* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download LEL 1 - Linguistics and English Language
Scottish Gaelic grammar wikipedia , lookup
Germanic strong verb wikipedia , lookup
Swedish grammar wikipedia , lookup
Lithuanian grammar wikipedia , lookup
Old Irish grammar wikipedia , lookup
French grammar wikipedia , lookup
Polish grammar wikipedia , lookup
Japanese grammar wikipedia , lookup
Macedonian grammar wikipedia , lookup
Old English grammar wikipedia , lookup
Modern Hebrew grammar wikipedia , lookup
Ancient Greek grammar wikipedia , lookup
Antisymmetry wikipedia , lookup
Hungarian verbs wikipedia , lookup
Navajo grammar wikipedia , lookup
Udmurt grammar wikipedia , lookup
Kannada grammar wikipedia , lookup
Italian grammar wikipedia , lookup
Chinese grammar wikipedia , lookup
Portuguese grammar wikipedia , lookup
Serbo-Croatian grammar wikipedia , lookup
Latin syntax wikipedia , lookup
Icelandic grammar wikipedia , lookup
Kagoshima verb conjugations wikipedia , lookup
Georgian grammar wikipedia , lookup
Lexical semantics wikipedia , lookup
Pipil grammar wikipedia , lookup
Spanish grammar wikipedia , lookup
English clause syntax wikipedia , lookup
LEL 1 Syntax 7: Auxiliaries in English and sentence structure Outline • Sentences cannot always be equated with VPs. Sentences with an auxiliary verb must contain more structure. • Auxiliary verbs and main verbs in English are in different positions in the sentence structure. • Embedded clauses must contain even more structure. • Main clauses in which the finite verb precedes the subject must contain the same amount of structure as embedded clauses. Sentence structure revisited Recap: It is possible to analyse simple sentences like Jane saw Mary or The man read a book as VPs. In accordance with X-bar theory, V is the head of the sentence in that case, the subject is the specifier of the VP and the object is the complement. VP (= S) V’ Subj V Obj Sentences with an auxiliary verb Can sentences with an auxiliary verb be analysed just as VPs as well? Pauline can sing the most difficult songs. The doctor will see the patient tomorrow. The weather may turn bad. Gerald has read that book seven times. They could if the auxiliary and the main verb formed one complex verb together, a compound. But that is extremely unlikely. Why main verb and auxiliary do not form a compound Main verb and auxiliary can be split by other syntactic material: Can Pauline sing that song properly? The doctor will probably not see the patient tomorrow. A true Verb-Verb compound such as stir-fry can never be split in this way: *Stir will Pauline fry those vegetables. *Pauline fried the vegetables stir again. Why main verb and auxiliary do not form a compound (cont’d) In an auxiliary verb – main verb sequence, it is the left-hand verb (the auxiliary) that can be inflected for tense: Pauline has to sing those songs. *Pauline to have sings those songs. The patient is seen by the doctor. *The patient been sees by the doctor. In a Verb-Verb compound, it is the right-hand verb that can be inflected for tense: Pauline stir-fried the vegetables yesterday. *Pauline stirred-fry the vegetables yesterday. She stir-fries too much for my liking. *She stirs-fry too much for my liking. Properties of English modal auxiliaries An analysis for sentences with auxiliaries suggests itself when we look more closely at some of the properties of English modal auxiliaries in particular. First, modals can only appear in forms carrying finite inflection. They do not occur as non-tensed forms, such as infinitives, past participles or present participles. *To can build a house is a great asset. *The sun seems to will shine. *Paul has never could do such a thing. *John’s maying to go to America is surprising. Properties of English modal auxiliaries (cont’d) A second property of English modals is that they must occupy a position in the sentence that is somewhat further to the left than the position occupied by main verbs. Iris will paint the door. Iris will not paint the door. *Iris not will paint the door *Iris paints not the door. Iris does not paint the door. Arthur could quickly open the tin. *Arthur opened quickly the tin. Arthur quickly opened the tin The I-position and the IP On the basis of these properties of modals, let us assume that sentences with a finite auxiliary contain an extra position to the left of the VP: the Infl-position, commonly abbreviated to I-position. In accordance with the X-bar theory for phrase structure, this position is the head of an Infl Phrase, commonly abbreviated to IP. The VP is in the complement position of this IP. IP I’ spec I VP V’ spec V compl Subject movement If sentences are IPs, and finite auxiliaries are in the I-position, then the subject in English must be in the specifier position of the IP, as it usually precedes the finite auxiliary: Iris will not paint the door. Arthur could quickly open the tin. But semantically speaking, the subject appears to act as the specifier of the Verb Phrase: it expresses the element that the Verb Phrase predicate says something about. So there is a paradox: the subject appears in the specifier of the IP, but it is interpreted as the specifier-of-the-VP. Subject movement (cont’d) Paradox: the subject appears in the specifier of the IP, but it is interpreted as the specifier-of-the-VP. Why not have our cake and eat it as well? Let’s claim that there are actually two instances of the subject in a sentence, one in spec-IP and one in spec-VP. The only difference between the two instances is that the first one is pronounced, while the second one is not. Another way of describing this situation is to say that the subject moves from spec-VP to spec-IP, and that such movement leaves a trace which is identical to the moved element except for being silent. The full IP Thus, the full structure of a sentence like Iris will paint the door is as follows, where tIris stands for ‘the trace of Iris’. IP I’ NP Iris I will VP V’ tIris V paint NP the door Where’s the verb? If finite auxiliaries are in the I-position, not the V-position, then what about sentences without an auxiliary, where the main verb is finite? Is this in the I-position, then? Not in English. In English, main verbs always remain in the Vposition, even when they are finite: Mary will often read a book in the evening. [will in I] *Mary reads often a book in the morning. [*reads in I] Mary often reads a book in the morning. [reads in V] Do-support In English, in sentences containing the negator not the I-position must be filled. The reluctance of main verbs to go into the I-position is so strong that, if there is no other auxiliary in a negated sentence, a dummy auxiliary is used to fill up the I-position. Mary has not read a book this morning. [auxiliary verb in I] *Mary reads not a book in the morning. [*main verb in I] Mary does not read a book in the morning. [dummy do in I] V-to-I movement in French French looks like English in having finite auxiliaries in the I-position and non-finite main verbs in the V-position: Jean a souvent donné ses livres à Marie. John has often given his books to Mary John has often given his books to Mary. But in sentences without an auxiliary in French, the main verb does go into the I-position: Jean donne souvent ses livres à Marie. John gives often his books to Mary John often gives his books to Mary. *Jean souvent donne ses livres à Marie. More than one auxiliary Sentences can contain more than one auxiliary verb: Mary has been working. Jane has been overlooked. Garry will be cycling. I could have cried. This will have to be considered. John should have been working. His resignation might have been accepted. The tramline was to have been completed years ago. More than one auxiliary (cont’d) For sentences with more than one auxiliary, we will assume that there is VP-recursion below the IP, as there is no clear evidence that non-finite auxiliaries occur in a distinct clausal position such as the I-position. Mary has not been seen for a while. *Mary has been not seen for a while. Garry will not be cycling to Birmingham. *Garry will be not cycling to Birmingham. Complementizers Embedded clauses can be introduced by a word that links them to the main clause: a complementizer. Thomas knew [that Betty would never go to Peru]. Corinne asked [if she could stay in Argentina a bit longer]. Stella did not know what to make of the film [since she had not read the book]. To accommodate complementizers in a sentence’s structure, we need another head position in front of the entire IP: the Cposition. In accordance with X-bar theory, C is the head of a phrase CP that contains a specifier position and a complement position. The complement position contains the IP. The full CP CP C’ spec C IP I’ spec I VP V’ spec V compl Subject-verb inversion In English interrogative sentences (questions), the finite verb must appear in front of the subject (unless the subject itself is questioned). Deborah has finished her work already. Has Deborah finished her work already? Paul will read the newspaper tonight. Will Paul read the newspaper tonight? Two hypotheses possible: A. The subject is lower in interrogatives than in declaratives. B. The finite verb is higher in interrogatives than in declaratives. Subject-verb inversion (cont’d) Hypothesis A: The subject is lower in interrogatives. Perhaps the subject stays in spec-VP in questions, rather than moving to spec-IP? No: the subject still precedes those elements that appear in between the I-position and the VP, such as often: Has John often done that? *Has often John done that? Subject-verb inversion (cont’d) Hypothesis B: the finite verb is higher in interrogatives. In declaratives the finite auxiliary is in I. Which higher position is available for the finite auxiliary in interrogatives? Answer: the finite verb is in C in interrogatives! Bonus of this assumption: it explains why there is never subjectverb inversion, even in interrogative clauses, if a complementizer already occupies C. I wonder [if Deborah has finished her work already]. *I wonder [if has Deborah finished her work already]. Do-support in interrogatives We have seen that 1. The finite verb in interrogative clauses (without a complementizer) must go into the C-position. 2. Main verbs in English never leave the V-position. Given this it is no surprise that in interrogatives without an auxiliary, dummy do appears again, this time to fill up the C-position: Frances likes sticky toffee pudding best. *Likes Frances sticky toffee best? Does Frances like sticky toffee pudding best? Some other languages allow main verbs to appear in C: Fietst Karel vaak langs de gracht? Dutch cycles Carl often along the canal Does Carl often cycle along the canal?