Download The Influence of Target Properties and the Possible

Survey
yes no Was this document useful for you?
   Thank you for your participation!

* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project

Document related concepts

Visual selective attention in dementia wikipedia , lookup

Neuromarketing wikipedia , lookup

Visual search wikipedia , lookup

Visual servoing wikipedia , lookup

Time perception wikipedia , lookup

Visual N1 wikipedia , lookup

Response priming wikipedia , lookup

C1 and P1 (neuroscience) wikipedia , lookup

Point shooting wikipedia , lookup

Eye tracking wikipedia , lookup

Process tracing wikipedia , lookup

P200 wikipedia , lookup

Superior colliculus wikipedia , lookup

Transcript
Lokaverkefni til BS–prófs
í sálfræði
The Influence of Target Properties and
the Possible Lateralization of Saccades on Saccadic Parameters
Does the Size or Salience of a Target influence Latency and Peak Velocity,
and are there any Left-Right Asymmetries?
Þeódóra A. Thoroddsen
Maí 2015
Háskóli Íslands
Heilbrigðisvísindasvið
Sálfræðideild
!
!
!
!
!
!
The Influence of Target Properties and
the Possible Lateralization of Saccades on Saccadic Parameters
Does the Size or Salience of a Target influence Latency and Peak Velocity, and are there any Left-Right Asymmetries?
!
!
!
Þeódóra A. Thoroddsen
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
Lokaverkefni til BS-gráðu í sálfræði
Leiðbeinandi: Árni Kristjánsson
!
Sálfræðideild
Heilbrigðisvísindasvið Háskóla Íslands
Maí 2015
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
Ritgerð þessi er lokaverkefni til BS-gráðu í sálfræði og er óheimilt að afrita
ritgerðina á nokkurn hátt nema með leyfi rétthafa.
!
© Þeódóra A. Thoroddsen, 2015
!
Prentun: Svansprent
Reykjavík, Ísland 2015
Saccadic eye movements, which rapidly and accurately guide our gaze from one point
of interest to another, allow us to interact with our visual environment. Latency and
peak velocity are among the most studied parameters of saccades with their
characteristics pretty well known. However, much less is known about how the possible
lateralization of saccades and how target properties, such as size and salience, influence
these parameters. In this study we conducted a systematic investigation of how the size
and the salience of a target influence latencies and peak velocity of saccades, while
taking into account any modulatory effects of eye dominance and target location. We
investigated saccades to targets with seven different saliencies, ranging from almost
white to black, and to targets with seven different sizes, ranging from 0.2° to 1.4°. The
targets appeared unpredictably to the left or the right of a fixation with their centers 8°
away from the screens centre. Our results showed that latencies became shorter towards
larger and more salient targets, though peak velocity seemed to remain unaffected. Thus
it is important to take target properties into account when investigating saccadic
parameters. Our results also showed that for at least some people ocular dominance can
explain left-right asymmetries in peak velocity, and thus it is also important to consider
this effect before pooling data across the two saccade directions. !
!
Preface
The two experiments in this Bachelor's thesis were done in collaboration with Dr. Árni
Kristjánsson and Dr. Ómar I. Jóhannesson. The custom-made script Dr. Ómar I.
Jóhannesson made in Matlab, to analyze the data of the eye-tracker, made this project
possible. He also gave me an Icelandic guide to the statistical software R, which was of
great help in the statistical analyses of this project. He showed me patience and gave me
guidance, while I was getting accustomed to the software, which I am incredibly
grateful for. !
Table of Contents
!
Introduction …….………………..……………………………..…..…….……..…….5
Method …………………………….………………………….……….……………..10
Participants …………………….……………………….……………………….10
Research Design ……………….………………………..…..…………………..10
Stimuli ……………………………….…………………..…..…………….……10
Equipment/measuring devices ……….……………………..…..………….……11
Procedure …………………….…………………………..…….………….…….11
Statistical Analyses ………….……………………………..…..………….…….12
Results …………………………….…………………………..…..………….………13
Experiment 1 ………………………………………………………..……….…..13
Experiment 2 …………………………..…….…….………….……….…..…….15
Discussion ………………………………………………………….…..……….……17
Conclusions ……………………….…………..………….………………..…………19
References ………………………………………….……….…..…………..………..20
!
4
All we can do, whether it is perceive, learn, think, remember or act, is made possible by
the integrated activity of the nervous system cells and the body's main control centre,
the brain (Carlson, 2012). In conjunction they regulate both non-conscious and
voluntary processes. The brain even forms our consciousness, allowing us to think and
learn. The brain has four main structures: the cerebrum, the diencephalon, the
cerebellum and the brain stem. The largest main structure is the cerebrum, which is
divided into left and right cerebral hemispheres by a central groove (Parker, 2007). One of the fundamental characteristics of the brain is its hemispheric asymmetry, so
the question of cerebral dominance for perceptual and cognitive processes has been
central to much research in cognitive psychology and neurosciences. Two long-standing
examples of these asymmetries are that the left hemisphere specializes in language and
the right hemisphere in spatial processing (Dien, 2008; Serrien, Ivry, & Swinnen, 2006).
The cerebrum has a heavily folded pattern that is unique in each person. The
grooves in the pattern outline four functional areas called frontal lobe, parietal lobe,
temporal lobe and the occipital lobe. The occipital lobe, which contains most of the
anatomical region of the visual cortex, is almost entirely devoted to vision. This might
not be surprising since vision provides the brain with more input than all the other
senses combined and more than half of the information in consciousness seems to enter
through the eyes (Parker, 2007).
The sequence of vision is in short as follows: when rays of light enter the eye, they
are refracted by the cornea and fine-focused by the transparent lens. Then they go
through the vitreous humour and shine an upside-down image onto the retina lining.
The retina contains over 120 million cone cells and 7 million rod cells, which convert
the light energy into nerve signals. Rods do not differentiate colors like cones, but they
are light sensitive. The fovea is a small, central pit responsible for sharp central vision,
where the cones are closely packed. Intermediate retinal cells connect the rods and
cones to the optic nerve, which transmits the image to the visual cortex where it is
turned upright. The optic chiasm is a crossover junction at the base of the brain, where
the left and the right optic nerves converge before traveling to the visual cortex (Parker,
2007).
!
5
!
Each eye sees a slightly different view of what is in front of them, since the eyes
are set apart. These views become more different as the object gets closer. The visual
centers of the brain use this comparison to determine the distance of the object. The
combination into a single image is called binocular vision. The total field of binocular
vision is 180-200 degrees wide, giving each eye a visual field of 140-150 degrees. They
overlap by about 100 degrees directly in front, which allows the visual cortex to
compare them to estimate the depth and distance of the object (Parker, 2007). When
defining the ocular/sighting dominance of a person, that person is asked to align a target
with a distant point and choose the eye that is better suited for the task (Mapp, Ono, &
Barbeito, 2003).
When exploring the visual field, the eyes move to locate, see and track stimuli in
the environment. Saccadic eye movements or saccades, which rapidly and accurately
guide the gaze from one point of interest in the visual field to another, allow us to
interact with our visual environment. They are used to fixate on a target that is stable in
the visual field and they hold the object on the fovea while we shift our fixation from
one point to another. Saccadic movements can be horizontal, vertical or oblique. They
are described by their latency (the time before their execution), their metrics (their
direction and amplitude), and their dynamic properties (their velocity, acceleration,
etc.). Smooth pursuit eye movements, on the other hand, are used to track targets that
are moving by keeping them on the fovea (Leichnetz and Goldberg, 1987; MacAvoy,
Gottlieb and Bruce, 1991). In contrast to elicited saccades, smooth pursuit eye
movements have low velocities, proceed smoothly and usually travel ipsilaterally to the
stimulated area of the brain (MacAvoy, Gottlieb and Bruce, 1991).
The brain processes this visually-guided behavior after the corresponding visual
structures receive the signals from the retina (Boch, Fischer and Ramsperger, 1984;
Boch and Fischer, 1986). Saccades appear to be quite stereotypical behaviors which has
led to attempts to characterize the saccadic waveform with a limited number of
parameters, with the main ones being velocity, amplitude and duration (Collewijn,
Erkelens, & Steinman, 1988). !
6
Peak velocity marks the highest velocity that the eye reaches during a saccade. The
relation between peak velocity and amplitude, or the “main sequence,” has been thought
of as a linear relationship, where an increase in peak velocity is a function of saccadic
amplitude for smaller saccades with a softer saturation (Bahill, Clark & Stark, 1975).
The actual value of peak velocity and the level of saturation can still vary greatly
(Bahill, Brockenbrough, & Troost, 1981; Boghen et al., 1974; Baloh et al., 1975). The
source of this problem is probably the variety in methods of recording and data analysis.
Among components that influence peak velocity are the directions of the saccades.
Adducting saccades tend to have higher peak velocities than abducting ones.
Centripedal (towards the primary position) saccades also tend to have higher peak
velocities than centrifugal ones (Collewijn, Erkelens, & Steinman, 1988).
The latency, sometimes called reaction time, of a saccade is the time from the target
onset to the initiation of a saccade. The elaboration of visual information, at different
stages of the cortical and subcortical level, takes time and can be reflected in the
reaction time of a saccade (Liversedge, Gilchrist, & Everling, 2011). The minimum
saccadic reaction time can be estimated by considering the neural delays in the shortest
pathway from the retina, through the cortex and down to the oculomotor plant. Break of
previous fixation, decision making and computation of size and direction are elements
of the saccadic reaction time process. Delay times, due to the type of stimulus, also have
to be taken into account (Boch, Fischer and Ramsperger, 1984; Boch and Fischer,
1986). The strength and type of a stimulus can have a great deal of impact, where the high-
contrast stimuli signals travel through the sensory system more quickly, resulting in less
sensory delay before the rise-to-threshold can start, than low-contrast stimuli. Likewise,
chromatic stimuli tend to elicit longer latency than luminance stimuli. Retinal
eccentricity also affects latency, with latency increasing as the target is located further
from the fovea. This function is actually U-shaped, meaning that the shortest latencies
would be for targets within about 2° and 10° from the fovea and relatively longer ones
within 1° and 2° (Liversedge, Gilchrist, & Everling, 2011). Target location probabilities
(Liu et al., 2010) and attentional load (Kristjánsson, Chen, & Nakayama, 2001) are also
factors that might affect saccadic reaction times.
7
Saccades have been classified as regular saccades, express saccades and
microsaccades. Regular saccades have reaction times between 150-300 ms with the
variation of about 20 ms or more. Microsaccades share the same characteristics as
regular saccades but have very low amplitude and are very short. Express saccades are
minimal latency saccades that are 70-80 ms in monkeys and around 100 ms in humans.
They are reflexive, goal directed and occur if a previous fixation point disappears some
time before a new target emerges, leaving a gap between them (Boch, Fischer and
Ramsperger, 1984; Boch and Fischer, 1986). When a central fixation point is turned off before the peripheral target appears,
shorter reaction times can be produced compared to when the fixation point overlaps
with the target (Fischer & Ramsperger, 1984). If the gap between the disappearance of
the fixation point and the appearance of the target is 200 ms or more, saccadic latency is
about 150 ms. If the fixation point disappears and the target appears simultaneously,
latency increases to about 200 ms and if an overlap occurs, latency increases to about
250 ms. This has been named the “gap effect” (Saslow, 1967).
The emphasis in this thesis is on regular saccades, which can be classified as anti-
and prosaccades. A prosaccade is made when a peripherally stimulus presents itself and
you saccade towards it. For example if a stimulus appears 8° to the right of a fixation
point, and you saccade towards it, you would be making a prosaccade. The amplitude of
the saccade would be 8° if the stimulus appears 8° away from the fixation point. An
antisaccade is a saccade in the opposite direction of a prosaccade that still has the same
amplitude. In the case above an antisaccade would be a saccade 8° to the left of the
fixation point (Jóhannesson, 2012). Latencies of antisaccades are usually longer than of
prosaccades, but several factors have been found which can slow prosaccades and speed
antisaccades (see Kristjánsson, Vanderbroucke & Driver, 2004; Kristjánsson, Chen &
Nakayama, 2001; Liu et al. 2010).
Earlier studies about the characteristics of saccades have shown that rightward
saccades have shorter latencies than leftward saccades, indicating that there is
a structural determinant for asymmetries in saccadic latencies (Pirozzolo & Rayner,
1980; Hutton & Palet, 1986). However, the view of hemispheric specialization in
saccadic control remains under debate, since the asymmetry in saccade latency has not
always been demonstrated.
8
A study by Weber and Fischer (1995) demonstrated leftward and rightward
saccadic latency differences for reactive saccades, but they were only elicited by a gap
of 100 or 200 ms indicating that it had something to do with the different proportion of
express saccades. A later study by Honda (2002) failed to demonstrate asymmetry in
saccade latency in data averaged over subjects, but managed to find it at the individual
level in tasks involving reactive or voluntary saccades. Yet other studies have not
managed to find any differences in latencies (De Clerck, Crevits, & Van Maele, 2000;
Constantinidis et al., 2003).
The potential role of eye dominance has also been raised. One study (Lazarev &
Kirenskaya, 2008) showed shorter latencies for leftward saccades in a right-eyed group
but no differences in a left-eyed group, yet another (Kolesnikova, Tereshchenko,
Latanov, & Shulgovskii, 2010) showed shorter latencies for leftward saccades in a lefteyed group but less clear results in a right-eyed group. The possible occurrence of spatial asymmetry in other parameters than latency has
not yet been fully considered. A study by Vergilino-Perez et al. (2012) tackled this
problem and consistently showed that both right-eyed groups made faster rightward
saccades, whereas both left-eyed groups made faster leftward saccades, indicating that
the saccades directed in the ipsilateral direction relative to the dominant eye sometimes
had faster peak velocities. This suggests that eye dominance is the main factor in this
asymmetry. As previous studies they found no evidence in favor of left-right
asymmetries in saccade latency.
It is difficult to draw conclusions about the possible lateralization of saccades since
there is such lack of consistent results, and thus the question still remains of whether
hemispheric specialization exists for the saccadic system. Latency and peak velocity are
among the most studied parameters of saccadic eye movements with their
characteristics pretty well know. However, much less is known about how target
properties, such as size and salience, influence them. Thus, in this study we conducted a
systematic investigation of how the size and the salience of a target influence latencies
and peak velocity of reactive saccades, while taking into account any modulatory effects
of eye dominance and target location.
!
9
Method
Participants
Participants in both experiments were unpaid volunteers naive to the aim of the
experiment. They were students of psychology at the University of Iceland and received
course credit for participating. All reported normal or corrected to normal vision. The
first experiment consisted of 10 participants (7 females and 3 males) with the mean age
of 24.8, ranging from 22 to 43 years. Two had a dominant left eye and eight had a
dominant right eye. The second experiment consisted of 10 participants (9 females and
1 male) with the mean age of 26, ranging from 22 to 45 years. Four had a dominant left
eye and six had a dominant right eye. !
Research Design
Two within-subject design experiments were conducted. In the first one the independent
variables were the size of the stimulus, which had seven levels ranging from 0.2° to
1.4°, the ocular dominance and the target location. The dependent variables were the
reaction time and peak velocity of the observers. In the second one the independent
variables were the salience of the stimulus, which had seven levels ranging form almost
white to black, the ocular dominance and the target location. The dependent variables
were also the reaction times and peak velocity of the observers.
!
Stimuli
In both experiments observers fixated on a 0.3° dot in the center of the display that
disappeared after 1000 – 1300 ms. The targets then appeared unpredictably to the left or
the right of the fixation with their centers 8° away from the screens center. Each target
lasted for 300 ms, after the eye fixated on it, before disappearing. In experiment 1 the
target varied randomly in size between 0.2°, 0.4°, 0.6°, 0.8°, 1.0°, 1.2° and 1.4°. The
RGB value of the screen was [255, 255, 255] and the value of the target was [0, 0, 0]. In
experiment 2 the size of the target was 1.0° and it varied in brightness form almost
white, on a white background, to black. The RGB value of the screen was [255, 255,
255] and the value of the target randomly changed between [0, 0, 0], [40, 40, 40], [80,
80, 80], [120, 120, 120], [160, 160, 160], [200, 200, 200] and [240, 240, 240]. !
10
Equipment/measuring devices
In both experiments a high speed (250 Hz, one datapoint every fourth ms) monocular
video eye-tracker from Cambridge research Systems was used. The spatial accuracy of
the tracker is about 0.125°-0.25°, with the horizontal range being ±40° and the vertical
being ±20°. The eye-tracker uses infrared light (930 nm), that comes from 2 diodes
identically positioned on each side of the infrared camera, which provides two sets of
the first Purkinje image, each opposite to the corresponding diode, to keep track of the
gaze. The eye-tracker software computes the length from the centre of the pupil to both
of the first Purkinje image changes, when the eye rotates, to compute the point of gaze.
Chin and forehead rests were used to minimize head movements, even though the eyetracker can correct those movements up to ±10 mm (Cambridge Research Systems,
2006). The viewing distance in both experiments was 53 cm.
Both experiments were run on the same Dell computer. The Intel Core Duo is 2.33
GHz, the working memory is 1.95 GB and the operating system is Microsoft Windows
XP 2002. In both experiments a 100 Hz, 19” Hansol CRT monitor (model 920D with
maximal resolution of 1280 x 1024 px) was used. When a target emerges on the screen a
signal is sent immediately to the eye-tracker, which is used as a timestamp for when the
target appeared, so that the x and y coordinates of the gaze are known with 4 ms
precision. The main software used was Matlab (v. 7.11.0, 2010b, 32 bit). The experiments
were programmed there and a custom-made program was used to analyze the
eye-tracker data. PsychToolBox was used to display the targets and keep track of when
they emerged and when they disappeared. A toolbox from Cambridge Research System
(2006) for Matlab was used to provide information about the x and y coordinates.
!
Procedure
In both experiments participants were led to a sound-proof booth with an eye-tracker
and inquired about their dominant eye. The eye-tracker was then adjusted accordingly
and the movement of the dominant eye of each participant was recorded while they used
their binocular vision. !
11
Observers were required to fixate on a small dot at the center of the display for
1000-1300 ms, after which the fixation point disappeared. Targets appeared
unpredictably to the left or the right of the fixation with their centers 8° away from the
screens centre and remained on the screen for 300 ms. Participants were asked to
respond as quickly and accurately as possible, and to return their fixation to the center
as soon as the target disappeared. If a saccade landed outside a circular region of 1.2°,
centered at the center of the screen, a warning message appeared telling the observer to
look directly at the center.
The first experiment assessed latencies and peak velocity of various target sizes,
ranging from 0.2° to 1.4° in seven steps, which appeared at random after the fixation
point disappeared. The second experiment assessed latencies and peak velocity of
various target saliency, ranging from almost white to black in seven steps, which also
appeared at random after the fixation point disappeared. The session in both
experiments consisted of 4 blocks, with 140 trials per block, and lasted about 30 to 40
minutes. Participants were told they could take breaks between blocks as needed.
!
Statistical Analyses
A custom-made script was written by Dr. Ómar I. Jóhannesson in Matlab to analyze the
data of the eye-tracker. The statistical program R 3.1.3, and its corresponding libraries,
were used in the statistical analyses. If a saccade latency was shorter than 100 ms, it was
considered anticipatory instead of stimulus driven and thus excluded from all analyses.
The mean of each task for each participant was computed and the saccades that deviated
more than 3 standard deviations (SD) from the corresponding mean were excluded and
thought of as the upper limit of valid saccades. The dominant and non-dominant eyes
had no performance differences, so the data for the eyes were combined. In both
experiments repeated measure ANOVAs, with the aov function, were used to compare
differences in the dependent measures with respect to the levels of the experimental
conditions. Tukey's post hoc tests were performed based on significant main effects; pvalues < .05 were considered statistically significant. The target location on the screen
(left or right) were also compared, with a paired t-test, to see if the location might
influence the dependent measures.
!
12
Results
Trials with saccadic latencies shorter than 100 ms and longer than 3 SD from the mean,
for each participant and task, where excluded from all analyses (1.4% of the data).
Repeated measure ANOVAs were used in both experiments, along with Tukey's posthoc comparison when appropriate. Two sample t-tests were used to see if there were any
performance differences for the dominant and non-dominant eye, and paired t-tests were
used to see whether the target location had any influences. !
Experiment 1
The main effect of the seven target sizes (0.2°, 0.4°, 0.6°, 0.8°, 1.0°, 1.2°, 1.4°) on the
mean reaction time was statistically significant (F(6,54) = 6.71; p < .001), with Tukey's
post-hoc comparisons indicating that the 0.2° target size had a significantly longer
latency than the rest at p < .001. Comparisons between the other six target sizes were
not significant. !
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
Figure 1. The mean reaction time of the seven target sizes. Latencies became shorter
towards larger target sizes, with the 0.2° target size having the longest latency
(M = 176 ms).
13
The dominant and non-dominant eyes had no significant performance differences in
reaction time (t(8) = .41; p = .692) and the target location did not significantly influence
the reaction time (t(9) = -.43; p = .674).
The main effect of the seven target sizes on peak velocity was not significant
(F(6, 54) = .32; p = .926), where peak velocity varied from 338°/sec to 340°/sec, and
the dominant and non-dominant eyes had no significant performance differences in peak
velocity either (t(8) = 1.02; p = .336). The target location, on the other hand, did significantly influence peak velocity
(t(9) = -3.64; p = .005), where peak velocity became higher when the target was to the
right of the screen (355°/sec) compared to the left of the screen (324°/sec). When the
observers were separated by their dominant eye, the peak velocity difference by target
location was not significant for the left-eyed group (t(1) = -.55; p = .679), but it was
significant for the right-eyed group (t(7) = -3.99; p = .005). No interaction effect was
found between the target location and the size of the target for the right-eyed group
(F(6, 49) = .30; p = .935).
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
Figure 2. Peak velocity by target location in the right-eyed group. Peak velocity became
higher when the target was to the right of the screen (352°/sec) compared to the left of the
screen (318°/sec).
14
Experiment 2
The main effect of the seven target saliencies ([0, 0, 0], [40, 40, 40], [80, 80, 80], [120,
120, 120], [160, 160, 160], [200, 200, 200], [240, 240, 240]) on the mean reaction time
was significant (F(6,54) = 12.25; p < .001). Tukey's post-hoc comparisons of the seven
target saliencies indicate that the least salient target ([0, 0, 0]) had a significantly longer
latency than the rest at p < .001, but comparisons between the other six target saliencies
were not significant.
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
Figure 3. The mean reaction time of the seven target saliencies. Latencies became shorter
towards more salient targets, where “1” represents the least salient target ([0, 0, 0]) and
“7” the most salient target ([240, 240, 240]), with the least salient target having the
longest latency (M = 186 ms). !
The dominant and non-dominant eyes had no significant performance differences in
reaction time (t(8) = -1.82; p = .106) and the target location did not significantly
influence it either (t(9) = -1.19; p = .264).
!
15
Finally, the main effect of the seven target saliencies on peak velocity was
significant (F(6,54) = 3.47; p = .006), but Tukey's post-hoc comparisons of the seven
target saliencies did not yield any significant differences in peak velocity, so the
difference was deemed inconclusive. !
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
Figure 4. Peak velocity by target salience, where “1” represents the least salient target
([0, 0, 0]) and “7” the most salient target ([240, 240, 240]). Peak velocity varies from
!
336°/sec to 347°/sec between the seven target saliencies.
The dominant and non-dominant eyes had no significant performance differences in
peak velocity (t(8) = -1.83; p = .105) and whether the target was to the left or to the
right on the screen did not significantly influence peak velocity (t(9) = .13; p = .899). !
16
Discussion
Latency, velocity, amplitude and duration are among the most studied parameters of
saccadic eye movements, so their characteristics are pretty well known (Collewijn,
Erkelens, & Steinman, 1988). However, much less is known about the influence of
target properties on these parameters. The main purpose of his study was to conduct a
systematic investigation of how target properties, like size and salience, influence
latencies and peak velocity of reactive saccades. The strength and type of a stimulus
have been shown to have an impact on latency, where for example high-contrast or
luminance stimuli have less sensory delay than low-contrast or chromatic stimuli
(Liversedge, Gilchrist, & Everling, 2011). In our study we investigated saccades to
targets with seven different saliencies ranging from almost white, on a white
background, to black and to targets with seven different sizes ranging from 0.2° to 1.4°.
Our results showed that latencies became shorter towards larger target sizes, with the
smallest (0.2°) target having the longest mean latency (176 ms), and also became
shorter towards more salient targets, with the least salient one having the longest mean
latency (186 ms). However, our results showed barely any differences in peak velocity
for both experiment 1 (the size study), where the peak velocity varied from 338°/sec to
340°/sec, and experiment 2 (the salience study), where the peak velocity varied from
336°/sec to 347°/sec. Thus peak velocity seems to remain unaffected by the size and
salience of a stimulus. These results stress the importance of knowledge about the
effects of stimulus properties on saccadic parameters for future investigations of these
parameters. It has been difficult to draw conclusions about the possible lateralization of
saccades since there is such lack of consistent results (Pirozzolo & Rayner, 1980;
Hutton & Palet, 1986; Weber & Fischer, 1995; Honda, 2002). The question still remains
of whether spatial asymmetry in parameters like latency and peak velocity exists for the
saccadic system, so we decided to see if eye dominance and target location had any
modulatory effects on peak velocity and latency in our study. As many previous studies
(De Clerck, Crevits, & Van Maele, 2000; Constantinidis et al., 2003; Vergilino-Perez et
al., 2012) no modulatory effects were found for latency in neither experiment. No
performance differences were found between the dominant and non-dominant eyes, nor
any influences of target location on peak velocity in experiment 2 (the salience study). 17
The target location did, on the other hand, influence peak velocity in experiment 1
(the size study), where peak velocity became higher when the target was to the right of
the screen compared to the left of the screen. With further inspection, the right-eyed
group had higher peak velocity when the target was to the right of the screen indicating
that the saccades directed in the ipsilateral direction relative to the dominant eye have
faster peak velocities, which is consistent with the results of Vergilino-Perez et al.
(2012). However, the left-eyed group showed no differences in peak velocity, which
might have been due to the fact that only 2 out of the 10 participants were left-eyed. A
future study might include more participants with a more even division of ocular
dominance. Another reason why only a part of the observers showed this pattern might
be because some people show clear ocular dominance, whereas other people may
present an alternating balance between both eyes (Mapp, Ono, & Barbeito, 2003; Seijas
et al., 2007). This study shows that at least for some people ocular dominance can
explain this left-right asymmetry in peak velocity, so it appears important from a
methodological point of view to consider the effect of ocular dominance before pooling
data across the two saccade directions, as is commonly done.
!
18
Conclusions
We conclude that latency can be influenced by target properties such as size and
salience, while peak velocity seems to remain unaffected, and thus it is important to
take these properties into account when investigating saccadic parameters. We also
conclude that at least for some people ocular dominance can explain left-right
asymmetries in peak velocity, and thus it is also important to consider the effect of
ocular dominance before pooling data across the two saccade directions.
!
!
!
19
References
Bahill, A. T., Brockenbrough, A., & Troost, B. T. (1981). Variability and development of
a normative data base for saccadic eye movements. Investigative Ophthalmology
& Visual Science, 21(1), 116-125.
Bahill, A. T., Clark, M. R., & Stark, L. (1975). The main sequence, a tool for studying
human eye movements. Mathematical Biosciences, 24(3), 191-204.
Baloh, R. W., Sills, A. W., Kumley, W. E., & Honrubia, V. (1975). Quantitative
measurement of saccade amplitude, duration, and velocity. Neurology, 25(11),
1065-1065.
Boch, R., & Fischer, B. (1986). Further observations on the occurrence of expresssaccades in the monkey. Experimental Brain Research, 63(3), 487-494.
Boch, R., Fischer, B., & Ramsperger, E. (1984). Express-saccades of the monkey:
reaction times versus intensity, size, duration, and eccentricity of their targets.
Experimental Brain Research, 55(2), 223-231.
Boghen, D., Troost, B. T., Daroff, R. B., Dell'Osso, L. F., & Birkett, J. E. (1974).
Velocity characteristics of normal human saccades. Investigative Ophthalmology
& Visual Science, 13(8), 619-623.
Cambridge Research Systems (2006). Video eyetracker toolbox. User Manual.
Rochester: Author.
Carlson, N. R. (2012). Physiology of Behavior 11th Edition. Pearson.
Collewijn, H., Erkelens, C. J., & Steinman, R. M. (1988). Binocular co-ordination of
human horizontal saccadic eye movements. The Journal of Physiology, 404(1),
157-182.
Constantinidis, T. S., Smyrnis, N., Evdokimidis, I., Stefanis, N. C., Avramopoulos, D.,
Giouzelis, I., & Stefanis, C. N. (2003). Effects of direction on saccadic
performance in relation to lateral preferences. Experimental brain research,
150(4), 443-448.
De Clerck, M., Crevits, L., & Van Maele, G. (2000). Saccades: is there a difference
between right and left? Neuro-Ophthalmology, 24(2), 327-330.
Dien, J. (2008). Looking both ways through time: the Janus model of lateralized
cognition. Brain and cognition, 67(3), 292-323.
20
Fischer, B., & Ramsperger, E. (1984). Human express saccades: extremely short
reaction times of goal directed eye movements. Experimental Brain Research,
57(1), 191-195.
Honda, H. (2002). Idiosyncratic left–right asymmetries of saccadic latencies:
examination in a gap paradigm. Vision research, 42(11), 1437-1445.
Hutton, J. T., & Palet, J. (1986). Lateral saccadic latencies and handedness.
Neuropsychologia, 24(3), 449-451.
Kolesnikova, O. V., Tereshchenko, L. V., Latanov, A. V., & Shulgovskii, V. V. (2010).
Effects of visual environment complexity on saccade performance in humans with
different functional asymmetry profiles. Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology,
40(8), 869-876.
Kristjánsson, Á., Chen, Y., & Nakayama, K. (2001). Less attention is more in the
preparation of antisaccades, but not prosaccades. Nature Neuroscience, 4(10),
1037–1042.
Kristjánsson, Á., Vandenbroucke, M. W. G., & Driver, J. (2004). When pros become
cons for anti-versus prosaccades: Factors with opposite or common effects on
different saccade types. Experimental Brain Research, 155(2), 231–244.
Lazarev, I. E., & Kirenskaya, A. V. (2008). The influence of eye dominance on saccade
characteristics and slow presaccadic potentials. Human Physiology, 34(2),
150-160.
Leichnetz, G. R., & Goldberg, M. E. (1987). Higher centers concerned with eye
movement and visual attention: cerebral cortex and thalamus. Reviews of
Oculomotor Research, 2, 365-429.
Liu, C.-L., Chiau, H.-Y., Tseng, P., Hung, D. L., Tzeng, O. J. L., Muggleton, N. G., &
Juan, C.-H. (2010). Antisaccade cost is modulated by contextual experience of
location probability. Journal of Neurophysiology, 103(3), 1438–1447.
Liversedge, S., Gilchrist, I., & Everling, S. (Eds.). (2011). The Oxford Handbook of Eye
Movements. Oxford University Press.
MacAvoy, M. G., Gottlieb, J. P., & Bruce, C. J. (1991). Smooth-pursuit eye movement
representation in the primate frontal eye field. Cerebral Cortex, 1(1), 95-102.
21
Mapp, A. P., Ono, H., & Barbeito, R. (2003). What does the dominant eye dominate? A
brief and somewhat contentious review. Perception & Psychophysics, 65(2),
310-317.
Jóhannesson, Ó. I. (2012). Saccade performance in the nasal and temporal hemifields
(Unpublished Master’s thesis). The University of Iceland, Reykjavik. Parker, S. (2007). The Human Body Book. London: Dorling Kindersley Limited. Pirozzolo, F. J., & Rayner, K. (1980). Handedness, hemispheric specialization and
saccadic eye movement latencies. Neuropsychologia, 18(2), 225-229.
Saslow, M. G. (1967). Effects of components of displacement-step stimuli upon latency
for saccadic eye movement. JOSA, 57(8), 1024-1029.
Seijas, O., de Liaño, P. G., de Liaño, R. G., Roberts, C. J., Piedrahita, E., & Diaz, E.
(2007). Ocular dominance diagnosis and its influence in monovision. American
Journal of Ophthalmology, 144(2), 209-216.
Serrien, D. J., Ivry, R. B., & Swinnen, S. P. (2006). Dynamics of hemispheric
specialization and integration in the context of motor control. Nature Reviews
Neuroscience, 7(2), 160-166.
Stanton, G. B., Deng, S. Y., Goldberg, E. M., & McMullen, N. T. (1989).
Cytoarchitectural characteristic of the frontal eye fields in macaque monkeys.
Journal of Comparative Neurology, 282(3), 415-427.
Vergilino-Perez, D., Fayel, A., Lemoine, C., Senot, P., Vergne, J., & Doré-Mazars, K.
(2012). Are there any left-right asymmetries in saccade parameters? Examination
of latency, gain, and peak velocity. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science
53(7), 3340-3348.
Weber, H., & Fischer, B. (1995). Gap duration and location of attention focus modulate
the occurrence of left/right asymmetries in the saccadic reaction times of human
subjects. Vision Research, 35(7), 987-998.
22