affective”. First, I’m wondering if by retrieval, he means an organism’s second encounter with a previously formed attitude or stimuli? If this is so, is there evidence that there is differential processing the first time one encounters a novel attitude-evoking stimuli than there is the second time that person 2/8 Attitudes ProSem Week 2/5 Comments/Questions encounters the same stimuli? i.e. perhaps the first reaction is purely affective, but presumably one has the chance to elaborate more/use cognition before subsequent reactions. Does the mere fact of this process itself, i.e. that one is able to cognitively elaborate on his or her attitude, change the substance of the resulting attitude, even though it may be essentially the same? I’m thinking of our usual example of one who, upon meeting a person from an outgroup (e.g. a student at the rival school), implicitly forms a negative affective opinion of that person and through subsequent interactions, is able to cognitively deduce that the negative affect is legitimate (e.g. that person manifested objective demonstrations of being intolerable/annoying/self-gratifying/etc). Does the mere fact that one was able to interact with that person and corroborate the initial impression at all change the nature of either impression? Does it provide legitimacy? This is in some ways related to the saliency/attribution component of the Jones et al. paper, in that “the specific cause of one’s feelings is not always evident, and they are therefore subject to processes of attribution”. 2. Related to my above question, I wonder what implications we think the issue of saliency has on informing policy? In Irene’s section, we talked a bit about how social psych research is often conducted out of context, though researchers make claims that their work can ultimately shape policy. Zajonc’s discussion of dissonance (“We buy the cars we ‘like’, choose the jobs and houses that we find ‘attractive’, and then justify those choices by various reasons that might appear convincing to others who never fail to ask us, ‘why this car?’ or ‘why this house?’) made me think of my own project, and how the use of dissonance might be used to explain a shift in what is salient to an individual. 3. Gawronski et al. discuss the importance of attention to context clues. On the contrary, Zajonc claims that affective reactions operate outside of attentive processes. Given these two competing roles of attention, to what extent is it or isn’t it important in evaluations and attitudes? Should efforts be made to control attention, or is it better to understand how attitudes are formed at the sub-attentional level? Jessica Jones 1) Zajonic posits that affect and cognition are partially independent systems and can consititude different effects in information processing (Zajonc, 1980). When it comes to forming attitudes then, it seems as though there are different ways in which attitudes are formed and the requirements for an attitude to take place. Per our discussion last week, then, would we be able to categorize attitude formation into mutually exclusive experiences- through cognitive efforts, through affect, or perhaps even behavior? For example, an infant liking milk is an attitude (affect), an infant realizing the stove is hot after touching it (behavior), or an infant refusing a certain food after experiencing it (cognitive). This seems to align with the discussion of explicit versus implicit and categorize attitudes by forming them in different ways. 2) Gawronski et al focus on showing automatic evaluations are highly dependent on the context in which it is encountered, adding to the notion of primacy and regency effects in attitude formation. The authors briefly note that attention may be a moderator of the effects observed, which makes me wonder if attention falls under the same category as self-awareness. Reading through the experiments studied, I understood the notion of context as an important factor, but one’s level of self-awareness to the particular context could also be incredibly relevant. Age, 3/8 Attitudes ProSem Week 2/5 Comments/Questions maturity, emotional intelligence are all factors of self-awareness, which to me, may play a large role in how we think about the role of context in attitude formation. If context is important, should one’s awareness to that specific context also be evaluated? 3) As I did last week, I leave these readings this week a bit overwhelmed by the specifics of particular attitudes formed at certain levels of cognition, social contexts, and before/after particular effects in place. Has there been any consensus in the last five years to a particular model of attitude formation? It is certainly a large construct that requires definitions before pursuing further study, and I can’t help but ponder how to wrap my head around the general construct of how attitudes are created, formed, and changed? Lindsay Labrecque 1. I found the Jones et al. (2009) article regarding implicit misattribution and evaluative conditioning to be particularly interesting especially as, implicit misattribution as a mechanism in the process of evaluative conditioning seems to be a similar cognitive process (although under different names) as the process that occurs in fear and anxiety disorders. At the risk of comparing the processes too much, it is interesting that in anxiety disorders, individuals typically avoid anxiety-inducing stimuli, at least from a behavioral perspective. It seems reasonable to imagine that these same individuals might attend to the stimuli cognitively, which may in turn heighten or maintain arousal. Drawing parallels from this to the Jones et al. (2009) paper, it seems to me that the significant eye tracker data may be a result of task demands. I am wondering about the utility of eye tracker technology as a method of examining implicit misattribution and about the meaning of fixations. Do eye fixations really measure or imply evaluation? In addition, I wonder how the EC in these studies was possibly impacted by the subjects’ possible familiarity with the pokemon cartoon used in the experiments (per Gawronski et al., 2010) given that it seems likely many participants had some prior contextual evaluations related to pokemon. 2. According to Zajonc (1980), “…models for affect preferences are formed and expressed only after and only as a result of considerable prior cognitive activity…I argue, along with Wundt and Cummings, that to arouse affect, objects need to be cognized very little – in fact, minimally” (p. 154). Gawronski et al. (2010) demonstrate various conditions under which automatic evaluations reflect initial or the addition of novel information. If Zajonc is “right,” that an affective preference for an object requires little cognitive processing, then what forms the basis of these automatic evaluations? Is is a valenced belief, an attitude, the context? What role, if any, does affect play in automatic evaluations? 3. Again, I found the Gawronski et al. (2010) paper quite interesting. However, I’m not sure if I fully buy into their assertion that “…attention to context cues is typically low during the encoding of initial information about a target object…” (p. 696). Specifically, how is it that these experiences are stored context-free? Do we not, to some degree, use context to interpret or as the basis of our evaluations? That is, does context not provide the cues for the appropriateness of information about a target object? If not contextual cues, then what are we encoding initial information from? Shana Stites 4/8 Attitudes ProSem Week 2/5 Comments/Questions Zajonc’s (1980) discussion of feelings versus thoughts, especially regarding the time course for each in relation to the other, reminded me of LeDoux’s “high road” and “low road.” Affect can be more immediate, with very little cognitive processing outside of basic sensory and perceptual processes or it can be more cognitive, the result of having thought about the context in which the affect was elicited. So cognition can accompany affect, but it need not do so. The seeming value of cognition over affect in our daily leads us to say we are rational creatures and yet we hold on to attitudes even after the cognitive basis of an attitude has been discredited. What is it about the “hot” affective processes that seem inferior to the “cold” cognitive processes, when both contribute so meaningfully to our success (ex: statements about the emotionality of women compared to men are not generally meant as compliments)? Both Zajonc (1980) and Gawronski et al. (2010) talked about the primacy of affect/evaluations. While the basic ideas about primacy of attitudes were similar, Gawronski et al. had definitely made good use of the intervening 30 years and additional research to come up with some potential reasons why that primacy effect would hold in certain situations but not in others. I read them in chronological order initially, but when I went back to look at the Zajonc paper after reading Gawronski et al., the statements made by the former could be interpreted through the representational model proposed by the latter. Because the Zajonc paper was written much longer ago, it seemed that in his discussion of the primacy of attitudes that he was referring more to what we currently call explicitly measured attitudes while Gawronski et al. were examining changes in implicitly measured attitudes. To what extent do the moderating effects of attention to context influence more explicitly measured attitudes? Jones et al. (2009) in their paper argued that evaluative conditioning (EC) and Pavlovian conditioning (PC) are distinct processes and that the requirements for PC are not necessarily the same for EC. Jones et al. mentioned that EC is more resistant to extinction than PC (this is unfortunate in terms of learned prejudicial attitudes). Other ideas associated with PC, but not mentioned specifically in the article, are stimulus generalization and stimulus discrimination. By this I mean the likelihood that following PC or EC that the “leaner” responds similarly to other similar stimuli or not (i.e. Pavlov’s dogs salivate to other similar stimuli). The Gawronski et al. article did address this to some extent, but discussed generalization and contextualization in terms of the environment. To what extent does an evaluative response to the stimulus itself generalize to other similar stimuli? Using these particular study stimuli, I would want to know what moderators would make people respond similarly to other Pokemon characters or other cartoons that weren’t actually from Pokemon. Phoenix van Wagnoner Zajonc has a section where he points out affective judgments are always about the self (p. 157). Yet we know that context matters in our evaluations and we can conflate evaluations between objects. An individuals mood is an obvious contextual element that would change an evaluation of an object. However, it seems nonrecursive in that the evaluation could impact mood and possibly change subsequent evaluations in close temporal proximity. I could see the case where mood has a strong effect on implicit associations, even overwhelming them. Can we control for mood in our attitude experiments without priming? 5/8 Attitudes ProSem Week 2/5 Comments/Questions As Zajonc pointed out Affective judgments often come before cognition and can become separated from content. This idea ties into Gawronski et al.’s argument that automatic evaluations “reflect the valence of counter- attitudinal information only in the context in which this information was learned (occasion setting) and the valence of initial experiences in any other context (renewal effect)” (p.933). Were does this leave us with implicit misattribution? Would evaluative conditioning that presents a counter attitudinal association be subtyped and context dependent? Can a counter attitudinal evaluation be considered an evaluation of the same object in the first place, given the different context? Are two temporally different evaluations of the same object in three dimensional space actually evaluating the same object at all? Could this be the reason we fail test retest reliability and parallel test reliability? What do meta-analyses tell us? Brooke Carter Zajonc (1980) describes the complicated interplay between affect and cognition. How do these two process apply to Gawronski et al.’s (2010) work on generalization and contextualization? Is the brain classifying various relationships between objects/valence/contexts in a cognitive, categorical manner or in a more general affective manner? Does the automatic nature of these evaluations shed any light on this question? Most, if not all, models of attitude change, decision making, belief formation, etc., that incorporate both affect and cognition view these two systems as independent processes. However, could they be viewed as a combined process that uses cognitive logic and probability to build a representation, but always results in an affective decision? Take the seemingly pure cognitive evaluation that “2 + 2 = 4” as an example. Does the belief that this representation is true come from an affective marker associated with certainty and confidence in one’s prior experience with arithmetic? Similarly, I would argue that incongruency of “2+2=5” just “feels” wrong in some sense. Do deliberations that are devoid of uncertainty and ambiguity create an affective state that we have named rationality or cognition? Both the Jones and Gawronski articles investigated the creation of evaluative associations from a bottom-up perspective. They varied stimulus attributes and relationships between stimuli to affect subsequent associations. However, many evaluative associations seem to build on previously held associations. What top-down factors influence the formation of new associations? How do they interact with the bottom-up factors studied in these papers? Do certain personal biases make the context more salient in certain situations, such as times when the object (CS) already holds a valence incongruent with the US? Alex Kirk 1. The article by Gawronski, Rydell, Vervliet, and De Houwer (2010) is actually remarkably relevant to the therapy mechanisms I study with regards to exposure therapy for anxiety disorders (e.g., inhibitory learning, much of which relates to Mark Bouton’s work). The idea of formulating two associations with a given stimulus (i.e., context-free and context-based) poses certain complications with regards to treatment outcomes, particularly with regards to fear-based disorders (e.g., phobias) in terms of maintaining long-term reductions in fear. This has typically 6/8 Attitudes ProSem Week 2/5 Comments/Questions been overcome by generalizing exposure therapy to multiple contexts in hopes of generalizing the effects. What I would be curious to better understand is: how might these sorts of learning effects apply to more diffuse, long-lasting beliefs? For instance, when it comes to discriminatory beliefs, or maybe long-term major depression, how might these findings inform the actual changing of beliefs that are often associated with the core of a person’s worldview either about themselves or the world in general? Can these extend beyond short-term laboratory effects? Is a greater temporal consistency required (i.e., continual violation of expectancies for a greater period of time)? 2. The concept behind evaluative conditioning (EC) is indeed interesting, where an attitude towards a stimulus forms or changes due to that object’s pairing with positively or negatively valenced stimuli (Jones, Fazio, & Olson, 2009). However, one thing I am curious to know is what EC brings to psychology beyond common cognitive theories such as schema theory, or behavioral theories such as relational frame theory (RFT), where associations among different stimuli/events can change the perceptions or attitudes towards those stimuli/events based on how they are related. The EC process itself sounds like an automatic one, and so what I am curious to know is what these findings (i.e., changing attitudes towards a stimulus based on how that stimulus is paired with other stimuli) might imply with regards to the Gawronski et al. (2010) article? That is, if attitudes can be formed/changed automatically and relatively easily simply based on associations; does this have implications for either renewal effects as delineated by Gawronski et al., or for attitude change via violation of expectancies? 3. The article by Zajonc (1980) has interesting implications for the work by Gawronski et al. (2010). Zajonc proposes that, while much of psychology considers affect to occur after cognitive operations have been performed, he delineates situations where affect may occur before such cognitive operations. With regards to the work by Gawronski et al., what I am curious to better understand is: what role might affect play in informing context effects of beliefs regarding certain stimuli? For example: if one were to perform a fear learning/extinction experiment, would belief change (i.e., cognitive recognition of no threat) via violation of expectancies precede affective change (i.e., reduced fear)? Or would this relationship be reversed where a change in affect leads to a change in belief? Maggie Tobias 1. A common theme in this week’s readings was that we don’t seem to form our attitudes in as neat and logical a way as we would like to think. Zajonc comes to the conclusion that we do not make many decisions, even large ones, based on a “detailed cognitive analysis” (172), implying that our preferences and decisions are instead based primarily on immediate affective responses. The Jones article builds upon this argument, showing how during this process we are prone to misattribute the cause for an affective response, leading to evaluative conditioning. Taken together, these ideas paint a picture of attitudes and preferences that might appear bleak; they seem to suggest that we aren’t nearly as logical and thoughtful when building attitudes as we might believe, and in fact, we likely hold certain attitudes due to mistaken attribution formation! Since it likely isn’t as simple as just deeming Jones’s EC and Zajonc’s affective responding outright maladaptive, it instead is important to consider the implications of each effect. Furthermore, what adaptive functions might these features hold? 7/8 Attitudes ProSem Week 2/5 Comments/Questions 2. Setting aside the implicit versus explicit attitudes discussion from last week’s reading, what does it mean for the idea of a “true attitude” that our automatic evaluations for the same object can change in different contexts and that our attitudes can be formed or changed due to mistaken attribution via evaluative conditioning? This also brings up general concerns for attitude measurement in research. 3. Is the idea that we could still have certain attitudes that are so strong as to be contextindependent simply incorrect, given this body of research? I think a very basic reaction to these readings might be to argue that we all have a few objects or concepts that we feel too strongly opinionated on to ever demonstrate any degree of malleability in relation to them. Is this possible, or more likely a story we tell ourselves to reaffirm the thoughtful and purposeful status of our attitudes and subsequent evaluative conduct? 8/8