* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Download Polymorphic mimicry in Papilio dardanus: mosaic
Genome (book) wikipedia , lookup
Koinophilia wikipedia , lookup
Genome evolution wikipedia , lookup
Hardy–Weinberg principle wikipedia , lookup
Designer baby wikipedia , lookup
Human genetic variation wikipedia , lookup
Quantitative trait locus wikipedia , lookup
Genetic drift wikipedia , lookup
Population genetics wikipedia , lookup
Dominance (genetics) wikipedia , lookup
Microevolution wikipedia , lookup
EVOLUTION & DEVELOPMENT 5:6, 579–592 (2003) Polymorphic mimicry in Papilio dardanus: mosaic dominance, big effects, and origins H. Frederik Nijhout Department of Biology, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708, USA Author for correspondence (e-mail: [email protected]) SUMMARY The mocker swallowtail, Papilio dardanus, has a female-limited polymorphic mimicry. This polymorphism is controlled by allelic variation at a single locus with at least 11 alleles. Many of the alternative morphs are accurate mimics of different species of distasteful butterflies. Geneticists have long been interested in the mechanism by which a single gene can have such diverse and profound effects on the phenotype and in the process by which these complex phenotypic effects could have evolved. Here we present the results of a morphometric analysis of the pleiotropic effects of the mimicry gene on the array of elements that makes up the overall pattern. We show that the patterns controlled by mimicking alleles are more variable and less internally correlated than those controlled by nonmimicking alleles, suggesting the two are subject to different degrees of selection and mutational variance. Analysis of the pleiotropic dominance of the alleles reveals a consistent pattern of dominance within a coevolved genetic background and a mosaic pattern of dominant and recessive effects (including overdominance) in a heterologous genetic background. The alleles of the mimicry gene have big effects on some pattern elements and small effects on others. When the array of big phenotypic effects of the mimicry gene is applied to the presumptive ancestral color pattern, it produces a reasonable resemblance to distasteful models and suggests the initial steps that may have produced the mimicry as well as the polymorphism. INTRODUCTION gene hypotheses. Clarke and Sheppard (1960d) suggested that the diverse phenotypic effects of this locus could be explained if it was actually a tightly linked cluster of genes, each with an independent effect on the phenotype. Such a supergene could arise by recombination events that move genes with related functions for pattern specification to a common region of a chromosome; recombination within such a gene cluster could be inhibited by an inversion. This way, each allele of the supergene would lock together a unique combination of alleles of the constitutive genes to achieve its particular phenotypic effect. The alternative hypothesis is that the mimicry gene is a regulatory gene that controls the expression of a number of unlinked genes that affect various aspects of the color pattern (Nijhout 1991). Each allele of this regulatory gene would control a different temporal or spatial pattern of expression in the genes it regulates. The regulated pattern genes could be monomorphic in a given geographic race, and differentiation of these genes between races could account for the observed breakdown of mimicry in interracial hybrids. The exact number of alleles of this gene is not known, because all possible crosses have not yet been done, but it appears at present that there are at least 11 alleles with major effects on the pattern. Most alleles have the same phenotypic effect wherever they are found across the geographic range of The female-limited polymorphic mimicry of the African Mocker Swallowtail, Papilio dardanus, represents one of the most spectacular and puzzling cases of evolution in the animal world. Papilio dardanus is broadly distributed throughout sub-Saharan Africa. The males of P. dardanus look identical across this entire range, but the females occur as at least 14 distinct morphs (Fig. 1). The majority of these morphs are Batesian mimics of several distasteful species of Danaidae and Acraeidae (Poulton 1924; Ford 1936; Bernardi et al. 1985). The remaining morphs, although highly distinctive, do not resemble any known species of butterfly and are not believed to be mimics. As many as six of the female morphs may occur together in a given population. Perhaps the most remarkable feature of the female color morphs of P. dardanus is that their diversity is controlled by allelic variation at a single locus, the mimicry gene, H. There are at least 11 alleles at this locus, and the various diploid combinations of these alleles account for the diversity of highly distinctive female color patterns. The mimicry gene There are at present two competing hypotheses about the nature of the mimicry gene: the supergene and the regulatory & BLACKWELL PUBLISHING, INC. 579 580 EVOLUTION & DEVELOPMENT Vol. 5, No. 6, November^December 2003 Fig. 1. The homozygous female forms of Papilio dardanus. The specimens are arranged in three groups based on their pattern. The black portions are considered pattern, and the colored portions are background. The names of the forms and their allelic designation are given below each specimen. The text refers to group 1 as the hippocoon, group 2 as the cenea group, and group 3 as the male-like or the planemoides group. A male is shown underneath group 2. Males are monomorphic and identical for all genotypes. the species, but three of these alleles (H c, H h, and H T) produce significantly different phenotypes in different populations (Clarke and Sheppard 1960a,b,c, 1962). The form ochracea appears to be controlled by the same allele (H c) as cenea. The form cenea is widespread and mimics the danaids Amauris echeria and A. albimaculata, whereas ochracea occurs in a small area in Northern Kenya where it mimics the darker local form, Amauris echeria septentrionis. The form hippocoon is probably controlled by the same allele (h) as hippocoonides. Hippocoon is more widespread than hippocoonides; their distributions do not overlap because the two forms mimic different geographic races of Amauris niavius. In addition, in the race polytrophus, there is a yellowish form of hippocoonides, called trimeni, which appears to be produced by modifier genes. Finally, the form lamborni from central Kenya may be controlled by the same allele (H T) as the more widespread form trophonius. The differences in the color patterns associated with these three alleles are presumably due to differences in the genetic background in which the alleles are expressed. The evolution of mimicry Batesian mimicry is believed to originate by means of an initial mutation that has a sufficiently big effect on the phenotype to give a passable resemblance to a protected model, followed by the accumulation and selection of mutations in modifier genes that progressively refine the mimicry (Fisher 1930; Carpenter and Ford 1933; Sheppard 1959; Clarke and Sheppard 1960c; Charlesworth and Charles- Nijhout worth 1975a; Turner 1977; Charlesworth 1994). Charlesworth and Charlesworth (1975b,c) calculated the conditions under which mimicry will evolve, and their calculations suggest that modifying mutations that refine the mimicry will be maintained if they are tightly liked to the gene that conferred the initial advantage, thus providing a plausible explanation for the evolution of a supergene. In P. dardanus, this simple scenario is complicated by the fact that mimicry is polymorphic and that the various morphs are not equally distributed among geographic races. Among the unresolved questions are (a) whether each of the 14 morphs evolved independently from a common ancestor or (b) whether some morphs evolved from others and (c) whether the geographic distribution of morphs due to independent origins in different regions or (d) to differential dispersal and extinction. Given the complexity and the wide geographic distribution of the polymorphism, it seems parsimonious to assume that each of the alleles arose only once and is identical across its entire geographic range and that the evolution of geographic races occurred after the evolution of the polymorphism. The alternative possibility, that the same polymorphic mimicry arose several times independently in different geographic races of a monomorphic ancestor, seems to be less probable. An intermediate between these two extremes is that a limited polymorphism of perhaps three or four alleles arose first, followed by a geographic dispersal and a subsequent evolution of additional alleles unique to particular geographic races. The latter is almost certainly true for the form ochracea (derived from cenea), hippocoonides (derived from hippocoon), lamborni (derived from trophonius), and of the nonmimetic male-like female morph from Ethiopia (a loss of mimicry). Whether the malelike females from Madagascar represent a primitive condition or are also due to a secondary loss of mimicry remains an open question (Vane-Wright and Smith 1991). The diversity of P. dardanus female forewing patterns can be classified into three groups (Fig. 1). The first one, which we will call the hippocoon group, is the largest and has a pattern of large bold areas of black and color. The black portions of the wing pattern constitute ‘‘pattern,’’ whereas the white or colored portions of the wing pattern constitute ‘‘background’’ (Nijhout 1994). Pattern diversity in this group consists primarily of diversity in the background color and to a lesser degree in differences in the width of the black areas of the pattern. The second is the cenea group, which consist of two forms, cenea and ochracea. Here the black portions of the pattern are greatly expanded and the background is reduced to an array of spots. The third group is the planemoides group. It has relatively simple patterns of dark areas in the proximal and distal portions of the wing, separated by a single band of background color. In this group we also include the male patterns and the male-like female patterns, which have the distal black pattern but not the proximal one. Polymorphic mimicry in P. dardanus 581 Although we know little about the nature and molecular mechanism of action of the mimicry gene, we can nevertheless learn a great deal about how this gene controls various aspects of the color pattern by systematic analysis of how different aspects of the color pattern vary under different breeding conditions. The beginnings of such an analysis, using morphometrics and a comparative analysis of pattern variation in color forms and interracial hybrids, is the subject of this article. This analysis suggests a hypothesis about the first steps in the evolution of mimicry and shows that the subsequent diversification of forms and refinement of their mimicry involved significant changes in the patterns of dominance of the many pleiotropic effects of the mimicry gene. MATERIALS AND METHODS Specimens for this study were obtained from collections and experimental crosses preserved in the British Museum, Natural History, the National Museum of Kenya, and the African Butterfly Research Institute (Nairobi, Kenya). Because populations of P. dardanus can be quite variable, summaries on every group of specimens reported here were either from collections from single locales or the progeny of single crosses. Specimens were photographed with a 3 megapixel digital camera, and morphometrics were done using digitization software developed for the purposes of this study. The landmarks measured are shown in Fig. 2. These landmarks were recorded as (x,y) coordinates, and distances between various pars of coordinates were calculated using a spreadsheet program (Microsoft Excel). In all cases, the effects of body size were removed by regressing the measures of the size of a pattern element on the mean of four distance measures of the length and separation between wing veins (Fig. 2B, dotted lines) and taking the residuals of the regression (using JMP, SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA). The data reported here refer to these residuals. Repeatability of measurement was established by digitizing 12 landmarks on 10 specimens, four different times, at intervals of a week to a month. The mean coefficient of variation (standard deviation 100/mean) of repeatability on 18 distances calculated from these measurements was 1.03, with a standard error of 0.37. Correlations reported here are Pearson product-moment correlations; 95% confidence intervals were estimated using Fisher’s z-transformation (Sokal and Rohlf 1981; Paulsen and Nijhout 1993). If the 95% confidence interval contained zero, the correlation was assumed to not be significantly different from zero. RESULTS The definition of the female forms has been based almost entirely on subjective qualitative descriptions of the color patterns shown in Fig. 1. To make this comparison more objective and to develop a quantitative database to study morphological evolution, we have undertaken an extensive morphometric analysis of pattern diversity and pattern 582 EVOLUTION & DEVELOPMENT Vol. 5, No. 6, November^December 2003 Fig. 2. Diagrammatic representation of the forewing pattern. (A) Names of the wing veins and the names of the pattern elements as used in Figs. 3 and 4, and Table 5. (B) Coordinates recorded for digitization of pattern landmarks (circles) and the distance measures (lines) used in Tables 1–4. The landmarks used for pattern element are the distances between a vein and the edge of a black portion of the pattern, measured along the midline of an intervenous area. These distances are indicated by solid lines, and the numbers on each line refer to the distance measurements used in Table 3. Dotted lines indicate distance measures used to calculate the general size of the wing. variation. Here we restrict our analysis to the patterns on the forewings, because these have undergone the greatest radiation in form during the evolution of mimicry. Correlated variation of pattern elements in males and female morphs The color patterns of P. dardanus are considerably simpler than those of the well-studied nymphalid butterflies. The elements of the nymphalid groundplan (Nijhout 1991) are absent, and the entire pattern consists of two regions of black pattern elements. One extends inward from the distal margin of the wing and is present in both males and females. The other, restricted to females, occupies the leading edge and proximal portion of the wing. As in other butterflies, color pattern development is compartmentalized by the wing veins (Nijhout 1994; Koch and Nijhout 2002). As a consequence, the portions of the pattern in each intervein region can become developmentally uncoupled to various degrees (Nijhout 1985). Elements of the color pattern that are developmentally uncoupled will exhibit a certain degree of independent variation from individual to individual. A high degree of correlated variation between two elements indicates that they share many developmental determinants. A low degree of correlated variation indicates that variation of each portion of the pattern is caused by different factors. Here we measure only phenotypic correlations. In other butterflies, phenotypic correlations are good predictors of genetic correlations (Paulsen 1994, 1996), suggesting that phenotypic variation is due to individual genetic variation rather than to individually different environmental effects on each portion of the pattern. The genetic correlation between different portions of the pattern indicates the degree to which those portions are genetically coupled; high genetic coupling is a constraint on the ability of those parts to respond independently to selection. The correlated variation in the width of the black margin in adjoining intervein regions in males and females of the monomorphic Madagascan race meriones are shown in Table 1. In males the correlated variation declines with distance. In females the patterns in the anterior part of the wing are correlated with each other, as are those in the posterior portion of the wing, but there is no correlated variation between those two regions of the wing. We were able to obtain sufficiently large series (430 specimens from a single locale) of males of two additional geographic races, polytrophus and tibbulus, for a comparative correlation analysis. These results are shown in Table 2. In tibbulus the correlations decline strongly with distance, whereas in polytrophus the patterns remain highly correlated despite increasing distance between them. Evidently, geographic races can differ considerably in the correlations among various portions of their color pattern. In all cases summarized in Tables 1 and 2, where the decline in correlation with distance is strong, there appears to Table 1. Correlations among the widths of the marginal pattern elements (taken along the midline of each wing cell) in females (A) and males (B) of the monomorphic nonmimicking meriones race A 6d 5d 5d 0.85 B 4d 3d 2d 6d 5d 0.92 5d 4d 3d 2d 4d 0.61 0.79 4d 0.82 0.91 3d −0.05 0.25 0.32 3d 0.73 0.81 0.93 2d 0.27 0.53 0.42 0.81 2d 0.79 0.75 0.84 0.87 1d 0.34 0.53 0.27 0.50 0.71 1d 0.68 0.56 0.55 0.56 0.82 Axes indicate the pattern element (see Fig. 2A). Bold numbers indicate correlations that are significantly different from zero. Nijhout Polymorphic mimicry in P. dardanus Table 2. Correlations among the widths of the marginal pattern elements (taken along the midline of each wing cell) of males of the races polytrophus (A) and tibbulus (B) B A 6d 5d 4d 3d 6d 2d 5d 4d 3d 583 The findings of Koch and Nijhout (2002) on the role of wing veins in compartmentalization of the color pattern of Papilio xuthus suggest that high correlated variation among the patterns in different wing compartments is likely to be the primitive condition in the Papilionidae. 2d 5d 0.81 5d 0.74 4d 0.73 0.88 4d 0.47 0.69 3d 0.66 0.74 0.91 3d 0.36 0.41 0.82 2d 0.64 0.76 0.85 0.89 2d 0.29 0.31 0.73 0.87 1d 0.60 0.67 0.77 0.77 0.93 1d 0.21 0.21 0.54 0.71 0.89 Axes indicate the wing cells (see Fig. 2A). Bold numbers indicate correlations that are significantly different from zero. be a greater step at wing vein M3 or M2, so that the patterns on each side of this boundary are significantly more highly correlated with each other than they are with patterns on the other side of this boundary. In many species of butterflies there is an abrupt change in the shape or color of pattern elements on either side of wing vein M3 (Nijhout 1991), suggesting that some of the determinants of the color pattern are different in the anterior and posterior portions of the forewing. We were also able to obtain a sufficiently large sample of females from two mimicking forms, cenea and hippocoonides, of the race polytrophus for correlation analysis. In these forms the color pattern is more complicated in that in each wing cells there are two black patterns: a distal pattern that extends in from the wing margin and a proximal pattern that extends out from the discal cell. The sizes of the patterns at the wing cell midlines were measured, as illustrated in Fig. 2, and the correlations among these are shown in Table 3. In the form cenea only 4 of 66 correlations were significantly different from zero, and in hippocoonides only 2 of 45 correlations were significantly different from zero. By chance alone one would expect three and two of the correlations to be significant (at the 5% level) in cenea and hippocoonides, respectively, so it appears that there are probably no significant correlations among any portions of the pattern in these two forms. The absence of correlated variation among pattern elements in mimicking forms stands in contrast to the neighbor and regional correlations observed in the nonmimetic patterns. It is possible that the reduction of internal correlations enabled the evolution of accurate mimicry. In the Nymphalidae there are generally low correlations among the various elements of the color pattern (Nijhout 1991; Paulsen and Nijhout 1993; Paulsen 1996), but it is not yet clear whether this is also the case for the simpler patterns of the Papilionida. Variability of the pattern Our studies on the correlated variation of pattern elements revealed a substantial amount of phenotypic variability in the various forms of P. dardanus. Assuming a similar mutation load, patterns that are subject to strong selection should exhibit less genetic and phenotypic variability than patterns that are under weaker selection. Accordingly, we measured the variability of the color pattern in mimetic and nonmimetic forms. We also measured the variability of patterns of three of the interracial crosses done by Clarke and Sheppard (1960b, 1963), in which they observed a breakdown of mimicry. The results are presented in Table 4 in the form of coefficients of variation. To provide a basis of comparison, we also measured the variability of several physical landmarks in the venation pattern of the wing whose absolute dimensions were in the same range as those of the various features of the color pattern that were measured (Fig. 2). The color patterns of the nonmimetic forms have approximately the same degree of variability as the structural features of the wing. Interestingly, the patterns of mimetic females are substantially more variable than those of the nonmimetic forms. This increased variability is not due to a greater variability of the substrate on which the pattern develops, because the coefficients of variation of their venation pattern are about the same as those of the nonmimetic forms. The variability of the pattern of naturally occurring imperfect mimics (niaviodes) and of the progeny of interracial crosses is greater than that of the pure female forms. Table 4 partitions the variability of the non-male-like forms into that attributable to variation in the marginal patterns (which they have in common with the male-like forms) and the basal patterns (which are unique to the female mimicking forms). It is clear that the basal patterns are more variable than the marginal patterns, although both sets of pattern elements are more variable in the mimetic than in the nonmimetic forms. Effects of the alleles of the mimicry locus If the diversity of female forms evolved by the sequential addition of mutations that affect different portions of the pattern, then it would be interesting to know whether these independent effects can be detected within today’s diversity and variation of the pattern. For the studies that follow, we established a character matrix for all the homozygous female forms (Table 5). In Table 5, the size of each pattern element is scored as the percent of the wing cell covered by the element, based on the average of a range of 10–20 specimens. In the 584 EVOLUTION & DEVELOPMENT Vol. 5, No. 6, November^December 2003 Table 3. Correlations among the widths of the pattern elements in females of the mimetic forms cenea (A) and hippocoon (B) from the race polytrophus A 1d(1) 2d(2) 3d(3) 4d(4) 5d(5) 6d(6) 1p(7) 2p(8) 3p(9) 4p(10) 2d(2) 0.65 3d(3) 0.53 −0.16 3d(4) 0.11 0.21 0.57 5d(5) 0.40 0.56 0.31 0.19 6d(6) 0.14 0.20 0.31 0.20 0.66 1p(7) −0.05 0.14 −0.03 0.33 0.03 −0.12 2p(8) 0.03 −0.24 −0.12 0.30 −0.17 −0.44 0.65 3p(9) −0.37 −0.16 −0.50 −0.30 −0.28 −0.01 0.23 0.13 4p(10) −0.11 −0.07 −0.41 −0.40 −0.13 −0.02 0.095 0.30 0.72 5p(11) −0.12 0.01 0.11 0.09 −0.17 0.11 −0.12 −0.35 −0.08 −0.23 6p(12) −0.34 0.27 −0.43 −0.07 −0.29 −0.09 0.10 0.30 0.23 0.22 3p(9) 5p(11) 5p(11) 0.09 B 1d(1) 2d(2) 3d(3) 5d(5) 6(6) 1p(7) 2p(8) 2d(2) 0.68 3d(3) 0.29 0.69 5d(5) −0.03 −0.14 0.01 6d(6) −0.01 −0.18 −0.22 0.53 1p(7) −0.08 0.18 0.45 0.01 0.04 2p(8) 0.06 0.03 −0.11 −0.06 0.089 −0.14 3p(9) −0.09 −0.09 0.00 −0.05 0.27 0.42 −0.35 5p(11) 0.08 0.16 0.28 −0.06 0.022 0.24 −0.29 0.04 6p(12) 0.07 0.08 0.12 0.08 0.20 0.25 0.02 −0.21 0.23 Pattern elements are coded as shown in Fig. 2A. Parenthetical numbers next to pattern element refer to distance measures shown in Fig. 2B. Bold numbers indicate correlations that are significantly different from zero. scoring system, 1 5 10%, 2 5 20%, and so on. In cases where the entire wing cell is black, the pattern is made up by fusion of the proximal and distal pattern elements in that wing cell. In such cases the score of each pattern element is given as the largest value recorded for that element in all genotypes. In addition, we measured the sizes of pattern elements for all the types of heterozygotes of whose identity we could be certain. These data are used to study the patterns of dominance relationships and the relative dimensions of the effects of the alleles of the mimicry gene. Dominance relationships within geographic races When crosses are made between different genotypes within a geographic race, the various alleles typically have a Nijhout Polymorphic mimicry in P. dardanus 585 Table 4. Coefficients of variation of the color pattern elements in Papilio dardanus Venation meriones (female) meriones (male) tibullus male polytrophus male polytrophus cenea polytrophus hippocoonides dardanus hippocoonides antinorii hippocoonides1 C&S broods 4687147812 C&S broods 4695147012 C&S brood 46832 All Pattern Elements 4.9 4.4 11.9 7.3 5.9 5.8 8.9 9.4 7.8 8.5 8.1 Proximal Patterns Distal Patterns F F F F 33.8 36.3 36.3 33.9 44.6 41.8 46.2 7.4 8.3 29.2 11.1 16.3 16.6 14.9 17.0 21.3 24.1 21.9 7.4 8.3 9.0 11.1 26.9 21.2 21.0 24.2 32.1 32.2 34.1 Names of geographic races are italicized; names of female forms are in roman letters. 1 This form is usually called niavioides and is an imperfect mimic. 2 Data from female offspring of interracial crosses by Clarke and Sheppard (1963) and preserved in the British Museum, Natural History. well-defined dominance relation to each other, and the various female morphs segregate as clear Mendelian traits (Clarke and Sheppard 1959, 1960a,b,e, 1962). Most heterozygous genotypes exhibit full dominance of one of the alleles, with only three notable exceptions. The niobe phenotype can be obtained with the niobe allele of the mimicry locus (H ni) but also as a heterozygote between the planemoides and trophonius alleles (H pl/H T), yielding the so-called synthetic niobe (Clarke and Sheppard 1960a). The heterozygote between the leighi and trophonius alleles (H l/H T) gives a unique phenotype called salaami, and the heterozygote between the planemoides and poultoni alleles (H pl/H p) gives a unique phenotype called dorippoides (Clarke and Sheppard 1960a). Table 5. Character matrix of pattern elements of the female forms of Papilio dardanus, and the two color forms of Papilio phorcas Wing Cell Form 1d 2pd 2ad 3d 4pd 4ad 5d 6d 7d 1p 2pp 2ap 3p 4pp 4ap 5p 6p Dp Dm Dd antinorii cenea dionysos dorippoides hippocoonides hippocoon lamborni leighi meriones natalica niobe ochracea planemoides poultoni trophonius salaami dorippoides synth niobe P. phorcas yellow P. phorcas green 1 4 1 1 2 2 2 2 1 2 1 3 1 2 1 1 1 1 4 4 2 4 2 2 4 4 3 4 2 4 2 4 2 4 2 2 2 2 4 4 2 4 2 2 5 5 3 6 2 6 2 4 2 5 4 3 2 3 5 5 3 4 4 2 4 4 4 4 3 4 4 4 2 4 4 4 2 4 5 5 3 4 1 1 1 1 1 1 4 1 1 4 2 1 1 1 1 1 5 5 3 4 1 1 1 1 1 1 4 1 1 5 1 1 1 1 1 1 5 5 3 6 2 2 2 1 3 2 5 3 2 6 3 2 2 1 2 2 5 5 4 5 4 3 4 4 4 4 5 4 4 4 4 3 3 1 3 4 5 5 5 6 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 6 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 0 4 0 0 0 0 0 4 0 2 0 3 3 1 0 0 0 0 3 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 1 0 1 1 0 0 0 0 0 2 1 0 0 0 0 1 1 0 1 0 1 0 0 1 0 1 0 0 0 2 0 0 0 0 0 4 4 4 4 0 4 4 4 2 4 3 4 0 4 1 0 0 2 2 0 3 3 2 3 0 3 3 2 2 3 3 2 0 2 0 0 0 1 1 0 1 1 1 1 0 1 1 1 1 2 1 1 0 1 0 0 0 2 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 2 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 2 1 3 1 3 3 3 3 3 1 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 2 3 1 3 3 3 3 3 2 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 0 3 2 0 3 3 3 3 0 3 3 3 1 3 3 3 0 3 3 3 The dimension of each pattern element is scored on a scale from 0 to 10, representing the fraction of the wing cell covered by black pattern, as described in text. Wing cells are coded as in Fig. 2A. Wing cells 2 and 4 often have an asymmetrical pattern and independent measurements were taken of the posterior (p) and anterior (a) halves (e.g. 2pd is the posterior portion of the pattern in cell 2d). 586 EVOLUTION & DEVELOPMENT Vol. 5, No. 6, November^December 2003 The dominance relationships of the effects of the mimicry gene on different portions of the pattern are illustrated in left-hand panel of Fig. 3, for all the heterozygous combinations for which we have been able to obtain clear morphological data from the literature and from unpublished breeding experiments. In most heterozygotes, one allele typically has a dominant effect on all elements of the color pattern, though there are several major exceptions to this general rule. The first is the planemoides (H pl) allele, which appears to have a mix of dominance effects on different portions of the pattern. The second is the case of the heterozygotes with unique phenotypes (the forms salaami, dorippoides, and synthetic niobe). In these forms, several pattern elements exhibit overdominance, meaning that the phenotypic value of the heterozygote is greater than that of either homozygote. Although overdominance for fitness is widespread and well understood (as heterozygote superiority), overdominance for morphological traits is exceptionally uncommon, and the developmental mechanisms by which it could arise are not obvious. The cenea (H c) allele exhibits two patterns of dominance with respect to members of the hippocoon group. It is dominant with respect to the hippocoon (H h) and natalica (H na) alleles and recessive to all the rest. This suggests that members of the hippocoon group belong to two genetic classes, perhaps reflecting different episodes in the evolution of mimicry. Dominance relationships between geographic races The relatively clean patterns of dominance obtained from crosses within a geographic race stand in contrast to the results obtained when crosses are made between geographic races. In such cases, one obtains highly variable heterozygous phenotypes that differ from the within-race crosses in that no two individuals are quite identical in their pattern and that the outlines of the patterns are often irregular and poorly defined. Clarke and Sheppard (1960b,c,e, 1963) suggested that this variable dominance was due to the fact that within a Fig. 3. Matrices of the pleiotropic effects of various alleles of the mimicry gene on different elements of the color pattern. Genotypes are grouped by allele in the left-most column (alleles are designated by the superscripts shown in Fig. 1). Many of the genotypes appear twice, grouped by each of their alleles. (Left) Dominance relationships among the alleles refer to the alleles in the left-most column: black, dominant; red, recessive; blue, codominant; green, overdominant; gray, no difference in pattern. Interracial hybrids and unique heterozygous patterns (see text) are indicated. (Right) Morphic effects of various alleles on different elements of the color pattern. Morphic effects refer to alleles in the left-most column: black, hypermorphic (large pattern dominant to small pattern); red, hypomorphic (large recessive to small); blue; codominant (intermediate between homozygotes); green, overdominant. In all cases, the overdominant pattern is smaller than that of both allelic homozygotes. Nijhout population the mimicry alleles occur in a coadapted genetic background. When this coadapted gene complex is broken up by hybridization between geographic races, the mimicry gene no longer produces a well-defined and stable phenotype. Figure 3 shows that the clean dominance relationships of pattern elements break down in heterozygotes from interracial crosses. Here we illustrate crosses using the ‘‘yellow’’ Hya allele (of the antinorii race from Ethiopia) and Hym allele (of the meriones race from Madagascar). In interracial hybrids, these alleles have different dominance relationships for different elements of the pattern: some elements are dominant, whereas others are recessive in the same heterozygote. The pleiotropic patterns of dominance are quite different for the two alleles, suggesting that these two male-like forms are genetically quite dissimilar and may therefore have independent evolutionary origins. This mosaic dominance of the pleiotropic effect of the mimicry gene may be related to the differential dominance of pleiotropic QTL effects described in mouse mandibles by Ehrlich et al. (2003). In P. dardanus, however, the mosaic dominance appears only in the aberrant genetic environment of interracial hybrids that show a breakdown of mimicry. The evolution of a coadapted gene complex that supports accurate mimicry within a geographic race appears to be associated with the loss of mosaic dominance. It is not obvious at present what is gained by having the same sign of dominance of all the pleiotropic effects of a gene, but it is a very clear evolutionary signal, so it must have had some consequences for fitness. Hyper- and hypomorphic effects In addition to the dominance effects on different portions of the pattern, each allele also has a unique effect on the dimensions of those pattern elements. An allele can have either a hypermorphic effect, if it makes a pattern element larger than the allele with which it is compared, or a hypomorphic effect, if it makes the pattern element smaller. It is most common for dominant alleles to have hypermorphic effects on the trait they control. The morphic effects of the various alleles of the mimicry gene on different portions of the pattern are illustrated in the right-hand panel of Fig. 3. The alleles clearly have a mixture of morphic effects on the different portions of the color pattern. Dominant alleles do not make all elements of the pattern larger than the recessive alleles with which they are juxtaposed. Indeed, most effects of dominant alleles are hypomorphic. The mixture of morphic effects indicates that the mimicry allele does not affect each pattern element through the same physiological mechanism. In many (but not all) cases, the morphic effects are similar in adjoining blocks of pattern elements, indicating, perhaps, that these blocks share a common genetic control. Polymorphic mimicry in P. dardanus 587 Big effects may give insight into the origin of mimicry Each allele of the mimicry gene either enlarges or reduces different portions of the color pattern, but the magnitude of this effect is not the same for all parts of the pattern. Insofar as the initial step in the evolution of mimicry is likely to have been due to a genetic effect of large magnitude, we investigated whether the relative dimensions of the phenotypic effects of modern mimicry alleles could be used to detect the footprint of the original mutation of big effect. We determined the size of the effect of an allele on a given pattern element by the difference of the score of that element in the homozygotes and all available heterozygotes for each allele. Difference scores of three or greater are given in Fig. 4. The big effects were found to be restricted to a relatively small set of locations on the wing (Fig. 4). The overall effect is to reduce the size of the marginal elements in wing cells 1, 2, 4, and 5 and the proximal element in wing cell 1 and to increase the size of the proximal element in wing cell 3. To determine whether these big effects could have produced a passable mimicry when superimposed on the ancestral pattern of P. dardanus, we need to understand what that ancestral pattern must have looked like. Most authors have assumed that the male pattern of P. dardanus represents the ancestral form of the pattern from which the female forms must have been derived (Ford 1936; Clarke and Sheppard 1960c; Turner 1963). This assumption is presumably based on the homogeneity of male forms across the species’ range and the fact that several geographic races possess male-like female forms. The pattern of male P. dardanus is, however, very different from that of other papilionids, and it seems more reasonable to seek the ancestral pattern among the species that are most closely related to P. dardanus. There is general agreement that P. dardanus is most closely related to P. phorcas and P. constantinus, although authors differ on which of these species is the sister species to P. dardanus (Vane-Wright and Smith 1991, 1992; Clarke et al. 1991; Caterino and Sperling 1999; Vane-Wright et al. 1999). Of the two, P. phorcas is polymorphic, with boldly patterned black and green males and dimorphic females (Fig. 5). One of the female morphs is male-like, and the other has a yellow and brown pattern that has a general similarity to that of related papilionids. Papilio constantinus is monomorphic, and both males and females have a pattern that resembles that of the brown female morph of P. phorcas. It seems possible, therefore, that the polymorphism of P. dardanus is not uniquely derived but is related to the pattern dimorphism of its sister species, P. phorcas. The ecological function of the polymorphic pattern of P. phorcas is unclear, because neither of the morphs appears to be a mimic. The black and green male morph is clearly the derived pattern, so it may be maintained by sexual selection, as is believed to be the case for the male pattern of P. dardanus 588 EVOLUTION & DEVELOPMENT Vol. 5, No. 6, November^December 2003 Fig. 4. Matrix of big effects of the various alleles on different elements of the color pattern. Gray, difference score 5 3; black, difference score 5 4. Diagram of forewing illustrates the locations and polarity of the big effects of the mimicry gene on the elements of the color pattern. (Ford 1953; Sheppard 1959). The polymorphic female form of P. phorcas is believed to have originated as a male-mimicking ‘‘transvestitism’’ from a primitively sexually dimorphic color pattern (Vane-Wright 1976; Clarke et al. 1985). If P. phorcas and P. dardanus share a recent common ancestor, then the evolution of a female polymorphic pattern in P. dardanus may have been facilitated by a preexisting polymorphism resembling that of P. phorcas. If this is the case, then the male pattern of P. dardanus may have been derived from the male pattern of P. phorcas and the female polymorphism of P. dardanus from the female dimorphism of P. phorcas. If we take the patterns of P. phorcas to represent the primitive extreme, then we may have a more reasonable basis from which to deduce the evolution of the P. dardanus patterns than if we assume the male P. dardanus pattern to be primitive. This possibility was explored by examining the big effects of the various alleles of the mimicry gene. Figure 6 illustrates the pattern modifications obtained when the pattern changes of big effect (from Fig. 4) are applied to the yellow female pattern of P. phorcas. The altered pattern bears a passable resemblance to a hippocoon-like target phenotype. So it seems possible that this small suite of changes could have given rise to the hippocoon class of patterns. Fig. 5. Patterns of Papilio phorcas. All males are of the black and green phenotype (A), whereas females are dimorphic and can be either black and green (A) or yellow and brown (B). Nijhout The transition from a phorcas-like pattern to a cenea-like pattern is more difficult. Using only the pattern elements that express the big effect, it would require an enlargement of 3p, 1p, 1d, and 4d and no change in 2d and 4d. This suite of changes is inconsistent with the observed correlation of changes within this set of pattern elements (3p becoming larger and all others becoming smaller, or vice versa with the alternative allele). A cenea-like pattern can, however, be achieved from a hippocoon-like pattern by increasing 1p, 1d, 2d, 4, and 5d and keeping 3p unchanged (Fig. 6). This change involves a simple partial reversal of the changes that led from phorcas-like to hippocoon-like, requiring only that the magnitude of the reversal is greater in cells 1 and 5 so that these cells become completely filled with the black pattern. To achieve the resemblance to a model, the altered black portion of the pattern must be accompanied by a change in background pigmentation. A suppression of background pigmentation to achieve a white field is sufficient. In both the prehippocoon and the pre-cenea patterns, relatively minor additional modifications need to be applied to achieve a remarkably close resemblance to a target model phenotype (Fig. 6). Whether these transformations resemble the actual effect of early mutations and whether they provide sufficient resemblance to distasteful species to improve the fitness of its bearer is, obviously, a matter of conjecture. It seems, Polymorphic mimicry in P. dardanus 589 nevertheless, that it is possible to evolve at least two of the major mimicry patterns in relatively few steps. The third main mimicry pattern is that of the planemoides form (Hpl). This could be derived most simply from the P. phorcas pattern by narrowing the distal black portion of the pattern along the entire length of the wing (Fig. 7) and by altering the background pigmentation to a reddish brown. The changes required to produce the planemoides pattern are relatively simple and are not related to the previous two. The scenario outlined above thus suggests that a hippocoon-like form arose first and that this form gave rise to the cenea-like form and that the planemoides-like form arose independently. So the first step in the evolution of mimicry could involve only a single locus. The hippocoon-like pattern is likely to have evolved twice (see Discussion), and the planemoides and cenea-like patterns may have originated once each. The further refinement of any one of the female forms could be accomplished by a relatively small number of additional genetic changes. DISCUSSION The mimetic wing patterns of P. dardanus have different patterns of variation and internal correlation than nonmi- Fig. 6. Imposing the big effects of the mimicry gene (see Fig. 4) on the presumptive ancestral Papilio phorcas-like pattern. A reasonably close resemblance to a hippocoon pattern is obtained in a single step. A single-step transformation to a cenea-like pattern requires pattern changes that are not supported by the big effects of the mimicry gene. Transformation of a hippocoon-like pattern to a cenea-like pattern, however, requires only reversals of some of the big effects. 590 EVOLUTION & DEVELOPMENT Vol. 5, No. 6, November^December 2003 Fig. 7. Derivation of the planemoides from Papilio phorcas is accomplished by simple diminution of the proximal and distal black areas (and a change in background color). metic ones. Mimetic patterns exhibit a higher degree of individual variability than nonmimetic patterns. If the degree of variability is a function of the mutation-selection balance, then these findings suggest that nonmimetic patterns either are under stronger selection or experience less mutational variance than mimetic patterns. It is possible that the greater individual variability of mimetic pattern is due to the fact that these genotypes are more protected against predators and thus less subject to selection and that there is, for some reason, stronger normalizing selection on nonmimetic and male patterns than on mimetic patterns. Alternatively, it is possible that mimetic patterns require the activity of many more genes than do nonmimetic ones and would therefore be more sensitive to standing genetic variation and newly introduced mutational variance. Mimetic wing patterns exhibit a lower degree of correlated variation among the various elements of the pattern than do nonmimetic wing patterns. If the pattern of genetic correlations resembles the pattern of phenotypic correlations, as is the case in other butterflies (Paulsen 1994, 1996), then these findings suggest few if any genetic correlations among the elements of the color pattern of the mimicking forms. The absence of correlation in the mimicking patterns may be due to the accumulation of the many genes with small effects that have been hypothesized to be responsible for refining the details of the mimicry (the second stage in the evolution of mimicry). If this is the correct interpretation, then it implies that these modifier genes affect the morphology of the color pattern on a spatial scale that is the same size or smaller than the scale at which these measurements were made. The various alleles of the mimicry gene affect the dimension of the pattern elements to different degrees. Here the effect is rather complex in that a given allele may increase the size of some pattern elements while decreasing the size of others. This is unlike the case in Heliconius, where dominant alleles typically increase the size of a pattern element (Nijhout et al. 1990), although a recent report shows substantial evolution of dominance in Heliconius (Naisbit et al. 2003). In P. dardanus the effect on the size of the pattern is independent of the dominance effect of the allele. Thus, a dominant effect can either increase or decrease the size of an element, with the latter being somewhat more common than the former. This mix of hyper- and hypomorphic pleiotropic effects of a given allele is difficult to explain on the basis of a common mechanism of action. This effect is consistent with both the supergene and the regulatory gene hypotheses. In either case, it can be best explained if variation in each pattern element is affected by at least some unique genes or by unique combinations of genes. Whether these genes are linked in a supergene, as in the case of Papilio memnon (Clarke et al. 1968; Clarke and Sheppard 1971), or whether they are dispersed across the genome and controlled by a polymorphic regulatory gene remains an open question. The alleles of the mimicry locus have a complex set of pleiotropic and dominance effects on various features of the forewing pattern. Each allele affects a different combination of pattern elements, and the size of each of the pattern elements affected by a given allele is altered to a different degree. Within a geographic race, the different alleles show a clear and unambiguous pattern of dominances, in which the degree of dominance is the same for all elements affected by a given allele. This stands in contrast to the mosaic pattern of dominance observed in interracial crosses, where a given allele may have a dominant effect on some elements and a recessive effect on others. The similarity of dominance effects within a race may be a manifestation of the so-called coadaptation of genes within a population. The mosaic of dominances in interracial crosses would then be due to a breakdown of this coadaptation when an allele is placed in a genetic background that is different from the one in which it evolved. If this is the correct interpretation, it suggests that the similarity of dominance effects within a population is an evolved property. It is possible that when a mimicry allele first originated, it had a mosaic of dominant and recessive pleiotropic effects on Nijhout different portions of the pattern, just as modern alleles have a mosaic of morphic effects on different parts of the pattern. Perhaps initially all dominant effects were associated with increases in the size of pattern elements and all recessive effects with decreases in size. A favorable combination of hyper- and hypomorphic pleiotropic effects could then be stabilized by the evolution of a genetic background that made all those effects dominant (e.g., Gilchrist and Nijhout 2001). This process would be repeated with the origin of each new mimetic locus, resulting in the order of dominance observed. This scenario for the evolution of dominance relations is different from the traditional Haldane’s sieve, which suggests that a new favorable allele can be established most easily if it is dominant than if it is recessive so that new phenotypes tend to be dominant over preexisting phenotypes (e.g., Clarke et al. 1985). Under both scenarios, the order of dominance is likely to reflect the order of origin of the phenotypes, but under Haldane’s sieve this would be a purely statistical effect, whereas under the alternative scenario suggested above, dominance is an evolved adaptation. The hippocoon pattern group is the largest and most diverse of the three pattern groups in P. dardanus (Fig. 1). The fact that the cenea allele (H c) is dominant to some alleles in the hippocoon group (H h and H na) and recessive to others suggests that the hippocoon-like phenotype may have evolved independently more than once. This interpretation is supported by the finding on the dimensions of the phenotypic effects of the various alleles on the pattern. Application of the big phenotypic effects to the presumptive primitive pattern of the P. dardanus ancestor suggests that a hippocoon-like mimic can be achieved in one step but that a cenea-like mimic most likely requires at least two evolutionary steps and is probably derived from a hippocoon-like pattern rather than independently from the ancestral pattern. It is possible, then, that the cenea allele (H c) evolved from the group that led to the hippocoon (H h) and natalica (H na) alleles, to which it is dominant. The other alleles in the hippocoon group may have been derived independently and at a later time, and this may explain why they are dominant to the cenea allele. This scenario is in accordance with the suggestion of Turner (1963) that the hippocoon form probably arose before any of the other mimetic forms. Clarke et al. (1985, 1991) argued that in Papilio phorcas, the male-like pattern of the female is the derived form. This suggests that the species may initially have been sexually dimorphic (with brown/yellow females and black/green males) and that a so-called transvestite evolutionary step (Vane-Wright 1976; Clarke et al. 1985) produced male-like females and was the origin of the female color dimorphism. Acknowledgments This work was supported by a grant from the Human Frontiers Science Program. I thank Alfried Vogler, Ali Cieslak, Toshio Polymorphic mimicry in P. dardanus 591 Sekimura, Dick Vane-Wright, Susan Paulsen, and, in particular, Steve Collins for many helpful and insightful discussions and Trey Greer for developing DIGIT, the digitization software used in this study. REFERENCES Bernardi, G., Pierre, J., and Nguyen, T. H. 1985. Le polymorphisme et le mimétisme de Papilio dardanus Brown. Bul. Soc. Entomo. France 90: 106–155. Carpenter, G. D. H., and Ford, E. B. 1933. Mimicry. Methuen, London. Caterino, M. S., and Sperling, F. A. H. 1999. Papilio phylogeny based on mitochondrial cytochrome oxidase I and II genes. Mol. Phylogene. Evol. 11: 122–137. Charlesworth, B. 1994. The genetics of adaptation: lessons from mimicry. Am. Nat. 144: 839–847. Charlesworth, D., and Charlesworth, B. 1975a. Theoretical genetics of Batesian mimicry. I. Single-locus models. J. Theoret. Biol. 55: 283–303. Charlesworth, D., and Charlesworth, B. 1975b. Theoretical genetics of Batesian mimicry. II. Evolution of supergenes. J. Theoret. Biol. 55: 305– 324. Charlesworth, D., and Charlesworth, B. 1975c. Theoretical genetics of Batesian mimicry. III. Evolution of dominance. J. Theoret. Biol. 55: 325– 337. Clarke, C., Clarke, F. M. M., Collins, S. C., Gill, A. C. L., and Turner, J. R. G. 1985. Male-like females, mimicry and transvestitism in butterflies (Lepidoptera: Papilionidae). Syst. Entomol. 10: 257–283. Clarke, C. A., Gordon, I. J., Smith, C. R., and Vane-Wright, R. I. 1991. Phylogenetic relationships of three African swallowtail butterflies, Papilio dardanus, P. phorcas and P. constantinus: new data from hybrids (Lepidoptera: Papilionidae). Syst. Entomol. 16: 257–273. Clarke, C. A., and Sheppard, P. M. 1959. The genetics of Papilio dardanus Brown. I. Race cenea from South Africa. Genetics 44: 1347–1358. Clarke, C. A., and Sheppard, P. M. 1960a. The genetics of Papilio dardanus Brown. II. Races dardanus, polytrophus, meseres, and tibullus. Genetics 45: 439–457. Clarke, C. A., and Sheppard, P. M. 1960b. The genetics of Papilio dardanus Brown. III. Race antinorii from Abyssinia and race meriones from Madagascar. Genetics 45: 683–698. Clarke, C. A., and Sheppard, P. M. 1960c. The evolution of mimicry in the butterfly Papilio dardanus. Heredity 14: 163–173. Clarke, C. A., and Sheppard, P. M. 1960d. Super-genes and mimicry. Heredity 14: 175–185. Clarke, C. A., and Sheppard, P. M. 1960e. The evolution of dominance under disruptive selection. Heredity 14: 23–87. Clarke, C. A., and Sheppard, P. M. 1962. The genetics of Papilio dardanus Brown. IV. Data on race ochracea, race flavicornis, and further information on races polytrophus and dardanus. Genetics 47: 909–920. Clarke, C. A., and Sheppard, P. M. 1963. Interactions between major genes and polygenes in the determination of the mimetic patterns of Papilio dardanus. Evolution 17: 404–413. Clarke, C. A., and Sheppard, P. M. 1971. Further studies on the genetics of the mimetic butterfly Papilio memnon. L. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 263: 35–70. Clarke, C. A., Sheppard, P. M., and Thornton, I. W. B. 1968. The genetics of the mimetic butterfly Papilio memnon. L. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 254: 37–89. Ehrlich, T. H., Vaughn, T. T., Koreishi, S. F., Linsey, R. B., Pletscher, L. S., and Cheverud, J. M. 2003. Pleiotropic effects on mandibular morphology. I. Developmental morphological integration and differential dominance. Mol. Dev. Evol. 296B: 58–79. Fisher, R. A. 1930. The Genetical Theory of Natural Selection. Oxford University Press, Oxford. Ford, E. B. 1936. The genetics of Papilio dardanus Brown (Lep.). Trans. Entomol. Soc. Lond. 85: 435–466. Ford, E. B. 1953. The genetics of polymorphism in the Lepidoptera. Adv. Genet. 5: 43–87. Gilchrist, M. A., and Nijhout, H. F. 2001. Nonlinear developmental processes as sources of dominance. Genetics 159: 423–432. 592 EVOLUTION & DEVELOPMENT Vol. 5, No. 6, November^December 2003 Haldane, J. B. S. 1924. A mathematical theory of natural and artificial selection. Part I. Trans. Cambr. Phil. Soc. 23: 19–41. Koch, P. B., and Nijhout, H. F. 2002. The role of wing veins in colour pattern development in the butterfly Papilio xuthus (Lepidoptera: Papilionidae). Eur. J. Entomol. 99: 67–72. Naisbit, R. E., Jiggins, C. D., and Mallett, J. 2003. Mimicry: developmental genes that contribute to speciation. Evol. Dev. 5: 269–280. Nijhout, H. F. 1985. Independent development of homologous pattern elements in the wing patterns of butterflies. Dev. Biol. 108: 146–151. Nijhout, H. F., Wray, G. A., and Gilbert, L. E. 1990. An analysis of the phenotypic effects of certain color pattern genes in Heliconius (Lepidoptera: Nymphalidae). J. Linn. Soc. 40: 357–372. Nijhout, H. F. 1991. The Development and Evolution of Butterfly Wing Patterns. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, DC. Nijhout, H. F. 1994. Developmental perspectives on the evolution of butterfly mimicry. BioScience 44: 148–157. Paulsen, S. M. 1994. Quantitative genetics of butterfly wing patterns. Dev. Genet. 15: 79–91. Paulsen, S. M. 1996. Quantitative genetics of the wing color pattern in the buckeye butterfly (Precis coenia and Precis evarete): evidence against the constancy of G. Evolution 50: 1585–1597. Paulsen, S. M., and Nijhout, H. F. 1993. Phenotypic correlation structure among elements of the color pattern in Precis coenia (Lepidoptera: Nymphalidae). Evolution 47: 593–618. Poulton, E. B. 1924. Papilio dardanus, the most wonderful butterfly in the world. J. East Afr. Uganda Nat. Hist. Soc. 209: 4–22. Sheppard, P. M. 1959. The evolution of mimicry; a problem in ecology and genetics. Cold Spring Harbor Symp. Quant. Biol. 24: 131–140. Sokal, R. R., and Rohlf, F. J. 1981. Biometry. Freeman, San Francisco, CA. Turner, J. R. G. 1963. Geographical variation and evolution in the males of the butterfly Papilio dardanus Brown (Lepidoptera: Papilionidae). Trans. R. Entomol. Soc. Lond. 115: 239–259. Turner, J. R. G. 1977. Butterfly mimicry: the genetical evolution of an adaptation. Evol. Biol. 10: 163–206. Vane-Wright, R. I. 1976. An alternative hypothesis on the evolution of Papilio dardanus Brown. Proc. R. Entomol. Soc. Lond. 41: 1–7. Vane-Wright, R. I., Raheem, D. C., Cieslak, A., and Vogler, A. P. 1999. Evolution of the mimetic African swallowtail butterfly Papilio dardanus: molecular data confirm relationships with P. phorcas and P. constantinus. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 66: 215–229. Vane-Wright, R. I., and Smith, C. R. 1991. Phylogenetic relationships of three African swallowtail butterflies, Papilio dardanus, P. phorcas and P. constantinus: cladistic analysis (Lepidoptera: Papilionidae). Syst. Entomol. 16: 275–291. Vane-Wright, R. I., and Smith, C. R. 1992. Occurrence and significance of natural hybrids between Papilio dardanus and P. phorcas (Lepidoptera: Papilionidae). Syst. Entomol. 17: 269–272.