* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Advertising and Publicity: Suggested New Applications for Tourism
Survey
Document related concepts
Transcript
Advertising and Publicity: Suggested New Applications for Tourism Marketers MARSHA D. LODA, WILLIAM NORMAN, AND KENNETH F. BACKMAN This research explores two of the basic tools used by tourism marketers: advertising and publicity. Message stimulus is the independent variable and consists of two parts: message presentation (i.e., publicity or advertising) and message sequencing (i.e., publicity only, publicity then advertising, advertising only, or advertising then publicity). Four dependent variables are organized in two categories: message acceptance (i.e., perceived credibility, message strength) and message response (i.e., attitude toward the destination, purchase intent). Results of the study show that publicity, in either presentation or sequencing, created significantly higher mean scores than advertising for credibility, message strength, and purchase intent. This study reaffirms that publicity is an important element in the tourism marketing mix. Furthermore, it suggests that a publicity-then-advertising strategy is most effective at persuading potential tourists to visit a specific destination. Keywords: communication; advertising; publicity; perceived credibility While different sources of information affect customers’ purchase decisions, it is in mass-media advertising that marketers often invest significant portions of marketing budgets (Knipp 1996). However, from previous studies, it can be inferred that print publicity is potentially more credible than print advertising (Lord and Putrevu 1993; Cameron 1994; Hallahan 1999). Research is neither abundant nor conclusive. The current research tests whether there is a difference between advertising and publicity on message acceptance and response and if the sequencing of message presentation (i.e., advertising then publicity or publicity then advertising) affects message processing. Four dependent variables are studied: message strength, perceived credibility, attitude toward the destination, and purchase intent. This study attempts to address marketing issues of importance to the tourism industry. Message credibility is critical because of the intangible nature of the tourism product and the risk associated with destination selection. THE ISSUE OF ADVERTISING VERSUS PUBLICITY The belief that publicity is more credible, persuasive, or effective than advertising is cited in marketing literature (Gartner 1993; Kotler, Bowen, and Makens 1996; Ries and Ries 2002) but without substantive empirical support. Marketing texts teach that publicity’s advantages over advertising include lower costs and increased visibility and that the third-party endorsement effect inherent in publicity is assumed to increase its credibility (Kotler 1993). These claims are countered by Hunt and Gruning (1993), who note the lack of research evidence that editorial copy has greater credibility than advertising. Hallahan (1999, p. 331) agrees and states, “despite widespread acceptance among practitioners, and general acknowledgement by academicians, little empirical research has been conducted to test claims about third-party endorsement or the superiority of news versus advertising.” Lord and Putrevu (1993) explain the need for greater recognition of the value and importance of publicity. They call for research to bring practitioners and scholars to a better understanding of what they call an often neglected promotional tool. Cho (2001) also examined the role of credibility of information sources in tourists’ information-search behavior along with the role of prior knowledge and need for information. Respondents rated 10 information sources, ranging from their own experience to magazines, newspapers, and the Internet, on a scale of 1 (not at all credible) to 7 (very credible). Results reveal that “the credibility of sources was found to have the most crucial influence on the selection and use of the source, compared with the effects of prior knowledge and need for information” (p. v). However, there was no distinguishing between advertising messages and editorial messages in the rating process. While some literature exists comparing the persuasive effects of advertising and publicity, it is not extensive. In all, 12 studies were located (Schwarz, Kumpf, and Bussmann 1986; Anderson and Abbott 1985; Hausknecht, Wilkinson, and Prough 1989; Salmon et al. 1985; Hennessey and Anderson 1990; d’Astous and Hebert 1991; Cameron 1994; Chaiken and Maheswaran 1994; Straughan, Bleske, and Zhao 1994; Chew, Slater, and Kelly 1995; Hallahan 1995, 1999). Among these, Dr. Marsha Loda was a principle in an advertising agency for 15 years and the director of marketing for the largest tourism business in North Carolina. She got her PhD in 2003 and is an assistant professor of marketing in the Hull College of Business at Augusta State University in Augusta, Georgia. Dr. William Norman is director of the Recreation, Travel and Tourism Institute at Clemson University in Clemson, South Carolina, where he also serves as associate professor. Dr. Kenneth F. Backman is associate professor at Clemson University in Clemson, South Carolina. His research interests include tourism marketing and community tourism development. Journal of Travel Research, Vol. 45, February 2007, 259-265 DOI: 10.1177/0047287506292688 © 2007 Sage Publications Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 11, 2016 260 FEBRUARY 2007 Hallahan (1999) reports that 2 studies supported that publicity messages are more credible than advertising, 7 were inconclusive, and 3 had methodology issues that confounded their results. Hallahan concludes, “the evidence suggests news does not uniformly outperform advertising” (p. 339). One exploratory study reported on the extent and role of publicity programs in the promotional strategy of 10 national tourism organizations. It reports that publicity programs conducted by destinations are often “deliberate, planned, methodical and coordinated with a clear set of objectives” (Dore and Crouch 2003, p. 137). Some respondents reported receiving a greater annual value from their publicity programs than from any other promotional strategy. Dore and Crouch call for further research to examine the impact of publicity with other forms of messages and to study consumer behavior issues including the effect of publicity on destination choice. THE IMPACT OF MESSAGE SEQUENCING Conversely, how consumers process and integrate various sources of information has been the subject of considerable study. Researchers examining this issue often use the expectancy-value (EV) theory of Fishbein and Ajzen (1975). This is a model of reasoned behavior or central-route processing wherein consumers carefully consider message content (Petty and Cacioppo 1986). In these cases, the success of persuasive communications (i.e., message acceptance) depends on the degree to which consumers accept message claims (Smith and Vogt 1995). Factors that influence message acceptance include perceived credibility and message strength (Fishbein and Ajzen 1975). The theory of reasoned action was extended to the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen and Driver 1992) that ads subjective norms and perceived behavioral control as predictors of actual behavior. According to Smith and Vogt (1995), the EV model identifies which variables are important in information response, that is, the degree to which attitudes are changed by persuasive communication. However, Anderson’s (1971) informationintegration theory describes how information from different sources (i.e., advertising and publicity) is combined when people form evaluations. According to information-integration theory, the sequence of exposure to informational stimuli is an important issue. In their integrated information response model, Smith and Swinyard (1982) propose that consumers’ belief strength can be affected by message sequencing. Exposure to a credible initial message source such as direct experience generates “a powerful information base for attitudinal development” (p. 84). Lord and Putrevu (1993) recognized the potential sequencing impact of publicity with advertising. They state that multiple promotional messages interact in such a way as to yield advantages for the publicity-then-advertising ordering relative to an advertising-then-publicity sequence. They call for specific research to confirm and better understand these potential sequencing effects. Using the EV model as a theoretical framework, Smith and Vogt (1995) studied the sequencing effects of advertising and negative word-of-mouth publicity on tourism messages. A test was conducted using four experimental groups: advertising only, negative word-of-mouth only, advertising then negative word-of-mouth, and negative word-of-mouth then advertising. Results revealed that negative word-of-mouth communication reduced the perceived credibility of advertising as well as brand attitudes and purchase intent for a specific vacation destination. This study was based on a prior experiment by Smith (1993), who studied the integration effects of advertising and trial for a cola product. HYPOTHESES According to Smith and Vogt (1995), the extent of persuasion that occurs from a promotional message depends on the degree to which consumers undergo message acceptance. Factors that influence message acceptance include perceived credibility (i.e., truthfulness and accuracy) and message strength (i.e., weak or strong reasoning in message claims), two dependent variables tested in this study. Fishbein and Ajzen’s (1975) model of reasoned behavior states that attitude leads to behavioral intent. Attitude toward the brand and purchase intent are common variables of marketing-research projects (Hallahan 1999). Therefore, attitude toward the destination (i.e., a predisposition to respond favorably or unfavorably) and purchase intent (i.e., a stage of motivation that precedes behavior) are studied. Message stimulus is the independent variable of this study and consists of two parts: message presentation (i.e., publicity or advertising) and message sequencing (i.e., publicity only, publicity then advertising, advertising only, or advertising then publicity). Four dependent variables are organized in two categories: message acceptance (i.e., perceived credibility, message strength) and message response (i.e., attitude toward the destination, purchase intent). Two hypotheses were examined concerning whether there is a difference in message acceptance and message response depending on how the message is presented (advertising or publicity) and whether sequencing matters. Hypothesis 1: There will be greater message acceptance for publicity only than for any other message sequence. Hypothesis 2: There will be greater message response for publicity only than for any other message sequence. METHODOLOGY The research was conducted as an experiment in the form of a posttest-only control group. To explore the hypotheses, five experimental groups were used: (1) advertising only, (2) publicity only, (3) advertising then publicity, (4) publicity then advertising, and (5) a control group. The posttest-only group design assured that subjects are not biased by previous exposures or learning effects through pretests (Kirk 1982). University student subjects were chosen because they generally represent a homogeneous group that travels frequently (Jamrozy 1996). The research consisted of five stages: selecting the specific destination to study, selecting attributes of that destination to include in stimulus materials, developing the stimulus materials, testing the stimulus materials, and data collection. Product and Attribute Selection Procedures for selecting the specific destination and destination attributes tested replicated those used in the Smith and Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 11, 2016 JOURNAL OF TRAVEL RESEARCH 261 TABLE 1 SCALE AND RELIABILITY MEASURES α Perceived credibility (3 items, scored from 1 to 7 [highest]) In the materials you just read: How credible do you think the claims were .90 Message strength (3 items, scored from –3 to +3 [highest]) I think the message/arguments in the materials were: Easy to understand/not easy to understand Strong reasons/weak reasons Clear/unclear .75 Attitude toward the destination (3 items, –3 to +3 [highest]) I feel Aruba as a vacation place is positive/negative I feel Aruba as a vacation place is good/bad I feel Aruba as a vacation place is interesting/uninteresting .92 Purchase intent (2 items scored from 1 to 7 [highest]) How likely are you to select Aruba as your vacation place How likely would you be to recommend Aruba as a vacation place to a friend .89 Vogt (1995) study. To select the destination, 27 subjects were surveyed concerning destinations they would realistically consider if they had the time and money to travel. Beach destinations dominated the pretest. Four islands received multiple mentions. The island of Aruba was selected because it received the least multiple mentions. A lesser known destination was desirable to limit preconceived attitudes. Another survey was conducted in which 21 different subjects were asked to determine which attributes were salient when choosing an island vacation destination. Following the free-elicitation procedure of Fishbein and Ajzen (1975), the respondents listed the attributes they considered important when selecting an island vacation. The five most frequently mentioned attributes were incorporated into stimulus materials. These attributes were activities (e.g., hiking, snorkeling, scuba diving, sightseeing), white sand beaches, good weather, inclusive and reasonable price, and interesting or native foods. Stimulus Materials Three one-page print advertisements and three one-page magazine publicity articles, each incorporating the five most salient attributes, were created and pretested. Following a quantitative analysis of 5 months of tourism advertisements and articles in Southern Living magazine, stimulus materials were designed to closely resemble one-page advertisements and articles in the magazine. Southern Living was chosen as the prototype after surveying a sample of 27 students on their magazine-reading habits. Stimulus materials were pretested and revised to arrive at one article and one advertisement. The final materials were rated by respondents as equally persuasive (using a scale of 1 to 10, where 1 is not at all persuasive and 10 is extremely persuasive). Both of the promotional messages were presented with four-color photography and in a full-page format. Measurement Scale Development The measures all used 7-point scales that were based on those used by Smith and Vogt (1995). Perceived credibility was measured with three Likert-type scales asking how truthful, accurate, and credible a specific stimulus was to the respondent. These measures were developed by Darley and Smith (1993) and Smith and Hunt (1978). Message strength was measured with a 3-item scale developed by Miniard, Bhatla, and Rose (1990) and Petty, Cacioppo, and Schumann (1983). This scale asked respondents to rate message claims from weak to strong. Attitude toward the destination was measured globally with three semantic differential scales (Osgood, Suci, and Tannenbaum 1967). Respondents used bipolar attribute fields to rate the destination as good, positive, and interesting. This measurement was developed by MacKenzie and Lutz (1989), Miniard, Bhatla, and Rose (1990), and Smith (1993). Purchase intent was measured with two 7-point scales developed by Smith (1993). The questions asked how likely the respondent was to select the destination and how likely he or she would be to recommend the destination to a friend. Responses range from not at all likely to extremely likely. For all multi-item measures, mean scores were combined and averaged to generate one statistic for each dependent variable. Five versions of the instrument were created and pretested (see table 1). Data Collection Procedures The study population for this research was students at a university in the southeastern United States. A sample size of 150 (30 per group) was used so that the central-limit theorem would apply and normal distribution would be assured for each group (Salkind 1994). As Smith and Vogt (1995) did, involvement was encouraged of all groups. To create reasonably high processing involvement, participants were asked to “consider yourself a person who has saved their funds, and now has the time and money to take a vacation this summer.” Participants were instructed to read the materials carefully, told to form an evaluation of the destination, and informed that they would be asked some questions about the destination later. Each participant was randomly assigned a booklet containing instructions, stimulus materials, and survey questions to be completed following exposure to the stimulus. Subjects were instructed to spend as much time as they needed to thoroughly read the stimulus materials (depending Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 11, 2016 FEBRUARY 2007 TABLE 2 FIGURE 2 RESULT OF MULTIVARIATE TESTS (WILKS’S LAMBDA) DIFFERENCES IN PERCEIVED CREDIBILITY BY TREATMENT GROUP F Significance Power 2.61 5.82 .018 .001 .85 1.00 Test Message acceptance Message response FIGURE 1 (M = 5.48) 5.4 (M = 5.33) 5.2 5.0 DIFFERENCES IN PURCHASE INTENT BY TREATMENT GROUP (M = 4.87) (M = 5.25) 5.4 ad only pub only ad then pub pub then ad Treatment 5.0 Mean (M = 4.97) 4.8 4.6 (M = 5.55) 5.8 5.6 Mean 262 4.6 (M = 4.77) 4.2 Note: Measured using a 3-item, 7-point scale with 1 as not at all truthful/accurate/credible and 7 as completely truthful/accurate/ credible. Dotted lines show areas of significant difference. (M = 4.30) 3.8 3.4 (M = 3.03) 3.0 ad only pub only ad then pub pub then ad control Treatment Note: Measured using a 7-point scale with 1 as zero likelihood and 7 as certain. Dotted lines show areas of significant difference. on the experimental group). Next, the dependent measures were collected (subjects were not allowed to turn back to the stimulus materials). Following exposure to the treatment, data were collected to measure the four dependent variables. ANALYSIS AND RESULTS Two MANOVAs were used to test the hypotheses (see table 2). The first hypothesis tested message acceptance (i.e., perceived credibility and message strength) without the presence of a control group (respondents could not rate these variables because they were not exposed to stimulus materials); it revealed that the relationship was significant (F = 2.61, p = .018). The second hypothesis tested message response (i.e., attitude toward the destination and purchase intent) and included a control group (respondents could rate these variables without seeing stimulus materials). It was also significant (F = 5.82, p = .001). Hence, according to this study, there is sufficient evidence to conclude that message acceptance and message response generated by tourism communication differ significantly depending on whether the message is presented in the format of an advertisement or an article and depending on which format is seen first. A series of one-way ANOVA tests were performed to identify areas of significance. Publicity consistently produced higher scores on all dependent variables than did advertising. Although the highest score for a variable may have been publicity then advertising rather than publicity only, the highest score for any dependent variable was always led by publicity in some form, never advertising only or advertising then publicity. Means scores for publicity then advertising were highest for three variables: perceived credibility (M = 5.48), message strength (M = 6.60), and attitude toward the destination (M = 6.73). For the variable purchase intent, publicity only received the highest mean score (M = 5.55). When the control group was used, all means plots followed a similar pattern (see example in figure 1), with treatment groups in this order: advertising only (lower), publicity only (higher), advertising then publicity (lower), publicity then advertising (higher), and control group (lowest). For those analyses that did not use a control group (perceived credibility and message strength), the same initial pattern was present (see example in figure 2). The advertising-only score was always lower than the publicity-only score, and the advertising-then-publicity score was always lower than the publicity-then-advertising score. While this pattern was present in all analysis results, the differences in mean scores were not always significant except for the control group, whose mean score was consistently, significantly lower than the treatment group. Significant findings are summarized in table 3. For the variable perceived credibility, publicity only was significantly higher than advertising only (p = .028), and publicity then advertising was significantly higher than both advertising only (p = .004) and advertising then publicity (p = .014). For message strength, publicity then advertising was significantly higher than advertising only (p = .003). For the variable attitude toward the destination, no stimulus-oriented treatment achieved or approached significance; only the control group was significantly different. Lastly, for the variable purchase intent, publicity only was significantly higher than both advertising only (p = .001) and advertising then publicity (p =. 012), while publicity then advertising was significantly higher than advertising only (p = .046). Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 11, 2016 JOURNAL OF TRAVEL RESEARCH 263 TABLE 3 SUMMARY OF SIGNIFICANT FINDINGS Dependent Variable Treatment Perceived credibility ad only ad only ad then pub ad only control ad only ad only pub only control Message strength Attitude toward the destination Purchase intent M 4.87 4.87 4.97 6.14 5.44 4.30 4.30 5.55 3.03 Treatment pub only pub then ad pub then ad pub then ad all pub only pub then ad ad then pub all M p 5.33 5.48 5.48 6.60 6.57a 5.55 5.25 4.77 4.97a .028 .004 .014 .003 .001 .001 .046 .012 .001 a. Average mean of all treatment groups. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION The results of the current study extend existing knowledge about how potential tourism consumers accept and respond to mass communication. First, this study compares the effects of tourism advertising to publicity. This issue is important because of the conflicting results reported by previous comparison studies of advertising and publicity in the general marketplace. This study is consistent with those that indicated publicity messages had greater credibility than advertising (Gartner 1993; Cameron 1994; Hallahan 1995). The current study adds to the body of knowledge about how consumers integrate different message sources. No other sequencing studies were located that combined advertising with publicity, although the practice is widely used in marketing. In today’s media-saturated environment, it is important for tourism marketers to understand how publicity and advertising affect each other. In the current study, the publicity-then-advertising sequence of information produced significantly higher means scores as compared to advertising-only information when subjects evaluated perceived credibility, message strength, and purchase intent of the ad. Indeed, the ability to increase the positive effects of advertising can be a major benefit of publicity that has not been adequately captured previously. Applications Based on these findings, there are various marketing implications from this study. Four major applications may help increase tourism marketing’s effectiveness when providing information to potential visitors. These are (1) publicity generally outperforms advertising; (2) to increase advertising’s effectiveness, precede it with publicity; (3) publicity is less effective when it follows advertising; and (4) campaigns must be planned well in advance. These applications support experiential suggestions by Ries and Ries (2002, p. xii), who said, “Advertising is a continuation of public relations by other means and should be started only after a PR program. . . . Furthermore, the theme of an advertising program should repeat the perceptions created in the mind of the prospect by the PR program.” Publicity generally outperforms advertising. Publicity about a tourist destination, in either presentation or sequencing, created significantly higher means scores than advertising for three of the four of dependent variables (perceived credibility, message strength, and purchase intent). Clearly, tourism marketers who do not have resources devoted to a publicity effort should consider doing so. In the face of the consistent results of the present study, one might question the reliance on advertising by tourism marketers. While it seems publicity provides distinct advantages over advertising, there are some practical disadvantages. As Lord and Putrevu (1993) note, publicity—while potentially more persuasive—is less under the control of tourism marketers. Advertising offers a controlled, consistent market presence that is difficult to achieve with publicity alone. However, this study clearly shows that the strongest tourism-marketing effect may be achieved by a publicity-then-advertising sequencing strategy. Increase advertising effectiveness with publicity. The information-sequencing component of the current study produced some potentially important implications for tourism advertising practices. Results showed the publicity-thenadvertising sequence to be more effective than the reversed sequence. When respondents viewed publicity about tourist areas before viewing ads, mean scores were significantly higher for perceived credibility, message strength, and importantly to marketers, purchase intent. In addition to the response generated, this finding could have sizeable implications for tourism-marketing budgets. For example, when one full-page advertisement in Southern Living magazine costs approximately $112,000, tourism marketers want to maximize the potential results of this investment. In today’s costly and competitive advertising environment, various strategies are used to increase an advertisement’s effectiveness, from copy testing to bold creative approaches. The current study points out that one strategy to increase advertising’s effectiveness may be overlooked: precede advertising with publicity. Do not follow advertising with publicity. Another consistent finding from this study indicates that while publicity is a valuable asset to a tourism-marketing program, it may not be worth the resources expended to acquire it if the publicity follows advertising. When advertising then publicity was found significantly different from publicity then advertising, the latter always influenced subjects more. Also, advertising only was never found significantly different from advertising then publicity, suggesting that the publicity in this sequence is dispensable. This study indicates that timing within a tourism-marketing campaign is critical. The Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 11, 2016 264 FEBRUARY 2007 credibility and effectiveness of publicity is markedly diminished if advertising is the first message viewed by a potential visitor. This is likely because of the perception bias created by advertising (Ray 1992). Or perhaps the publicity primes the customer, and then the advertising reinforces what the publicity stated. Campaigns must be planned well in advance. Publicity should precede tourism advertising for maximum impact as suggested by this research. Therefore, tourism marketing campaigns must be planned well in advance to take advantage of these findings. Publicity can play off the same message theme or concept as the advertising. However, the message for an advertising campaign must occur well enough in advance to allow for an aggressive publicity campaign to be developed and implemented before the advertising begins. Limitations This research is subject to two major limitations. It is limited to studying a tourism product promoted in print advertising and print publicity. In addition, as Lord and Putrevu (1993) state, the issues to be addressed in totally comparing advertising and publicity are broad and cannot be adequately treated in a single study. This study focuses on four informational constructs: message strength, perceived credibility, attitude toward the destination, and purchase intent. Hybrid messages and other forms of media are beyond the scope of this study. Suggestions for Further Research While implications reported here should be important in the communication and tourism fields, this study was limited by focusing only on print media, specifically magazines, with a student population in a laboratory setting concerning a tourism product. Based on this, the study needs to be replicated in other settings, with other populations, and with other products. The impact of influential media such as informal word of mouth, the Internet, hybrid messages, negative publicity, and other sources commonly encountered in tourism communication should be incorporated in future research. Certainly, measuring real behavior as opposed to purchase intent would increase the study’s importance. Therefore, longitudinal research is recommended to accurately capture the persuasiveness of tourism communication leading to behavioral change. In conclusion, this study supports previous anecdotal and inconclusive reports (Gartner 1993; Cameron 1994; Hallahan 1999) indicating that publicity is an important element in the marketing mix. It is hoped that tourism marketers will find benefit from this study by paying more attention to the positive effects of publicity and to the timing of messages within a campaign in an effort to maximize the effectiveness of tourism advertising and influence purchase intent. The most significant new finding suggests that a publicity-then-advertising sequence could be the most effective strategy for promoting tourism destinations. Tourism marketers need to develop marketing themes enough in advance to allow for a publicity campaign, using a similar message as the advertising, to precede ad placement. REFERENCES Ajzen, I., and B. Driver (1992). “Application of the Theory of Planned Behavior to Leisure Choice.” Journal of Leisure Research, 24 (3): 207–24. Anderson, N. H. (1971). “Integration Theory and Attitude Change.” Psychological Review, 78: 171–206. Anderson, P., and S. M. Abbott (1985). “Comparing Infomercial and Commercial on a Newspaper Cable Television Channel.” In Marketing Communications—Theory and Research, edited by Michael J. Houston and Richard J. Lutz. Chicago: American Marketing Association Proceeding Series, pp. 140–44. Cameron, G. T. (1994). “Does Publicity Outperform Advertising? An Experimental Test of the Third-Party Endorsement.” Journal of Public Relations Research, 6 (3): 185–207. Chaiken, S., and D. Maheswaran (1994). “Heuristic Processing Can Bias Systematic Processing: Effects of Source Credibility, Argument Ambiguity and Task Importance on Attitude Judgment.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66 (3): 460–73. Chew, C., M. S. Slater, and K. A. Kelly (1995). “Advertising versus Product Publicity: The Effects on Credibility and Purchase Intent.” Paper presented at the meeting of the Association for Education in Journalism and Mass Communication, August 1995, Washington, DC. Cho, M. (2001). “The Role of Prior Knowledge, Need for Information and Credibility of Information Sources in Tourists’ Information Search Behavior.” Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA. Darley, W. K., and R. E. Smith (1993). “Advertising Claim Objectivity: Antecedents and Effects.” Journal of Marketing, 57 (4): 100–13. d’Astous, A., and C. Hebert (1991). “Une Etude Comparative des Effets de la Publicité Ecrite Conventionnelle et du Public-Reportage.” In Marketing: Proceedings of the Annual Conference of the Administrative Sciences Association of Canada, edited by T. Schellinick. Niagara Falls, Ontario: Administrative Sciences Association of Canada, pp. 102–12. Dore, L., and G. I. Couch (2003). “Promoting Destinations: An Exploratory Study of Publicity Programmes Used by National Tourism Organizations.” Journal of Vacation Marketing, 9 (2): 137–51. Fishbein, M., and I. Ajzen (1975). Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Gartner, W. C. (1993). “Image Formation Process.” Journal of Travel & Tourism Marketing, 2: 191–215. Hallahan, K. (1995). “Content Class as a Contextual Cue in the Cognitive Processing of Publicity versus Advertising.” Journal of Public Relations Research, 11 (4): 293–320. ——— (1999). “No, Virginia, It’s Not True What They Say About Publicity’s ‘Implied Third-Party Endorsement’ Effect.” Public Relations Review, 25: 331–49. Hausknecht, D. M., J. B. Wilkinson, and G. E. Prough (1989). “Advertorials: Do Consumers See the Wolf in the Sheep’s Clothing?” In 1989 AMA Educators’ Proceedings: Enhancing Knowledge Development in Marketing, edited by P. Bloom. Chicago, IL: American Marketing Association, pp. 308–12. Hennessey, J. E., and S. C. Anderson (1990). “The Interaction of Peripheral Cues and Message Arguments on Cognitive Responses to an Advertisement.” In Advances in Consumer Research, edited by Marvin E. Goldbert, Gerald Corn, and Richard Pollay. Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, pp. 17, 237–43. Hunt, T., and J. E. Gruning (1993). Public Relations Techniques. Forth Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace College Publishers. Jamrozy, Ute (1996). The Effectiveness of Tourism Destination Advertisements: The Role of Feeling Responses. Dissertation. Kirk, R. E. (1982). Experimental Design. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole. Knipp, Helmut (1996). “Pouring Over Budget Line Items Will Cut Costs.” Hotel & Motel Management, 211 (8): 20–24. Kotler, P. (1993). Marketing Management: Analysis, Planning, Implementation and Control. 7th ed. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Kotler, P., J. Bowen, and J. Makens (1996). Marketing for Hospitality and Tourism. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. Lord, K.R., and S. Putrevu (1993). “Advertising and Publicity: An Information Processing Perspective.” Journal of Economic Psychology, 14: 57–84. MacKenzie, S. B., and R. J. Lutz (1989). “An Empirical Examination of the Structural Antecedents of Attitude toward the Ad in an Advertising Pretesting Context.” Journal of Marketing, 53 (2): 48–65. Miniard, P. W., S. Bhatla, and R. Rose (1990). “On the Formation and Relationship of Ad and Brand Attitudes: An Experimental and Causal Analysis.” Journal of Marketing Research, 27: 290–303. Osgood, C. E., G. J. Suci, and P. H. Tannenbaum (1967). The Measurement of Meaning. Urbana, IL: Univeristy of Illinois Press. Petty, R. E., and J. T. Cacioppo (1986). Communication and Persuasion: Central and Peripheral Routes to Attitude Change. New York/Berlin: Springer-Verlag. Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 11, 2016 JOURNAL OF TRAVEL RESEARCH 265 Petty, R. E., J. T. Cacioppo, and D. Schumann (1983). “Central and Peripheral Routes to Advertising Effectiveness: The Moderating Role of Involvement.” Journal of Consumer Research, 10: 135–46. Ray, M. L. (1992). Advertising and Communication Management. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Ries, A., and L. Ries (2002). The Fall of Advertising and the Rise of PR. New York: Harper Business. Salkind, N. J. (1994). Exploring Research. New York: Macmillan College Publishing Co. Salmon, C. T., L. N. Reid, J. Pokrwczynski, and R. Willett (1985). “The Effectiveness of Advocacy Advertising Relative to News Coverage.” Communication Research, 12: 546–67. Schwarz, N., M. Kumpf, and W. Bussmann (1986). “Resistance to Persuasion as a Consequence of Influence Attempts in Advertising and Non-Advertising Communications.” Psychology, A Quarterly Journal of Human Behavior, 23: 72–76. Smith, R. E. (1993). “Integrating Information from Advertising and Trial.” Journal of Marketing Research, 30: 204–19. Smith, R. E., and Hunt, S. D. (1978). “Attributional Processes and Effects in Promotional Situations.” Journal of Consumer Research, 5: 149–58. Smith, R. E., and W. R. Swinyard (1982). “Information Response Models: An Integrated Approach.” Journal of Marketing, 46 (1): 81–93. Smith, R. E., and C. A. Vogt (1995). “The Effects of Integrating Advertising and Negative Word-of-Mouth Communications on Message Processing and Response.” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 4 (2): 133–51. Straughan, D., G. L. Bleske, and X. Zhao (1994). “Modeling Format and Source Effects of an Advocacy Message.” Education in Journalism and Mass Communication, August, 46–52. Downloaded from jtr.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 11, 2016