* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download The overviews in Chapter 1: 1. How natural selection shapes
Dual inheritance theory wikipedia , lookup
Genome (book) wikipedia , lookup
Genetic drift wikipedia , lookup
Polymorphism (biology) wikipedia , lookup
Human genetic variation wikipedia , lookup
Heritability of IQ wikipedia , lookup
Group selection wikipedia , lookup
Population genetics wikipedia , lookup
Koinophilia wikipedia , lookup
The overviews in Chapter 1: 1. How natural selection shapes behavior. 2. How learning influences behavior. 3. How learning is aided in groups through cultural transmission of information. Example 1 of natural selection working on animal behavior: Parasite pressure, in populations of Hawaiian crickets, changes mating behavior. (Or does it? See last slide in this series.) Parasite pressure in populations of Hawaiian crickets changes mating behavior Parasitic fly finds male crickets by their mating call Males with weaker calls survive better (but attract fewer females) Selective balance favors mostly silent males Silent males acquire “satellite” behavior to get females (On some islands lots of parasitic flies, on other islands few) “File” generates sound when wing rubbed One assumes allelic variation in the genes controlling wing ridges. Some individuals have deeper ridges and make slightly louder sounds, others shallower ridges. Why doesn’t natural selection drive all individuals to have the maximum ridge and greatest sound? Competing pressures! Perhaps deep ridges require more force to rub. Crickets with deep ridges can’t call for as long. Given variation, predation pressure then favors individuals with the shallow ridge allele (and are not parasitized by flies). Frequency of the shallow ridge alleles increases in population. Eventually get mostly silent males. Ethological philosophy: Did natural selection operate to change a behavior? Example 2 of natural selection working on animal behavior: Mole rats live in arid or in better environments Arid environment scarce resources rats should be more Xenophobic. Use aggression index as surrogate for xenophobia From a scientific presentation perspective, what is wrong with this diagram? BTW, for “clinical” rat and mouse behavioral testing, some companies make turn-key systems. The Noldus automated aggression tester for mice. From the sales brochure: ”The Tube Test is a well-known test paradigm designed to measure (altered) social dominance and investigate social hierarchies in mice (Lindzey et al., 1961). Traditionally, data are collected by human observers. The Automated Tube Test is a novel apparatus that fully automates the validated tube test procedures” Match sessions: two mice are placed in their own start box, walk through the tube, and compete in forcing the other back into its start box. Example of learning: grasshopper food preference experiments Give hoppers choice of better diet or crappy diet. 1. Match dishes with colored cards & odors, or 2. Scramble cards, odors and dishes Citral or coumarin odor, Brown or green cards Consistent cue pairing – hoppers go right to the better food Which of Tinbergen’s questions is relevant for this learned behavior? (function) Cultural transmission of learned information The key distinction is vertical transmission between generations. Text example: Rat 2 smells food odor on rat 1; figures out (& remembers) food with that smell is good to eat. Chapter 2 For natural selection to change the behavior of a population of individuals: 1. There must be variation in the trait. 2. Trait variants lead to differential fitness. (environmental control!)** 3. The trait is all or partially under genetic control. ** myopia in humans! New behaviors under genetic control can spread very quickly, even if the increase in fitness is small! Group hunting in African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus). Generation time ≈ 5 years. 10% advantage 100% of population has behavioral trait in 500 years. In the Pacific NW, Different populations of garter snakes (same species) vary in their food preference: slugs good OR slugs no good A genetic /innate preference: hybrids are intermediate. If a snake in the coastal population showed up with a mutated allele that changed its olfactory preference such that slug = good, given the abundance of slugs near the coast, that snake would have much more food available, and leave more genes in the next generation. If the reproductive success of the individuals with that allele was just 1% more than “normal” snakes, in 10,000 years the coastal population would be composed almost entirely of slugs-are-OK snakes. BUT… Is there one best trait among variants? Often multiple selection pressures will lead to two or more stable phenotypes, each an equally successful behavioral strategy. (more on this later) This begs the question: How much of a behavior is under genetic control? In some cases, artificial selection experiments can provide data (a theoretical experiment example)… Pop variation in how fast a novel object is approached. Only slow ones (80 seconds or slower) allowed to breed. Mean of original, initial population 60 seconds. Mean of truncated breeder population 90 seconds. Still get fast approach individuals After truncated population allowed to breed, mean of 2nd generation = 70 seconds. Note: mean shifted faster, from 90 to 70 seconds! Artificial selection did not get rid of all of the fast approach phenotype. The heritability of approach speed: Xgen2 = 70 s Xgen1 = 60 s Xsel = 90 s Xgen2 – Xgen1 = 70 – 60 = 10 s Xsel - Xgen1 = 90 – 60 = 30 s (achieved) (goal) Heritability = = 10/30 = 33% How much of a behavior is under genetic control? In addition to artificial selection, one can measure the consistency of traits from parents to offspring (An actual experiment) … Some swallows aggregate in large breeding colonies, others in small colonies. Learned? Innate? Random? Offspring do what their parents did: Cross-fostered birds raised in “opposite” colony – parental preference persists: Note: The way the parent-offspring relationship is expressed is as a “correlation”. We correlate the X and Y variables. How? A simple math/stats technique will produce a “least-squares” linear regression line, fit to the data values. A correlation coefficient reports how well the line represents the trend in the data. 1 is perfect correlation. 0 is no correlation How much of a behavior is under genetic control? Sometimes there are “natural” experiments … Some guppies live permanently in low-predation areas and others in high predation areas. Waterfalls gene flow barrier. Physiological and behavioral differences in the populations of guppies (Poecilia reticulata) & two other species. Behavioral response to predation is increase school size. Trinidad transplant experiment: high predation zone 1957 Arima site guppies No guppies previously, no predators. Turure site 1990’s Very rapid evolution (or shift in gene frequency) to the behavioral traits best suited to a low predation environment. Turure site guppies Kinship effects on behavior: The extreme case of eusociality. Social insects & NMRs • apparent altruistic behavior • Classes or caste individuals give up reproduction Haplo-diploid system or inbreeding high sibling relatedness. Sociobiology: Conceptual breakthrough by Dawkins (The Selfish Gene) & E.O. Wilson, 1970’s. Social behaviors driven by degree of relatedness. Natural selection works on the individual to increase inclusive (direct + indirect) fitness. But… Not the last word, and much social progressive pressure not to accept. Do related species exhibit the same behaviors? If much or all of some behaviors are under genetic control, and related species have many genes in common, you would expect this. Behavioral phylogeny!