* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Reflex Facilitation During Eyeblink Conditioning and Subsequent
Learning theory (education) wikipedia , lookup
Behaviorism wikipedia , lookup
Milgram experiment wikipedia , lookup
Human vestigiality wikipedia , lookup
Psychophysics wikipedia , lookup
Experimental psychology wikipedia , lookup
Psychological behaviorism wikipedia , lookup
Operant conditioning wikipedia , lookup
Behavioral Neuroscience 2002, Vol. 116, No. 6, 1052–1058 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0735-7044/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0735-7044.116.6.1052 Reflex Facilitation During Eyeblink Conditioning and Subsequent Interpositus Nucleus Inactivation in the Rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus) Jan Wikgren Timo Ruusuvirta University of Jyväskylä University of Helsinki Tapani Korhonen University of Jyväskylä In eyeblink conditioning in the rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus), not only is a conditioned response (CR) acquired, but also the original reflex is modified as a function of training. In Experiment 1, by comparing unconditioned responses in unpaired and paired groups, 3 types of reflex facilitation were distinguished. One type was linked to exposure to the unconditioned stimuli (USs) and/or experimental setting. The 2nd type was related to the formation of the memory trace for conditioned eyeblink. The 3rd type was linked to the conditioned stimulus immediately preceding the US in the paired group. In Experiment 2, reversible inactivation of the interpositus nucleus (IPN) abolished the CR and reduced the CR-related reflex facilitation, indicating that the latter depends on the plasticity of the IPN. by reflex modification, a change in the unconditioned responses (URs; Harvey, Gormezano, & Cool-Hauser, 1985; Weisz & LoTurco, 1988; Weisz & McInerney, 1990; Weisz & Walts, 1990). This is defined as a change in the latency and/or amplitude of the UR. In the case of eyeblink conditioning with a relatively short interstimulus interval (less than 1 s), modification in the form of reflex facilitation occurs (Schreurs, Oh, Hirashima, & Alkon, 1995). The previous findings indicate the existence of at least three types of reflex facilitation, the first type being linked simply to mere exposure to the experimental setting (including presentations of the US; Schreurs et al., 1995). This type could be termed experience-related reflex facilitation, as it refers to reflex facilitation that is not related to the forward CS–US pairings. The second type is linked to the temporal proximity of the CS, causing greater URs in paired trials as compared with US-alone trials, even in the phase of learning at which CRs have not yet emerged (e.g., Weisz & LoTurco, 1988). We refer to this as CS-mediated reflex facilitation, which is most likely linked to emotive learning of the association between the CS and the aversiveness of the US. The third type occurs in the phase of learning at which CRs emerge. It is linked to URs in US-alone trials, which are higher in their amplitude after extensive training than before such training (Schreurs et al., 1995). As this type of reflex facilitation is most probably linked to sensory learning, it is termed CR-related reflex facilitation. The contribution of emotive learning in reflex facilitation is further supported by the finding that whereas the interpositus nucleus (IPN) is necessary for the conditioned eyeblink response (e.g., Anderson & Steinmetz, 1994; Steinmetz, Lavond, Ivkovich, Logan, & Thompson, 1992; Weisz & LoTurco, 1988), the amygdala is involved in both emotive learning and reflex facilitation (Weisz, Harden, & Xiang, 1992). The present study examines different types of reflex facilitation within a single experiment with the aim of excluding those types of reflex facilitation that are dependent on the sensorimotor learn- Different dichotomies of associations during classical conditioning have been proposed, such as diffuse– discrete, preparatory– consummatory, or autonomic–somatic (Brandon & Wagner, 1991; Konorski, 1967). The rationale for these dichotomies is that in any conditioning procedure that involves an emotionally significant unconditioned stimulus (US), there are at least two types of learning taking place in parallel: emotive and sensorimotor (Brandon & Wagner, 1991). These types, it has been suggested, are relatively independent and differ in terms of the brain sites critical to their emergence (cerebellum for sensorimotor learning, amygdala and related areas for emotive learning). However, it has been demonstrated that both types of learning are involved in learning that seems only sensorimotor in nature, such as classical conditioning of the eyeblink response in rabbits (Gormezano, Schneiderman, Deaux, & Fuentes, 1962). This involves the pairing of a conditioned stimulus (CS; e.g., a tone) with a US (e.g., an airpuff toward the cornea). Repetitious temporal forward pairing of these stimuli results in the acquisition of a conditioned response (CR), that is, a movement of the nictitating membrane (NM) as a response to the CS. For example, Brandon and Wagner (1991) demonstrated the involvement of emotive learning in eyeblink conditioning by finding that the context CS (the emotive CS) potentiated the learning of the eyeblink reflex to a new CS. In addition to changes in CR amplitude, another means of studying the emotive learning in eyeblink conditioning is provided Jan Wikgren and Tapani Korhonen, Department of Psychology, University of Jyväskylä, Jyväskylä, Finland; Timo Ruusuvirta, Cognitive Brain Research Unit, Department of Psychology, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland. This study was supported by the Graduate School of Psychology, Turku, Finland. We thank Lauri Viljanto, Asko Tolvanen, Piia Astikainen, Satu Barman, and Michael Freeman for their help. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jan Wikgren, Department of Psychology, University of Jyväskylä, P.O. Box 35, 40351 Jyväskylä, Finland. E-mail: [email protected] 1052 REFLEX FACILITATION IN EYEBLINK CONDITIONING ing circuit (IPN). In Experiment 1, we compared the development of reflex facilitation over the time course of the paired treatment with its development over the time course of the explicitly unpaired treatment. In Experiment 2, we reversibly inactivated the IPN by a cold probe in well-trained rabbits to reveal those types of reflex facilitation that are linked to the normal functioning of this nucleus. We made four predictions. First, some reflex facilitation should occur solely because of the experience of the experimental setting during both treatments (experience-related reflex facilitation). Second, in the paired treatment, reflex facilitation should be more vigorous when the CS immediately precedes the US than when the US is presented alone (CS-mediated reflex facilitation). Third, after a robust level of CR is achieved in the paired treatment, reflex facilitation should be higher in the US-alone trials than in the corresponding trials in the unpaired treatment (CR-related reflex facilitation). Fourth, the reversible inactivation of the IPN in well-trained rabbits should affect CR-related reflex facilitation but not necessarily CS-mediated reflex facilitation. Experiment 1 Method Subjects. The subjects were 21 adult New Zealand albino rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) weighing 2.5–3.7 kg at the time of surgery. The animals were individually housed in metal cages on a 12:12-hr light-dark cycle, with free access to food and water. All the experiments were carried out in accordance with the European Union Directive 86/609/EEC regarding the care and use of animals for experimental procedures (Netherlands Centre Alternatives to Animal Use, 2002). The Ethics Committee for Animal Research of the University of Jyväskylä, Jyväskylä, Finland gave its consent to the study. Surgery. The animals were anesthetized with intramuscular injections of a ketamine–xylazine cocktail (Ketaminol, 50 mg/ml, 5.6 ml; Rompun, 20 mg/ml, 2.2 ml; physiological saline, 2.2 ml). The initial dosage was 3– 4 ml, and the anesthesia was maintained by additional injections of 2 ml every 20 – 40 min. After a deep general anesthesia had been achieved, we placed the animals in a stereotaxic instrument (David Kopf Instruments, Tujunga, CA) with the bregma 1.5 mm above the lambda. A longitudinal incision was made to reveal the skull, onto which the headstage (designed to hold the minitorque potentiometer, an airpuff delivery nozzle and tone tubing) was cemented using four stainless steel anchoring screws. Cold probes were implanted only for the unpaired group (they served as subjects in Experiment 2). It is our experience that implantation of a cold probe near the IPN does not affect subjects’ learning capabilities (in Experiment 2, their learning curve was normal). As both groups otherwise underwent the same surgical protocols, there should be no other differences between the groups than those caused by their respective experimental treatments. The construction of the cold probe was based on that presented by Zhang, Ni, and Harper (1986). The shaft of the probe was not warmed, as it has been shown that lesioning the cerebellar lobule HVI, which the probe penetrated, does not interfere with the acquisition of the conditioned eyeblink response (R. E. Clark, Zhang, & Lavond, 1992). In short, the cold probe consists of two stainless steel tubes, one inside the other. The inner tube delivers the coolant at a distance of 1 mm from the tip of the probe, which is sealed by solder. The coolant exits through a plastic tube attached to the outer cannula at a Y-shaped junction. The coolant used was freonlike 1,1,1,2-tetrafluoroethane (KLEA R-134-A). The implantation followed a method of recording-electrode implantation proposed by Korhonen (1991). The cold probe was implanted near the IPN using the coordinates of 0.5 mm anterior and 5.0 mm lateral to the lambda. Anal- 1053 gesics (Temgesic, 0.3 mg/ml) were provided 2 hr after surgery and additionally if needed. Experimental procedure. The subjects were given at least 1 week to recover after surgery before the commencement of the experimental procedures. On the first day, adaptation to the experimental situation was done by placing the animals in a Plexiglas restraining box in a soundproof conditioning chamber. The rabbits were divided in two groups: the unpaired (UP) group (n ⫽ 9) and the classical conditioning (CC) group (n ⫽ 11). A UP session consisted of 70 presentations of the tone (1000 Hz, 85 dB, 350 ms) and 70 presentations of the airpuff (2.1 N/cm2 source pressure, 100 ms) given in a pseudorandom order with an intertrial interval (ITI) varying between 15 and 25 s (mean ITI ⫽ 20 s). A CC session consisted of 60 CC trials, in which the tone was followed by the airpuff and 10 CS-alone and 10 US-alone test trials in a pseudorandom order. The paired trials in the CC group were presented in a delayed fashion so that the stimuli coterminated. The ITI varied between 30 and 50 s (mean ITI ⫽ 40 s). The subjects were treated for five successive daily sessions. Peak amplitude values for CRs and URs were defined as the maximum extension of the NM during a period of 250 ms immediately following the CS or US. Any movement of the NM exceeding 0.5 mm was counted as a response. Trials in which NM movement exceeded 0.3 mm during the 100 ms immediately prior to the trial were excluded from the analysis. Statistical procedures. URs were analyzed with respect to the different trial types in the three phases of the experiment. The trial types consisted of the US presented alone in the UP group (US/UP) and in the CC group (US/CC) and the US paired with the CS in the CC group (CS ⫹ US/CC). The phases of the experiment consisted of the first trial in the first session (Phase A), the last trial in the first session (Phase B), and the last trial in the last session (Phase C). The last trial in the last session in the CC group was not compared with the US-alone trials in the other conditions because of the presence of conditioned motor responding in that group. Single-trial samples were used to minimize the possibility of interaction between the CS and the US at the beginning of the experiment and thereby to capture the responses when they were most naive. Experience has shown that in our setting, 60 paired trials are usually not enough for the development of a CR. Therefore, by the last trial in the first session, the URs should not yet be influenced by a learned sensorimotor response. For the statistical analyses, analysis of variance for repeated measures was used. When the CC group was compared with the UP group, treatment (CC vs. UP) served as a between-subjects factor in a mixed model. In addition, t tests were used to further assess differences between groups (independent samples) or trial types during a certain training phase (paired samples). Histology. After the experiments, the animals were anesthetized with an intramuscular injection of ketamine–xylazine cocktail and then overdosed with an intravenous injection of pentobarbital. The rabbits were then perfused via the ascending aorta with saline followed by 10% Formalin. The brains were removed and fixed in Formalin–sucrose solution for at least 1 week. Frozen coronal sections of 100 m were taken from the site of the cold probe. Slices were mounted on gelatinized slides and stained with cresyl violet. The locations of the cold probes were determined according to the stereotaxic atlas by Shek, Wen, and Wisniewski (1986). Results Conditioned responding. No CRs exceeding the criterion were observed in either of the groups during the first session. The analyses of the URs, therefore, are not contaminated by learned overt motor responses. By the fifth session, the mean (⫾SEM) CR percentage in the CC group was 77.4% ⫾ 7.2. No CRs above the baseline level were observed in the UP group. Unconditioned responding. Figure 1 depicts the development of the UR amplitude for the US period during the experiment in both groups and trial types. As can be seen, common to both groups and to both trial types in the CC group is the tendency of 1054 WIKGREN, RUUSUVIRTA, AND KORHONEN Figure 1. Mean (⫾SEM) peak amplitudes of the unconditioned responses (in millimeters) in unconditioned stimulus (US)-alone trials in the classical conditioning group (CC; squares), CS ⫹ US trials in the CC group (triangles), and US trials in the unpaired group (UP; circles) plotted as a function of the training phase in Experiment 1. The mean peak amplitudes are plotted pairwise, as they were analyzed. Asterisks indicate significant differences between the NM amplitudes (* p ⬍ .05, ** p ⬍ .01). CS ⫽ conditioned stimulus; NM ⫽ nictitating membrane; A ⫽ the first trial in the first session; B ⫽ the last trial in the first session; C ⫽ the last trial in the last session. the UR to increase in amplitude as a function of exposure to the experimental setting, as indicated by the significant main effect of training phase (A, B, and C) in both groups: in the CC group, F(2, 20) ⫽ 25.25, p ⬍ .01; in the UP group, F(2, 16) ⫽ 13.06, p ⬍ .01. In the CC group, the UR amplitude grew more rapidly in the CS ⫹ US trials than in the US-alone trials, as indicated by the significant Training Phase (A, B, and C) ⫻ Trial Type (US-alone vs. CS ⫹ US) interaction, F(2, 20) ⫽ 6.38, p ⬍ .05. A t test for paired samples further revealed that the differences between the trial types was significant only for the last trial of the first training session, t(10) ⫽ 3.83, p ⬍ .01 (left-hand side in Figure 1). Furthermore, there was no significant difference in the last trial of the first session between the US/CC and US/UP conditions, indicating that the presence of the CS was a necessary factor in this phase of training to maintain reflex facilitation. Comparison of the US-alone trials between the CC and UP groups is shown in the center of Figure 1. The Training Phase (A, B, and C) ⫻ Group (CC vs. UP) interaction was significant, F(2, 36) ⫽ 2.88, p ⬍ .05. A t test for independent samples further revealed a significant difference between these groups in the last trial of the last session, t(18) ⫽ 2.24, p ⬍ .05, by which time the CC group had already acquired a robust level of conditioned responding. Taken together, these results indicate the presence of reflex facilitation, especially in relation to the US-alone trials in the CC group. When the CS ⫹ US/CC and US/UP trials were compared (right-hand side of Figure 1), the difference in UR amplitude was significant for the last trial of the first session, t(18) ⫽ 2.24, p ⬍ .05, but not for the first trial of the first session, where the subject had not yet experienced paired presentation of the stimuli in the CC group. The finding that the later URs were higher in amplitude in all conditions suggests the presence of experience-related reflex facilitation. The presence of CS-mediated reflex facilitation was indicated by the finding that the UR in the CC group was facilitated more if the US was preceded by the CS, despite the same initial level of responding in all conditions. Although no difference was found in URs to the US-alone trials between paired and unpaired treatments, an indication of the US–UR circuit having not yet been modified, these responses were higher in amplitude in the paired treatment by the end of training. This indicates the presence of CR-related reflex facilitation and, presumably, a relatively permanent modification of the US–UR circuit. These results are in accordance with those of Schreurs et al. (1995), who found CR-related reflex facilitation in US-alone trials when a robust level of CRs was achieved but not when the extent of conditioning was low (17% CRs after the first day of conditioning in their data). The data suggest that CS-mediated reflex facilitation, at this phase of learning, reflects a conditioned change in the organism. Whatever this change may be, it affects the US processing in a facilitative manner. To investigate whether reflex facilitation is dependent on the functioning of the IPN (the plausible core of the sensorimotor learning circuit), we set up Experiment 2. The aim was to test whether the amount of reflex facilitation is altered when the CR is blocked by inactivation of the IPN. Because the IPN is essential for forming and maintaining the association between the CS and the US, Experiment 2 should show whether any type of reflex facilitation is influenced by sensorimotor learning. Discussion Method These results indicate that three types of reflex facilitation were present during conditioning and that it is possible to dissociate them by taking samples from appropriate phases of conditioning. Subjects and surgery. The subjects were the same 9 adult female New Zealand albino rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) that were used in the UP group in Experiment 1. Experiment 2 REFLEX FACILITATION IN EYEBLINK CONDITIONING Procedure. After the unpaired treatment, the rabbits were trained in the same manner as the CC group in Experiment 1. After reaching the learning criterion of eight out of nine consecutive CRs, with the addition of one overtraining session, the rabbits underwent a session that involved cooling of the IPN. The cooling session was divided into three blocks: precooling, cooling, and postcooling. Each block consisted of 30 trials (4 CS only, 4 US only, and 22 CS ⫹ US trials). CRs were recorded from CS period in CS-only and CS ⫹ US trials, and UR peak amplitudes were recorded separately from the US-alone and CS ⫹ US trials, as in Experiment 1. After the first block, the cold probe was activated and the gas flow was adjusted so that the temperature in the cold probe fell below 5 °C. This took about 2 min. After the cooling block, the experiment was again interrupted for about 2 min to allow the temperature to return to normal. Generally, the subjects showed no CRs during cooling, but, in practice, the temperature fluctuates to some extent, which causes release from blocking in some of the trials. To ensure that the URs measured in the paired trials during cooling were not influenced by sudden CRs, the trials with NM movement exceeding 0.5 mm during the CS period were excluded from the analyses. Statistical procedure. We used repeated measures. For CRs, the three blocks of the cooling session formed the cooling variable. URs in both trial types (URs in paired trials and in US-alone trials) were analyzed separately in the same manner as the CRs. Afterward, we ran an additional analysis in which trial type was used as an additional variable. To provide an objective measure for the functioning of the cooling, we performed t tests for paired samples on each subject’s CR values during the cooling session (before vs. during cooling). Single trials were treated as cases. In all statistics, an alpha level of .05 was applied. Results Histology. The cold probe was correctly located (distance less than 2 mm from the IPN) in 6 out of 9 subjects (see Figure 2). Consistently, statistically significant reduction of the CR because of cooling was found only in these animals. Consequently, data from only these 6 subjects were analyzed further. Conditioned responding. Figure 3 illustrates the average peak amplitudes of CRs before, during, and after cooling. A significant main effect of cooling, F(2, 10) ⫽ 7.91, p ⬍ .01, was found. Unconditioned responding. The peak amplitude of the URs in both the US-alone and CS ⫹ US trials in the three phases of the cooling session are shown in Figure 4. The interaction of cooling and trial type was significant, F(2, 10) ⫽ 8.89, p ⬍ .01, as were the 1055 Figure 3. Mean (⫾SEM) peak amplitudes of the conditioned responses (in millimeters) in well-trained animals with accurate cold probe implantation during the different blocks in the cooling session as measured in conditioned stimulus ⫹ unconditioned stimulus trials (22 trials per block) in Experiment 2. main effects of both cooling, F(2, 10) ⫽ 11.13, p ⬍ .01, and trial type, F(2, 10) ⫽ 11.33, p ⬍ .05. Analyses performed for both trial types separately indicated that the effect of cooling was significant for the US-alone trials, F(2, 10) ⫽ 23.04, p ⬍ .01, but not for the CS ⫹ US trials, F(2, 10) ⫽ 0.79, p ⫽ .48. Discussion Cooling of the IPN in well-trained animals abolished the CR and significantly reduced the amplitude of URs in the US-alone trials but not in the CS ⫹ US trials. Thus, cooling of the IPN both abolished the actual discrete conditioned skeletal muscle response and reduced the CR-related reflex facilitation but had no effect on CS-mediated reflex facilitation. This indicated that the CR-related reflex facilitation, in particular, is dependent on a functional IPN. Consistently, in Experiment 1, this type of reflex facilitation was associated with the presence of a robust CR, as it was not observed in the initial phase of training. General Discussion Figure 2. Locations of the cold probe tips in all 9 experimental subjects in Experiment 2. Circles stand for the subjects in which conditioned responding was abolished during probe activation. The cold probe tip locations for the animals that did not show statistically significant abolition of conditioned response during cooling are marked with rectangles; their data were excluded from further analysis. A ⫽ millimeters anterior to lambda; M ⫽ midline; IP ⫽ interpositus nucleus; DE ⫽ dentate nucleus; FA ⫽ fastigial nucleus. The present experiments indicated the presence of three types of reflex facilitation. The first type, experience-related reflex facilitation, emerged after 60 US-presentations even in the UP group, indicating that the animals’ experience of the US and/or of the context in which the US was presented must have been sufficient for this effect. A possibility that the context might also play a role in this type of reflex facilitation is based on the finding that the CR amplitude, at least, can be increased by the context (Brandon & Wagner, 1991; Schreurs et al., 1995). Thus, not only selective sensitization of the US–UR circuit but also an association between 1056 WIKGREN, RUUSUVIRTA, AND KORHONEN Figure 4. Mean (⫾SEM) peak amplitudes of the unconditioned responses (in millimeters) in unconditioned stimulus (US)-alone trials and CS ⫹ US trials as a function of phase in the cooling session in Experiment 2. CS ⫽ conditioned stimulus. the US and its context might contribute to experience-related reflex facilitation. The second type of reflex facilitation, CR-related reflex facilitation, was linked to the emergence of the CR. Pairing the CS and the US resulted in more vigorous URs in the US-alone trials but, in line with the previous studies (Schreurs et al., 1995), only after a robust level of conditioning was reached. Consistently, the emergence of CR-related reflex facilitation was linked to the same neural circuit, that is, the IPN, that is necessary for the emergence of the CR itself, as it was affected by the temporal inactivation of this nucleus. This result is in contrast with the unaffected URs in the US-alone trials after the IPN lesion found in most of the earlier studies (e.g., G. A. Clark, McCormick, Lavond, & Thompson, 1984; R. E. Clark et al., 1992; Lavond, Hembree, & Thompson, 1985; Steinmetz et al., 1992; Yeo, Hardiman, & Glickstein, 1985). This finding is, however, in line with the study by Ivkovich, Lockard, and Thompson (1993), who reported such a declining trend in UR amplitudes to US-alone presentations. One might argue that the prior unpaired treatment in Experiment 1 led to this result; namely, given that reflex facilitation might be context related to some extent, one might propose that the IPN is involved not only in the storage of the CS–US association but also in that of the context–US association. However, in our recent experiment (Wikgren & Korhonen, 2001), the reversible inactivation of the IPN after the unpaired treatment was not found to affect the UR amplitude in US-alone trials but, in line with the present study, did so after the paired treatment. This evidence further suggests that the CS–US association formed by the IPN was linked to the processing of the US presented alone as well. Also, the third type of reflex facilitation (CS-mediated reflex facilitation) seems to have an associative basis. It was found to emerge at an early phase of the paired treatment, even before a robust level of CR was achieved but only after several CS–US pairings had been presented. A lack of its emergence during the very first trial of the experiment excludes a possibility that merely the presence of a tone led, in a nonspecific manner, to more vigorous URs, making valid comparisons between unpaired and paired trials impossible (Young, Cegavske, & Thompson, 1976). In contrast, even earlier studies have indicated that reflex facilitation can be specific to the physical features of the CS immediately preceding the US (Weisz & LoTurco, 1988; Weisz & McInerney, 1990), a key feature necessary for the CS acting as a signal. The exact nature of the CS-mediated reflex facilitation remains to be seen. One possibility, however, is that this type of reflex facilitation might be related to a conditioned emotive state, that is, to the ability of the US not only to possess sensory and response-eliciting value as such but also to induce aversiveness (Richardson & Thompson, 1984; Thompson, Thompson, Kim, Krupa, & Shinkman, 1998). This dissociation, at least in one direction, between the emotive and nonemotive value of a US has been further indicated by the finding that stimulation of the dorsal accessory olive as a US can be associated with a CS without signs of aversiveness (Mauk, Steinmetz, & Thompson, 1986). Thus, in the present study, it is possible that the remaining emotive aspects of the US might have become associated with the CS, independent of the behavioral response. This interpretation is further supported by the relative independence of the CS-mediated reflex facilitation on the cooling of the IPN (Wikgren & Korhonen, 2001), indicating that the neural bases necessary for the related association must be other than the IPN. In the present data, this interpretation is supported, as the UR amplitude in CS ⫹ US trials did not alter during cooling of the IPN. What, then, might anatomically contribute to CS-mediated reflex facilitation? It seems that the related pathways diverge from those necessary for CR acquisition and maintenance of the CR at the lower levels of the CS pathway. Whereas reflex facilitation can be induced by the electrical stimulation of the cochlear nucleus as a CS, this cannot be done when the nuclei located later along this pathway (the superior olive, inferior colliculus, or medial geniculate nucleus) are stimulated (Nowak, Kehoe, Macrae, & Gormezano, 1999). This finding further converges with the findings from another line of research, fear-potentiated startle effect (Davis, 1998); it refers to the findings that the amplitude of a UR to an intensive stimulus (the startle reflex), such as a loud tone, can be modified by inducing a state of fear. More specifically, pairing a neutral stimulus with the startle-eliciting noise, the startle reflex becomes stronger in paired trials, as measured by freezing behavior in rats or facial electromyograph responses in humans. Similarly to reflex facilitation, the startle-eliciting stimulus elicits the related behavior via the lower levels of the CS pathway. The related neural circuit consists of the connection between the sensory neurons in the cochlear root nucleus and the neurons in the nucleus reticular pontis caudalis (Rosen, Hitchcock, Sananes, Miserendino, & Davis, 1991), which, in turn, has a connection with the motor nuclei and the amygdala. The central nucleus of the amygdala in particular seems to be a critical region for the acquisition and maintenance of the fear-potentiated startle effect, as suggested by lesions to the amygdala or its connections with the startle pathway (Hitchcock & Davis, 1991; Hitchcock, Sananes, & Davis, 1989). REFLEX FACILITATION IN EYEBLINK CONDITIONING Given that both the emotive aspects of learning and the early parts of the auditory pathway contribute to CS-mediated reflex facilitation and the fear-potentiated startle effect, it is possible that they are similar also in the respect of both being dependent on the same critical region of the brain, the amygdala. Consistently, animals with amygdalar lesion are compromised in CR acquisition and in exhibiting reflex facilitation during aversive conditioning (Weisz et al., 1992), which indicates that the amygdala might be involved in learning the aversiveness of the US during eyeblink conditioning. The role of the amygdala in reflex facilitation is further supported by the finding that the electrical stimulation of the amygdala prior to a US presentation facilitates the UR (Whalen & Kapp, 1991). The neural substrate for this effect seems to consist of the projections of the amygdalar central nucleus to the lateral tegmental field in the thalamus, which, in turn, projects to a variety of cranial motor nuclei (Hopkins & Holstege, 1978; Takeuchi, Satoda, Tashiro, Matsushima, & Uemura-Sumi, 1988). Thus, by way of these connections, the amygdala might modulate various reflexes, such as the NM reflex (Kapp, Supple, & Whalen, 1994; Whalen & Kapp, 1991), independently of the IPN. Three types of reflex facilitation were found. Experience-related reflex facilitation developed as a function of time spent in the experimental setting with aversive stimuli and/or the resulting sensitization of the US–UR circuit. In contrast, CR-related reflex facilitation, in particular, was linked to the emergence of the CR and consistently was affected by the inactivation of the neural circuit necessary (IPN) for the CR development and maintenance. CS-mediated reflex facilitation, despite emerging at an early phase of learning after a few repetitions of CS–US pairings, was not affected by the IPN inactivation. By being dependent on forward CS–US pairings but not on the IPN, CS-mediated reflex facilitation might reflect emotive aspects of learning and, thereby, be based on the same neural substrates as other forms of emotive learning, such as fear-potentiated startle reflex. References Anderson, B. J., & Steinmetz, J. E. (1994). Cerebellar and brainstem circuits involved in classical eyeblink conditioning. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 5, 251–273. Brandon, S. E., & Wagner, A. R. (1991). Modulation of a discrete Pavlovian conditioned reflex by a putative emotive Pavlovian conditioned stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 17, 299 –311. Clark, G. A., McCormick, D. A., Lavond, D. G., & Thompson, R. F. (1984). Effects of lesions of cerebellar nuclei on conditioned behavioral and hippocampal neuronal responses. Brain Research, 291, 125–136. Clark, R. E., Zhang, A. A., & Lavond, D. G. (1992). Reversible lesions of the cerebellar interpositus nucleus during acquisition and retention of a classically conditioned behavior. Behavioral Neuroscience, 106, 879 – 888. Davis, M. (1998). Anatomic and physiologic substrates of emotion in an animal model. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology, 15, 378 –387. Gormezano, I., Schneiderman, N., Deaux, E., & Fuentes, I. (1962, October 12). Nictitating membrane: Classical conditioning and extinction in the albino rabbit. Science, 138, 33–34. Harvey, J. A., Gormezano, I., & Cool-Hauser, V. A. (1985). Relationship between heterosynaptic reflex facilitation and acquisition of the nictitating membrane response in control and scopolamine-injected rabbits. Journal of Neuroscience, 5, 596 – 602. Hitchcock, J. M., & Davis, M. (1991). The efferent pathway of the 1057 amygdala involved in conditioned fear as measured with the fearpotentiated startle paradigm. Behavioral Neuroscience, 105, 826 – 842. Hitchcock, J. M., Sananes, C. B., & Davis, M. (1989). Sensitization of the startle reflex by footshock: Blockade by lesions of the central nucleus of the amygdala or its efferent pathway to the brainstem. Behavioral Neuroscience, 103, 509 –518. Hopkins, D. A., & Holstege G. (1978). Amygdaloid projections to the mesencephalon, pons and medulla oblongata in the cat. Experimental Brain Research, 32, 529 –547. Ivkovich, D., Lockard, J. M., & Thompson R. F. (1993). Interpositus lesion abolition of the eyeblink conditioned response is not due to effects on performance. Behavioral Neuroscience, 107, 530 –532. Kapp, B. S., Supple, W. F., Jr., & Whalen, P. J. (1994). Effects of electrical stimulation of the amygdaloid central nucleus on neocortical arousal in the rabbit. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108, 81–93. Konorski, J. (1967). Integrative activity of the brain. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Korhonen, T. (1991). A method for rapid implantation of multielectrode systems. Physiology & Behavior, 49, 401– 403. Lavond, D. G., Hembree, T. L., & Thompson, R. F. (1985). Effect of kainic acid lesions of the cerebellar interpositus nucleus on eyelid conditioning in the rabbit. Brain Research, 326, 179 –182. Mauk, M. D., Steinmetz, J. E., & Thompson, R. F. (1986). Classical conditioning using stimulation of the inferior olive as the unconditioned stimulus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 79, 2731–2742. Netherlands Centre Alternatives to Animal Use. (2002). Council directive of 24 November 1986 on the approximation of laws, regulations and administrative provisions of the member states regarding the protection of animals used for experimental and other scientific purposes (86/609/ EEC). Retrieved September 20, 2002, from http://www.nca-nl.org/ English/Docs/86-609-eec_en.pdf Nowak, A. J., Kehoe, E. J., Macrae, M., & Gormezano, I. (1999). Conditioning and reflex modification of the rabbit nictitating membrane response using electrical stimulation in auditory nuclei. Behavioural Brain Research, 105, 189 –198. Richardson, R. T., & Thompson, R. F. (1984). Amygdaloid unit activity during classical conditioning of the nictitating membrane response in rabbit. Physiology & Behavior, 32, 527–539. Rosen, J. B., Hitchcock, J. M., Sananes, C. B., Miserendino, M. J. D., & Davis, M. A. (1991). A direct projection from the central nucleus of the amygdala to the acoustic startle pathway: Anterograde and retrograde tracing studies. Behavioral Neuroscience, 105, 817– 825. Schreurs, B. G., Oh, M. M., Hirashima, C., & Alkon, D. L. (1995). Conditioning-specific modification of the rabbit’s unconditioned nictitating membrane response. Behavioral Neuroscience, 109, 24 –33. Shek, J. W., Wen, G. Y., & Wisniewski, H. M. (1986). Atlas of the rabbit brain and spinal cord. Basel, Switzerland: Karger. Steinmetz, J. E., Lavond, D. G., Ivkovich, D., Logan, C. G., & Thompson, R. F. (1992). Disruption of classical eyelid conditioning after cerebellar lesions: Damage to a memory trace system or a simple performance deficit? Journal of Neuroscience, 12, 4403– 4426. Takeuchi, Y., Satoda, T., Tashiro, T., Matsushima, R., & Uemura-Sumi, M. (1988). Amygdaloid pathway to the trigeminal motor nucleus via the pontine reticular formation in the rat. Brain Research Bulletin, 21, 829 – 833. Thompson, R. F., Thompson, J. K., Kim, J. J., Krupa, D. J., & Shinkman, P. G. (1998). The nature of reinforcement in classical conditioning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 70, 150 –176. Weisz, D. J., Harden, D. G., & Xiang, Z. (1992). Effects of amygdala lesions on reflex facilitation and conditioned response acquisition during nictitating membrane response conditioning in rabbit. Behavioral Neuroscience, 106, 262–273. Weisz, D. J., & LoTurco, J. J. (1988). Reflex facilitation of the nictitating 1058 WIKGREN, RUUSUVIRTA, AND KORHONEN membrane response remains after cerebellar lesions. Behavioral Neuroscience, 102, 203–209. Weisz, D. J., & McInerney, J. (1990). An associative process maintains reflex facilitation of the unconditioned nictitating membrane response during the early stages of training. Behavioral Neuroscience, 104, 21– 27. Weisz, D. J., & Walts, C. (1990). Reflex facilitation of the rabbit nictitating membrane response by an auditory stimulus as a function of interstimulus interval. Behavioral Neuroscience, 104, 11–20. Whalen, P. J., & Kapp, B. S. (1991). Contributions of the amygdaloid central nucleus to the modulation of the nictitating membrane reflex in the rabbit. Behavioral Neuroscience, 105, 141–153. Wikgren, J., & Korhonen, T. (2001). Interpositus nucleus inactivation reduces unconditioned response amplitude after paired but not explicitly unpaired treatment in rabbit eyeblink conditioning. Neuroscience Letters, 308, 181–184. Yeo, C. H., Hardiman, M. J., & Glickstein, M. (1985). Classical conditioning of the nictitating membrane response of the rabbit: I. Lesions of the cerebellar nuclei. Experimental Brain Research, 60, 87–98. Young, R. A., Cegavske, C. F., & Thompson, R. F. (1976). Tone-induced changes in excitability in abducens motoneurons and of the reflex path of nictitating membrane in rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus). Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 90, 424 – 434. Zhang, J., Ni, H., & Harper, R. M. (1986). A miniaturized cryoprobe for functional neuronal blockade in freely moving animals. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 16, 79 – 87. Received April 27, 2000 Revision received March 26, 2002 Accepted May 13, 2002 䡲